Abstract
Background
To ascertain good and appropriate healthcare for both women and men implementation of gender perspectives in medical education is needed. For a successful implementation, knowledge about students' attitudes and beliefs about men, women, and gender is crucial. The aim of this study was to compare attitudes to gender and gender stereotyping among Dutch and Swedish male and female medical students.
Methods
In this cross-sectional study, we measured the attitudes and assumptions about gender among 1096 first year medical students (616 Dutch and 480 Swedish) with the validated Nijmegen Gender Awareness in Medicine Scale (N-GAMS). The response rate was 94% in the Netherlands and 93% in Sweden. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the scores between Dutch and Swedish male and female students. Linear regressions were used to analyze the importance of the background variables.
Results
There were significant differences in attitudes to gender between Dutch and Swedish students. The Swedish students expressed less stereotypical thinking about patients and doctors and the Dutch were more sensitive to gender differences. The students' sex mattered for gender stereotyping, with male students in both countries agreeing more with stereotypical statements. Students' age, father's birth country and mother's education level had some impact on the outcome.
Conclusions
There are differences between cultures as well as between men and women in gender awareness that need to be considered when implementing gender in medical education.
This study suggests that to arouse the students' interest in gender issues and make them aware of the significance of gender in medical work, the examples used in discussions need to be relevant and challenging in the context of the specific country. Due to different levels of knowledge and different attitudes within the student population it is important to create a climate for dialogue where students feel permitted to disclose their ideas and attitudes in order to become aware of what these are as well as their possible consequences on interaction and decision-making in medical work.
Keywords: Medical curricula, medical students, gender implementation, gender, attitudes, stereotypical thinking
Background
Differences between men and women regarding biology, living conditions, behaviours and risk-taking are important causes of gender disparities in health and illness [1,2]. The impact of gender is not limited to reproduction-related disorders but also relevant in conditions such as cardiovascular disease, psychiatric disorders and cancer. Moreover, gender has an impact on medical communication, influencing patients' symptom presentations [3,4] as well as doctors' conduct and interpretations of patients' complaints and signs [5,6]. For example, physicians are more likely to interpret men's symptoms as organic and women's as psychosocial [5,7], and female patients are less often considered for referral or further investigation than men with similar symptoms, which raises questions of unequal care and gender bias [6,8,9].
As the importance of gender in health care has become established knowledge, attention has been paid to the implementation of gender issues in medical schools. Research reports and political documents are pointing towards the need for gender perspectives in medical education in order to ascertain good and appropriate health care for both men and women and increase future doctors' gender awareness [2,10]. Gender awareness means that physicians have the knowledge and ability to recognize and incorporate gender as an essential determinant of health and illness into their daily practice [11]. Gender awareness also denotes being aware that stereotyped assumptions and beliefs about men's and women's behaviour, skills, and needs are widespread in society and reframing people's thinking and perceptions. Since gender-stereotypical thinking has the inherent risk of thwarting and biasing medical assessments, gender awareness implies reflecting on one's own attitudes and preconceptions about men and women, patients as well as doctors and other staff [9,12].
In most countries and medical schools the process of including gender aspects in medical curricula has started only recently. Reports from Canada, the Netherlands, Sweden and Australia describe the achievements and work undertaken, for example in pedagogy and the development of databases with paper-cases that incorporate gender aspects [11,13-15]. The implementation of gender is not always a smooth process; it is sometimes met with doubts and scepticism [15]. In a Swedish study, where a majority of the medical students considered gender to be an important topic with relevance, another group of students were hesitant and some even expressed reluctant and negative views about the subject [16]. To achieve successful implementation, it is necessary to investigate attitudes towards and values about gender issues [12,17]. Recently the Nijmegen Gender Awareness Scale in Medicine, (N-GAMS) for measuring medical students' attitudes and values concerning gender was developed and validated in the Netherlands [11]. The scale was suggested as a tool for making a baseline assessment of gender awareness when implementing gender perspectives, as well as an evaluative assessment after the integration of gender. When used in a cross-sectional study in the Netherlands, N-GAMS showed that male medical students held stronger gender stereotypes than their female peers [11]. In the present paper N-GAMS is used to compare gender awareness in first-term Dutch and Swedish students.
The Netherlands and Sweden are two countries in the front row when it comes to implementing gender within medical education. The need for gender perspectives in medical curricula has been acknowledged at governmental level in both countries. The Dutch Ministry of Health initiated a nationwide project for implementing gender issues in Dutch medical schools in 2002; the project had its centre at Radboud University, Nijmegen Medical Centre [11]. In Sweden, the government has initiated several assessments of education about gender in medical schools and this has had effects on local university policies. In 2001, Umeå University medical school decided to mainstream gender perspectives into the medical curriculum, and a committee was set up to lead this work [14].
Even though the Netherlands and Sweden are both wealthy European countries, gender relations in the two countries show differences. The Dutch two-third earner model implies that men work full-time while women work part-time and take care of their own children or grandchildren [18]. Dutch women's labour market participation started rather late in the 1980s, and among all industrialized countries the Netherlands ranks first in the list for part-time work among women [19,20]. In Sweden, almost as many women as men (80%) participate in paid work and every other woman of working age works full-time [21]. Day-care facilities and support for Swedish parents in terms of pregnancy and parental leave are well developed and widely used since the 1960s [20].
Feminist research argues that social policies in different countries can be seen as representations of gender values and ideology on a structural level [22]. Social policies may affect individuals' behaviour, for example how they choose to study, work, or organize their private lives, and, in turn, people's behaviours have an impact on general assumptions and expectations of men and women, as well as of how gender differences are perceived [23]. For medical education this means that there might be country differences in students' attitudes to and perceptions of gender. To explore this, the aim of this study is to compare attitudes to gender and gender stereotyping among Dutch and Swedish male and female medical students.
Methods
This study had a cross-sectional design and data were collected by a questionnaire. First-year medical students at Umeå University in Sweden, from autumn 2006 to spring 2009 (n = 516, 46% men), and at Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre in the Netherlands, during 2006-2007 (n = 657, 32% men), were invited to participate. The students were informed about the study when attending an ordinary lecture during the first week in medical school. Participation was voluntary and those who agreed to participate stayed on after the lecture to anonymously answer the questionnaire. The Ethical Committee at Umeå University has approved this study.
Questionnaire
The questionnaire measured gender awareness by means of the Nijmegen Gender Awareness in Medicine Scale (N-GAMS), previously elaborated in the Netherlands by way of exploratory factor analyses [11]. Reliability and validity were established and in its final form N-GAMS contains 32 statements answered by a 5-point Likert-scale (ranking from 1 = totally disagree to 5 = totally agree). It measures three dimensions of gender awareness: Gender Sensitivity (GS), focusing on students' attitudes towards gender concerns in health care; Gender Role Ideology towards Patients (GRI-P), measuring gender-stereotypical thinking about patients; and Gender Role Ideology towards Doctors (GRI-D), measuring gender-stereotypical thinking about doctors (Table 1). A high score on the GS scale implies more gender sensitivity. On the GRI-P and GRI-D subscales a higher score indicates agreeing more with gender stereotypes. In the present study one item was added to the GRI-D subscale to obtain an equal number of statements about male and female doctors.
Table 1.
GS, Gender sensitivity (excluded items*, items scored in reverse_R) | |
---|---|
GS1_R | addressing differences between men and women creates inequity in health care* |
GS2 | physicians' knowledge of gender differences in illness and health increases quality of care* |
GS3_R | physicians should only address biological differences between men and women |
GS4_R | in non-sex-specific health disorders the sex/gender of the patient is irrelevant |
GS5_R | a physician should confine as much as possible to biomedical aspects of health complaints of men and women |
GS6_R | physicians do not need to know what happens in the lives of men and women to be able to deliver medical care* |
GS7_R | differences between male and female physicians are too small to be relevant |
GS8_R | especially because men and women are different, physicians should treat everybody the same |
GS9_R | physicians who address gender differences are not dealing with the important issues |
GS10_R | in communicating with patients it does not matter to a physician whether the patients are men or women |
GS11_R | in communicating with patients it does not matter whether the physician is a man or a woman |
GS12_R | differences between male and female patients are so small that physicians can hardly take them into account |
GS13 | for effective treatment, physicians should address gender differences in etiology and consequences of disease* |
GS14_R | it is not necessary to consider gender differences in presentation of complaints* |
GRIP, Gender role ideology towards patients (excluded item*) | |
GRIP1 | male patients better understand physicians' measures than female patients |
GRIP2 | female patients compared to male patients have unreasonable expectations of physicians |
GRIP3 | women more frequently than men want to discuss problems with physicians that do not belong in the consultation room |
GRIP4 | women expect too much emotional support from physicians |
GRIP5 | male patients are less demanding than female patients |
GRIP6 | women are larger consumers of health care than is actually needed |
GRIP7 | men do not go to a physician for harmless health problems |
GRIP8 | medically unexplained symptoms develop in women because they lament too much about their health |
GRIP9 | female patients complain about their health because they need more attention than male patients |
GRIP10 | it is easier to find causes of health complaints in men because men communicate in a direct way |
GRIP11 | men appeal to health care more often with problems they should have prevented* |
GRID, Gender role ideology towards doctors (excluded item*, item added _A) | |
GRID1 | male physicians put too much emphasis on technical aspects of medicine compared to female physicians |
GRID2 | female physicians extend their consultations too much compared to male physicians |
GRID3 | male physicians are more efficient than female physicians |
GRID4 | female physicians are more empathic than male physicians |
GRID5 | female physicians needlessly take into account how a patient experiences disease |
GRID6 | male physicians are better able to deal with the work than female physicians* |
GRID7 | female physicians are too emotionally involved with their patients |
GRID8_A | compared to female physicians, male physicians are too hurried in their consultations |
*items removed during factor analysis due to low factor loading.
_R items scored in reverse, i.e. the more you agree, the lower your gender sensitivity score.
_A item added to the original N-GAMS scale to achieve an equal number of statements about male and female doctors.
The questionnaire also contained questions about the students' and their parents' socio-cultural background, i.e., students' sex, age, country of birth, civil status and whether they had children or not, and parents' country of birth, educational level and working time. The educational level of the parents was divided into three categories: "primary" when the highest education was primary school, "intermediate" when the highest education was secondary or intermediate secondary school, and "higher" for higher secondary school or university. The parents' working time was reported as "no paid work", "part-time" (< 36 hrs/week) or "full-time" (≥ 36 hrs/week).
Validation of the scale
To check the N-GAMS scale for our sample including both Dutch and Swedish medical students, exploratory factor analyses were performed. The three-factor structure was confirmed, the new item loaded as expected together with the other GRI-D statements, but seven other statements were excluded due to low factor loadings (< .4) or cross-loading (Table 1). Factor analysis was repeated without the excluded items and all factor loadings were >.437. The three factors extracted explained 42.5% of the variance (GS 9.6%, GRI-P 28.4% and GRI-D 4.5%). The reliability scores of the subscales with Cronbach's α were: GS-scale α = .76 (9 items), GRI-P scale α = .89 (10 items) and GRI-D scale α = .89 (7 items). GRI-D and GRI-P were significantly correlated (r = .657, p < 0.000) supporting a common ground for gender stereotypes towards physicians and patients. However, extracting three components also indicated a distinction between both subscales.
Analysis
Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare results from multiple groups. The N-GAMS scores between Dutch and Swedish male and female students was compared and interaction effects between country of study and student's sex was analysed.
To analyse the relationship between the socio-cultural background variables and outcome on subscales of the N-GAMS we performed linear regressions since the outcome variables are interval variables. We used a model with students' and parents' background variables as independent variables and the outcome on GS, GRI-P and GRI-D as dependent variables. Since ANOVA showed differences between the Dutch and Swedish students we stratified our analyses by country. All statistical analyses of the data were performed in SPSS 17.0.
Results
In total 1096 (616 Dutch and 480 Swedish) students participated in the study, a response rate of 94% in the Netherlands and 93% in Sweden (Table 2). The response rate among men and women was similar in each country (not shown in table).
Table 2.
Variable | Categories | The Netherlands | Sweden | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age, mean (SD) | 18.8 (1.70) | 22.7 (3.70) | < 0.001† | |
Sex, % (n) | Male | 30.5 (188) | 46.0 (221) | < 0.001Ω |
Female | 69.5 (428) | 54.0 (259) | ||
Sexual orientation | Heterosexual | 98.2 (605) | 94.6 (452) | 0.001 Ω |
Other | 1.8 (11) | 5.4 (26) | ||
Civil status | Single | 97.1 (598) | 78.3 (376) | < 0.001 Ω |
Cohabiting/married | 2.9 (18) | 21.7 (104) | ||
Children | Yes | 0 (0) | 4.2 (20) | < 0.001 Ω |
No | 100 (612) | 95.8 (457) | ||
Birth country student | Country of study | 94.6 (575) | 95.8 (459) | 0.341 Ω |
Other | 5.4 (33) | 4.2 (20) | ||
Birth country mother | Country of study | 90.3 (543) | 91.9 (440) | 0.389 Ω |
Other | 9.7 (58) | 8.1 (39) | ||
Birth country father | Country of study | 89.1 (541) | 91.0 (436) | 0.302 Ω |
Other | 10.9 (66) | 9.0 (43) | ||
Highest education mother | Primary | 1.9 (12) | 2.3 (11) | < 0.001 Ω |
Intermediate | 45.9 (283) | 15.9 (76) | ||
Higher | 52.1 (321) | 81.8 (392) | ||
Highest education father | Primary | 1.1 (7) | 5.0 (24) | < 0.001 Ω |
Intermediate | 30.6 (187) | 19.5 (93) | ||
Higher | 68.2 (417) | 75.5 (360) | ||
Mother's working status | No paid work | 18.2 (111) | 10.0 (47) | < 0.001 Ω |
Part-time | 66.7 (407) | 21.1 (99) | ||
Full-time | 15.1 (92) | 68.9 (324) | ||
Father's working status | No paid work | 6.5 (39) | 9.0 (42) | 0.067 Ω |
Part-time | 8.8 (53) | 5.8 (27) | ||
Full-time | 84.6 (507) | 85.2 (396) | ||
Total | 56.2 (616) | 43.8 (480) |
Age presented as means (SD), other factors % (n).
Percentages are valid percent, missing values excluded, therefore the sum of n is not always the sum of participants. Most missing values concern father's working status (2.9%, n = 32).
†p value calculated using t-test
Ω p values calculated using Chi-Square, level of significance p < 0.05
The students' age ranged from 16 to 50 years (M = 21), the Swedish students were older than the Dutch (M = 23 years versus M = 19 years) and the age span was larger in Sweden. There were more female students in both samples but the preponderance of women was considerably more pronounced in the Netherlands, where about 70% of the participants (and eligible students) were women. It was more common that Swedish students were cohabiting or married, had children, or that they reported a sexual orientation other than heterosexual, compared to the Dutch peers. In both Sweden and the Netherlands the proportion of students with a mother or father born abroad was small, and in both countries the non-native parents had their origin in many different countries in Europe, Asia, North America, South America and Africa (not shown in table). The parents' level of education was higher in Sweden than in the Netherlands and the difference was especially large between the Swedish and Dutch mothers. Concerning parents' working status, a large difference was also found between the mothers. A majority of the Swedish students had full-time working mothers but the most common among the Dutch was that mothers worked part-time.
Since none of the Dutch students had children this variable was excluded in further analyses.
The outcome on N-GAMS showed significant differences between Dutch and Swedish students on all three subscales when compared by ANOVA (Table 3). The Dutch students reported higher gender sensitivity, i.e., they had higher scores on the GS subscale. Dutch students also had a higher score than the Swedish students on the GRI-P and GRI-D subscales, indicating that Dutch students held more gender-stereotypical attitudes towards both doctors and patients. When comparing N-GAMS scores for male and female students, a significant difference was found on the GRI-P scale, where the male students demonstrated more stereotypical attitudes than female students. No significant interaction effects were found. The mean values for the subscales showed that Swedish female students had the lowest values on GRI-P, i.e., they expressed the least stereotypical thinking towards patients. Thereafter came the Swedish male students, Dutch female students and finally the Dutch male students. The mean values for the GRI-D subscale showed the same pattern. The Swedish female students agreed the least and the Dutch male students agreed the most with stereotypical thinking about male and female doctors.
Table 3.
The Netherlands | Sweden | Main effect country | Main effect sex | Interaction effect | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male (n = 188) Mean (SD) |
Female (n = 428) Mean (SD) |
Male (n = 221) Mean (SD) |
Female (n = 259) Mean (SD) |
F (df) | sig | η2 | F | sig | η2 | F | sig | η2 | |
GS | 3.43 (0.58) | 3.43 (0.53) | 3.30 (0.66) | 3.37 (0.64) | 5.87 (3, 1054) | 0.016 | 0.006 | NS | NS | ||||
GRIP | 2.52 (0.59) | 2.27 (0.51) | 1.96 (0.74) | 1.70 (0.59) | 221.34 (3, 1076) | < 0.001 | 0.171 | 43.97 | < 0.001 | 0.039 | NS | ||
GRID | 2.44 (0.60) | 2.43 (0.55) | 2.07 (0.76) | 2.00 (0.72) | 93.96 (3, 1073) | < 0.001 | 0.081 | NS | NS |
Univariate analysis of variance used, significant results shown.
GS: gender sensitivity, GRI-P: gender role ideology towards patients, GRI-D: gender role ideology towards doctors.
N represents total number of participants, missing values excluded: GS 3.8%, n = 38, GRI-P 1.5%, n = 16, GRI-D 1.7%, n = 19.
When exploring whether the socio-cultural background variables were related to outcome on GS, GRI-P and GRI-D, linear regressions stratified by country were used. Within this model (Table 4) age was important for GS in both countries, with older students scoring higher, i.e. being more gender sensitive. Father's country of birth had an impact on GS in the Dutch sample, i.e., Dutch students with non-native fathers were less gender-sensitive. The students' sex was significant for the outcome on GRI-P in both countries, but explained this score to a greater extent in the Netherlands than in Sweden. In Sweden, older students also expressed less stereotypical thinking about patients. Mothers' education was the largest contributor in our model among the Swedish students, and students with intermediate- or high-educated mothers disagreed more with stereotypical thinking about patients and doctors than did students with low-educated mothers.
Table 4.
Variable | Categories | GS | GRIP | GRID | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dutch | Swedish | Dutch | Swedish | Dutch | Swedish | ||
Sex | Man (ref) | ||||||
Woman | -0.013 | 0.077 | -0.217*** | -0.189*** | 0.005 | -0.043 | |
Age | 0.158*** | 0.186** | -0.025 | -0.190** | 0.040 | -0.084 | |
Birth country | Country of study (ref) | ||||||
Other | 0.059 | 0.008 | 0.001 | 0.052 | -0.025 | 0.084 | |
Sexuality | Heterosexual (ref) | ||||||
Other | -0.007 | 0.095 | 0.003 | -0.067 | -0.028 | 0.015 | |
Civil status | Single (ref) | ||||||
Cohabiting/Married | 0.046 | -0.023 | -0.042 | 0.029 | -0.060 | 0.002 | |
Birth country father | Country of study (ref) | ||||||
Other | -0.166* | -0.094 | 0.045 | 0.008 | 0.124 | -0.013 | |
Birth country mother | Country of study (ref) | ||||||
Other | -0.003 | 0.053 | 0.025 | -0.063 | -0.068 | -0.020 | |
Education father | Low (ref) | ||||||
Intermediate | -0.080 | 0.098 | -0.254 | 0.064 | 0.084 | 0.095 | |
High | -0.023 | 0.172 | -0.290 | 0.039 | 0.032 | 0.087 | |
Education Mother | Low (ref) | ||||||
Intermediate | 0.018 | -0.111 | 0.305 | -0.332** | 0.231 | -0.261* | |
High | 0.102 | -0.068 | 0.289 | -0.319* | 0.179 | -0.231 | |
Working status father | No work (ref) | ||||||
Part-time | -0.025 | 0.041 | 0.069 | -0.033 | 0.115 | -0.068 | |
Full-time | 0.031 | 0.092 | 0.107 | -0.017 | 0.150* | -0.048 | |
status mother | No work (ref) | ||||||
Part-time | 0.024 | -0.115 | -0.023 | -0.091 | 0.026 | -0.090 | |
Full-time | -0.026 | -0.130 | 0.029 | -0.073 | 0.029 | -0.071 | |
R2 total | 0.036 | 0.031 | 0.041 | 0.052 | 0.005 | -0.009 |
GS: gender sensitivity, GRI-P: gender role ideology towards patients, GRI-D: gender role ideology towards doctors.
Numbers shown are standardized regression coefficient (Beta).
*p < 0.05
** p < 0.01
***p < 0.001
However, as indicated by R2 in Table 4 (degree of explanation), in this model the socio-cultural variables did not contribute much to the outcome on N-GAMS.
Discussion
The Dutch and Swedish medical students differed in gender awareness and showed significant differences in outcome on all three N-GAMS subscales. Dutch students reported higher gender sensitivity but they also expressed more gender-stereotypical attitudes towards patients and doctors. There were differences between men and women in stereotypical thinking, and in both countries female students disagreed more with stereotypical statements about patients. The pattern was that Swedish female students expressed the least stereotypical thinking, followed by Swedish male students, Dutch female students and finally the Dutch male students. The students' age had some impact on outcome in both countries, father's country of birth was related to gender sensitivity among the Dutch students, and mother's education was related to stereotypical thinking among the Swedish students.
On method
Data for this study were collected during the students' first week in medical school, implying that the results mirror the gender awareness the students had when entering their studies. The large sample and high response rate was a strength.
N-GAMS was carefully elaborated and validated in a Dutch context [11], but our factor analysis showed that N-GAMS could be translated and used also among Swedish medical students. Factor structure was the same and the loadings similar in the Swedish and the Dutch samples. The items that needed to be excluded from the subscales had low loading in both samples. From GRI-P and GRI-D, one item from each subscale needed to be excluded (Table 1). The item added to the GRI-D subscale loaded as expected and seemed to add something to the scale. The GS subscale needed the largest revision, with five items excluded due to low factor loading. The remaining nine items all concern whether differences between men and women are relevant to consider in clinical work. In fact, "sensitivity for gender differences" might be a more appropriate name than "gender sensitivity" for this subscale.
Using a scale as a tool for exploring gender attitudes and stereotypes obviously has both strengths and limitations. The N-GAMS scale was suggested to be used for research purposes and to evaluate gender awareness raising courses and one obvious strength with using such an instrument is that a large number of medical students could be included in the study. We also found that N-GAMS could be translated and used outside the Netherlands, meaning that the instrument makes it possible to compare medical students in different countries and cultural settings. However, the methods needs certain reflection; when understanding the differences we found between Dutch and Swedish students the background variables used in the questionnaire do not in themselves explain the results. Still, the significant differences in students' characteristics (Table 2) provide clues to the context of the medical students' attitudes and awareness. While the N-GAMS scale could be used for example as a baseline assessment to evaluate gender awareness raising courses, or to make a comparison of a large amount students, qualitative research might give further depth and provide more explanations through highlighting the social discourses and reasoning that lies behind the students results and the difference between the Netherlands and Sweden.
On Results
The fact that the students in Nijmegen compared to the students in Umeå were more sensitive to gender differences and also more gender stereotyping of patients and doctors might have different explanations. Since the students came from all over Sweden and the Netherlands and answered the questionaire during their first days of medical school it is reasonable to believe that the differences reflect national differences rather than University differences. As outlined in the introduction, Sweden and the Netherlands are both welfare states, but the Netherlands has less far-reaching gender equality legislation and a more obviously gendered distribution of chores and duties than Sweden [19,20,23]. Childcare and household work are the responsibilities of Dutch women who to a large extent work part-time, while most Dutch men are the main breadwinners and work full-time. In Sweden a majority of both women and men work full-time while the children are taken care of at day-care centres [21]. Conditions in society and people's behaviour have an impact on people's attitudes, expectations and values [22], and inasmuch as Dutch students see more differences between men and women in everyday activities it seems reasonable that they perceive gender differences as more relevant and also score higher for GS than the Swedish students do.
While the statements in the GS subscale consider the importance of biological and communicative gender differences for the clinical work, the statements in the GRIP and GRID subscales contain rather outspoken and evaluative statements about how male and female patients and doctors are, for example that patients or doctors of one gender are "better", "too much" or "less", compared to the opposite gender (Table 1). Agreeing with the statements implies believing in gender differences, but also accepting a hierarchy in the differences - the features of one gender are described as being superior to and more desirable than the features of the other. In line with common gender stereotypes in society [24,25], the statements describe men as more competent, effective, instrumental and trustworthy than women, who are seen as more emotional, worried and in need of more attention and time to talk. That the Swedish students disagreed more than the Dutch with gender-stereotypical statements was not so surprising. The dominant political discourse about gender in Sweden proclaims that women and men are equal and should be treated equally [26]. These social norms reasonably influence not only students' values but also what they consider to be suitable and political correct answers in an enquiry.
Although communal conceptions about men and women underpin attitudes towards both patients and doctors, the students' gender stereotyping of patients was more pronounced than their stereotyping towards doctors. Previous research shows that people's self-reports of their instrumental and communal traits are usually less gender-stereotypical than are their estimates of the "typical person" [24,27]. Perhaps students in the first term already identify with the group of "doctors", and knowing the diversity of their own features they avoid judging doctors according to preconceptions about typical male and female behaviours?
The finding that male students held stronger gender stereotypes towards patients than their female peers is consistent with the previous Dutch study (using N-GAMS) of medical students [11], and with other research comparing gender-stereotypical attitudes in men and women [24,25]. In the present study both male and female students scored in the direction of disagreeing with stereotypes, but the female students stated more clearly that they disagree. The reason that men admit more to gender stereotypes could be that such stereotypes are in general more positive towards men, which is also true for the statements on the GRI-P subscale [25].
Proportions of women in full-time work and educational level in both men and women are often used as indicators of gender equality in a country [19,20,23]. We therefore expected that these background variables would contribute to explaining the differences in gender awareness between the medical students in the Netherlands and Sweden. However, no such clear pattern emerged. How can that be understood?
It is well described in research that prevailing norms and beliefs about gender in a society are created and maintained by multiple, complementary processes acting simultaneously and at different levels of analysis, e.g., individual, societal, political, normative and cultural levels [22,24,25]. Changes on a single level will not be sufficient to eliminate or change the gendered norm patterns or attitudes. For instance, even if political decisions aiming at gender equality have had an impact on women's career choices as well as on men's participation in traditional female tasks [19,20], studies on gender stereotypes show that the core structure of beliefs about typical men and typical women have been largely unchanged for decades [24,25,27]. Gender stereotypes have a high momentum of inertia and change slowly. Transferring this reasoning to our study, Swedish and Dutch students with parents acting in contrast to traditional gender roles are still living in societies where gendered segregation of duties is a prevailing pattern and where cultural beliefs about gender are exposed in everyday talk as well as in newspapers, books, films, and other media. Thus it becomes hard to relate the differences in attitudes between the Dutch and Swedish students to one or the other of the characteristics used as background factors. Moreover, within each country of study, the variation among the students in socio-cultural background is small and a majority of the students have similar background characteristics (Table 2). When the countries are separated and the background variables analysed in linear regression, most students are grouped in the same categories, which would explain that even the model including all background variables does not explain more of the variation in outcome on N-GAMS (Table 4). Still, our study indicates that mothers' educational level is important for gender attitudes.
Implications for education
It is important to know the students' attitudes as a take-off point in the implementation of gender in the medical curricula [12,17]. Different attitudes between the Dutch and Swedish students informs us that medical education has to take cultural and societal differences between countries into account, and even when comparing two welfare western states there are differences in values and preconceptions about gender. Inasmuch as the goal in education about gender is to make students interested and aware of the significance of gender in medical work, the examples used in discussions need to be trustworthy, relevant and challenging in the context of the specific country.
In our study the male students agreed more with gender-stereotypical statements about patients than their female peers, which is in line with previous research showing that men to a larger extent than women hold on to gender-stereotypical ideas [11,24]. Earlier studies have shown that male students are less knowledgeable about gender issues and also more sceptical about the implementation of gender in education than female students [15,16]. We suggest that these phenomena are related. Lack of insight and knowledge implies difficulties in understanding the relevance of gender in medical work, and not understanding the relevance makes you less interested in, and more sceptical about, education on gender. Furthermore, lack of interest means that you are less motivated to reflect on your own attitudes and values concerning gender and you are thus more subject to stereotyped preconceptions about men's and women's needs, wishes, and behaviour. Is it possible to break through this circular process? Yes, to some extent at least. Previous research claims that students become more positive and interested as they learn more and get used to discussing gender [15,16]. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is a lack of research about whether tendencies to make stereotyped assessments of patients decrease as a result of the implementation of gender aspects in education.
Since gender-stereotypical assessments might lead to bias and misjudgements of patients, as well as misunderstandings in collegial cooperation, it is important to find out how to help students to reduce judgments that are grounded in stereotypes [9,12]. Reflecting about one's own reactions to patients in clinical situations might be a tool that helps students to see their own attitudes and the consequences these might have for medical treatment. Such reflections can proceed from the students' own experiences from e.g. clinical training in hospitals and primary care, in prefabricated paper-cases, or in symptom presentations from simulated patients.
Our experiences from gender education indicate that, parallel to the importance of good examples to discuss and reflect on, it is crucial to create a climate for dialogue where students feel permitted to disclose ideas and attitudes, including such ideas as are not "politically correct" in a specific context. If problematic ideas and reactions are not disclosed, the students' chances to catch sight of their own attitudes and values, and to discuss them, will be small and as a result the education will have less impact. On the other hand, when a student realizes that her/his own, or other students' or staff members', values and preconceptions might lead to problems for patients, then s/he usually becomes curious and interested.
Further research about the effects of gender perspectives in education on students' attitudes and behaviour need to focus the gender topics included as well as the education methods used.
Conclusions
This study shows that the Nijmegen Gender Awareness Scale in Medicine (N-GAMS) can be used for international comparisons of medical students' attitudes and values concerning gender.
There are differences between cultures as well as between men and women in gender awareness that need to be considered when implementing gender in medical education.
Based on these results we suggest that to arouse the medical students' interest in gender issues and make them aware of the significance of gender in medical work it is crucial that the examples used in discussions are trustworthy, relevant and challenging in the context of the specific country. Due to different levels of knowledge and different attitudes within the student population it is important not only with good examples to discuss and reflect on, but also to create a climate for dialogue where students feel permitted to disclose their ideas and attitudes in order to become aware of what these are as well as their possible consequences for their future profession.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
All five authors have made contributions to the study design, acquisition and interpretation of data. TLJ and KH was responsible the Dutch and Swedish data respectively. JA was responsible for the coordination of data and performed the statistical analysis. PV developed and validated the N-GAMS scale and made substantial contributions to the analysis. JA drafted the manuscript and EJ and KH assisted in writing the manuscript. All authors have been involved in revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content and have read and approved the final manuscript.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:
Contributor Information
Jenny Andersson, Email: jenny.andersson@fammed.umu.se.
Petra Verdonk, Email: p.verdonk@vumc.nl.
Eva E Johansson, Email: eva.johansson@fammed.umu.se.
Toine Lagro-Janssen, Email: A.Lagro-Janssen@elg.umcn.nl.
Katarina Hamberg, Email: katarina.hamberg@fammed.umu.se.
Acknowledgements and funding
The research was supported by grants from the Swedish Resarch Council.
References
- Doyal L. What makes women sick: gender and the political economy of health. London: Macmillan; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Sen G, Östlin P, George A. Unequal, unfair, ineffective and inefficient, gender inequity in health: why it exists and how we can change it. Final report to the WHO Commission on Social Determinants of Health 2007. [Available from: http://www.who.int/social_determinants/resources/csdh_media/wgekn_final_report_07.pdf]
- Hall J, Roter D. Do patients talk differently to male and female physicians? A meta-analytic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2002;48:217–224. doi: 10.1016/S0738-3991(02)00174-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danielsson U, Bengs C, Lehti A, Hammarström A, Johansson E. Struck by lightning or slowly suffocating - gendered trajectories into depression. BMC Fam Pract. 2009;10:56. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-10-56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamberg K, Risberg G, Johansson E, Westman G. Gender bias in physicians' management of neck pain: a study of the answers in a Swedish national examination. J Womens Health Gend Based Med. 2002;11:653–666. doi: 10.1089/152460902760360595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiaramonte G, Friend R. Medical students' and residents' gender bias in the diagnosis, treatment, and interpretation of coronary heart disease symptoms. Health Psychol. 2006;25:255–266. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colameco S, Becker L, Simpson M. Sex bias in the assessment of patient complaints. J Fam Pract. 1983;16:1117–1121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daly C, Clemens F, Lopez Sendon J, Tavazzi L, Boersma E, Danchin N. et al. Gender differences in the management and clinical outcome of stable angina. Circulation. 2006;113:490–498. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.561647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamberg K. Gender bias in medicine. Womens Health. 2008;4:237–243. doi: 10.2217/17455057.4.3.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The National Board of Health and Welfare. A gender perspective on Swedish health care. (Swedish title: Jämställd vård? Könsperspektiv på hälso- och sjukvården) Stockholm; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Verdonk P, Benschop YWM, De Haes H, Lagro-Janssen TLM. Medical students' gender awareness. Sex Roles. 2008;8:222–234. [Google Scholar]
- Verdonk P, Benschop Y, de Haes H, Lagro-Janssen T. From gender bias to gender awareness in medical education. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14:135–152. doi: 10.1007/s10459-008-9100-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips S. Evaluating women's health and gender. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002;187:22–24. doi: 10.1067/mob.2002.127366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamberg K. Gender perspective mostly lacking at Umeå Medical School. The faculty play a key role when implementing suggestion for improvement. (Swedish title: Få och spridda genusinslag i Umeås läkarprogram. Lärarna nyckelgrupp för att sjösätta förslagen till förbättringar) Lakartidningen. 2003;100:4084–4085. 4088-4090. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainer J. Gender and the medical curriculum: a rural case study. Women Health. 2003;37:67–87. doi: 10.1300/J013v37n04_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamberg K, Johansson E. Medical students' attitudes to gender issues in the role and career of physicians: a qualitative study conducted in Sweden. Med Teach. 2006;28:635–641. doi: 10.1080/01421590600871007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson E, Hamberg K. From calling to a scheduled vocation: Swedish male and female students' reflections on being a doctor. Med Teach. 2007;29:1–8. doi: 10.1080/01421590601181463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdonk P, de Rijk A, Klinge I, de Vries A. Sickness absence as an interactive process: gendered experiences of young, highly educated women with mental health problems. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73:300–306. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visser J. The first part-time economy in the world: a model to be followed? Journal of European Social Policy. 2002;12:23–42. doi: 10.1177/0952872002012001561. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Evertsson M, England P, Mooi-Reci I, Hermsen J, de Bruijn J, Cotter D. Is Gender Inequality Greater at Lower or Higher Educational Levels? Common Patterns in the Netherlands, Sweden, and the United States. Social Politics: International studies in gender, state and society. 2009;16:210–241. doi: 10.1093/sp/jxp008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Statistics Sweden. Women and men in Sweden, Facts and figures 2008. 2008. Örebro.
- Connell RW. Gender. Cambridge: Polity; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Nordenmark M. Does gender ideology explain differences between countries regarding the involvement of women and of men in paid and unpaid work? International Journal of Social Welfare. 2004;13:233–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-6866.2004.00317.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ridgeway C Correll S Unpacking the gender system: A theoretical perspective on gender beliefs and social relations Gender & Society 200418510–531.22250032 [Google Scholar]
- Glick P, Fiske S, Mladinic A, Saiz J, Abrams D, Masser B. et al. Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:763–775. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.5.763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lagerberg R, Randecker E. Sweden - Up North, Down to earth. Stockholm: Swedish Institute; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Spence J, Buckner C. Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2000;24:44–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]