Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2011 Dec 19;109(3):770–774. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112173109

Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries

Vegard Skirbekk a,b,1, Elke Loichinger a,b,c, Daniela Weber a,b
PMCID: PMC3271876  PMID: 22184241

Abstract

Comparing the burden of aging across countries hinges on the availability of valid and comparable indicators. The Old Age Dependency Ratio allows only a limited assessment of the challenges of aging, because it does not include information on any individual characteristics except age itself. Existing alternative indicators based on health or economic activity suffer from measurement and comparability problems. We propose an indicator based on age variation in cognitive functioning. We use newly released data from standardized tests of seniors' cognitive abilities for countries from different world regions. In the wake of long-term advances in countries’ industrial composition, and technological advances, the ability to handle new job procedures is now of high and growing importance, which increases the importance of cognition for work performance over time. In several countries with older populations, we find better cognitive performance on the part of populations aged 50+ than in countries with chronologically younger populations. This variation in cognitive functioning levels may be explained by the fact that seniors in some regions of the world experienced better conditions during childhood and adult life, including nutrition, duration and quality of schooling, lower exposure to disease, and physical and social activity patterns. Because of the slow process of cohort replacement, those countries whose seniors already have higher cognitive levels today are likely to continue to be at an advantage for several decades to come.


The world population is growing older (1, 2). Comparisons of the burden of aging across countries hinge on the availability of valid and comparable indicators. Demographic indicators like the old-age dependency ratio and median age are widely used to characterize and rank how old countries are. Based on these measures, the populations of Germany, the United States, and Japan are much older than those of India, China, or Mexico. However, the fact that these indicators are exclusively based on chronological age distributions limits their usefulness in terms of drawing conclusions about the consequences of and possible responses to population aging. Alternative approaches to comparing the extent of aging across countries are based on subjective health, life expectancy, and economic activity (35). These studies show that different countries can be considered to be the oldest in the world depending on how aging indicators are defined. Such measures, however, can be influenced by culture- and nation-specific interpretations of health level and disability and by business cycle fluctuations.

In contrast to existing studies, we here compare the extent of aging across nations according to age variation in cognitive abilities. Recently released surveys allow us to compare country-level variation in seniors’ cognitive functioning across populations with younger and older age distributions.

Studies have found that cognitive ability levels predict individual productivity better than any other observable individual characteristics and that they are increasingly relevant for labor market performance (610). This finding applies to a variety of countries and settings, including poorer countries and rural settings (11, 12). In the wake of long-term advances in countries’ industrial composition and technological advances, the ability to handle new job procedures is now of high and growing importance, which increases the importance of cognition over time (7, 13).

The growing importance of seniors for the labor market and the fact that certain cognitive abilities decline considerably during late adult ages are the reasons why we focus our study on the population that is 50 y and older (1418). The length of time for which individuals can retain high cognitive performance will influence the age until which they can potentially stay active in the labor market. We use standardized questions based on representative surveys from different world regions. These international comparable surveys of seniors include English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA), Health and Retirement Study (HRS), World Health Organization (WHO) Study on global AGEing and adult health (SAGE), and Survey of Health, Aging and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), which together allow us to cover almost one-half (45.5%) of the world population (see SI Materials and Methods for more details). These surveys include a measure of cognitive ability that is operationalized comparably across all surveys, namely, immediate recall of a certain number of given words, which is a measure of short-term memory (19). Other variables that measure cognitive abilities, like delayed word recall or fluency, are either not included in every survey or not measured in a comparable way. Analysis for countries where these measures can be compared corroborates the results we get for immediate word recall (see Figs. S1 and S2, Tables S1 and S2, and SI Results for more details). This study compares seniors’ age variation in cognitive abilities across countries from both developed and emerging economies by using results from standardized testing procedures. The inclusion of seniors from world regions with chronologically younger populations has become possible only recently with the release of SAGE.

Immediate recall has been shown to be important for a variety of outcomes, ranging from financial decisionmaking to the risk of developing dementia (2023). Moreover, technological advances and changes in working procedures imply that the importance of the ability to learn and remember is increasing (24). Employers are particularly concerned that their employees are able to learn new work procedures and process new information (25), which also suggests that employers view the ability to immediately recall information as advantageous to labor market performance.

Results

Fig. 1 shows the age variation in immediate recall across countries and country regions. It depicts the proportion of words (out of 10 read out nouns) which the respondents are able to recall within 1 min (18 countries) and 2 min (UK and the US; 95% of the US participants completed the task within 1 min. See SI Materials and Methods for more details). In Table 1 summary statistics for all used variables of our dataset are provided.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Mean age-group–specific immediate recall scores (values between 0 and 1, where a score of 0.4 means being able to recall 40% of the given words). Curves are smoothed by using spline interpolations. Logistic regression to test for significant age-related decline, significance levels P < 0.001. Analysis of variance to test for differences between countries, significance levels P < 0.01.

Table 1.

Summary statistics of the survey subsets

HRS
SAGE
SHARE (Northern Europe)
SHARE (Continental Europe)
SHARE (Southern Europe)
Measurements United States China India Mexico Denmark, England, Ireland, Sweden Austria, Belgium, Czech Republic, France, Germany, Netherlands, Poland, Switzerland Greece, Italy, Spain
Year 2006/07 2007–2009 2007–2009 2007–2009 2006/07 2006/07 2006/07
Sample size 17,995 13,367 7,150 2,306 4,736 14,948 6,153
Females, % 58.3 53.1 49.4 60.5 53.4 55.2 54.6
Birth cohorts 1901–1956 1910–1959 1909–1957 1904–1959 1907–1957 1903–1957 1905–1957
None of the  words recalled, % 0.7 1.3 1.0 2.8 0.4 1.9 2.9
All words  immediately  recalled, % 0.7 0.4 0 0 0.9 0.4 0.5

The findings highlight a statistically significant age-related decline in all countries within the 50–85 age interval. Immediate recall age trajectories for northern and continental European countries and the United States run parallel to those of southern Europe, Mexico, and China, whereas the decline in India proceeds at a much slower pace. Our findings show statistically significant differences in the levels of cognitive performance between countries. Thus, seniors in the United States and northern and continental European countries have the highest immediate recall ability, whereas their peers in China, India, Mexico, and southern Europe perform worse.

To take the observed differences in cognitive functioning of seniors into account when comparing aging across countries, we propose an additional indicator that focuses on cognition and demographic change: cognition-adjusted dependency ratio (CADR). For this measure, the denominator is composed of everyone who is 15 to 49 y old and the 50+ population (no upper age bound) with good cognitive functioning (approximated by those who are able to recall at least one-half of the words in the test). The numerator consists of the number of persons aged 50+ who recall fewer than one-half the words in the test.

Our aging indicator is presented and compared with the usual old-age dependency ratio (OADR), which is defined as the ratio of the number of persons aged 65+ to the number of persons between the ages of 15 and 64, in Table 2. The rank ordering of the different countries and regions changes as we apply the alternative aging indicator: Mexico, India, and China do less well compared with the rank order based on the OADR, whereas the United States and continental and northern Europe do better.

Table 2.

Different measures for the burden of aging

Rank (ratio)
Country/group CADR OADR
United States of America 1 (0.10) 4 (0.19)
Northern Europe (Denmark, England, Ireland, Sweden) 2 (0.12) 5 (0.24)
India 3 (0.14) 1 (0.07)
Mexico 3 (0.14) 2 (0.09)
China 5 (0.15) 3 (0.12)
Continental Europe (Austria, Belgium, Czech Republic, France, Germany, Netherlands, Poland, Switzerland) 6 (0.18) 6 (0.25)
Southern Europe (Greece, Italy, Spain) 7 (0.32) 7 (0.27)

Source: Population data for the year 2005 from UN (2009) and for England for the year 2005 from the Office for National Statistics (2010); survey data from HRS, SAGE, and SHARE.

This result implies that although continental European countries have a larger population share above the age of 65 than China, their lower CADR would suggest that these countries are effectively “younger.” That is, they have a lower share of seniors with poor cognitive performance.

Discussion

One potential source of explanation for the international variation in seniors’ cognitive functioning levels are life-course differences among the cohorts we consider. Present-day seniors in different countries have varying experiences with respect to a large number of influences, including their average length and quality of schooling, nutrition (prenatal, early life, and adult), exposure to famines, disease, and pollutants, physical and social activity patterns, and whether working conditions have been stimulating or detrimental to cognitive performance (2637).

An increase in cognitive performance among successive cohorts describes a phenomenon that has been observed in many countries for an extended period (3840). In the United States, such evidence began with comparisons of test performances of conscripts from World War I and World War II, with continued cognitive improvements having been documented for most of the 20th century. Successive cohorts in western countries have been shown to generally perform better at cognitive tests for long periods, whereas in other countries, cohort improvements have only been documented for recent cohorts. The likely later onset of cognitive improvement would follow the observed delayed onset of drivers of cognitive improvements, including mortality decline, universal education, improved nutrition, and better economic conditions (4044).

In some countries, particularly in India, the cohorts presently 50 y and older have grown up during periods of widespread poverty and deprivation and where mortality levels were high, which could lower the overall levels of cognition among the cohorts presently old (41, 45). At the same time, large socio-economic mortality differentials within a population could imply that the population is positively selected at a more advanced age. Given that those with higher cognitive performance live longer, this could entail a “flatter” age-cognition curve (46, 47), such as for India (4850).

A few studies suggest that there has been a leveling off and reversal in cognitive increases among recent generations of younger men in some countries; however, it will take several decades until these cohorts attain senior age (51, 52).

Education has been identified as significantly raising levels of cognitive functioning, including memory (16, 53, 54). The countries in our study with better cognitive functioning levels are also the countries with higher educational attainment. Northern Europeans and Americans have globally the highest educational attainment among their 50+ population, whereas education levels are much lower in the Chinese and Mexican senior populations. Epidemiological research has identified low educational attainment as an important risk factor for low cognitive functioning and Alzheimer's disease (55, 56). Education is related to better cognitive performance in late life, and researchers relate the effect to occupational complexity and the acquisition of a lifelong ability to sustain attention and conceptualize problems. Although it is uncertain whether education affects the rate of decline (57), it can affect the cognitive level for all age groups (53, 54). Being mentally active, through courses and cognitive training, has been shown to improve cognitive functioning among older people (33, 5860). Education may increase the synaptic density in the neocortical association cortex and, therefore, delay cognitive decline and dementia by several years (61). At the same time, lower childhood intelligence in early life appears to be a reliable proxy for lower cognitive ability later in life (6264).

Later born cohorts with higher cognitive functioning will eventually replace older cohorts with poorer cognitive performance. If this trend continues, cognitive performance is likely to improve along cohort lines at senior adult ages (65). In Mexico, where time series data allow comparison of successive cohorts, we find that individuals of the 1941–45 cohorts at age 60 were able to remember on average 4.2 words, whereas those born 1946–50 were able to recall 5.1 words at the same age. The same holds for England's 60-y-olds: Here, we find an increase from 6.0 to 6.3 recalled words for the 1941–45 relative to the 1946–50 cohorts. Overall, these developments suggest that there will be a universal increase in cognitive functioning among seniors in the coming decades. However, as cohort replacement is a slow process, the countries whose seniors have higher cognitive levels today are likely to continue to have an advantage for several decades ahead.

Age-related norms (such as at which age a person is regarded as being “old”) can vary by countries and cultures (66, 67). They may be influenced by age-related laws and regulations, including official retirement ages, which vary significantly between countries (68). Retiring at older ages can imply that one stays mentally active until higher ages, which could improve the level of cognitive functioning until higher ages (69).

Conclusion

The current study's shift in focus from chronological age distributions to actual cognitive functioning at older ages leads to a relevant additional possibility to compare aging across countries. This shift in perspective is crucial because it changes focus from predictable changes in the demographic age structure toward the importance of improving and maintaining cognitive abilities. Because the adjustment for aging requires long-term investments and changes in training policies and lifestyles, it is essential to implement policies and efforts that prepare societies for an older population by maintaining cognitive abilities throughout the life cycle (70, 71).

The degree to which demographic aging translates into societal challenges depends to a considerable extent on the age at which mental functioning becomes significantly impaired. Technological improvements increasingly allow seniors to participate longer in the working life (72, 73). Normal aging, however, also tends to involve a decline in certain cognitive abilities, where technological innovations are less likely to be able to compensate to a significant extent for cognitive decline. At the same time, the need for cognitive fitness seems to continue to increase. Nations that are truly challenged by aging may be those where the cognitive performance among their seniors is poor; not those who have chronologically older age structures.

Materials and Methods

A growing number of surveys are focusing on the elderly (for an overview of selected cross-national and single-country databases, see ref. 1). However, the number of surveys that contain the information we need for our analysis is limited (Table 3). The main reasons for excluding data sources are (i) The survey only includes people above 60 or 65, which is higher than our lower age bound of 50 y and (ii) the measure for cognitive ability is not included in the survey or not comparable to our measure of word recall.

Table 3.

Overview of all used datasets

Dataset Country/region Year Sample size
ELSA (English Longitudinal Study of Aging) England 2006/07 9,771
HRS (Health and Retirement Study) United States 2006/07 18,469
MHAS (Mexican Health and Aging Study) Mexico 2003 13,704
SAGE (WHO Study on global Aging and adult health) China, India, Mexico 2007/09 32,696
SHARE (Survey of Health, Aging and Retirement in Europe) Europe 2006/07 26,515

Data Sources and Variables.

ELSA, representative for the population aged 50+ of England, consists of four waves (2002–2009) (74). HRS is representative for the 50+ population of the United States. It started 1992 and was conducted every year until 1996. Thereafter it was done only every other year. So far, 11 waves are available. For our purpose we took the RAND HRS dataset, a user-friendly subset of HRS (75). SHARE is a European survey that is representative of the participating countries’ population aged 50+. The first survey was conducted in 11 countries in 2004/2005. Three more countries were added for the second wave in 2006/07. We divided the individual country files of SHARE into three regional datasets: continental (Austria, Belgium, the Czech Republic, France, Germany, the Netherlands, Poland, and Switzerland), southern (Greece, Italy, and Spain) and northern Europe (Denmark, Sweden, and Ireland) (76). Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) consists of two waves. The baseline survey was conducted in 2001, the follow-up in 2003. The survey population is representative of Mexicans aged 50+ (77). SAGE was initiated by WHO to collect longitudinal information on health and well-being of adults (18+ with an emphasis on 50+). We use this data for China, India, and Mexico (78).

ELSA is part of the Northern European country group. All “don't know” and “refused” answers are coded as missing in all surveys. For cross-country comparison we use the 2006/07 waves to compare cognitive performance by age and country/region for a similar period. MHAS was only used for obtaining cohort differences in cognitive functioning. All provided results are gained by including the provided cross-sectional individual sample weights or individual sample weights for longitudinal investigations.

CADR.

As described above, we introduce CADR, which is formally defined by the following equation.

graphic file with name pnas.1112173109uneq1.jpg

where mx represents the memory score of person x, agex represents the age of person x, and P is the population.

Trends in Cognitive Abilities.

We use all four waves of ELSA and calculate the mean immediate recall score at age 60 for the cohorts born between 1941 and 1945 and the cohorts born between 1946 and 1950. MHAS data for 2001 and 2003 are combined with SAGE data for 2007/09, because both surveys are representative for the 50+ population in Mexico. The cohorts born between 1941 and 1945 and the cohorts born between 1946 and 1950 are analyzed in an analogous manner to ELSA.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the support of Victoria Schreitter and Gwen Fisher. This paper uses data from the WHO Study on global AGEing and adult health (SAGE) and SHARE release 2.3.0, as of November 13, 2009. SHARE data collection in 2004–2007 was primarily funded by the European Commission through its fifth and sixth framework programs by Project Numbers QLK6-CT-2001-00360; RII-CT-2006-062193; and CIT5-CT-2005-028857). Additional funding provided by US National Institute on Aging Grants U01 AG09740-13S2, P01 AG005842, P01 AG08291, P30 AG12815, Y1-AG-4553-01, OGHA 04-064, and R21 AG025169 and by various national sources is gratefully acknowledged (see http://www.share-project.org for a full list of funding institutions). The Health and Retirement Study is sponsored by National Institute on Aging Grant NIA U01AG009740 and is conducted by the University of Michigan. The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing was developed by a team of researchers based at University College London, the Institute of Fiscal Studies, and the National Centre for Social Research. The funding is provided by the National Institute on Aging in the United States (Grants 2RO1AG7644-01A1 and 2RO1AG017644) and a consortium of UK government departments coordinated by the Office for National Statistics. The developers and funders of ELSA and the Archive do not bear any responsibility for the analyses or interpretations presented here. We acknowledge support by a Starting Grant of the European Research Council, Grant Agreement 241003-COHORT.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1112173109/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.United Nations . World Population Ageing 2009. New York: United Nations; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kapteyn A. What can we learn from (and about) global aging? Demography. 2010;47(Suppl):S191–S209. doi: 10.1353/dem.2010.0006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sanderson WC, Scherbov S. Average remaining lifetimes can increase as human populations age. Nature. 2005;435:811–813. doi: 10.1038/nature03593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vaupel JW, Loichinger E. Redistributing work in aging Europe. Science. 2006;312:1911–1913. doi: 10.1126/science.1127487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sanderson WC, Scherbov S. Demography. Remeasuring aging. Science. 2010;329:1287–1288. doi: 10.1126/science.1193647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schmidt FL, Hunter J. General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86:162–173. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Spitz-Oener A. Technical change, job tasks, and rising educational demands: Looking outside the wage structure. J Labor Econ. 2006;24:235–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gatewood RD. Human Resource Selection. 5th Ed. Thomson Learning,Mason, OH; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Scott JC, Reynolds DH. The Handbook of Workplace Assessment: Selecting and Developing Organizational Talent. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Neisser U. Rising scores on intelligence tests. Am Sci. 1997;85:440–447. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Behrman JR, Ross D, Sabot R. Improving quality versus increasing the quantity of schooling: Estimates of rates of return from rural Pakistan. J Dev Econ. 2008;85:94–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tansel A. Wage employment, earnings and returns to schooling for men and women in Turkey. Econ Educ Rev. 1994;13:305–320. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bekman E, Bound J, Machin S. Implications of skill-biased technological change: International evidence. Q J Econ. 1998;113:1245–1279. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Prull MW, Gabrieli JDE, Bunge SA. Age-related changes in memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In: Craik FIM, Salthouse TA, editors. Handbook of Aging and Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ; 2000. pp. 91–153. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Souchay C, Isingrini M, Espagnet L. Aging, episodic memory feeling-of-knowing, and frontal functioning. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:299–309. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Park DC, et al. Mediators of long-term memory performance across the life span. Psychol Aging. 1996;11:621–637. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Maitland SB, Intrieri RC, Schaie KW, Willis SL. Gender differences and changes in cognitive abilities across the adult life span. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2000;7:32–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Anderson ND, Craik FIM. Memory in the aging brain. In: Tulving E, Craik FIM, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press; 2000. pp. 411–425. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Old SR, Naveh-Benjamin M. Age-related changes in memory - Experimental approaches. In: Hofer SM, Alwin DF, editors. Handbook of Cognitive Aging - Interdisciplinary Perspectives. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brækhus A, Øksengård AR, Engedal K, Laake K. Subjective worsening of memory predicts dementia after three years. Nor Epidemiol. 1998;8:189–194. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fein G, McGillivray S, Finn P. Older adults make less advantageous decisions than younger adults: Cognitive and psychological correlates. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13:480–489. doi: 10.1017/S135561770707052X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Panza F, et al. Current epidemiology of mild cognitive impairment and other predementia syndromes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:633–644. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.8.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.O'Brien JT, et al. Do subjective memory complaints precede dementia? A three-year follow-up of patients with supposed ‘benign senescent forgetfulness’. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1992;7:481–486. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Machin S, Van Reenen J. Technology and changes in skill structure: Evidence from seven OECD countries. Q J Econ. 1998;113:1215–1244. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Munnell A, Sass SA, Soto M. Employer Attitudes towards Older Workers: Survey Results. Center for Retirement Res, Chestnut Hill, MA; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Erickson KI, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Samir, KC et al. Projection of populations by level of educational attainment, age, and sex for 120 countries for 2005-2050. Demogr Res. 2010;22:383–472. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Robertson A, et al., editors. WHO Reg Publ Eur Ser. 2004. Food and health in Europe: A new basis for action. (WHO Regional Office Europe, Copenhagen), No. 96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Amarantos E, Martinez A, Dwyer J. Nutrition and quality of life in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56:54–64. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.suppl_2.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Acemoglu D. Patterns of skill premia. Rev Econ Stud. 2003;70:199–230. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Black RE, et al. Maternal and Child Undernutrition Study Group Maternal and child undernutrition: Global and regional exposures and health consequences. Lancet. 2008;371:243–260. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61690-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Meijer WA, van Boxtel MPJ, Van Gerven PWM, van Hooren SAH, Jolles J. Interaction effects of education and health status on cognitive change: A 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13:521–529. doi: 10.1080/13607860902860821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ball K, et al. Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study Group Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271–2281. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Engelhardt H, Buber I, Skirbekk V, Prskawetz A. Social involvement, behavioural risks and cognitive functioning among older people. Ageing Soc. 2010;30:779–809. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lumey LH, Stein AD, Susser E. Prenatal famine and adult health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2011;32:237–262. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031210-101230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rahu K, Rahu M, Pullmann H, Allik J. Effect of birth weight, maternal education and prenatal smoking on offspring intelligence at school age. Early Hum Dev. 2010;86:493–497. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Broadberry SN. Forging up, falling behind and catching up. A sectoral analysis of Anglo-American productivity differences, 1870-1990. Res Econ Hist. 1997;17:1–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tuddenham RD. Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. Am Psychol. 1948;3:54–56. doi: 10.1037/h0054962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Flynn JR. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations. What IQ tests really measure. Psychol Bull. 1987;101:171–191. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sacuiu S, et al. Secular changes in cognitive predictors of dementia and mortality in 70-year-olds. Neurology. 2010;75:779–785. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f0737c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.United Nations . World Population Prospects, the 2010 Revision. New York: United Nations; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Maddison A. Monitoring the World Economy 1820-1992. Paris: Org Econ Coop Dev; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lutz W, Goujon A Samir KC, Sanderson W. Vienna Yearbook of Population Research 2007. Verlag der Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften, Vienna: 2007. Reconstruction of populations by age, sex and level of educational attainment for 120 countries for 1970–2000; pp. 193–235. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Uvin P. The state of world hunger. Nutr Rev. 1994;52:151–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.1994.tb01411.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Visaria PM. Mortality and fertility in India, 1951-1961. Milbank Mem Fund Q. 1969;47:91–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Calvin CM, et al. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: Systematic review with meta-analysis. Intl J Epidemiol. 2011;40:626–644. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Deary IJ, Weiss A, Batty GD. Intelligence and personality as predictors of illness and death. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2010;11:53–79. doi: 10.1177/1529100610387081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Subramanian SV, et al. The mortality divide in India: The differential contributions of gender, caste, and standard of living across the life course. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:818–825. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.060103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Saikia N, Ram F. Determinants of adult mortality in India. Asian Population Studies. 2010;6:153–171. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Subramanian SV, Ackerson LK, Subramanyam MA, Sivaramakrishnan K. Health inequalities in India: The axes of stratification. Brown J World Aff. 2008;14:127–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Teasdale TW, Owen DR. A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;39:837–843. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sundet JM, Barlaug DG, Torjussen TM. The end of the Flynn effect?: A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence. 2004;32:349–362. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ceci SJ. How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:703–722. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Glymour MM, Kawachi I, Jencks CS, Berkman LF. Does childhood schooling affect old age memory or mental status? Using state schooling laws as natural experiments. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62:532–537. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.059469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cullum S, et al. Decline across different domains of cognitive function in normal ageing: Results of a longitudinal population-based study using CAMCOG. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:853–862. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200009)15:9<853::aid-gps211>3.0.co;2-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Launer LJ, et al. Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer's disease: Results from EURODEM pooled analyses. EURODEM Incidence Research Group and Work Groups. European Studies of Dementia. Neurology. 1999;52:78–84. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Van Dijk KRA, Van Gerven PWM, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van der Elst W, Jolles J. No protective effects of education during normal cognitive aging: Results from the 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Psychol Aging. 2008;23:119–130. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Schaie KW, Willis SL. Can decline in adult intellectual functioning be reversed? Dev Psychol. 1986;22:223–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Le Carret N, Lafont S, Mayo W, Fabrigoule C. The effect of education on cognitive performances and its implication for the constitution of the cognitive reserve. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23:317–337. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2303_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Bennett DA, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60:1909–1915. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000069923.64550.9f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Katzman R. Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1993;43:13–20. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.1_part_1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Whalley LJ, et al. Childhood mental ability and dementia. Neurology. 2000;55:1455–1459. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.10.1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Richards M, Shipley B, Fuhrer R, Wadsworth MEJ. Cognitive ability in childhood and cognitive decline in mid-life: Longitudinal birth cohort study. BMJ. 2004;328:552–554. doi: 10.1136/bmj.37972.513819.EE. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Schooler C, Mulatu MS, Oates G. The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychol Aging. 1999;14:483–506. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Finkel D, Reynolds CA, McArdle JJ, Pedersen NL. Cohort differences in trajectories of cognitive aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62:286–294. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.5.p286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Liefbroer AC, Billari FC. Bringing norms back in: A theoretical and empirical discussion of their importance for understanding demographic behaviour. Popul Space Place. 2010;16:287–305. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Van Dalen HP, Henkens K, Schippers J. Productivity of older workers: Perceptions of employers and employees. Popul Dev Rev. 2010;36:309–330. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2010.00331.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.OECD . Live longer, work longer. Ageing and Employment Policies. Paris: Org Econ Coop Dev; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rohwedder S, Willis RJ. Mental Retirement. J Econ Perspect. 2010;24:119–138. doi: 10.1257/jep.24.1.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.D'Anci KE, Rosenberg IH. B vitamins in the prevention of cognitive decline and vascular dementia. In: Bendich A, Deckelbaum RJ, editors. Preventive Nutrition: The Comprehensive Guide for Health Professionals. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 2010. pp. 325–334. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Williams JW, Plassman BL, Burke J, Holsinger T, Benjamin S. Preventing Alzheimer's Disease and Cognitive Decline. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD; 2010. Publication No. 10-E005. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.de Zwart BCH, Frings-Dresen MHW, van Dijk FJH. Physical workload and the aging worker: A review of the literature. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1995;68:1–12. doi: 10.1007/BF01831627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Metter EJ, Conwit R, Tobin J, Fozard JL. Age-associated loss of power and strength in the upper extremities in women and men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997;52:B267–B276. doi: 10.1093/gerona/52a.5.b267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Scholes S, et al. Technical Report (Wave 3): Living in the 21st Century: Older People in England: The 2006 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. London: National Centre for Social Research; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Health and Retirement Study . Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan; Available at http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/ [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Börsch-Supan A, Hank K, Jürges H. A new comprehensive and international view on ageing: Introducing the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe. Eur J Ageing. 2005;2(4):245–253. doi: 10.1007/s10433-005-0014-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mexican Health and Aging Study . Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania; Available at http://www.mhas.pop.upenn.edu/english/home.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.WHO Study on Global Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE) (WHO, Geneva) Available at http://www.who.int/healthinfo/systems/sage/en/index.html.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES