Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011 Nov 23;13(1):51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136

Normalization as a canonical neural computation

Matteo Carandini 1, David J Heeger 2
PMCID: PMC3273486  NIHMSID: NIHMS348079  PMID: 22108672

Abstract

There is increasing evidence that the brain relies on a set of canonical neural computations, repeating them across brain regions and modalities to apply similar operations to different problems. A promising candidate for such a computation is normalization, in which the responses of neurons are divided by a common factor that typically includes the summed activity of a pool of neurons. Normalization was developed to explain responses in the primary visual cortex and is now thought to operate throughout the visual system, and in many other sensory modalities and brain regions. Normalization may underlie operations such as the representation of odours, the modulatory effects of visual attention, the encoding of value and the integration of multisensory information. Its presence in such a diversity of neural systems in multiple species, from invertebrates to mammals, suggests that it serves as a canonical neural computation.


The brain has a modular design. The advantages of modularity are well known to engineers: modules that can be replicated and cascaded, such as transistors and web servers, lie at the root of powerful technologies. The brain seems to apply this principle in two ways: with modular circuits and with modular computations. Anatomical evidence suggests the existence of canonical microcircuits that are replicated across brain areas, for example, across regions of the cerebral cortex1,2. Physiological and behavioural evidence suggests that canonical neural computations exist — standard computational modules that apply the same fundamental operations in a variety of contexts. A canonical neural computation can rely on diverse circuits and mechanisms, and different brain regions or different species may implement it with different available components.

Two established examples of canonical neural computations are exponentiation and linear filtering. Exponentiation, a form of thresholding, operates at the level of neurons and of networks3 — for example, in the mechanism that triggers eye and limb movements46. This operation has multiple key roles: maintaining sensory selectivity7, decorrelating signals8 and establishing perceptual choice9,10. Linear filtering (that is, weighted summation by linear receptive fields) is a widespread computation in sensory systems. It is performed, at least approximately, at various stages in vision11, hearing12 and somatosensation13. It helps to explain a vast number of perceptual phenomena14. It may also play a part in sensorimotor15 and motor systems16.

A third kind of computation has been seen to operate in various neural systems: divisive normalization. Normalization computes a ratio between the response of an individual neuron and the summed activity of a pool of neurons. Normalization was proposed in the early 1990s to explain non-linear properties of neurons in primary visual cortex1719. Similar computations20 had been proposed previously to explain light adaptation in retina2124, size invariance in the fly visual system25 and associative memory in the hippocampus26. Evidence that has accumulated since then suggests that normalization plays a part in a wide variety of modalities, brain regions and species.

Here, we review this evidence and suggest that normalization is a canonical neural computation in sensory systems and possibly also in other neural systems. We introduce normalization by describing results from the olfactory system of invertebrates. We then describe its operation in retina, in primary visual cortex, in higher visual cortical areas and in non-visual cortical areas, and discuss its role in sensory processing and in the modulatory effects of attention. Finally, we review the multiple mechanisms and circuits that may be associated with normalization, and the behavioural measurements that are captured well by normalization. Two independent sections define the basic elements of the normalization equation and the many roles that have been proposed for normalization in relation to optimizing the neural code.

Normalization in the invertebrate olfactory system

The fruitfly (Drosophila melanogaster) senses odours through receptor neurons, each of which expresses a single odorant receptor. Receptor neurons project to the antennal lobe, a brain region that is analogous to the olfactory bulb in vertebrates. The response R of an antennal lobe neuron increases with the activity I of the receptor neurons that drive it27:

R=γInσn+In (1)

The parameters γ, σ and n determine the shape of the response curve (FIG. 1a). A mask odorant that does not drive an antennal lobe neuron nonetheless suppresses the responses to a test odorant that does drive the neuron (FIG. 1b). The interaction of the two odorants is accurately described27 by the normalization equation:

R=γInσn+Imn+In (2)

Here, I is the response of receptor neurons that drive the antennal lobe neuron (which responds to the test odorant), and Im are the pooled responses of the other receptor neurons (which respond to the mask odorant). As Im appears as an additive term in the denominator, increasing it has the same effect as increasing σ in equation 4: it shifts the response curve to the right on a logarithmic scale (FIG. 1b).

Figure 1. Normalization in the olfactory system of the fruitfly.

Figure 1

a | Responses of olfactory neurons in the antennal lobe to a single test odorant, as a function of activity in the presynaptic receptor neuron. γ and σ are constants (shown by the dotted line and the arrow). b | Responses of olfactory neurons in the antennal lobe to test odorant in the presence of mask odorants of increasing concentration (lower concentrations are shown by lighter colours). Curves are fits of the normalization model (equation 5), with Im free to vary with mask concentration. Data from REF. 27.

Normalization in the retina

The retina needs to operate in a wide range of light intensities. Across visual environments (for example, an overcast night and a sunny day) light intensities range over 10 factors of 10 (REFS 28,29). Within a given visual scene the variation is typically smaller, but still more than a factor of 10 (REF. 30). The retina uses anatomical solutions (rod and cone photoreceptors and an adjustable pupil) for some of this wide range. For the rest of the range, light adaptation adjusts the sensitivity of neurons through normalization (FIG. 2a), resulting in responses that represent contrast (deviations from the mean over recent time and local space) rather than absolute intensities.

Figure 2. Normalization in retina.

Figure 2

a | Light adaptation operates on light intensity to produce a neural estimate of contrast (multiple arrows indicate light intensities from multiple locations). b | Responses of a turtle cone photoreceptor to light of increasing intensity. The intensity of the coloured squares reflects background intensity. Curves are fits of normalization model (equation 5) with n = 1. c | Light adaptation moves the operating point to suit images of differing intensity. Histograms on abscissa indicate distributions of light intensity for a sinusoidal grating under dim illumination (shown in blue) and bright illumination (shown in green). Histograms on ordinate indicate distributions of responses, which are more similar to one another than the light intensity distributions. d | The same data as in part c | plotted as a function of local contrast (Weber contrast) rather than light intensity. Light adaptation makes responses roughly proportional to local contrast. The linear approximation given by equation 6 is shown (indicated by the dotted line). e | Contrast normalization operates on the neural estimate of contrast and normalizes it with respect to the standard deviation (sd) of nearby contrasts (multiple arrows indicate local contrast from multiple locations). f | Effects of contrast normalization. Responses of a neuron in lateral geniculate nucleus (which receives input from the retina) as a function of grating contrast and size. deg, degrees. Data in part b, from REF. 24. Data in part f, from REF. 40.

Photoreceptor responses increase with the intensity of a stimulus (FIG. 2b). Their sensitivity (the position of the intensity–response curve on the intensity axis) depends on background light intensity2224,31,32 (FIG. 2b). Increasing background intensity shifts the response curve to the right on a logarithmic axis23,24,31,32, placing the steepest portion of the curve (where the cell is most sensitive to changes) near the background intensity (FIG. 2b). As with a mask odorant in insect olfaction, equation 5 fits this effect well. Here, the background intensity constitutes the additive term Im in the denominator, and the exponent n is 1.

Normalization makes photoreceptors adjust their operating point to discount mean light intensity (FIG. 2c). For example, consider two images differing only by a scaling factor (for example, because of different illumination33). Their distributions of light intensities are shifted laterally on a logarithmic axis (FIG. 2c). Normalization makes the photoreceptor responses shift accordingly, so that the responses to both images are similar34.

This adjustment of sensitivity approximates a neural measure of visual contrast (FIG. 2c,d). If background light is sufficiently high (Im >> σ), equation 5 can be rewritten (through a Taylor series) as:

RRmαC (3)

Here, Rm is the response to the background light Im. C is local contrast, the relative deviation of local light intensity from the mean:

C=(IIm)Im (4)

The approximation in equation 6 assumes that local contrast C is moderate (so that C2 and higher can be ignored). This condition is often met in natural scenes, in which local contrast is typically low30.

The key factor in determining local contrast is the spatial and temporal scale over which one computes the mean light intensity Im. Computing it over a scale that is too wide or long would entail large deviations from the mean, so equation 6 would no longer apply and light adaptation would no longer provide a measure of contrast. At the other extreme — computing local contrast over a tight spatial scale and brief temporal scale — estimates of mean intensity would become unreliable because of noise (variability inherent in the photoreceptor responses)29.

Following light adaptation, signals in retina are thought to feed into a second normalization stage, which performs contrast normalization (FIG. 2e). This stage adjusts responses to one location in an image based on the contrast in a surrounding spatial region3540. Conceptually, it is useful to consider contrast normalization as separate from light adaptation29,30, but mechanistically the two stages may overlap in bipolar cells. Light adaptation is thought to operate in photoreceptors and bipolar cells, whereas contrast normalization is thought to occur in bipolar cells and ganglion cells41, and to become stronger in subsequent stages of visual processing.

Under contrast normalization, responses are no longer proportional to local contrast Cj (the output of the first normalization stage). Instead, the response Rj of neuron j is divided by a constant σ plus a measure of overall contrast40:

Rj=γΣiwiCiσ+ΣkαkCk2 (5)

Here, the weights wi (positive or negative) define the spatial profile of the summation field (typically, a centre-surround difference of Gaussians), and the weights αk (positive) define the spatial profile of the suppressive field (typically, a large Gaussian40). The responses of neurons at the output of the retina (as measured in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN)) are characterized well by this equation, in which the normalization in the denominator corresponds to the standard deviation of contrasts over a region of the visual field42.

A common way to probe contrast normalization is to use gratings that vary in overall contrast and size (FIG. 2f). As predicted by the model, increasing grating contrast leads to response saturation when gratings are shown in a large window, but not when they are shown in a small window40 (FIG. 2f). For small windows, local contrast is zero in most of the suppressive field, so the denominator has a small role in equation 8. For larger stimuli, increasing grating contrast increases local contrast not only in the numerator but also in the denominator, and responses saturate. Response saturation, therefore, is due to contrast and not to the evoked response: it is strongest for largest stimuli, which evoke weaker responses than smaller stimuli.

Normalization in primary visual cortex

Normalization is thought to operate not only in retina but also at multiple subsequent stages along the visual pathway. Indeed, the normalization model was first developed to account for the physiological responses of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1)1719,4345.

Here, we describe the normalization model for a population of V1 neurons differing in preference for stimulus position and orientation. This characterization of the responses of neural populations4648 encompasses previous descriptions of single neurons19,43. In the model, the responses of a population of V1 neurons are given by:

R(x,θ)=D(x,θ)nσn+N(x,θ)n (6)

Here, x and θ indicate the preferred position and orientation of each neuron in the population (the only two stimulus attributes that we consider in this simplified explanation). The numerator contains the stimulus drive D, which results from each neuron's summation field and determines the selectivity for stimulus position and orientation. The normalization factor N in the denominator, in turn, is determined by the suppressive field α(x,θ), which provides weights with which to pool the stimulus drive received by each of the neurons (BOX 1). The normalization factor typically responds to a broader set of stimuli than the summation field. So a neuron that responds to stimuli with particular spatial positions and orientations can be suppressed by stimuli with a broader range of spatial positions and orientations. The exponent n in V1 neurons is generally between 1.0 and 3.5, with an average of about 2 (REFS 4850).

Normalization explains why responses of V1 neurons saturate with increasing stimulus contrast, irrespective of stimulus orientation, and therefore irrespective of firing rate49 (FIG. 3a). Normalization explains this behaviour1719,43 because both the stimulus drive and the normalization factor are proportional to grating contrast c:

R=γ(θφ)cnσn+cn (7)

Here, γ is the neuron's tuning curve for orientation, which depends on the difference between the neuron's preferred orientation θ and the stimulus orientation φ. This characterization of the responses is a separable function of contrast and orientation, so the responses saturate with contrast regardless of stimulus orientation (FIG. 3a), and orientation tuning is invariant with contrast51,52.

Figure 3. Normalization in primary visual cortex.

Figure 3

a | Contrast saturation. Responses as a function of grating contrast for gratings having optimal orientation (shown in red) and suboptimal orientation (shown in yellow). b | Cross-orientation suppression. Responses to the sum of a test grating and an orthogonal mask grating (colours indicate mask contrast, from 0% (shown in yellow) to 50% (shown in dark red)). c | Transition from drive to suppression. Grating 1 had optimal orientation and grating 2 had suboptimal orientation. Grating 2 could provide some drive to the neuron when presented alone (shown in yellow) but became suppressive when grating 1 had moderate contrasts (shown in red). d | Surround suppression. A grating contained in a central disk was surrounded by a grating in an annulus. The annulus elicited minimal responses when presented alone, but suppressed responses to the central disk. e | Effects of normalization on population responses. Each dot indicates the response of a population of neurons selective for a given orientation, and each panel indicates the population responses to a stimulus. Stimuli are gratings of increasing contrast, presented alone (top) or together with an orthogonal grating (bottom). Data in part a from REF. 43; data in part b from REF. 56; data in part c from REF. 43; data in part d from REF. 142; data in part e from REF. 48.

Another non-linear phenomenon that is captured by normalization is cross-orientation suppression (FIG. 3b). The responses of a V1 neuron to a test grating that drives responses are suppressed by superimposing on the test grating a mask grating that is ineffective in eliciting responses when presented alone — for example, because its orientation is orthogonal to the neuron's preferred orientation43,5356. Normalization explains this effect because the suppression in the denominator increases with both test contrast ct and mask contrast cm, whereas the stimulus drive increases only with test contrast ct (the summation field does not respond to the mask):

R=γctnσn+cmn+ctn (8)

The effect of increasing mask contrast, therefore, is to increase the denominator, which shifts the curve to the right on a logarithmic contrast axis (FIG. 3b). This is remarkably similar similar to the effect in the fruitfly antennal lobe of adding a non-preferred odorant (FIG. 1b), or increasing background light intensity in photoreceptors (FIG. 2b). In all of these cases, the responses to a preferred stimulus are effectively divided by the strength of a non-preferred stimulus.

Normalization also explains the more general case in which the mask does provide some drive to the neuron, for example, because its orientation is close to the preferred one (FIG. 3c). In this case, normalization makes an important prediction43,57: the mask should evoke activity when presented alone, but it should become suppressive in the presence of a more effective stimulus. To see how this prediction arises, consider the response of a neuron (equation 9) to the sum of two stimuli with contrasts c1 and c2:

R=w1c1+w2c2σ+c1+c2 (9)

w1 >> w2 measure the degree to which stimulus 1 and 2 drive the neuron (and for simplicity we are setting the exponent n to 1). When stimulus 1 is absent (c1 = 0), responses increase with stimulus 2 contrast. If instead stimulus 1 has sufficiently high contrast c1, one can ignore the term w2c2 in the numerator. Therefore c2 appears only in the denominator, where it exerts a purely suppressive effect.

Another widespread phenomenon in V1 that is explained by normalization is surround suppression58,59 (FIG. 3d). A neuron's responses to stimuli inside the summation field can be suppressed by placing additional stimuli in the surrounding region60,61. Normalization accounts for this phenomenon58 (FIG. 3d) for similar reasons to those discussed in relation to cross-orientation suppression: the suppressive field covers a larger region of visual space than does the summation field.

Normalization correctly predicts that the V1 population exhibits strong winner-take-all competition in response to sums of stimuli with different contrasts48 (FIG. 3e). When a low-contrast vertical grating is added to a high-contrast horizontal grating, the population responses mostly reflect the high-contrast grating (FIG. 3e), even though the low-contrast grating was perfectly able to elicit strong responses when presented alone (FIG. 3e). Normalization provides winner-take-all competition because the presence of multiple stimuli effectively raises the constant in the denominator, reducing the sensitivity of the neurons to the point that the weaker stimuli become unable to drive them (FIG. 3b). An exponent n > 1 strengthens this effect, but normalization can provide winner-take-all competition even when the exponent in the numerator is 1 (see FIG. 3e).

Normalization in other cortical areas

There is evidence for normalization downstream from area V1, and particularly in visual cortical area MT6264, where neurons are selective for visual motion (speed and direction). An established model of MT responses62,63 involves a summation field that operates on the population activity of V1, followed by a normalization stage. The summation field determines the selectivity for velocity, and the normalization stage helps to make this selectivity independent of spatial pattern63. The presence of normalization in MT would explain a number of suppressive phenomena that have been observed65, but it is challenging to determine whether normalization is computed de novo in MT or simply inherited from the V1 inputs64. Indeed, normalization in area V1 can profoundly shape the pattern of population activity (FIG. 3d), and thereby have strong effects on MT neurons that receive inputs from V1 (REF. 48).

Other examples of normalization in visual areas beyond V1 lie in the ventral pathway, where visual neurons are thought to perform a series of transformations that lead to object recognition66. Models of object recognition propose that information is combined from multiple features to construct object representations. In one such model67, a large set of summation fields extract features, and each successive visual cortical area integrates features by a non-linear pooling operation. The non-linear pooling operation selects the largest responses (winner-take-all) and feeds those forward to the next stage of integration. This winner-take-all competition can result from the normalization model48 (FIG. 3d), particularly when the exponent is large68 (BOX 2). Normalization, in this model, serves a role in the non-linear pooling that underlies feature integration. As predicted by such a model, there is evidence for suppression in ventral stream area V4 and the inferotemporal cortex (IT) when a non-preferred stimulus (or object) is presented with a preferred stimulus69,70. Some of the best current computer vision systems for visual object recognition follow a similar architecture as the model of the ventral visual pathway, with alternating stages of linear filtering and non-linear pooling through normalization. Normalization plays an important part in these computer vision systems as it enhances the accuracy of object recognition71.

There is also evidence for normalization in cortical areas that are devoted to other sensory modalities, and in particular in primary auditory cortex7274 (A1). It has been known for a long time that responses of A1 neurons to sounds depend on sound intensity and context. For example, the spectrotemporal receptive field provides only a rough approximation to an A1 neuron's responses, and it seems to change depending on the stimulus set that is used to measure it72,73,75. Responses of A1 neurons show a roughly logarithmic dependence on sound intensity (the commonly used decibel scale), but the responses can be shifted to the right on the logarithmic intensity axis by increasing intensity of a background noise76. These non-linear behaviours are qualitatively reminiscent of normalization: we have seen similar effects in the olfactory system (FIG. 1) and in two stages of the visual system (FIGS 23). In fact, the deviations from linearity can be characterized quantitatively by the normalization equation74. First, A1 responses increase with a measure of spectrotemporal auditory contrast that is analogous to spatiotemporal visual contrast (equations 6 and 7). Second, A1 responses saturate with auditory contrast (equation 4). Third, the responses can be suppressed by superimposing a noise stimulus that does not evoke responses in the neuron when presented alone (equation 5). Normalization may be a widespread phenomenon in auditory processing: similar divisive effects as those seen in mammalian area A1 have been characterized in the auditory midbrain of the owl77.

Normalization seems to be at work not only in individual sensory modalities but also at the stage of multisensory integration57. A prime example is provided by neurons in the cortical medial superior temporal area (MST) of primates, which are thought to integrate visual cues and vestibular cues about head, body and eye movements to estimate heading. Some of these neurons are driven more strongly by visual cues, and others are driven more strongly by vestibular cues, but many seem to have their activity modulated by both modalities. The nature of the interaction is divisive, as in equation 12. In the numerator there is a contribution by both modalities that is often biased in favour of one modality, whereas the denominator sums signals from both modalities. Normalization thus explains why neurons that integrate sensory inputs from different modalities weigh each modality depending on its strength78, why multisensory enhancement decreases with stimulus intensity (also known as the `principle of inverse effectiveness')57, and possibly why stimuli of the non-preferred modality can both drive the cell when presented alone and suppress it when presented with a stimulus of the preferred modality. We have already illustrated analogous effects of normalization in V1 (FIG. 3c).

In addition to sensory systems, there is evidence that the cortex uses normalization when encoding the value associated with different actions79,80. For example, neurons in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) of the macaque monkey encode the value of saccadic eye movements, but they are modulated by relative rather than absolute value: the neural representation of saccadic value is explicitly dependent on the values of the other available alternatives. The responses of a LIP neuron increase with the reward value (magnitude of juice reward) of a saccade target presented within the neuron's response field, and the responses decrease with the value of saccade targets simultaneously presented outside the response field. The normalization equation fits the responses to various combinations of reward values well. Other models (such as those based on absolute value or relative value computed differently from the normalization equation) do not fit the data as well. Rational choice theory assumes that options are assigned absolute values that are independent of the values of other available alternatives. However, animals and humans violate this assumption, suggesting that normalization may help to explain violations of rational choice theory79.

Normalization and visual attention

The normalization model has been extended to explain how responses in visual cortex are modulated by attention46. In the extended model, attention multiplicatively enhances the stimulus drive before normalization. As the normalization model can exhibit winner-take-all competition (FIG. 3e), even a small enhancement can provide a substantial advantage to the attended stimulus.

A typical experiment that probes spatial attention involves two spatially separate stimuli, and attention is directed towards one of them (FIG. 4a). Recordings in visual cortex indicate that the neurons that respond to the attended stimulus show stronger responses (FIG. 4b,c). To model this experiment, consider a population of neurons whose responses R(x,θ) depend on preferred position x and preferred orientation θ (equation 9). The stimulus drive D(x,θ) is largest for the neurons whose preferred positions and orientations match the stimuli (FIG. 4d). The effect of attention is simulated in the model by an array of attentional gain factors A(x,θ). This array depends on the range of attended locations and features; in the case of spatial attention (as in this example) it is narrow in space and broad in orientation (FIG. 4e). In other conditions it can instead be narrow in orientation (or other features) and broad in space (feature-based attention). The attentional gain factors multiply the stimulus drive, so that they affect both the numerator and the denominator of the normalization equation (FIG. 4f). The resulting output firing rates R(x,θ) are largest at the attended location (FIG. 4g).

Figure 4. Attentional modulation of responses in visual cortex and predictions of the normalization model of attention.

Figure 4

a | A typical attention experiment. A pair of gratings is presented, one on each side of the fixation point (shown by a dot). The task engages attention around one of the gratings (shown by a red circle). One of the gratings lies in the summation field of a recorded neuron (shown by a dashed circle). b | In some experiments, attending to the stimulus in the summation field (shown by a red curve) changes contrast gain (leftward shift) relative to attending the opposite side (shown by a blue curve). The normalization model of attention predicts this result when the attended region is large and the stimulus is small relative to the summation and suppressive fields (shown in the inset). c | In other experiments, attention changes response gain (upward scaling). The model predicts this result for large stimulus size and small attended region (shown in the inset). d | Stimulus drive D(x,θ) for a population of neurons indexed by their preference for stimulus position x (abscissa) and orientation θ (ordinate) (the grey level indicates the stimulus drive for each neuron). e | Attentional gain factors A(x,θ) when attending to the stimulus on the right (the red circle in a) without regard to orientation (light grey indicates a value of 1 and white indicates a value >1). The attentional gain factors are multiplied point-by-point (×) with the stimulus drive. f | Normalization factors N(x,θ) are computed from the result of this multiplication, by pooling over space and orientation (shown by the asterisk) through convolution with the suppressive field α(x,θ). g | The output firing rates R(x,θ) of the population can be computed by dividing the stimulus drive (÷) by the normalization factors. Figure is modified, with permission, from REF. 46 © (2009) Cell Press.

Although conceptually simple, the model can explain a seeming discrepancy in the literature about the effects of attention on responses of neurons in visual cortex (FIG. 4b–c). Different experiments have reported ostensibly conflicting outcomes46: a change in contrast gain (FIG. 4b), a change in response gain (FIG. 4c) or a combination of the two. According to the model, the discrepancy stems from differences in the size of the stimulus and the spread of attention (that is, the extent of the attentional gain factors), relative to the sizes of the summation and suppressive fields. The model predicts that attention increases contrast gain when the stimulus is small and attention is broad (FIG. 4b), because the attentional gain factors multiply the stimulus drive and the normalization factor by the same amount. The model predicts that attention increases response gain when the stimulus is large and attention is narrowly focused (FIG. 4c), because the attentional gain factors multiply the entire summation field (which appears in the numerator of the normalization equation) but only the centre of the suppressive field (in the denominator). These predictions were further confirmed by a behavioural study that systematically manipulated stimulus size and attention spread81.

Circuits and mechanisms underlying normalization

A large body of research has sought to identify the specific biophysical and cellular mechanisms that are responsible for normalization. Much of this research has focused on the role of GABA-mediated inhibition, which has often been suggested to underlie the operation of division. This emphasis has proven useful in the olfactory system of the fruitfly27 but less so in mammalian V1, where GABA inhibition seems to play little or no role in essential aspects of normalization82. Therefore, it is unlikely that a single mechanistic explanation will hold across all systems and species: what seems to be common is not necessarily the biophysical mechanism but rather the computation. Moreover, in some systems (for instance, the visual system) normalization seems to result from multiple circuits and mechanisms operating in concert and cascading across multiple stages. In the visual system, for example, contrast normalization is thought to be progressively strengthened in retina, LGN, V1 and MT. Below, we review some key mechanisms that have been proposed, with an emphasis on research performed in visual cortex, where the debate is most open.

Although there are notable exceptions (for example, much of light adaptation operates in single photoreceptors), normalization generally involves pooling a larger set of signals than those received by any single neuron. Therefore, its most useful explanations are those based on networks of neurons.

A key question concerns the basic arrangement of such networks (FIG. 5a,b). The normalization equation operates on neural signals (both in the numerator and in the denominator) that have not themselves been normalized. A simple way to obtain such signals would be through a feedforward network that taps them before they have been subjected to normalization (FIG. 5a). Such an arrangement has been proposed for the visual system of the housefly (Musca domestica)25, for the olfactory system of the fruitfly83 and for some aspects of normalization in the mammalian visual cortex84. However, normalization can also be achieved in a feedback circuit (FIG. 5b). It is well known to electrical engineers that gain control can be implemented using either a feedforward or a feedback system. A feedback circuit has been traditionally proposed for primary visual cortex, where signals in the denominator have been thought to originate from lateral feedback within V1 (REFS 19,4345) or from feedback from higher visual areas85. The origin of divisive signals is generally hard to distinguish based on responses alone, that is, unless the underlying anatomy is known and the signals themselves can be manipulated at various stages in the circuit. Some models, therefore, are agnostic as to the origin of divisive signals18,25.

Figure 5. Some networks and mechanisms that have been proposed for normalization.

Figure 5

a | The connections underlying normalization can be arranged in a feedforward manner, in which signals contributing to the denominator have not been normalized themselves. b | An alternative configuration involves feedback. The function f performs the appropriate transformation of signals so that they can be multiplied by the input, giving rise to division in steady state43,44. c | A resistor–capacitor (C) circuit and its transformation of an impulse into an exponential response. Conductance g determines both response gain and time constant. d | Effect of stimulus contrast on impulse responses of a lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) neuron. Increasing contrast (left part) causes impulse responses to be weaker and faster, both in the model (middle part) and in the data (right part). e | Synaptic depression as a mechanism for normalization. Depression changes the relationship between presynaptic current and postsynaptic current (arbitrary units) in a divisive way. f,g | Noise as a mechanism for normalization (arbitrary units). The transformation between stimulus-driven membrane potential (g) and firing rate (f) depends on signals originating from the rest of the brain in the form of `ongoing activity', modelled from the point of view of a single neuron as noise added to the membrane potential (shown by the inset Gaussian curve in g). Data in part d from REF. 35; data in part e from REF. 84; data in parts f and g from REF. 143.

A clue to the nature of divisive signals is given by their timing and sensory properties. For example, in visual cortex, normalization signals that originate near a neuron's preferred position (responsible, for example, for cross-orientation suppression (FIG. 3b)) resemble LGN responses more than V1 responses: they exhibit broad selectivity for stimulus attributes56, lack of adaptability56 and extremely short delay86. These characteristics might suggest a feedforward arrangement (FIG. 5a). Conversely, normalization signals that reach a neuron from a broader region of visual space (responsible, for example, for surround suppression (FIG. 3d)) resemble V1 responses in many ways, such as orientation selectivity59, susceptibility to adaptation87 and longer delay88. These characteristics suggest a feedback arrangement (FIG. 5b). It is possible, therefore, that phenomena such as cross-orientation suppression and surround suppression might be mediated by different mechanisms that operate in concert within the computational framework of normalization.

A second set of questions concerns the nature of the biophysical mechanisms that perform division. One of the very first proposals was shunting inhibition25,43,45. Shunting inhibition increases membrane conductance without introducing depolarizing or hyperpolarizing synaptic currents (FIG. 5c). Conductance increases could be obtained either through channels with a reversal potential close to the resting potential25,43 (for example, GABA type A (GABAA) receptors permeable to Cl ions) or by concomitant increases in excitation and inhibition, balanced so that there is an increase in conductance with no net synaptic current45. It is easy to see how conductance controls the gain of membrane potential responses, as this follows directly from Ohm's law: the membrane potential response V to a synaptic input current I is scaled by membrane conductance g as V = I / g. It is less obvious to see how conductance controls the gain of firing rate responses, because spiking itself introduces large, albeit brief, conductance increases89. It is now agreed that the effect of conductance increases on firing rates is divisive, but only if the source of increased conductance varies in time90. This variation could be achieved if the conductance changes were evoked by the noisy activity of other neurons91.

The shunting inhibition hypothesis makes a strong prediction: that normalization should affect not only the amplitude of the responses but also their time course. Increasing the conductance of a resistor–capacitor circuit such as the cellular membrane reduces not only the gain but also the time constant of the responses (FIG. 5c). The reduction in time constant is another way to reduce responsiveness, as briefer responses allow for less temporal summation. This prediction is valid in the retina during light adaptation35,92 and during contrast normalization35 (FIG. 5d). The evidence for conductance increases in normalization, however, is mixed. In V1, for example, intracellular measurements show that conductance does grow with stimulus contrast, but it is not invariant with orientation93,94 as it would be if it reflected only the strength of normalization.

More generally, inhibition seems to have a role in some but not all forms of normalization. In the olfactory system of the fruitfly, normalization seems to be due to presynaptic inhibitory connections between neurons in the antennal lobe83, because blocking inhibition with a GABA antagonist greatly reduces the suppressive effect of a mask stimulus27. However, in V1 the normalization mechanisms underlying contrast saturation (FIG. 3a) or cross-orientation suppression (FIG. 3b) do not seem to rely on GABAA inhibition: they are unaffected by blockage of GABAA receptors82. Inhibition in V1 may contribute to surround suppression95 (FIG. 3d), but this remains controversial96.

Alternative mechanisms have been proposed that could explain normalization phenomena without relying on inhibition. Some of these explanations rely on non-linearities in the afferent input27,84,97. In particular, a mechanism that could provide the appropriate non-linearity is synaptic depression98: if a synapse is engaged in transmitting both test signals and mask signals, its effectiveness is reduced in a way that resembles the divisive effect required by normalization84 (FIG. 5e). Explanations of this kind, however, can only explain divisive effects provided by the same afferents that feed the numerator of the normalization equation. In area V1, for example, they could explain phenomena of cross-orientation suppression (FIG. 3b) but not phenomena of surround suppression (FIG. 3d).

Other possible mechanisms rely on the effect of fluctuations in membrane potential on firing rate responses51 (FIG. 5f,g). The membrane potential of neurons is not only dependent on the afferent signals that are meant to drive the neuron but also on other signals originating from the rest of the brain in the form of ongoing activity99. In neurons such as those in area V1, the resulting fluctuations in membrane potential are essential in making the neuron fire: without them, many stimuli would evoke membrane potential fluctuations that are too small to reach spike threshold100,101. Consequently, the amplitude of ongoing activity controls the responsiveness of the neurons. The ongoing activity is weaker in V1 when stimulus contrast increases51, so the neurons become less responsive, mimicking divisive suppression.

Finally, an intriguing possibility is that normalization in some systems may rely on amplification rather than suppression. In visual cortex, for example, a canonical microcircuit has been proposed1,2 to amplify and shape responses inherited from the LGN. This circuit and more recently proposed circuits, such as one centred on balanced amplification102, suggest an alternative path to normalization. Instead of increasing normalization by increasing suppression, these circuits may increase normalization by decreasing amplification.

Behavioural evidence for normalization

Psychophysical studies of visual pattern perception have paralleled research on the neurophysiological response properties of neurons in the visual cortex. The prevailing view has been that judgments about pattern discrimination and pattern appearance are limited by neural signals in early visual cortical areas such as area V1. Consistent with this view, it has been possible to use normalization equations that summarize responses of V1 neurons to make specific predictions about human perception.

The simultaneous contrast–contrast illusion, for example, is created by surrounding a central texture patch with a textured background103. When the central texture patch is surrounded by a high-contrast pattern, the bright points of the central patch appear dimmer and, simultaneously, the dark points appear lighter. To explain this illusion with the normalization model, we must adopt a decision rule, a testable hypothesis that predicts behavioural performance from the pooled activity of a population of neurons. It has been proposed that perceived contrast is a monotonic function of the average activity of neurons with summation fields centred on the patch. The normalization model suggests that each of these neurons is suppressed by the pooled activity of a larger number of neurons, including those with summation fields in a surrounding spatial region. The responses of these surrounding neurons increase with background contrast. Hence, when there is a high-contrast background the suppression is stronger, and perceived contrast of the central texture patch is lower. Indeed, the normalization model has been used to explain the appearance of this illusion104.

The psychophysics of superimposed and surround suppression parallels the physiology. Surround suppression, as measured behaviourally, depends on whether the orientations and spatial frequencies of target and surround are matched103106, unlike a superimposed mask for which the suppression is largely nonspecific107. Surround suppression, as measured behaviourally, is also slightly delayed, unlike a superimposed mask for which the suppression is immediate106. Both of these results parallel the physiological findings86,88.

Analogous behavioural markers of normalization should be measurable in the other domains, for example, olfactory behaviour in the fruitfly while manipulating the concentrations of two or more odorants, and choice behaviour in human and non-human primates while manipulating the reward values of two or more alternative options.

Deficits in computations related to normalization have been linked with amblyopia108110, epilepsy111,112, major depression113,114 and schizophrenia67,115121. This suggests the possibility that the origin of these brain disorders may lie not in a particular brain area or system (such as the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia), but instead in computational deficits, and that normalization may be one of the fundamental computations that is compromised in these disorders.

Discussion

We have seen that divisive normalization is a widespread computation in disparate sensory systems, and that it may also play a part in cognitive systems (for example, those that encode value). Why is normalization so widespread? A tempting answer would be to see it as a natural outcome of a very common mechanism or network; a canonical neural circuit. However, there seem to be many circuits and mechanisms underlying normalization and they are not necessarily the same across species and systems. Consequently, we propose that the answer has to do with computation, not mechanism. Normalization is thought to bring multiple functional benefits to the computations that are performed by neural systems (BOX 2). Some of these benefits may be more important for some neural systems than for others.

Some of the literature that we have reviewed concerns aspects of normalization that are still the subject of intense research. A key set of questions concerns circuits and mechanisms that result in normalization. As reviewed above, these circuits and mechanisms are understood for some systems but not for others. Understanding these circuits and mechanisms is fundamental, especially if, as suggested earlier, deficits in normalization are indeed at the root of psychiatric, neurological or developmental disorders.

Another question for further exploration is the degree to which normalization resembles — in computational terms or in underlying circuitry — the more general non-linear interactions that constitute `gain modulation'. Gain modulation is the multiplicative control of one neuron's responses by the responses of another set of neurons. It arises in a wide range of contexts122, including in the interaction of proprioceptive and visual signals in the parietal cortex15,123, in the coordinate transformation needed for visually guided reaching15,122,123 and in invariant object recognition122. A variety of studies address the computational advantages of gain modulation15,124,125 and its possible underlying biophysical mechanisms90,91,122,126. In its simpler forms, normalization is a special case of gain modulation in that the signals controlling gain are a superset of the signals that determine the responses. However, when the normalization model is expanded to include distinct gain factors, as in the normalization model of attention, it incorporates more general aspects of gain modulation.

Because normalization is a computation that is repeated modularly in a number of brain systems, we propose that it should be considered a canonical neural computation. Under this proposal, normalization should be added to a short list of known canonical neural computations. These include not only two well-established computations that we have already mentioned — linear filtering and exponentiation — but also other computations, such as recurrent amplification, associative learning rules, coincidence detection, population vectors and constrained trajectories in dynamical systems. Identifying and characterizing more modular computations of this kind will provide a toolbox for developing a principled understanding of brain function.

Understanding canonical neural computations could help us to understand brain function in a number of ways. First, it would provide a single language to describe the functional specialization of different brain areas. Second, as we have discussed above, a computational understanding of normalization provides a platform for characterizing behaviour and for characterizing cellular mechanisms. Finally, understanding canonical neural computations such as normalization may also shed light on psychiatric, neurological and developmental disorders. If this hypothesis is correct for some of these disorders, then elucidation of these neural computations, and of the underlying mechanisms and microcircuits, is a fundamental mission.

Online at-a-glance summary

  • Normalization computes a ratio between the response of an individual neuron and the summed activity of a pool of neurons.

  • The normalization model was developed to explain responses in the primary visual cortex (V1), and has been seen to operate in a variety of other regions of the visual system: light adaptation in the retina, contrast normalization in the retina and lateral geniculate nucleus, and visual processing in higher visual cortical areas beyond V1.

  • Normalization has also been proposed to be at the root of the modulatory effects of visual attention on neural responses in visual cortex.

  • Normalization is seen in multiple species and brain regions. These include olfactory processing and representation in the fruitfly antennal lobe, the encoding of value in posterior parietal cortex, multisensory integration of visual motion and vestibular signals, and auditory processing in primary auditory cortex.

  • A variety of different (feedforward and feedback) neural circuits and mechanisms might perform normalization, including presynaptic inhibition, shunting inhibition, synaptic depression, changes in the amplitude of ongoing activity and balanced amplification.

  • The effects of normalization can be measured behaviourally.

  • The computational benefits of normalization include maximizing sensitivity, providing invariance with respect to some stimulus dimensions at the expense of others, facilitating the decoding of a distributed neural representation, facilitating the discrimination among representations of different stimuli, providing max-pooling (winner-take-all competition) and reducing redundancy.

  • Understanding canonical neural computations such as normalization may shed light on psychiatric, neurological and developmental disorders.

Box 1 | The normalization equation.

The normalization model is defined by a simple equation, the normalization equation. This equation specifies how the normalized response Rj of neuron j depends on its inputs Dk (which are not normalized):

Ri=γDjnσn+ΣkDkn (10)

The numerator is the neuron's driving input Dj; in sensory systems, this driving input provides the stimulus drive to the responses. Its units depend on the system under study. They could be in units of stimulus intensity in a sensory system, or in spikes per s if the input is considered to arise from another neuron. The denominator is a constant σ plus the normalization factor, which is the sum of a large number of inputs Dk, the normalization pool. The constants γ, σ and n constitute free parameters that are typically fit to empirical measurements: γ determines overall responsiveness, σ prevents division by zero and determines how responses saturate with increasing driving input, and n is an exponent that amplifies the individual inputs.

This operation is called `normalization' by analogy to normalizing a vector. In this vector, each element of the vector is one of the inputs (Dj) and the exponent is n = 2. If the normalization factor is the same for all neurons in the population, then the activity of the whole population of neurons is scaled (normalized) by the same number.

In a sensory system, the normalization equation is often used in combination with a summation field (or linear receptive field) that determines the neuron's selectivity for stimulus attributes. In that case the stimulus drive Dj to neuron j is taken to be a weighted sum of sensory inputs:

Dj=ΣkwjkIk (11)

Here, the Ik are activities of afferent neurons, and the weights wjk specify the summation field of neuron j. A number of variations of the normalization equation have been applied to model different systems:

Different inputs Dk can be assigned different weights αjk in the normalization pool. These weights define a suppressive field. The suppressive field may differ across neurons (hence the subscriptj). For example, neurons in primary visual cortex whose summation fields are centred on different spatial locations would have suppressive fields that are centred at corresponding locations.

A baseline response β can be added to the numerator (to account for the spontaneous activity of the inputs). Doing so allows the normalization pool to affect not only the driven responses but also the resting (spontaneous) activity of the neuron.

The exponent n can be the same in the numerator and denominator, as in equation 1, and it is applied to the individual inputs before being pooled. The exponent can also be applied to the output of the entire normalization pool, or different exponents m, n and p can be used in the numerator and denominator.

The normalization signal can be averaged over a period of recent time to model adaptation, such as light adaptation in retina or contrast adaptation in visual cortex. Combining these variations results in the following equation:

Rj=γ(ΣkwjkIk)n+βσn+(ΣkαjkIkm)p (12)

This expression encompasses various implementations of the model that have been found to characterize different neural systems. When applying the model to a given neural system, however, it is best to strive for simplicity and have as few parameters as possible — for example, as in equation 1.

A typical way to characterize systems exhibiting normalization is to fit the normalization equation to the responses of a neuron to stimuli that drive that neuron, to stimuli that drive other nearby neurons, and to various combinations of these stimuli. More recent approaches involve fitting the equation to the responses of a large population of neurons at once48.

Box 2 | Normalization and neural coding.

Theoreticians have offered several (not mutually exclusive) rationales for normalization, most of which are related to coding efficiency.

Maximizing sensitivity. Normalization adjusts the gain of the neural responses to efficiently use the available dynamic range, maximizing sensitivity to changes in input. Light adaptation in the retina enables high sensitivity to subtle changes in visual features over a huge range of intensities (FIG. 2). Normalizing reward values yields a representation that can distinguish one dollar from two dollars and one million dollars from two million dollars, that is, widening the effective dynamic range of the reward system.

Invariance with respect to some stimulus dimensions. Normalization in the antennal lobe of the fly is thought to enable odorant recognition and discrimination regardless of concentration27,127. Normalization in retina discards information about the mean light level to maintain invariant representations of other visual features35,128 (for example, contrast). Normalization in V1 discards information about contrast to encode image pattern (for example, orientation)17,19,48,49 (FIG. 3e), optimizing discriminability regardless of contrast129. Normalization in the visual cortical area MT is thought to encode velocity independent of spatial pattern62,63. Normalization in the ventral visual pathway may contribute to object representations that are invariant to changes in size, location, lighting and occlusion68.

Decoding a distributed neural representation. Visual area MT is thought to encode visual motion in the responses of a population of neurons tuned for different speeds and directions. These responses can be interpreted as discrete samples of a probability density function in which the firing rate of each neuron is proportional to a probability; the mean of the distribution estimates stimulus velocity and the variance of the distribution measures the uncertainty in that estimate63. Mean and variance can be computed simply as weighted sums of the firing rates if they are normalized to sum to a constant, the same constant for any stimulus.

Discriminating among stimuli. Normalization can make the neural representations of different stimuli more readily discriminable by a linear classifier27,127,129,130. The response of n neurons to a stimulus is a point in n-dimensional space. The points belonging to similar stimuli cluster together. A linear classifier discriminates stimulus categories by drawing hyperplanes between the clusters. This is difficult if some stimuli evoke strong responses (points far from the origin) but others evoke weak responses (points near the origin); the hyperplane that defines the boundary far from the origin can fail near the origin, and vice versa. Normalization prevents this problem from arising.

Max-pooling (winner-take-all). Normalization can cause a neuronal population to operate in two regimes, averaging the inputs when these are approximately equal and computing a winner-take-all competition (max-pooling, selecting the maximum of inputs) when one input is considerably larger than the rest48 (FIG. 3e). Max-pooling is thought to operate in multiple neural systems and to underlie perceptual decisions by selecting the neuronal subpopulation (or psychophysical channel) with the largest responses131135. Models of object recognition propose multiple stages of max-pooling (winner-take-all)67. Attention might interact with normalization to shift the computation from averaging to max-pooling, thus selecting the subpopulation with largest responses and suppressing the rest, similar to the `biased competition' model136.

Redundancy reduction. A neural representation is thought to be inefficient when its outputs are redundant137139. Normalization can reduce this redundancy. Normalization contributes to statistical independence of V1 responses to natural images140,141. Natural images tend to be similar at nearby locations (for example, along a contour). Consequently, the response of a V1 summation field can be estimated from the responses of V1 summation fields that are selective for nearby locations. Normalization removes the dependence, making the representation more efficient140,141. Likewise, responses of the population of antennal lobe neurons (post-normalization) in the fly olfactory system are more statistically independent than are the (pre-normalization) olfactory receptor neurons27,127.

Acknowledgements

We thank K. Harris, E. Simoncelli, J. Linden, R. Wilson and F. Rieke for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by an Advanced Investigator award from the European Research Council (to M.C.) and by US National Institutes of Health grants R01-EY016752 and R01-EY019693 (to D.J.H.). M.C. holds the GlaxoSmithKline/Fight for Sight Chair in Visual Neuroscience.

Biographies

Matteo Carandini is the GlaxoSmithKline/Fight for Sight Chair in Visual Neuroscience at University College London, UK. His research uses a variety of electrical and optical methods to characterize the computations performed by neural circuits, the interplay between sensory-driven activity and ongoing activity in visual cortex, and how neural activity supports visually guided behaviour. He obtained an M.Sc. in mathematics from the University of Rome, Italy (1990) and a Ph.D. in neural science, from New York University, USA (1996). He carried out postdoctoral work at Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA, and worked as an assistant professor at the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology, Zurich, Switzerland (1998–2002) and as a research scientist in the Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California, USA (2002–2006). He received a McKnight Scholar award in 2005 and was made a European Research Council Advanced Investigator in 2009. Since 2011 he has been a Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator.

David J. Heeger is Professor of Psychology and Neural Science at New York University. His research spans an interdisciplinary cross-section of engineering, psychology and neuroscience, using functional MRI and computational theory to quantitatively investigate brain and behaviour. He received a B.A. in mathematics (1983) and a Ph.D. in computer science from the University of Pennsylvania, USA (1987). He carried out postdoctoral work at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), USA, and has worked as a research scientist at the US National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) Ames Research Center and as an assistant and then associate professor at Stanford University, California, USA (1991–2002). In 1987 he received the David Marr Prize in computer vision and in 1994 the Alfred P. Sloan Fellowship in neuroscience. He has also been awarded the Troland Award in psychology from the National Academy of Sciences (2002) and the Sokol Award in the Sciences from New York University (2006).

Footnotes

Competing interests statement The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Attention The cognitive process of selecting one of many possible stimuli or events. In this Review, we focus on visuospatial attention. Rigorous methods have been developed for quantifying the effects of attention on performance.

Local contrast (Also known as Weber contrast). An image that is obtained by subtracting the intensity at each location by the mean intensity averaged over a nearby region and dividing the result by that mean intensity.

Summation field A region of sensory space that provides drive to a neuron. In many sensory systems, neurons derive their stimulus selectivity from a weighted sum of sensory inputs. The summation field comprises the weights in this sum.

Suppressive field A region of sensory space providing suppression. In the normalization model, responses are suppressed by a weighted sum of activity of a population of neurons. The suppressive field comprises the weights in this weighted sum.

Grating contrast (Also known as Michelson contrast). The contrast of a grating is given by twice the mean intensity minus the lowest intensity divided by the highest intensity. This is often expressed as a percentage. A 100% contrast grating is one in which the black bars have zero intensity.

Response saturation Neural responses that increase with the strength of the input but progressively level off with very strong inputs. Normalization controls the strength at which responses saturate.

Normalization factor A weighed sum of activity of a population of neurons, as determined by the suppressive field.

Winner-take-all A neural computation in which the response depends on the maximum of the inputs.

MT (Middle temporal area). Primate cortical area in which most neurons are selective for speed and direction of visual motion.

V4 Primate visual cortical area in which neurons respond selectively to combinations of visual features. The modulatory effects of attention on neural activity have been extensively studied in V4.

IT (Inferotemporal cortex). A region of primate cortex in which neurons respond selectively to pictures of objects, faces and complex combinations of visual features.

Spectrotemporal receptive field The receptive field of auditory neurons, which is typically defined in terms of sound frequency and time.

MST (medial superior temporal area). Primate cortical area in which neurons combine information about visual motion, head movements and eye movements.

Heading Trajectory of movement through the environment.

Lateral intraparietal area Primate cortical area in which neural activity depends on visual input, eye movements, attention to visual input, intention to make an eye movement and factors that affect when and where to move the eyes (including the expected probability and magnitude of a reward).

Rational choice theory Economic model of decision making.

Attentional gain factors A key component of the normalization model of attention. Each attentional gain factor corresponds to a particular spatial location and sensory feature, and has a value that is larger than one when that location and/or feature is attended.

Gain control Change in gain that multiplies or divides the amplitude of the response to an input.

Ongoing activity Fluctuations in neural activity in the absence of any change in sensory inputs or task demands.

References

  • 1.Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D. A functional microcircuit for cat visual cortex. J. Physiol. 1991;440:735–769. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Douglas RJ, Koch C, Mahowald M, Martin KAC, Suarez HH. Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits. Science. 1995;269:981–985. doi: 10.1126/science.7638624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wang XJ. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron. 2002;36:955–968. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01092-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hanes DP, Schall JD. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science. 1996;274:427–430. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5286.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lo CC, Wang XJ. Cortico-basal ganglia circuit mechanism for a decision threshold in reaction time tasks. Nature Neurosci. 2006;9:956–963. doi: 10.1038/nn1722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cisek P. Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: a computational model. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:9761–9770. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5605-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Priebe NJ, Ferster D. Inhibition, spike threshold, and stimulus selectivity in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2008;57:482–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wiechert MT, Judkewitz B, Riecke H, Friedrich RW. Mechanisms of pattern decorrelation by recurrent neuronal circuits. Nature Neurosci. 2010;13:1003–1010. doi: 10.1038/nn.2591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Smith PL, Ratcliff R. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stanford TR, Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Costello MG, Salinas E. Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neurosci. 2010;13:379–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.2485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Carandini M, et al. Do we know what the early visual system does? J. Neurosci. 2005;25:10577–10597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Depireux DA, Simon JZ, Klein DJ, Shamma SA. Spectro-temporal response field characterization with dynamic ripples in ferret primary auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2001;85:1220–1234. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.DiCarlo JJ, Johnson KO. Receptive field structure in cortical area 3b of the alert monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;135:167–178. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00162-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Graham NVS. Visual Pattern Analyzers. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pouget A, Snyder LH. Computational approaches to sensorimotor transformations. Nature Neurosci. 2000;3:1192–1198. doi: 10.1038/81469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bizzi E, Giszter SF, Loeb E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Saltiel P. Modular organization of motor behavior in the frog's spinal cord. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18:442–446. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)94494-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Heeger DJ. In: Computational Models of Visual Processing. Landy M, Movshon JA, editors. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 1991. pp. 119–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Albrecht DG, Geisler WS. Motion sensitivity and the contrast-response function of simple cells in the visual cortex. Vis. Neurosci. 1991;7:531–546. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800010336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Heeger DJ. Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex. Vis. Neurosci. 1992;9:181–197. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800009640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study introduced the normalization model and showed through simulations that it could explain numerous properties of neurons in primary visual cortex.
  • 20.Grossberg S. Nonlinear neural networks: principles, mechanisms and architectures. Neural Netw. 1988;1:17–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Naka KI, Rushton WA. S-potentials from luminosity units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae) J. Physiol. 1966;185:587–599. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Baylor DA, Fuortes MGF. Electrical responses of single cones in the retina of the turtle. J. Physiol. 1970;297:77–92. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Boynton RM, Whitten DN. Visual adaptation in monkey cones: recordings of late receptor potentials. Science. 1970;170:1423–1426. doi: 10.1126/science.170.3965.1423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Normann RA, Perlman I. The effects of background illumination on the photoresponses of red and green cones. J. Physiol. 1979;286:491–507. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Reichardt W, Poggio T, Hausen K. Figure-ground discrimination by relative movement in the visual system of the fly. Part II. Towards the neural circuitry. Biol. Cybern. 1983;46:1–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McNaughton BL, Morris RGM. Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system. Trends Neurosci. 1987;10:408–415. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Olsen SR, Bhandawat V, Wilson RI. Divisive normalization in olfactory population codes. Neuron. 2010;66:287–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study demonstrated normalization in the fly olfactory system, and showed how it may benefit the population code for odours (see also REF. 127).
  • 28.Rodieck RW. The First Steps in Seeing. Sinauer; Sunderland, Massachusetts: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rieke F, Rudd ME. The challenges natural images pose for visual adaptation. Neuron. 2009;64:605–616. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mante V, Frazor RA, Bonin V, Geisler WS, Carandini M. Independence of luminance and contrast in natural scenes and in the early visual system. Nature Neurosci. 2005;8:1690–1697. doi: 10.1038/nn1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Burkhardt DA. Light adaptation and photopigment bleaching in cone photoreceptors in situ in the retina of the turtle. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:1091–1105. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01091.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Schneeweis DM, Schnapf JL. The photovoltage of macaque cone photoreceptors: adaptation, noise, and kinetics. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:1203–1216. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-04-01203.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Shapley RM, Enroth-Cugell C. In: Progress in Retinal Research. Osborne N, Chader G, editors. Vol. 3. Pergamon; Oxford, UK: 1984. pp. 263–346. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Laughlin S. A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron's information capacity. Z. Naturforsch. C. 1981;36:910–912. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mante V, Bonin V, Carandini M. Functional mechanisms shaping lateral geniculate responses to artificial and natural stimuli. Neuron. 2008;58:625–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Shapley RM, Victor JD. The effect of contrast on the transfer properties of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. 1978;285:275–298. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Shapley RM, Victor J. How the contrast gain modifies the frequency responses of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. 1981;318:161–179. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Baccus SA, Meister M. Fast and slow contrast adaptation in retinal circuitry. Neuron. 2002;36:909–919. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Demb JB. Multiple mechanisms for contrast adaptation in the retina. Neuron. 2002;36:781–783. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01100-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bonin V, Mante V, Carandini M. The suppressive field of neurons in lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:10844–10856. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3562-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study extended the normalization model to the responses of subcortical neurons, and demonstrated its similarity with earlier models of retinal contrast gain control.
  • 41.Beaudoin DL, Borghuis BG, Demb JB. Cellular basis for contrast gain control over the receptive field center of mammalian retinal ganglion cells. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2636–2645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4610-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bonin V, Mante V, Carandini M. The statistical computation underlying contrast gain control. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:6346–6353. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0284-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Movshon JA. Linearity and normalization in simple cells of the macaque primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:8621–8644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08621.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study measured responses of neurons in primary visual cortex of primates using stimuli expressly designed to test the normalization model, and found that the model provided good quantitative fits. This study also formalized the resistor–capacitor model of normalization (see REF. 45).
  • 44.Heeger DJ. Modeling simple cell direction selectivity with normalized, half-squared, linear operators. J. Neurophysiol. 1993;70:1885–1897. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.5.1885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Carandini M, Heeger DJ. Summation and division by neurons in primate visual cortex. Science. 1994;264:1333–1336. doi: 10.1126/science.8191289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ. The normalization model of attention. Neuron. 2009;61:168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study showed how the normalization model can be extended to account for the modulatory effects of visual attention.
  • 47.Sit YF, Chen Y, Geisler WS, Miikkulainen R, Seidemann E. Complex dynamics of V1 population responses explained by a simple gain-control model. Neuron. 2009;64:943–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Busse L, Wade AR, Carandini M. Representation of concurrent stimuli by population activity in visual cortex. Neuron. 2009;64:931–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study showed that the normalization model can quantitatively describe the combined activity of large populations of neurons in V1, and that the responses (and the model) can exhibit winner-take-all behaviour. It also demonstrated normalization in responses from human area V1.
  • 49.Albrecht DG, Hamilton DB. Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function. J. Neurophysiol. 1982;48:217–237. doi: 10.1152/jn.1982.48.1.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sclar G, Maunsell JHR, Lennie P. Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey. Vision Res. 1990;30:1–10. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Finn IM, Priebe NJ, Ferster D. The emergence of contrast-invariant orientation tuning in simple cells of cat visual cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:137–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sclar G, Freeman RD. Orientation selectivity in the cat's striate cortex is invariant with stimulus contrast. Exp. Brain Res. 1982;46:457–461. doi: 10.1007/BF00238641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bauman LA, Bonds AB. Inhibitory refinement of spatial frequency selectivity in single cells of the cat striate cortex. Vision Res. 1991;31:933–944. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90201-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bonds AB. Role of inhibition in the specification of orientation selectivity of cells in the cat striate cortex. Vis. Neurosci. 1989;2:41–55. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800004314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Morrone MC, Burr DC, Maffei L. Functional implications of cross-orientation inhibition of cortical visual cells. I. Neurophysiological evidence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Sci. 1982;216:335–354. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1982.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Freeman TC, Durand S, Kiper DC. Carandini, M. Suppression without inhibition in visual cortex. Neuron. 2002;35:759–771. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00819-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ohshiro T, Angelaki DE, Deangelis GC. A normalization model of multisensory integration. Nature Neurosci. 2011;14:775–782. doi: 10.1038/nn.2815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study summarized decades of research in multisensory integration and showed that numerous aspects of the responses of multisensory neurons could be described by the normalization model.
  • 58.Cavanaugh JR, Bair W, Movshon JA. Selectivity and spatial distribution of signals from the receptive field surround in macaque V1 neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:2547–2556. doi: 10.1152/jn.00693.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Cavanaugh JR, Bair W, Movshon JA. Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:2530–2546. doi: 10.1152/jn.00692.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kapadia MK, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD. Dynamics of spatial summation in primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:12073–12078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.21.12073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sceniak MP, Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley R. Contrast's effect on spatial summation by macaque V1 neurons. Nature Neurosci. 1999;2:733–739. doi: 10.1038/11197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Heeger DJ, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA. Computational models of cortical visual processing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1996;93:623–627. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Simoncelli EP, Heeger DJ. A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT. Vision Res. 1998;38:743–761. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00183-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rust NC, Mante V, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA. How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns. Nature Neurosci. 2006;9:1421–1431. doi: 10.1038/nn1786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Britten KH, Heuer HW. Spatial summation in the receptive fields of MT neurons. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:5074–5084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-12-05074.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Zoccolan D, Kouh M, Poggio T, DiCarlo JJ. Trade-off between object selectivity and tolerance in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:12292–12307. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1897-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nature Neurosci. 1999;2:1019–1025. doi: 10.1038/14819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kouh M, Poggio T. A canonical neural circuit for cortical nonlinear operations. Neural Comput. 2008;20:1427–1451. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.02-07-466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Reynolds JH, Desimone R. Interacting roles of attention and visual salience in V4. Neuron. 2003;37:853–863. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00097-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zoccolan D, Cox DD, DiCarlo JJ. Multiple object response normalization in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:8150–8164. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2058-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Jarrett K, Kavukcuoglu K, Ranzato MA, LeCun Y. What is the best multi-stage architecture for object recognition?; Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. Comput. Vis; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.David SV, Mesgarani N, Fritz JB, Shamma SA. Rapid synaptic depression explains nonlinear modulation of spectro-temporal tuning in primary auditory cortex by natural stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:3374–3386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5249-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Ahrens MB, Linden JF, Sahani M. Nonlinearities and contextual influences in auditory cortical responses modeled with multilinear spectrotemporal methods. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:1929–1942. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3377-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rabinowitz NC, Willmore BD, Schnupp JW, King AJ. Contrast gain control in auditory cortex. Neuron. 2011;70:1178–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study demonstrated that the responses of neurons in the auditory cortex share many similarities with those in the visual cortex and that these can be described by the normalization equation.
  • 75.Blake DT, Merzenich MM. Changes of AI receptive fields with sound density. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:3409–3420. doi: 10.1152/jn.00233.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Phillips DP. Neural representation of sound amplitude in the auditory cortex: effects of noise masking. Behav. Brain Res. 1990;37:197–214. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90132-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Asadollahi A, Mysore SP, Knudsen EI. Rules of competitive stimulus selection in a cholinergic isthmic nucleus of the owl midbrain. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:6088–6097. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0023-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Morgan ML, Deangelis GC, Angelaki DE. Multisensory integration in macaque visual cortex depends on cue reliability. Neuron. 2008;59:662–673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Glimcher P. Foundations of Neuroeconomic Analysis. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Louie K, Grattan LE, Glimcher PW. Reward value-based gain control: divisive normalization in parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:10627–10639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1237-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Herrmann K, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ. When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nature Neurosci. 2010;13:1544–1559. doi: 10.1038/nn.2669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Katzner S, Busse L, Carandini M. GABAa inhibition controls response gain in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:5931–5941. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5753-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Olsen SR, Wilson RI. Lateral presynaptic inhibition mediates gain control in an olfactory circuit. Nature. 2008;452:956–960. doi: 10.1038/nature06864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Senn W. A synaptic explanation of suppression in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:10053–10065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10053.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Angelucci A, Levitt JB, Lund JS. Anatomical origins of the classical receptive field and modulatory surround field of single neurons in macaque visual cortical area V1. Prog. Brain Res. 2002;136:373–388. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(02)36031-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Smith MA, Bair W, Movshon JA. Dynamics of suppression in macaque primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:4826–4834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5542-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Durand S, Freeman TC, Carandini M. Temporal properties of surround suppression in cat primary visual cortex. Vis. Neurosci. 2007;24:679–690. doi: 10.1017/S0952523807070563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Bair W, Cavanaugh JR, Movshon JA. Time course and time-distance relationships for surround suppression in macaque V1 neurons. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:7690–7701. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-20-07690.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Holt GR, Koch C. Shunting inhibition does not have a divisive effect on firing rates. Neural Comput. 1997;9:1001–1013. doi: 10.1162/neco.1997.9.5.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Silver RA. Neuronal arithmetic. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:474–489. doi: 10.1038/nrn2864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron. 2002;35:773–782. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00820-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Sperling G, Sondhi MM. Model for visual luminance discrimination and flicker detection. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 1968;58:1133–1145. doi: 10.1364/josa.58.001133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Anderson J, Carandini M, Ferster D. Orientation tuning of input conductance, excitation and inhibition in cat primary visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;84:909–931. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Monier C, Chavane F, Baudot P, Graham LJ, Fregnac Y. Orientation and direction selectivity of synaptic inputs in visual cortical neurons: a diversity of combinations produces spike tuning. Neuron. 2003;37:663–680. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00064-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Haider B, et al. Synaptic and network mechanisms of sparse and reliable visual cortical activity during nonclassical receptive field stimulation. Neuron. 2010;65:107–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Ozeki H, Finn IM, Schaffer ES, Miller KD, Ferster D. Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression. Neuron. 2009;62:578–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Priebe NJ, Ferster D. Mechanisms underlying cross-orientation suppression in cat visual cortex. Nature Neurosci. 2006;9:552–561. doi: 10.1038/nn1660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Abbott LF, Varela JA, Sen K, Nelson SB. Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science. 1997;275:220–224. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Ringach DL. Spontaneous and driven cortical activity: implications for computation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009;19:439–444. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Carandini M. Amplification of trial-to-trial response variability by neurons in visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 2004;2:e264. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Ringach DL, Malone BJ. The operating point of the cortex: neurons as large deviation detectors. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:7673–7683. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1048-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Murphy BK, Miller KD. Balanced amplification: a new mechanism of selective amplification of neural activity patterns. Neuron. 2009;61:635–648. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Chubb C, Sperling G, Solomon JA. Texture interactions determine perceived contrast. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1989;86:9631–9635. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.23.9631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Xing J, Heeger DJ. Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement. Vision Res. 2001;41:571–583. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00270-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Solomon JA, Sperling G, Chubb C. The lateral inhibition of perceived contrast is indifferent to on-center/off-center segregation, but specific to orientation. Vision Res. 1993;33:2671–2683. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90227-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Petrov Y, Carandini M, McKee S. Two distinct mechanisms of suppression in human vision. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:8704–8707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2871-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Foley JM. Human luminance pattern-vision mechanisms: masking experiments require a new model. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 1994;11:1710–1719. doi: 10.1364/josaa.11.001710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Polat U, Sagi D, Norcia AM. Abnormal long-range spatial interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res. 1997;37:737–744. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00154-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Ellemberg D, Hess RF, Arsenault AS. Lateral interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2002;42:2471–2478. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00227-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Heimel JA, Saiepour MH, Chakravarthy S, Hermans JM, Levelt CN. Contrast gain control and cortical TrkB signaling shape visual acuity. Nature Neurosci. 2010;13:642–648. doi: 10.1038/nn.2534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Porciatti V, Bonanni P, Fiorentini A, Guerrini R. Lack of cortical contrast gain control in human photosensitive epilepsy. Nature Neurosci. 2000;3:259–263. doi: 10.1038/72972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Tsai JJ, Norcia AM, Wade A. American Epilepsy Society 63rd Annual Meeting. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Bubl E, Tebartz Van Elst L, Gondan M, Ebert D, Greenlee MW. Vision in depressive disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;10:377–384. doi: 10.1080/15622970701513756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Golomb JD, et al. Enhanced visual motion perception in major depressive disorder. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:9072–9077. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1003-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Must A, Janka Z, Benedek G, Keri S. Reduced facilitation effect of collinear flankers on contrast detection reveals impaired lateral connectivity in the visual cortex of schizophrenia patients. Neurosci. Lett. 2004;357:131–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Yoon JH, et al. Diminished orientation-specific surround suppression of visual processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:1078–1084. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Yoon JH, et al. GABA concentration is reduced in visual cortex in schizophrenia and correlates with orientation-specific surround suppression. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:3777–3781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6158-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Butler PD, et al. Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:495–504. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Dakin S, Carlin P, Hemsley D. Weak suppression of visual context in chronic schizophrenia. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:R822–R824. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Tadin D, et al. Weakened center-surround interactions in visual motion processing in schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:11403–11412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2592-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Butler PD, Silverstein SM, Dakin SC. Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;64:40–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Salinas E, Thier P. Gain modulation: a major computational principle of the central nervous system. Neuron. 2000;27:15–21. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J. Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1997;20:303–330. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Deneve S, Pouget A. Basis functions for object-centered representations. Neuron. 2003;37:347–359. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01184-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Salinas E. Fast remapping of sensory stimuli onto motor actions on the basis of contextual modulation. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:1113–1118. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4569-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Kepecs A, Raghavachari S. Gating information by two-state membrane potential fluctuations. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3015–3023. doi: 10.1152/jn.01242.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Luo SX, Axel R, Abbott LF. Generating sparse and selective third-order responses in the olfactory system of the fly. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:10713–10718. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005635107. doi:10.1073/pnas.1005635107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study explored the benefits of normalization in the fly olfactory system (see also REF. 27) in terms of coding for different odours
  • 128.Troy JB, Enroth-Cugell C. X and Y ganglion cells inform the cat's brain about contrast in the retinal image. Exp. Brain Res. 1993;93:383–390. doi: 10.1007/BF00229354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Ringach DL. Population coding under normalization. Vision Res. 2010;50:2223–2232. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.DiCarlo JJ, Cox DD. Untangling invariant object recognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:333–341. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Shaw ML. In: Attention and Performance VIII. Nickerson RS, editor. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, New Jersey: 1980. pp. 277–296. [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Palmer J. Attention in visual search: Distinguishing four causes of a set-size effect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1995;4:118–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Palmer J, Verghese P, Pavel M. The psychophysics of visual search. Vision Res. 2000;40:1227–1268. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00244-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Verghese P. Visual search and attention: a signal detection theory approach. Neuron. 2001;31:523–535. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00392-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Baldassi S, Megna N, Burr DC. Visual clutter causes high-magnitude errors. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e56. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Simoncelli EP, Olshausen BA. Natural image statistics and neural representation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:1193–1216. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Barlow HB. In: Sensory communication. Rosenblith WA, editor. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 1961. pp. 217–234. [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Geisler WS. Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008;59:167–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Lyu S, Simoncelli EP. Nonlinear extraction of independent components of natural images using radial gaussianization. Neural Comput. 2009;21:1485–1519. doi: 10.1162/neco.2009.04-08-773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Schwartz O, Simoncelli EP. Natural signal statistics and sensory gain control. Nature Neurosci. 2001;4:819–825. doi: 10.1038/90526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; This study showed how normalization can reduce redundancy in a population. Without normalization, responses of pairs of neurons in primary visual cortex to natural images would not be independent. Normalization makes them more independent.
  • 142.Carandini M. In: The Cognitive Neurosciences. Gazzaniga MS, editor. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 2004. pp. 313–326. [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Carandini M. Melting the iceberg: contrast invariance in visual cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:11–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES