Abstract
This ‘For Debate’ paper starts by recognizing the growing trend towards considering alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder. We argue that the adoption of this model results from focusing on those in treatment for alcohol dependence rather than considering the larger number of people in the general population who meet criteria for alcohol dependence at some point in their lives. The majority of the general population who ever experience alcohol dependence do not behave as though they have a chronic relapsing disorder: they do not seek treatment, resolve their dependence themselves and do not relapse repeatedly. We suggest that caution is therefore needed in using the chronic relapsing disorder label. Our primary concerns are that this formulation privileges biological aspects of dependence to the detriment of psychological and social contributions, it inhibits much-needed developments in understanding alcohol dependence and leads to inefficient distributions of public health and clinical care resources for alcohol dependence. We invite debate on this issue.
Keywords: Alcohol dependence, alcoholism, biomedicalization, brain disease, chronic relapsing disorder, DSM-V, epidemiology, treatment
INTRODUCTION
There appears to be a growing acceptance that alcohol dependence is a chronic relapsing disorder [1–8]; but is this description an accurate label to apply? Does it capture the core features of this problem [9–12], and is it useful? In this ‘For Debate’ paper we will explore why this term may be attractive, as well as its limitations. We suggest that the majority of people with alcohol dependence do not behave as though they have a chronic relapsing disorder. Further, we will argue that conceiving of alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder may be detrimental to our attempts to understand the nature of this phenomenon, and to decisions on what to do about it.
WHERE DOES THE CONCEPT COME FROM?
The principal source of the chronic relapsing disorder model of alcohol dependence may be neurobiology research. There have been several high-profile papers, published by leaders in the field in influential journals [1–4], describing addiction as a brain disease and as a chronic relapsing disorder, and the two can be easily conflated. Neurobiology has made great strides in understanding the impact of substances of abuse on the brain, and these findings have advanced a fundamentally biological explanation of addiction. However, as has been discussed eloquently by Kalant [13], there are limitations to how far neurobiology can take us towards understanding a problem that has social and psychological as well as biological roots. The neurobiological chronic relapsing disorder perspective tends towards reductionist rather than integrative conceptions of dependence. In addition, we argue that this emphasis on the biological component of an intrinsically interdisciplinary problem results from and reinforces an emphasis on the more severe end of the continuum of dependence problems. It is usually those with severe dependence who are under study in neuroscience. This focus may narrow rather than broaden our understanding, as it implies a categorical separation of the addicted (or the alcoholic) from the non-addicted (the favoured terms within neuroscience), because some people have this problem while others do not. This is at odds with clinically based attention to severity of dependence, prevailing conceptions of the nature of dependence itself, and also what is known in population health sciences.
Other strands of research have also contributed to the adoption of the conception of alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder. McLellan [14,15] makes the valid point that people with alcohol and drug dependence who show up in specialized addictions settings will often relapse repeatedly. An analogy is drawn with other health conditions, such as hypertension, which is regarded in a completely different fashion by society at large, and by the medical community, yet displays a similar chronicity to that seen in some people with alcohol dependence. Further, the success of treatment for hypertension appears to be judged by a different set of criteria than those applied to alcohol treatment. The latter is usually judged as successful if a single treatment course leads to long-term or permanent remission and the former is judged successful if the hypertensive patient has reduced symptoms during the time they are taking the treatment. McLellan is careful, however, to specifically avoid a discussion of the nature of dependence or addiction in an editorial in this journal [15] and to concentrate instead on the ways in which treatment responses are conceptualized and evaluated. The JAMA paper [14] is additionally concerned with advocacy that dependence is suitable for insurance, just like other chronic medical problems, and this paper becomes cited as providing evidential support to the chronic relapsing disorder formulation (e.g. [2]). McLellan's work also clearly embraces the need to provide a much broader continuum of care for those with addictions concerns [16,17].
AN ALTERNATE VIEW
Our approach to alcohol dependence comes primarily from a population health perspective. That is, when the course of alcohol dependence is examined for the entire population of people who meet criteria for this disorder, do they behave as though they have a chronic relapsing disorder? The majority of people who meet criteria for alcohol dependence at some point in their life: (i) do not seek treatment [18–21]; (ii) resolve their alcohol dependence without any formal treatment or similar help [19,22,23]; and (iii) do not relapse repeatedly to alcohol dependence [19,22,24,25]. We stress here that this is not to say that some people with alcohol dependence do not relapse repeatedly and that a chronic care model of treatment would be ill-advised for this subpopulation—just that most people who experience alcohol dependence do not relapse again and again. If we want to understand the nature of alcohol dependence, or other addictive behaviours, then it is important to examine the full range of people experiencing the problem, thus including but not restricted to those with severe dependence only.
COMMON GROUND
Is there a middle ground between these two views of alcohol dependence that may be appreciated when one considers the component terms—chronic, relapsing and disorder? For many people who experience alcohol dependence, there is indeed chronicity in the sense that this is not just a short-lived episode responsive to some sort of quick fix. Alcohol dependence usually takes many years to develop and be maintained [26–34] before it is subsequently overcome by most people on their own [19,22,23]. Drinking behaviour is dynamic and modified by a multitude of influences at different levels of intervention among those who are alcohol-dependent. Difficulties in controlling consumption are characteristic of many of those who have dependence and are fundamental to our understanding of it, and great suffering is involved in their personal struggles to limit the associated harms. Relapse in the sense of setbacks in these struggles is not at all unusual. Finally, alcohol dependence is clearly a disorder in diagnostic terms and disordered in the sense that it conflicts with deeply held values and goals and does damage to them [35].
It is not difficult to see why viewing alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder has appeal, as the formulation attests to important aspects of the phenomenon. Therein lies its limitation, as it does not capture accurately the apparent experience of most people affected by alcohol dependence, and thus potentially obscures rather than illuminates the full range of problems of dependence. It is almost as though the term ‘chronic relapsing disorder’ takes on a life of its own, holding more meaning than when its constituent terms are used in isolation. When applied by leaders of the field in non-specialist addiction journals, it is used to communicate the essential nature of dependence [1,2]. Finer-grained attention to the long-term course of the behaviour in the general population as observed in epidemiological studies provides a dramatically different and more heterogeneous picture from that attained in biologically orientated clinical research studies in treatment populations. This disjoint between the understanding of what is alcohol dependence in clinical and general population settings is at the root of our concern with adopting the model of a chronic relapsing disorder for alcohol dependence.
THIS TENSION IS NOT NEW
The different perspectives held by addictions researchers with a clinical versus a population health perspective are not new. Room [36] referred to these as the two worlds of alcohol problems at a time early in the development of the modern epidemiological study of alcohol dependence. These different perspectives have far-reaching consequences, because the picture that is derived of alcohol dependence is very different depending on the researcher's or policy maker's orientation. Fundamentally, it comes down to how we define the population with alcohol dependence—does it consist of only those who are seen in treatment or does it comprise everyone who meets criteria for alcohol dependence in the general population? If one restricts the perspective to just those in specialized addictions services then it may be useful to regard alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder, although even here severity of dependence has long been accepted to be a key determinant of likelihood of relapse [37]. When the scope of alcohol dependence is widened to include all those who meet diagnostic criteria within an epidemiological survey then the concept of a chronic relapsing disorder does not seem as applicable. It is important to be clear that this broader epidemiological perspective emphasizes the need to be appreciative of the full continuum of alcohol problems (up to and including severe alcohol dependence), and not any sort of false dichotomy between those who alcohol dependent and those who are not.
WHY DOES THIS MATTER?
For public health
The danger of conceptualizing alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder is that it directs attention to a subgroup of those with severe dependence, and thus potentially undermines public health strategic responses for both those with dependence and those whose drinking is harmful or hazardous rather than dependent [38]. A consideration of the entire continuum of alcohol problems (including both those at risk of problems and those with alcohol dependence) is the necessary context for a population perspective on alcohol control, not least because population-level interventions are an important source of benefit for dependent drinkers as well as for other at-risk drinkers [39,40]. Further, it is this larger population of at-risk drinkers who cause the majority of costs to society that result from alcohol consumption, simply because of their numbers (referred to as the prevention paradox; [41–43]). The best evidence regarding ways to reduce the prevalence of alcohol problems in our society rests almost entirely with public health initiatives (such as reducing availability, taxation, drinking and driving legislation and the provision of brief interventions; [40]). Any model of alcohol dependence that takes us away from this recognition of the primary importance of public health interventions in the prevention and management of alcohol problems in the general population may thus impair our ability to effectively address these significant societal concerns.
For clinical care
There are a number of reasons why it matters whether or not we adopt a chronic relapsing disorder model of alcohol dependence for clinical care. A concentration on the chronic relapsing nature of alcohol dependence may lead to a preponderance of resources going towards those with severe dependence and other co-occurring health (including mental health) issues. The appropriate balance of resources requires an appreciation that dependence occurs on a continuum in which there are many more people with mild to moderate than severe behavioural dysfunction [38,44]. This continuum of severity does not in any way imply a necessary progression from early and mild to late and severe. A range of services of varying intensities is needed to address the continuum of severity of alcohol dependence [16,17].
It could be argued that a chronic relapsing model might promote appropriate care for people with severe alcohol dependence (and other co-occurring health issues) in a specialized addictions setting. Dependence may not, however, be at the root of complicated and long-standing problems, and holistic orientations more appropriate for mainstream rather than specialist services may be called for when issues such as broken relationships, employment, housing, violence, physical health complications and co-occurring mental health issues come into play. It is also possible to envision chronic care packages which are attuned to the longer-term needs of the majority with dependence who often do not have complex co-occurring health issues and where extensivity rather than intensivity of intervention matters more [45,46]. Various researchers have been involved in the design and evaluation of continuing care protocols [47–49] which see ‘acute’ interventions at times of particular difficulty supplemented with brief efforts to support longer-term self-management. This clinical research effectiveness evidence base is, however, at an early stage of development, even though this may resemble how people actually do use treatment services, and health services research will also be needed to explore appropriate service provision models.
In a primary care setting, it is unknown whether the clinician may be more or less likely to ask about use of alcohol and intervene where indicated, if they think that alcohol dependence is a chronic relapsing disorder. While the therapeutically committed clinician who feels well supported in their role may be more likely to do so, others may be less likely to [50]. The majority of people who are identified with alcohol dependence in general practice will not be chronically relapsing nor display the wider needs of those in specialist treatment services. The label does not convey optimism about scope for positive change among either those with relatively moderate or severe dependence. There is a danger that it fosters low expectations which become self-fulfilling prophecies [51–53]. Brief treatments offered by generalists for those with dependence are in need of further development [54], as are online treatment interventions [55]. The concept that a chronic relapsing disorder model will lead to more and more appropriate treatment is an assumption that needs open consideration.
For research
The chronic relapsing disorder model has its roots in the examination of people in specialized addictions treatment settings. There is profound selection bias here. Severity of alcohol dependence does drive people into treatment, but this is one influence among many. People in treatment are more likely to have other concurrent mental health and substance use concerns [56–58] as well as to have experienced other problems in their lives, particularly just prior to help-seeking [59–63]. Put simplistically, people choose to enter into treatment because they have problems (or are made to go because somebody else determines they have problems). Those in treatment often have a complex set of difficulties above and beyond their alcohol dependence and it may be these co-occurring problems that lead to the increased likelihood that they will relapse and show up in treatment repeatedly, rather than alcohol dependence per se[56,62,64–67]. Notwithstanding acknowledgement of extensive comorbidities and the dominant understanding of the multi-faceted nature of dependence, there is a tendency to see alcohol dependence as the root cause of many of the associated difficulties. It is rather odd that we base our understanding of alcohol dependence on those most severely afflicted by other problems. Perhaps we would do better to give greater emphasis in our attempts to understand dependence to the young adult populations where the majority of dependence is to be found, relative to middle-aged individuals who end up in treatment often beset by accumulated life problems [26,27,65]. These difficulties may have originated prior to the onset of dependence and make causal contributions to its development [68]. It is also worth considering whether alcohol dependence seen in young adults is something of a measurement artefact, an earlier manifestation of the same phenomenon seen at older ages or whether the onset of additional problems changes dependence in a fundamental way, or some combination of these possibilities [65,69,70].
There is a clear need to understand more effectively the development of alcohol dependence over the life-course in people who do not seek treatment. This is particularly true as it may well be that dependence is just as modifiable, or indeed more so, among those who do not attend treatment services. This can be seen in studies exploring the natural history of alcohol dependence [22,23,25,71]. Evidence-based interventions designed for use with treatment-seeking populations share important characteristics or may be the same as those effective for those not seeking help. For example, Motivational Enhancement Therapy, one of the most well-evaluated psychosocial treatments for alcohol dependence, is simply an elongated version of the Drinkers Check-Up, designed originally to support non-treatment change [72,73]. At least among those who change successfully, the social, behavioural and motivational mechanisms of change are probably very similar with and without the support of treatment services [71,74–77]. The problem is that we do not really understand what it is that differentiates people whose alcohol dependence will chronically relapse from those who will resolve it successfully by themselves or with a little-well designed help. The forthcoming revision of DSM-V combines the conceptually distinct domains of dependence and other types of problems in a new category of disorder [78]. We share concern that this will probably lead to a diminution of attention to problems other than dependence [79], and this will probably make the chronic relapsing disorder label even more inappropriate for those diagnosed as having disorders. It is unclear what the consequences of this change will be for the life-course study of drinking behaviour.
Finally, the adoption of the chronic relapsing disorder model appears to have implications for the allocation of resources for conducting research [80]. Regarding alcohol dependence as a chronic relapsing disorder may be one of the contributing factors to the ongoing biomedicalization of alcohol problems. It has been posited that the result of this biomedicalization is that the large majority of funding for research goes to biobehavioural research to the detriment of research exploring the psychological and sociological components driving alcohol dependence [80]. Given that dependence on alcohol is fundamentally a behaviour, albeit co-determined by the interaction of biological, psychological and social influences, this is a real problem.
CONCLUDING REMARKS
Caution is needed when using the chronic relapsing disorder term, as the value of this perspective is diminished when one considers the problems probably attendant on its careless application. Restricting attention to those with ‘complicated alcohol dependence’, which may be a better way of thinking about and referring to this population, has profoundly adverse consequences for both science and clinical and public health interventions. Insufficient attention has been paid to the entire distribution of those with alcohol dependence. We believe that a chronic relapsing disorder model is not a useful conception for understanding the experience of the majority of people who have difficulties with alcohol dependence at some point in their life and that the influence of age and age-associated problems linked to dependence would benefit from further elaboration. Further, adopting a chronic relapsing disorder model may lead to an imbalance in considering the factors contributing to alcohol dependence, shifting focus away from psychological, social and environmental contributors towards an overemphasis on neurobiological perspectives. In addition, it is unclear whether a chronic relapsing disorder perspective serves some other useful purpose, such as leading to reduced stigma for those with alcohol dependence or to increasing the likelihood of accessing effective treatment. Until there is some evidence that adopting this model of alcohol dependence provides researchers, practitioners and policy makers with significantly more benefits than it does potential harms, we suggest that alcohol dependence itself should not be regarded as a chronic relapsing disorder.
Acknowledgments
Support to CAMH for salary of scientists and infrastructure has been provided by the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. The views expressed here do not necessarily reflect those of the Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. John Cunningham is also supported as the Canada Research Chair on Brief Interventions for Addictive Behaviours. Jim McCambridge is supported by a Wellcome Trust Research Career Development fellowship in Basic Biomedical Science (WT086516MA).
Declarations of interest
None.
References
- 1.Leshner AI. Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science. 1997;278:45–7. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Volkow ND, Li TK. Drugs and alcohol: treating and preventing abuse, addiction and their medical consequences. Pharmacol Ther. 2005;108:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2005.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schuckit MA. Alcohol-use disorders. Lancet. 2009;373:492–501. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60009-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Koob GF, Sanna PP, Bloom FE. Neuroscience of addiction. Neuron. 1998;21:467–76. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80557-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wrase J, Makris N, Braus DF, Mann K, Smolka MN, Kennedy DN, et al. Amygdala volume associated with alcohol abuse relapse and craving. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1179–84. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Breese GR, Sinha R, Heilig M. Chronic alcohol neuroadaptation and stress contribute to susceptibility for alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacol Ther. 2011;129:149–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2010.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Enoch MA. The role of GABA(A) receptors in the development of alcoholism. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ducci F, Goldman D. Genetic approaches to addiction: genes and alcohol. Addiction. 2008;103:1414–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02203.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 10.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edn revised. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Edwards G, Gross MM. Alcohol dependence: provisional description of a clinical syndrome. BMJ. 1976;1:1058–61. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.6017.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Edwards G. The alcohol dependence syndrome: a concept as stimulus to enquiry. Br J Addict. 1986;81:171–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1986.tb00313.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kalant H. What neurobiology cannot tell us about addiction. Addiction. 2010;105:780–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02739.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McLellan AT, Lewis DC, O'Brien CP, Kleber HD. Drug dependence, a chronic medical illness: implications for treatment, insurance, and outcomes evaluation. JAMA. 2000;284:1689–95. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.13.1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.McLellan AT. Have we evaluated addiction treatment correctly? Implications from a chronic care perspective. Addiction. 2002;97:249–52. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McLellan AT. Reducing heavy drinking: a public health strategy and a treatment goal? J Subst Abuse Treat. 2007;33:81–3. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.McLellan AT. Treatment given high priority in new White House drug control policy. Interview by Bridget Kuehn. JAMA. 2010;303:821–2. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cunningham JA, Breslin FC. Only one in three people with alcohol abuse or dependence ever seek treatment. Addict Behav. 2004;29:221–3. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(03)00077-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dawson DA, Grant BF, Stinson FS, Chou PS, Huang B, Ruan WJ, et al. Recovery from DSM-IV alcohol dependence: United States, 2001–2002. Addiction. 2005;100:281–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00964.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cohen E, Feinn R, Arias A, Kranzler HR. Alcohol treatment utilization: findings from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;86:214–21. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ko JY, Martins SS, Kuramoto SJ, Chilcoat HD. Patterns of alcohol-dependence symptoms using a latent empirical approach: associations with treatment usage and other correlates. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2010;71:870–8. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dawson DA. Correlates of past-year status among treated and untreated persons with former alcohol dependence: United States, 1992. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:771–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cunningham JA, Lin E, Ross HE, Walsh GW. Factors associated with untreated remissions from alcohol abuse or dependence. Addict Behav. 2000;25:317–21. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(98)00130-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Grant BF. Rates and correlates of relapse among individuals in remission from DSM-IV alcohol dependence: a 3-year follow-up. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:2036–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00536.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.de Bruijn C, van den Brink W, de Graaf R, Vollebergh WA. The 3-year course of alcohol use disorders in the general population: DSM-IV, ICD-10 and the Craving Withdrawal Model. Addiction. 2006;101:385–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Age of alcohol-dependence onset: associations with severity of dependence and seeking treatment. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e755–63. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fillmore KM. Alcohol Use across the Life Course: A Critical Review of 70 Years of International Longitudinal Research. Toronto, Ontario: Addiction Research Foundation; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Merline A, Jager J, Schulenberg JE. Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood. Addiction. 2008;103:84–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02178.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Andreasson S, Allebeck P, Brandt L. Predictors of alcoholism in young Swedish men. Am J Public Health. 1993;83:845–50. doi: 10.2105/ajph.83.6.845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jacob T, Koenig LB, Howell DN, Wood PK, Haber JR. Drinking trajectories from adolescence to the fifties among alcohol-dependent men. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009;70:859–69. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.McAweeney MJ, Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, Puttler LI, Wong MM. Individual and partner predictors of recovery from alcohol-use disorder over a nine-year interval: findings from a community sample of alcoholic married men. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66:220–8. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Penick EC, Knop J, Nickel EJ, Jensen P, Manzardo AM, Lykke-Mortensen E, et al. Do premorbid predictors of alcohol dependence also predict the failure to recover from alcoholism? J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2010;71:685–94. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Schuckit MA, Smith TL. Onset and course of alcoholism over 25 years in middle class men. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;113:21–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vaillant GE. A 60-year follow-up of alcoholic men. Addiction. 2003;98:1043–51. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00422.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Heather N. Addictive disorders are essentially motivational problems. Br J Addict. 1992;87:828–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1992.tb01974.x. discussion 833–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Room R. Measurement and distribution of drinking patterns and problems in general populations. In: Edwards G, Gross MM, Keller M, Moser J, Room R, editors. Alcohol-Related Disabilities. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1977. pp. 62–87. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Edwards G. A later follow-up of a classic case series: D. L. Davies's 1962 report and its significance for the present. J Stud Alcohol. 1985;46:181–90. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1985.46.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Institute of Medicine. Broadening the Base of Treatment for Alcohol Problems. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1990. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Room R, Babor T, Rehm J. Alcohol and public health. Lancet. 2005;365:519–30. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)17870-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Babor TF, Caetano R, Casswell S, et al. Alcohol: No Ordinary Commodity—Research and Public Policy. Oxford and London: Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kreitman N. Alcohol consumption and the prevention paradox. Br J Addict. 1986;81:353–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1986.tb00342.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rose G. Sick individuals and sick populations. Int J Epidemiol. 1985;14:32–8. doi: 10.1093/ije/14.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Stockwell T, Toumbourou JW, Letcher P, Smart D, Sanson A, Bond L, et al. Risk and protection factors for different intensities of adolescent substance use: when does the Prevention Paradox apply? Drug Alcohol Rev. 2004;23:67–77. doi: 10.1080/09595230410001645565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Willenbring ML. A broader view of change in drinking behavior. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:84s–6s. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.McLellan AT, McKay JR, Forman R, Cacciola J, Kemp J. Reconsidering the evaluation of addiction treatment: from retrospective follow-up to concurrent recovery monitoring. Addiction. 2005;100:447–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.McKay JR. Is there a case for extended interventions for alcohol and drug use disorders? Addiction. 2005;100:1594–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McKay JR, Van Horn DH, Oslin DW, Lynch KG, Ivey M, Ward K, et al. A randomized trial of extended telephone-based continuing care for alcohol dependence: within-treatment substance use outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2010;78:912–23. doi: 10.1037/a0020700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Scott CK, Dennis ML. Results from two randomized clinical trials evaluating the impact of quarterly recovery management checkups with adult chronic substance users. Addiction. 2009;104:959–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02525.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cacciola JS, Camilleri AC, Carise D, Rikoon SH, McKay SR, McLellan AT, et al. Extending residential care through telephone counseling: initial results from the Betty Ford Center Focused Continuing Care protocol. Addict Behav. 2008;33:1208–16. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cartwright AKJ. the attitudes of helping agents towards the alcoholic client: the influence of experience, support, training, and self-esteem. Br J Addict. 1980;75:413–31. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rosenthal R. Covert communication in classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubicles. Am Psychol. 2002;57:839–49. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.57.11.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rosenthal R, Jacobson L. Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and Pupil's Intellectual Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Leake GJ, King AS. Effect of counselor expectations on alcoholic recovery. Alcohol Health Res World. 1977;11:16–22. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Saitz R. Alcohol screening and brief intervention in primary care: absence of evidence for efficacy in people with dependence or very heavy drinking. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2010;29:631–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2010.00217.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Linke S, McCambridge J, Khadjesari Z, Wallace P, Murray E. Development of a psychologically enhanced interactive online intervention for hazardous drinking. Alcohol Alcohol. 2008;43:669–74. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dawson DA, Grant BF, Stinson FS, Chou PS. Estimating the effect of help-seeking on achieving recovery from alcohol dependence. Addiction. 2006;101:824–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chan YF, Dennis ML, Funk RR. Prevalence and comorbidity of major internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and adults presenting to substance abuse treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2008;34:14–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.12.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Dufour MC, Compton W, et al. Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:807–16. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Gorman DM, Brown GW. Recent developments in life-event research and their relevance for the study of addictions. Br J Addict. 1992;87:837–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1992.tb01978.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Tucker JA, Vuchinich RE, Pukish MM. Molar environmental contexts surrounding recovery from alcohol problems by treated and untreated problem drinkers. J Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1995;3:195–204. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Timko C, Finney JW, Moos RH, Moos BS. Short-term treatment careers and outcomes of previously untreated alcoholics. J Stud Alcohol. 1995;56:597–610. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Weisner C. The role of alcohol-related problematic events in treatment entry. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1990;26:93–102. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(90)90116-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Finney JW, Moos RH. Entering treatment for alcohol abuse: a stress and coping model. Addiction. 1995;90:1123–240. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.90912237.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Cunningham JA, Blomqvist J. Examining treatment use among alcohol-dependent individuals from a population perspective. Alcohol Alcohol. 2006;41:632–5. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi HY. Subtypes of alcohol dependence in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;91:149–58. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Weisner C, Matzger H, Kaskutas LA. How important is treatment? One-year outcomes of treated and untreated alcohol-dependent individuals. Addiction. 2003;98:901–11. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Weisner C. Toward an alcohol treatment entry model: a comparison of problem drinkers in the general population and in treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1993;17:746–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb00833.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.McCambridge J, McAlaney J, Rowe R. Adult consequences of late adolescent alcohol consumption: a systematic review of cohort studies. PLoS Med. 2011;8:e1000413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Babor TF, Caetano R. Subtypes of substance dependence and abuse: implications for diagnostic classification and empirical research. Addiction. 2006;101:104–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01595.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Caetano R, Babor TF. Diagnosis of alcohol dependence in epidemiological surveys: an epidemic of youthful alcohol dependence or a case of measurement error? Addiction. 2006;101:111–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01599.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Klingemann H, Sobell MB, Sobell LC. Continuities and changes in self-change research. Addiction. 2010;105:1510–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02770.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Miller WR, Sovereign RG, Krege B. Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers: II. The Drinker's Check-up as a preventive intervention. Behav Psychother. 1988;16:251–68. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People to Change Addictive Behavior. New York: Guilford Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Miller WR. Why do people change addictive behavior? The 1996 H. David Archibald Lecture. Addiction. 1998;93:163–72. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9321632.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Orford J. Excessive Appetites: A Psychological View of Addictions. New York: John Wiley; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Miller WR, Carroll KM. Drawing the scene together: ten principles, ten recommendations. In: Miller WR, Carroll KM, editors. Rethinking Substance Abuse: What the Science Shows, and What We Would Do about It. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. pp. 293–312. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Toward a comprehensive model of change. In: Miller WR, Heather N, editors. Treating Addictive Behaviors: Process of Change. New York: Plenum Press; 1986. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- 78.O'Brien C. Addiction and dependence in DSM-V. Addiction. 2011;106:866–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03144.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Edwards G. The trouble with drink: why ideas matter. Addiction. 2010;105:797–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02773.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Midanik LT. Biomedicalization and alcohol studies: implications for policy. J Public Health Policy. 2004;25:211–28. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.jphp.3190021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]