Abstract
Our recent work showed that close relationships result in shared cognitive and neural representations of the self and one’s mother in collectivistic individuals (Zhu et al., 2007, Neuroimage, 34, 1310–7). However, it remains unknown whether close others, such as mother, father and best friend, are differentially represented in collectivistic brains. Here, using functional magnetic resonance imaging and a trait judgment task, we showed evidence that, while trait judgments of the self and mother generated comparable activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and anterior cingulate (ACC) of Chinese adults, trait judgments of mother induced greater MPFC/ACC activity than trait judgments of father and best friend. Our results suggest that, while neural representations of the self and mother overlapped in the MPFC/ACC, close others such as mother, father and best friend are unequally represented in the MPFC/ACC of collectivistic brains.
Keywords: close other, self, medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, fMRI
INTRODUCTION
Close relationships influence life quality by providing physical and emotional support (Berscheid and Reis, 1998). Behavioral studies suggest that close relationships also shape and create self-concept by evolving shared cognitive elements of the self and close others such as mother, father, and best friend (Aron, 2002). Self-expansion implicated in close relationships helps to enhance potential efficacy and to accomplish goals by including to some extent close other’s resources, perspectives, and identities as a part of the self (Aron et al., 2004).
Given the important role of close relationships in an individual’s life, close relationships may generate unique neural representation of close others in human brains. Indeed, recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research has shown that perception of kin faces activated posterior cingulate and cuneus relative to perception of friend faces, suggesting extended processing of close others in the perceptual domain (Platek and Kemp, 2009). Other studies have shown evidence for shared neural representations of mental aspects (i.e. trait) of the self and close others in the prefrontal cortex in the human brain (Zhu et al., 2007; Ng et al., 2010). Using a self-referential task that requires trait judgments of the self and others (Rogers et al., 1977), previous studies found that, relative to trait judgments of a public person, self-trait judgments were associated with greater activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and rostral anterior cingulate (ACC) (Kelley et al., 2002; Macrae et al., 2004; Heatherton et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 2006; Northoff et al., 2006; Han et al., 2008; Jenkins et al., 2008; Ma and Han, 2011). Recent work suggests that the MPFC activity involved in self-referential processing is influenced by cultural values. For example, the degree of MPFC activity associated with general relative to contextual self-descriptions correlated with subjective ratings of individualism/collectivism cultural values (Chiao et al., 2009a). Temporarily heightening individualism/collectivism cultural values also modulated MPFC activity underlying explicit self-judgments in bilcutural participants (Chiao et al., 2009b).
More closely related to the current work, Zhu et al. (2007) found that the MPFC activity increased to trait judgments of mother compared to trait judgments of a public person but was comparable for trait judgments of the self and mother. These effects were evident in Chinese with the interdependent self-construals but not in English-speaking Westerners with the independent self-construals. More recent studies further showed that, relative to priming collectivism cultural values, priming individualism cultural values increased differentiation between the self and mother (Ng et al., 2010) or between the self and father (Harada et al., 2010) in the MPFC in bicultural individuals. The brain imaging findings indicate that collectivism cultural values may result in shared neural representations of the self and close others in the MPFC, whereas individualism cultural values may lead to differential neural representations of the self and close others in the MPFC. Taken together, the brain imaging findings suggest a neural basis for the interdependent self-construals in the collectivistic cultures (Markus and Kitayama, 1991).
However, previous studies only examined the neural representation of mother or father in relation to the self. This leaves an unresolved question about whether close others, such as mother, father and friends, are represented in collectivistic brains in a similar vein. Although humans have concerns about mother, father and best friend since preadolescent (Hodges et al., 1999), the concepts of close others may be constructed differently in human brains. For example, although peers comprise an important social context that helps to foster identity and to socialize youth into adult roles (Steinberg and Silverberg, 1986; Brown, 2004), the interpersonal significance of parents may be greater than that of peers (Rosenberg, 1979). Parental influence on adolescent personality development is also deeper and more enduring than that of peers (Brown, 1990). Thus, parents may be represented more strongly in the human brain compared to friends given the different social experiences one has with his/her parents and friends. In addition, social interactions between a child and his/her mother are different from those between a child and his/her father as mothers provide more direct care to their children than fathers do in most societies (Greary, 2000). For example, in Chinese culture, the care of infants and young children is exclusively the duty of mothers (Hou, 1987). Fathers have little to do with nurturing the young. Instead, fathers are expected to educate their children and teaching of children must be strict. Consequently, children may show more intense emotion toward their mothers than toward their fathers. The difference in children’s interactions with mother and father may modulate the neural representations of mother and father so that MPFC activity associated with trait judgments of the mother may be greater than that associated with trait judgments of the father.
Using fMRI and the trait judgment task, the current work tested the hypothesis that close others such as mother, father and best friend are differentially represented in collectivistic (i.e. Chinese) brains in relation to the self. We monitored hemodynamic responses to trait judgments of the self and close others. The region of interest (ROI) in the MPFC was first defined by contrasting trait judgments of the self and a public person. Signal intensity in the MPFC was then calculated respectively for mother-, father- and best-friend-judgments. We were particularly interested in whether the MPFC engaged in self-judgments was also involved in trait judgments of others and whether MPFC activity was equally strong for trait judgments of mother, father and best friend.
METHODS
Subjects
Thirty-two healthy Chinese college students (16 women, aged between 19 and 27 years, mean = 22.7, s.d. = 1.9) participated in this study. All were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Informed consent was obtained prior to scanning. This study was approved by a local ethics committee.
Stimuli and procedure
Subjects were first imaged while performing trait judgment tasks, followed by a memory test about 40 min after the scanning procedure. The stimuli used in the scanning procedure consisted of trait adjectives that were presented through an LCD projector (Shenzhan Sinorad Medical Electronics Inc., P. R. China) onto a rear-projection screen viewed with an angled mirror positioned on the head-coil. 240 positive and 240 negative trait adjectives consisting of 2–4 Chinese characters were selected from established personality trait adjective pools (Liu, 1990). The adjectives were classified into 10 lists of 48 words matched in word length and valence. Six lists of words were pseudo-randomly selected for trait judgment tasks while the remaining four lists of words were used in the memory test. After the scanning procedure, subjects were given a ‘surprising’ memory test. Thirty-two trait adjectives used in each judgment task were randomly selected and intermixed with 192 new trait adjectives for the memory test that required identification of old vs new items presented in a random order by pressing one of two buttons.
A box-car design consisted of six functional scans during the imaging procedure (Figure 1). Each scan consisted of six sessions of trait judgment tasks. Each session of 28 s began with a 4-s instruction that required subjects to judge if an adjective was proper to describe the self, mother, father, a gender-matched best friend or Xiang Liu (a well-known Chinese athlete). A font judgment (bold- vs light-faced) was included to control for semantic processing and motor responses. Eight trait adjectives were presented in each session. The judgment tasks were intervened by null sessions of 10 s during which subjects passively viewed two rows of asterisks (*). The order of the judgment tasks was counterbalanced using a Latin Squire design. Each trial consisted of a ‘cue’ word (either self, mother, father, best friend’s name, Liu or font) above a trait adjective presented for 2 s at the center of the screen. The two successively trait adjectives were intervened by the cue for 1 s when subjects had to make a response by pressing one of the two buttons with the right index or middle finger. All instructions and traits were in Chinese. Each of the Chinese characters in the cue word and trait words subtended a visual angle of 0.34° × 0.45° (width × height) and 0.85° × 1.14°, respectively at a viewing distance of 80 cm. Each symbol used in the null session subtended 0.23° × 0.23° (small ones) or 0.56° × 0.56° (large ones). All words and symbols were white on a black background.
Fig. 1.
Illustration of stimuli and procedure used in our study. Each scan consisted of six sessions of different judgment tasks. Two successive sessions were intervened by a null session of 10 s.
The Individualism and Collectivism Attitude Scale (Triandis, 1995) with a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree) was used to estimate the degree of endorsement of individualistic and collectivistic values. The intimate relationship between the self and others was measured using the Chinese Interpersonal Relationship Affective subscale (a 9-point Likert-type scale with 1 = to the lowest degree and 9 = to the highest degree, Zhang, 2001), which includes questions such as ‘how long do you know this person?’, ‘how familiar are you with this person?”, “how important is this person to you?”, “how strongly does this person influence you?’, etc.
fMRI data acquisition and analysis
Scanning was performed at Peking University First Hospital, on a GE 3-T scanner with a standard head coil. Thirty-two transversal slices of functional images covering the whole brain were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence (64 × 64 × 32 matrix with spatial resolution of 3.75 × 3.75 × 4 mm, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 24 × 24 cm, flip angle = 90°). Anatomical images were obtained using a 3D FSPGR T1 sequence (256 × 256 × 128 matrix with spatial resolution of 0.938 × 0.938 × 1.4 mm, TR = 7.4 ms, TI = 450 ms, TE = 3.0 ms, flip angle = 20°).
SPM2 (the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK) was used for data analysis. Functional images were realigned to the first scan to correct for head movement between scans and were co-registered with each participant’s anatomical scan. Functional images were then normalized into a standard anatomical space (2 × 2 × 2 mm3 isotropic voxels) using bilinear interpolation based on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. Normalized data were then spatially smoothed using a Gaussian filter with a full-width at half maximum (FWHM) parameter set to 8 mm.
Statistical analyses used a hierarchical random-effects model with two levels. In the first level of each subject, the onsets and durations of each session were modeled using a General Linear Model (GLM) according to stimulus conditions. All conditions (self-judgments, mother-judgments, father-judgment, best-friend-judgment, Liu-judgment, font-judgment and rest) were included in the model. A box-car function was used to convolve with the canonical hemodynamic response in each condition. Effects at each voxel were estimated and regionally specific effects were compared using linear contrasts in individual subjects using a fixed effect analysis. Random effect analyses were then conducted based on statistical parameter maps from each individual subject to allow population inference. Significant activations were identified using a threshold of P < 0.05 [family-wise error (FWE) corrected].
The contrast of self-judgment vs Liu-judgment was first calculated using a random effect model to define the brain regions involved in self-referential processing, which were used as ROIs. Signal intensity of parameter estimates was then extracted from the ROI in the MPFC [defined as the cluster above the threshold of P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) shown in the contrast of self- vs Liu-judgments with the peak at x/y/z = 0/40/18, MNI coordinates] linked to mother-, father-, best-friend- and font-judgments using MarsBaR 0.38 (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net). A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) of contrast values (mother-/father-/best-friend-judgments vs font-judgments) was conducted with judgment (mother-, father-, best-friend-judgments) as independent within-subjects variables. Whole-brain statistical parametric mapping analyses were also conducted to calculate contrasts of mother-, father-, best friend- vs Liu-judgments and contrasts of self- vs mother-, father-, best friend-judgments.
RESULTS
Behavioral performances of trait judgments and memory tests are shown in Table 1. One way ANOVA of the proportions of ‘yes’ responses during the scanning procedure using Task (self, mother, father, best friend or Liu) as an independent within-subjects variable showed a significant main effect of Task [F(4, 120) = 4.465, P < 0.01]. Post-hoc analyses confirmed that the proportion of ‘yes’ response was slightly higher to self- than to father- (P = 0.045), best-friend- (P = 0.067) and Liu-(P = 0.016) judgments, but did not differ from mother-judgment (P > 0.05). Reaction times were longer to trait judgments (self, mother, father, best friend and Liu) than to font-judgment [F(5, 150) = 18.096, P < 0.001].
Table 1.
Mean behavioral performances (s.d.) during the scanning procedure, memory test and subjective rating
Self | Mother | Father | Best friend | Liu-Xiang | Font | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Performance during scanning | ||||||
‘Yes’ responses (%) | ||||||
All | 50.7 (9.9) | 46.2 (8.1) | 44.1 (10.4) | 45.4 (9.0) | 44.5 (9.3) | |
Reaction time (ms) | ||||||
All | 1143 (158) | 1120 (101) | 1110 (117) | 1150 (114) | 1167 (124) | 999 (129) |
Performance in memory test | ||||||
Total recognition score (%) | ||||||
All | 44.5 (19.3) | 38.6 (18.2) | 36.6 (16.3) | 36.1 (20.3) | 29.2 (16.7) | 7.0 (8.4) |
One way ANOVA of corrected recognition scores (the proportion of hits minus false alarms) of the memory test conducted after the scanning procedure showed a significant main effect of Task [F(5, 150) = 54.451, P < 0.001], suggesting that corrected recognition scores were higher for words used for trait judgments (self, mother, father, best friend or Liu) than those used for font-judgments. Post hoc pairwise analyses showed that trait adjectives used in the self-judgment task were remembered better than those used in trait judgments of mother, father, best friend and Liu (P’s < 0.05). Recognition scores of the adjectives related to mother- and father-judgments were higher compared to that related to Liu-judgments (P’s < 0.05) but did not differ significantly from that related to best friend (P > 0.1).
Subjects scored significantly higher on the collectivistic subscale than on the individualistic subscale [5.48 ± 0.62 vs 4.82 ± 0.48, t(31) = 4.98, P < 0.001]. Intimacy scores were significantly higher for personal familiars (mother, father or best friend) than Liu [F(3, 90) = 408.195, P < 0.001]. Intimacy scores were higher on mother (8.31 ± 0.57) and father (7.90 ± 1.04) compared to best friend (7.12 ± 0.89) (P’s < 0.01). We also examined if behavioral performances associated with self and close other judgments during the scanning procedure correlated with subjective ratings of cultural values and intimacy. We only found that RTs to mother-judgments negatively correlated with rating scores of intimacy with mother (r = −0.585, P < 0.001), suggesting that the more intimate with mother, the faster subjects responded to mother-judgments during the scanning procedure.
Whole brain analysis first showed that self- vs Liu-judgments identified significant activation in the MPFC and the ACC (Figure 2A and Table 2), consistent with previous studies (Kelley et al., 2002; Zhu et al., 2007; Han et al., 2008; Ng et al., 2010; Ma and Han, 2011). To examine whether the MPFC involved in self-referential processing was differentially engaged in trait judgments of close others, signal intensity of parameter estimates in the MPFC was calculated for mother-, father-, best friend- and font-judgments, respectively. An ANOVA of the contrast values (mother-/father-/best-friend-judgments vs font-judgments) indicated that MPFC activity related to mother-, father-, and best-friend-judgments was significantly different [F(2, 30) = 5.851, P = 0.022]. Post hoc pairwise analyses further showed that MPFC activity was greater to mother- than father- and best-friend-judgments (both P < 0.05, Figure 2B) but did not differ between father- and best-friend-judgments (P > 0.5).
Fig. 2.
(A) Increased MPFC activation in association with self- vs Liu-judgments. The top figure shows MPFC activation in an anatomical structure. The bottom figure shows BOLD responses in the MPFC to self- and Liu-judgments. (B) Contrast values in association with self-, mother-, father-, best-friend-judgments vs. font-judgment; (C) Increased MPFC activation was observed when contrasting mother- vs Liu-judgments; No increase activity was observed when contrasting father-/best-friend-judgments with Liu-judgments; (D) Increased MPFC activation was observed when contrasting self- vs father-judgments and self- vs best-friend-judgments, whereas the contrast of self- vs mother-judgments did not show any significant activation.
Table 2.
Brain activations shown in various contrasts (P < 0.05 FWE corrected)
Condition/region | Voxel no. | x | y | z | Z-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Self- vs Liu-judgments | |||||
MPFC/ACC | 515 | 0 | 40 | 18 | 6.47 |
Mother- vs Liu-judgments | |||||
MPFC | 33 | 0 | 42 | 20 | 4.97 |
ACC | 92 | −2 | 26 | 30 | 5.82 |
Self- vs Father-judgments | |||||
MPFC/ACC | 37 | −6 | 46 | 20 | 4.95 |
Self- vs Best-Friend-judgments | |||||
MPFC/ACC | 28 | −6 | 34 | 24 | 4.76 |
Self- vs Font-judgments | |||||
Superior MPFC | 1851 | −8 | 42 | 50 | 6.33 |
Left inferior frontal | 354 | −38 | 24 | −14 | 5.98 |
Left temporal | 35 | −48 | −60 | 20 | 4.78 |
Precuneus | 251 | −6 | −64 | 16 | 5.65 |
Caudate | 648 | −10 | 16 | 8 | 6.97 |
Mother- vs Font-judgments | |||||
Superior MPFC | 1381 | −12 | 56 | 34 | 6.33 |
Left inferior frontal | 232 | −46 | 26 | −6 | 6.01 |
Left temporal | 12 | −64 | −30 | −2 | 4.89 |
Precuneus | 406 | −8 | −54 | 28 | 5.81 |
Caudate | 573 | −12 | 14 | 8 | 5.90 |
Father- vs Font-judgments | |||||
Superior MPFC | 1213 | −10 | 44 | 46 | 6.06 |
Left inferior frontal | 319 | −46 | 26 | −4 | 5.88 |
Left temporal | 52 | −52 | −24 | −6 | 4.96 |
Precuneus | 53 | −6 | −54 | 30 | 4.88 |
Caudate | 655 | −14 | 14 | 12 | 6.09 |
Best Friend vs Font-judgments | |||||
Superior MPFC | 816 | −10 | 46 | 52 | 6.21 |
Left inferior frontal | 188 | −46 | 26 | −6 | 5.76 |
Left temporal | 146 | −52 | −26 | −4 | 5.88 |
Precuneus | 351 | −8 | −54 | 28 | 5.77 |
Caudate | 424 | −14 | 6 | 14 | 5.52 |
x, y, z = MNI coordinates; MPFC = medial prefrontal cortex.
To assess if any other brain regions underlying semantic processing and the processing of general personal knowledge and memory retrieval during trait judgments differentiated between close others, we calculated BOLD signals associated with close other judgments in the brain areas identified in the contrast of self- vs font-judgments (e.g. the dorsal MPFC, left inferior frontal cortex, left temporal cortex, precuneus and caudate nucleus, see Table 2). However, the signal intensity in these ROIs failed to show any significant differences between mother-, father-, and best-friend-judgments (P’s > 0.05).
To examine whether close others were differentially encoded in the MPFC relative to the public person, whole brain analysis was calculated to contrast close-other-judgments vs Liu-judgments. We found that MPFC and ACC activity was greater to mother- than to Liu-judgments (Figure 2C and Table 2). However, the contrast between father-/best-friend-judgments and Liu-judgments did not show any significant activation at the same threshold. Moreover, to assess whether close others were differentially encoded in the MPFC in relation to the self, contrasts of self-judgments vs close-other-judgments were conducted. We found that MPFC activity did not differ between self- and mother-judgments but was greater to self- than to father-/best-friend-judgments (Figure 2D and Table 2).
To examine whether the neural activity associated with the self or close others can predict subjective evaluations of one’s own cultural values and intimacy with others, we calculated correlations between subjective rating scores and MPFC activity from the ROI defined in the contrast in association with self and close other judgments but did not find any significant correlations. We also conducted regression analyses to explore if any other brain areas, besides the ventral MPFC, were correlated with cultural values and subjective feelings of intimacy. We entered either rating scores of the collectivistic subscale, individualistic subscale and intimacy or the differential rating score (i.e. collectivistic subscale minus individualistic subscale) as repressors. However, these analyses failed to show any significant activation using a cluster-level threshold of P < 0.05 (corrected for multiple comparison).
DISCUSSION
Cultural values and behavioral performances
Cultural neuroscience studies investigate whether and how cultural values and beliefs modulate brain activity involved in multiple cognitive processes and behaviors (Chiao and Ambady, 2007; Han and Northoff, 2008, 2009; Chiao, 2009). One way to uncover cultural influences on neural activity is to compare subjects from two countries in which one cultural value dominates others (e.g. individualism or collectivism, Zhu et al., 2007; Chiao et al., 2009a). However, the globalization of economy and education has made it possible that college students in most countries are exposed to different cultural values and beliefs. Thus it is critical to assess which cultural value was dominant in our subjects even though they were recruited in China where collectivistic cultural values are dominant. Questionnaire measurements of the current work suggest endorsement of collectivistic cultural values in our Chinese subjects, consistent with the proposal of previous social psychological studies (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995). Questionnaire measurements also suggest subjective feeling of stronger intimate relationship between the self and parents than that between the self and best friend, possibly due to greater interpersonal significance of parents (Rosenberg, 1979) and/or stronger parental influence on personal development (Brown, 1990). However, although mother and father play different roles in nurturing the young (Hou, 1987; Greary, 2000), our subjects reported similar rating scores of intimacy with mother and father.
The behavioral performances obtained inside the scanner showed evidence of slower responses to trait-judgments compared to font-judgments. Trait-judgments required enhanced semantic processing of words relative to font-judgments and thus took longer time for responses. However, reaction times did not differ between trait judgments of the self and close others, suggesting that different trait judgments were comparable in task demands and overall difficulty. However, subjects showed better memory of trait adjectives associated with the self than those associated with others. This typical self-referential effect in memory performances suggest elaborated encoding during trait judgments and/or better retrieval of self-related information in the memory task.
Overlap between the self and mothers
The increased MPFC activity to self-judgments compared to trait judgments of the public person replicated previous findings (Kelley et al., 2002; Northoff et al., 2006; Han et al., 2008; Jenkins et al., 2008; Ma and Han, 2011), providing evidence for the involvement of the MPFC in encoding self-related information during trait judgments. In addition, our fMRI results showed increased MPFC activity to mother-judgments relative to trait judgments of the public person, indicating that the MPFC is differentially involved in representation of mother compared to a familiar public figure. Moreover, our fMRI results confirmed the overlap of representations of the self and mother in the MPFC as the contrast of self- versus mother-judgments did not show any significant activation in this brain region. Together with our previous findings (Zhu et al., 2007; Ng et al., 2010), the fMRI results indicate a shared neural representations of the self and mother in collectivistic brains. This provides a neural basis for the overlap of conceptual representations of the self and mother in collectivistic cultures (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). The shared representations of the self and mother in the MPFC also support the self-expansion model (Aron et al., 2004) that posits that in close relationships the close others become part of the self.
Differential neural representation of close others
Most importantly, our findings showed evidence that close others such as mother, father, and best friend are unequally represented in the MPFC of collectivistic brains. Specially, MPFC activity increased to mother-judgments compared to public person judgments whereas the activity in the same brain region failed to distinguish between farther/best friend and the public person during trait judgments. Moreover, activity in the MPFC that was engaged in self-referential processing was greater to mother-judgments relative to farther- and best-friend-judgments. Taken together, the results indicate that representation of mother in the MPFC was stronger than that of other close others such as father and best friend. Our results further showed that the activity differentiating between mother and father/best friend was specific to the ventral part of the MPFC that was defined in the contrast of self-judgments versus public person judgments. The brain regions defined in the contrast of self-judgments versus font-judgments including the dorsal part of the MPFC, posterior cingulate and caudate nucleus, which were involved in semantic processing and the processing of general personal knowledge and memory retrieval, did not differentiate between mother-, father- and best-friend-judgments. Thus, it seems that only the ‘self-area’ defined in the contrast of self-judgments vs public person judgments was engaged to differentiate between close others.
It has been proposed that the ventral region of the MPFC contributes to the encoding of self-relevance of stimuli (Nortorff et al., 2006; Han and Northorff, 2009). In consistent with this, Macrae et al. (2004) found that trait words judged as suitable to describe the self evoked greater MPFC activity relative to trait words judged as unsuitable to describe the self. A recent work showed further that the MPFC mediates the processing of personal closeness. Krienen et al. (2010) asked participants to make inferences about friends and strangers who were either similar or dissimilar to oneself in political attitudes, personality, lifestyle, occupation and education. They found that the ventral region of the MPFC and rostral ACC increased to friends compare to strangers regardless whether the friends were similar or dissimilar to the self. The findings underscore the functional role of the MPFC activity in encoding the social relevance of the person (closeness) to oneself. The current study complemented previous research by showing that the MPFC also differentiate between close others. Thus the MPFC not only classifies persons in terms of close others vs strangers but also engages in encoding how close a familiar other is. The greater MPFC activity link to mother than father/best friend in relation to the self may arise from stronger behavioral connections during development between the self and mother (Geary, 2000) and provide a neural basis for the excessive concern of closeness with mother over other close others (Hodges et al., 1999).
In consistent with differential neural representations of mother and best friend, our subjects reported stronger intimate feelings to mother than to best friend. However, differential neural representations of mother and father in the MPFC were observed even though questionnaire measurements of intimacy suggest similar close relationship between the self and mother/father. Thus markers of different close relationship can be found at the neural level (i.e. MPFC activity) even when such difference could not be disclosed in the questionnaire measurements of intimacy between mother and father. In addition, our fMRI results failed to reveal differential neural representation of father and best friend whereas questionnaire measurements of intimacy suggest different close relationship between father and the best friend. The inconsistency between subjective feelings and brain activity may arise from the influence of social desires on explicit subjective reports of social feelings. Alternatively, brain regions outside the MPFC may play a more critical role in encoding the intimacy between the self and close others. Given that self-report on behavioral surveys based on subjective Likert scales is strongly influenced by the reference group (Heine et al., 2002), future research may also investigate Asian participants in Western cultural contexts to examine the neural representation of close others when referenced to a different cultural group.
Recent studies have shown evidence that socio-cultural contexts strongly shape neural basis of multiple levels of human cognition (Chiao and Ambady, 2007; Han and Northoff, 2008; Chiao, 2009). The current work further suggest that different social interactions between the self and individuals closely related to the self can also shape the neurocogntive processes of the close others. To what degree are neural representations of close others in the MPFC modulated by cultural values and beliefs? This cannot be addressed by our brain imaging results as the current work recruited subjects from only one culture. We conducted regression analyses but failed to find significant activations that correlated with subjective ratings of individualism/collectivism values across subjects. We also classified our subjects into two groups based on a median split on the cultural values and compared MPFC activity in associated with self and close other judgments between the two groups. However, this did not reveal reliable difference in MPFC activity between the two groups with high and low collectivism cultural values. This is possibly due to the small variation of cultural values among our subjects. Future research may investigate the difference in neural representations of close others between two cultural groups and employed a large sample to assess individual differences in brain activity underlying personal relatedness in close relationship.
CONCLUSIONS
The current fMRI research showed evidence that close others such as mother, father and best friend are differentially represented in human MPFC. Representation of mother’s traits overlaps with that of the self in the MPFC whereas representations of father and best friend’s traits do not. The distinct neural representations of mother, father and best friend in collectivistic brains may arise from one’s different daily experiences with close others, and may affect social interactions between the self and close others. Our results support the proposal that the MPFC mediates a function to represent self-relatedness of others (Northoff et al., 2006; Han and Northoff, 2009; Krienen et al., 2010) and can be used as an objective measurement of close relationships, complementing subjective evaluations of close relationships.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by National Basic Research Program of China (973 Program 2010CB833903), National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 30630025, 30828012, 30910103901), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities.
REFERENCES
- Aron A. Self and close relationships. In: Leary MR, Tangney J, editors. Handbook of Self and Identity. New York: Guilford Press; 2002. pp. 442–61. [Google Scholar]
- Aron A, McLaughlin-Volpe T, Mashek D, Lewandowski G, Wright SC, Aron EN. Including others in the self. European Review of Social Psychology. 2004;15:101–32. [Google Scholar]
- Berscheid E, Reis HT. Attraction and close relationships. In: Fiske S, Gilbert D, Lindzey G, editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology. 4th edn. Boston: McGraw Hill; 1998. pp. 193–281. [Google Scholar]
- Brown B. Peer groups. In: Feldman S, Elliott G, editors. At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1990. pp. 171–96. [Google Scholar]
- Brown B. Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In: Lerner R, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. 2nd edn. New York: Wiley; 2004. pp. 363–94. [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Ambady N. Cultural neuroscience: parsing universality and diversity across levels of analysis. In: Kitayama S, Cohen D, editors. Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 237–54. [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY. Cultural neuroscience: a once and future discipline. Progress in Brain Research. 2009;178:287–304. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17821-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Harada T, Komeda H, et al. Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Human Brain Mapping. 2009a;30:2813–20. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Harada T, Komeda H, et al. Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009b;22:1–11. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC. Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:55–77. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Mao L, Gu X, Zhu Y, Ge J, Ma Y. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience. 2008;3:1–15. doi: 10.1080/17470910701469681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Northoff G. Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Review Neuroscience. 2008;9:646–54. doi: 10.1038/nrn2456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Northoff G. Understanding the self: a cultural neuroscience approach. Progress in Brain Research. 2009;178:203–12. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17814-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada T, Li Z, Chiao JY. Differential dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal representations of the implicit self modulated by individualism and collectivism: an fMRI study. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5:257–71. doi: 10.1080/17470910903374895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heatherton TF, Wyland CL, Macrae CN, Demos KE, Denny BT, Kelley WM. Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2006;1:18–25. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heine SJ, Lehman DR, Peng K, Greenholtz J. What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;82:903–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodges EVE, Finnegan RA, Perry DG. Skewed autonomy – relatedness in preadolescents’ conceptions of their relationships with mother, father, and best friend. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:737–48. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.3.737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hou DYF. Fatherhood in Chinese culture. In: Lamb ME, editor. The father’s role: cross-cultural perspective. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 1987. pp. 227–45. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins AC, Macrae CN, Mitchell JP. Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgments of self and others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2008;105:4507–12. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708785105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton TF. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:785–94. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krienen FM, Tu PC, Buckner RL. Clan mentality: evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex responds to close others. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:13906–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2180-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y. Modern Lexicon of Chinese Frequently-used Word Frequency. Beijing: Space Navigation Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Ma Y, Han S. Neural representation of self-concept in sighted and congenitally blind adults. Brain. 2011;134:235–46. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrae CN, Moran JM, Heatherton TF, Banfield JB, Kelley WM. Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:647–54. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implication for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review. 1991;98:224–53. [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron. 2006;50:655–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng SH, Han S, Mao L, Lai JCL. Dynamic bicultural brains: a fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture priming. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 2010;13:83–91. [Google Scholar]
- Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage. 2006;31:440–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platek SM, Kemp SM. Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI study of familiar, familial, and self-face recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:849–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers TB, Kuiper NA, Kirker WS. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1977;35:677–88. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.35.9.677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg M. Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Silverberg SB. The vicissitudes of autonomy in early adolescence. Child Development. 1986;57:841–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1986.tb00250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Triandis HC. Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder: Westview Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang ZX. Chinese cognition on interpersonal relationship: a multi dimensional investigation. In: Yang CF, editor. Interpersonal Relationship, Affection and Trust of the Chinese: from an Interactional Perspective. Taipei, Taiwan: Yuen Liu Publishing Co.; 2001. pp. 15–79. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu Y, Zhang L, Fan J, Han S. Neural basis of cultural influence on self representation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1310–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]