Abstract
Background
A healthy diet, low in saturated fat and high in fiber, is a popular medical recommendation in preventing cardiovascular disease (CVD). One approach to motivating healthier eating is to raise individuals’ awareness of their CVD risk and then help them form specific plans to change.
Objectives
The aim was to explore the combined impact of a Web-based CVD risk message and a fully automated planning tool on risk perceptions, intentions, and saturated fat intake changes over 4 weeks.
Methods
Of the 1187 men and women recruited online, 781 were randomly allocated to one of four conditions: a CVD risk message, the same CVD risk message paired with planning, planning on its own, and a control group. All outcome measures were assessed by online self-reports. Generalized linear modeling was used to analyze the data.
Results
Self-perceived consumption of low saturated fat foods (odds ratio 11.40, 95% CI 1.86–69.68) and intentions to change diet (odds ratio 21.20, 95% CI 2.6–172.4) increased more in participants allocated to the planning than the control group. No difference was observed between the four conditions with regard to percentage saturated fat intake changes. Contrary to our expectations, there was no difference in perceived and percentage saturated fat intake change between the CVD risk message plus planning group and the control group. Risk perceptions among those receiving the CVD risk message changed to be more in line with their age (change in slopeindividual = 0.075, P = .01; change in slopecomparative = 0.100, P = .001), whereas there was no change among those who did not receive the CVD risk message.
Conclusion
There was no evidence that combining a CVD risk message with a planning tool reduces saturated fat intake more than either alone. Further research is required to identify ways in which matching motivational and volitional strategies can lead to greater behavior changes.
Trial Registration
International Standard Randomized Controlled Trial Number (ISRCTN): 91154001; http://www.controlled-trials.com/ISRCTN91154001 (Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/62sBoGeOO)
Keywords: Risk perceptions, cardiovascular disease, planning, saturated fat intake
Introduction
Cardiovascular disease (CVD) is a leading cause of death among adults [1]. A healthy diet, low in saturated fat and high in fiber, is a popular medical recommendation in preventing CVD.
One approach to increase motivation to change is to improve awareness of the risk associated with an unhealthy lifestyle [2,3]. Risk analogies such as Heart-Age (HA) combine aspects of absolute and relative CVD risk and have been found effective in communicating future CVD risk [4,5]. In a recent study, those at higher actual CVD risk who received a HA risk analogy were more aware of their future CVD risk than were those exposed to a percentage CVD risk score [4].
Although many people report having good intentions to eat more healthily, these are not always translated into action [6,7]. Action plans, also known as implementation intentions, are strategies that can bridge the gap between intention and behavior. A meta-analysis of 94 studies showed that implementation intentions had a medium to large effect on goal achievement [8]. Fear appeals may also facilitate change when they are combined with specific instructions on what action to take [9]. While earlier studies explored the value of using action plans [7,10], more recently there has been a greater interest in the characteristics and mechanisms underlying effective plans [11-18], such as the creation of a strong cue–response relationship [12,13].
Research has also investigated the impact of self-efficacy on behavior change. According to the health action process approach model, action self-efficacy acts on the motivational part of decision making, whereas maintenance self-efficacy acts on the volitional part of the behavior [3]. While some studies report higher self-efficacy in participants making an implementation intention [19,20], others find no difference [7].
Implementation intention research to date has been largely offline (paper and pencil) with little focus and mixed results when their effectiveness has been tested online [21,22]. In a study conducted in an occupational setting, use of online implementation intentions backfired, such that participants who did not form an implementation intention exercised significantly more than participants who formed an implementation intention [21]. In an online dietary intervention, implementation intentions were combined with a text message reminder service leading to a reduction in perceived saturated fat intake and portion sizes [22]. The present study is one of a few studies designed to act on both the motivational and volitional phase of behavior change [23]. We offered a risk communication message to create more appropriate risk perceptions and to increase intention to change, and then helped individuals change their dietary behavior by forming specific plans on how to achieve this. This is also one of the few studies that compared the independent and combined short-term effects of an online health risk communication message and an online implementation intention tool on the promotion of healthy eating in an obese population, who are more likely to be at risk of developing CVD.
Objectives
The primary aim of this investigatory study was to test whether participants could form plans via a fully automated Web-based planning tool (PT) and to assess the short-term effects of combining a CVD risk message (Heart-Age, HA) with the planning tool (HA+PT) on participants’ saturated fat intake, measured by a 2-item scale (TIS) and a food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) over a period of 3 weeks. A secondary aim was to assess the effects of the heart-age risk message and planning tool on participants’ risk perceptions, self-efficacy, intentions to change saturated fat intake, and intentions to test cholesterol and blood pressure levels. We expected that the heart-age message would primarily change risk perceptions and participants’ intentions to change, while the planning tool would act primarily on self-efficacy and behavior. We wanted to explore whether participants could form Web-based plans and whether the combined HA+PT intervention would have a greater impact than either the heart-age message or the planning tool alone.
Methods
Participants
We invited 1187 participants through an online recruitment agency to log in to an open access website to take part in the study. The self-report eligibility criteria included age (30–60 years), obesity (body mass index [BMI] ≥29 kg/m2), not having a diagnosis of a heart condition or cancer or being pregnant, and being computer savvy. We chose obese participants because they were likely to benefit from heart-health information [24]. To help minimize any imbalance effects created by smokers receiving a higher heart-age score, a UK-representative sample of smokers was distributed across the four conditions of the study.
Design and Procedures
This study was conducted between the middle of January and the end of February 2009 and has been registered retrospectively. It was a Web-based, randomized, between-groups study designed to assess the difference in saturated fat intake between four experimental conditions. No participant–experimenter contact was present. Participants were given online instructions and completed each week’s session from the convenience of their home computer. At week 1 (recruitment), participants were recruited by an online agency, signed an online consent form [25], and completed an online questionnaire on their current saturated fat intake, risk perceptions, self-efficacy, and intentions to change their dietary intake. They also received educational information on the importance of a healthy diet low in saturated fat (Multimedia Appendix 1).
At week 2 (intervention), those participants who returned to the website were randomly allocated, using a computer-generated list of random numbers, into one of four conditions: (1) control group (CG), (2) PT condition, (3) HA risk message condition, and (4) HA+PT condition. Allocation of the participants in the four conditions was also stratified to balance by age group (30–45 years or 46–60 years) and gender. In the groups that received the HA risk message, participants filled out online information on their age, gender, weight, height, prescribed blood pressure medication, family history of heart and vascular disease, smoking status, self-prevalent diabetes, self-reported total and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels, and systolic blood pressure. They then received feedback on their future CVD risk in the form of the HA risk message. Participants in the PT condition were asked to identify a list of situations in which they would like to change their saturated fat intake and match these situations with a list of behaviors. Participants in all conditions were asked to fill out a shorter version of the questionnaire asked at baseline. At week 2, participants completed the session once and were not able to revisit the website to make any changes (eg, to create more plans).
At week 5 (follow-up), participants were asked to complete a follow-up assessment. They received £15 on study completion and were entered in a prize draw for vouchers (£200).
Interventions
The Heart-Age Risk Message Condition
Heart-age, which is described in more detail elsewhere [26], is the age corresponding to someone of the same gender with the same CVD risk level but with normal risk factors. The definition of normal is based on the following profile: not smoking, not diabetic, systolic blood pressure 125 mmHg (midpoint of normal range: 120–130 mmHg), total serum cholesterol 180 mg/dL (4.66 mmol/L; between normal range of 160–200 mg/dL or 4.14–5.18 mmol/L), and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol 45 mg/dL (1.17 mmol/L). For example, a 61-year-old man who smokes and has no other risk factors has a 10-year CVD risk of 10% and the HA of a 73-year-old man. In the HA condition, users filled in an online questionnaire and received feedback in form of the HA risk message (Figure 1).
The Planning Tool Condition
Participants who received the PT selected from a list of 13 situations, in which they were tempted to eat unhealthily and then chose an approach to change their behavior from a list of 13 solutions. For every situation–solution pair chosen, a line was drawn visually linking the two together [27]. Participants were asked to complete at least 3 situation–solution pairs.
The solutions were based on constructs from the processes of change model (eg, counterconditioning, stimulus control, and helpful relationships) [28]. Some nutritionally based behaviors were also included from an accredited site [29] after review by an expert nutritionist. The list of situations consisted of both situational cues (eg, having lunch) focusing on the “when and where” and motivational cues (eg, feeling bored) linked to the reasons (“why”) for performing a specific behavior [30]. Motivational cues were divided into three main situations: (1) experiencing positive affect, (2) experiencing negative affect, and (3) being faced with cravings [31,32]. The situations were translated into “if” statements (eg, “If I’m having breakfast”) and the list of solutions was translated into “then” statements (eg, “then I will tell myself I can eat healthily”). Figure 2 shows the PT.
Control Group Condition
Participants in the CG received educational information on the importance of a healthy diet low in saturated at week 1 and filled out the same online questionnaires as the rest of the experimental conditions at all study weeks.
Outcome Measures
Saturated fat intake, as the primary outcome measure, was assessed at baseline and follow-up by two measures. First, a self-report index of food [33] was used to record the frequency of consumption of 63 common foods. This FFQ has good test–retest reliability (r = .62, P < .01) [33] and validity when compared with 10-day weighed records [34,35]. Second, a two item scale (TIS) (r = .78, P < .001) was adapted from a previous study [34]. Participants were asked to report their agreement in consumption of low saturated fat foods (“I have eaten foods low in saturated fat...”) followed by frequency in consumption of these foods (“How often did you eat foods low in saturated fat?”). The correlation between the two measures was –0.320 (P < .001) at week 1 and –0.291 (P < .001) at week 5. Negative correlations are due to reverse scales used for the self-perceived items.
CVD risk perceptions measured participants’ perceived risk in an absolute sense and comparative with their age group [4]. The first item (Q1) examined perceptions of individual CVD risk (“I think that my chances of getting heart disease in the short term are...”). The second item (Q2) compared participants’ risk perceptions against those of other people of their age (“Compared to an average person of my age and sex, my chances of getting heart disease are...”). Responses were measured on a 7-point Likert scale at weeks 1 and 2.
Intention to reduce saturated fat intake was measured at weeks 1, 2, and 5 on a 7-point Likert scale via 10 items, which were highly intercorrelated (Cronbach alpha = .92), so were analyzed as a composite score. At follow-up, there were two further questions on participants’ intentions to assess their cholesterol and blood pressure over the next month.
Action and maintenance self-efficacy were modified from previous research [3,36-38]. Action self-efficacy (alpha = .84), which was measured at all study times, consisted of 4 items focusing on confidence to overcome obstacles. Maintenance self-efficacy (alpha = .89), assessed only at follow-up, consisted of 11 items exploring confidence in sustaining change in the face of difficulties. Items were measured on a 4-point scale (not at all, barely true, mostly true, exactly true).
Planning and outcome expectancies items were adapted from previous research [3,36-38] and measured on a 4-point scale. Planning comprised 2 items: “I have my own plan regarding (1) when, (2) how to reduce my saturated fat intake.” Outcome expectancies consisted of 11 items linked to the positive and negative expectancies of reducing saturated fat intake (eg, “If I reduce my saturated fat intake”... “food won’t taste as good,” “I will feel good”).
Feedback on the intervention was assessed at week 2 and at follow-up on a 7-point Likert scale (ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree). Participants were asked to rate the intervention in terms of its emotional impact, personal relevance, interest, trustworthiness, credibility, and enjoyment. All items were adapted from previous studies [39-41].
Statistical Considerations
Analysis of the outcome measures was restricted to those respondents who completed the follow-up assessments. Response to the CVD risk perceptions was analyzed using a generalized linear model with a cumulative logistic link function and multinomial distribution. Baseline scores and heart risk-adjusted age were included as covariates. As with all the analyses other potential covariates (eg, smoking, BMI, social economic status) were retained if significant in the model. Similar models were used for the intention-to-change and intention-to-test questions, and self-efficacy, planning, and feedback items, but omitting the heart risk-adjusted age covariate.
Mean change in self-perceived saturated fat intake within a group was assessed using analysis of variance with baseline included as a covariate. The groups were compared using another generalized linear model with a cumulative logistic link function and multinomial distribution. Data from the index of food was summarized to yield the total calorie intake per participant and the percentage of total energy intake contributed by saturated fat. We analyzed all of these data using analysis of variance models with baseline covariates always included and any other significant covariates retained. All analysis was carried out using version 9.1.3 of SAS software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).
Local Research Ethical Review Requirement
The study protocol (Multimedia Appendix 1) was approved by an independent research ethics committee (Colworth Research Ethics Committee) in the South of England on December 4, 2008 (Multimedia Appendix 2). All research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki [42].
Results
Participant Baseline Characteristics
At week 1, we invited 1187 people to participate through an online recruitment agency, of whom 1027 completed the initial questionnaire and were invited to take part in the study. At week 2, a total of 781 participants revisited the website and were allocated to one of four conditions. At week 2, 32 of these participants did not complete the online session. At week 5, a total of 581 participants returned to complete the follow-up questionnaire. We excluded 21 participants from the statistical analysis because they did not complete the whole session or due to inaccurate calorie intake reporting (<500 kcal or >5000 kcal per day). The numbers of participants completing each week are shown in Figure 3.
There was no significant difference in percentage saturated fat intake between participants who completed only the week 1 assessment and those who completed the week 5 measures (P = .79). The mean percentage saturated fat intake at week 1 (baseline) was 15.4%, much higher than the UK recommended levels [43]. Table 1 shows participants’ baseline characteristics.
Table 1.
Overall | Control group | Planning group | Heart-Age group | Planning plus Heart-Age group | F3,777 statistic (P value) | |
Number | 781 | 195 | 195 | 197 | 194 | |
Age (years)a, mean (SD) | 46.89 (8.26) | 47.05 (8.48) | 47.06 (8.11) | 46.91 (8.00) | 46.56 (8.52) | 0.15 (.93) |
BMIb (kg/m2)a, mean (SD) | 35.71 (5.71) | 35.72 (5.40) | 35.51 (6.15) | 36.49 (6.42) | 35.08 (4.64) | 2.10 (.1) |
Smokers (%) | 25.61 | 25.13 | 25.64 | 25.26 | 26.40 | 0.10c (.99) |
a No significant differences found in participants’ baseline characteristics (P > .05).
b Body mass index.
c Chi-square test (c2 3) statistic and P value.
Planning Tool
All participants allocated to the PT condition were able to formulate their Web-based plans with an average of 3.9 plans. Participants selected a range of motivational and situational cues. The most frequently chosen situations were “If I’m feeling hungry” (99/747, 13%), “If I’m getting a snack” (97/747, 13%), “If I’m having lunch” (71/747, 10%) “or dinner” (74/747, 10%), “If I’m craving a high-fat food” (66/747, 9%), and “If I’m feeling down or upset” (72/747, 10%).
The most frequently selected solutions were “Then I will go for fruit” (149/747, 19.7%), “Then I will find out about a lower-fat option” (105/747, 14.1%), “Then I will go for grilled/steamed poultry or fish” (85/747, 11%), “Then I will distract myself with something else” (70/747, 9%), and “Then I will tell myself if I try hard I can eat healthily” (66/747, 9%).
Time Spent Online
At week 1, participants spent an average of 12.44 (SD 9.77) minutes online. At week 2, the CG spent the least time online (mean 4.19, SD 2.43 minutes), followed by the PT (mean 7.84, SD 5.18 minutes), the HA (mean 10.91, SD 8.46 minutes), and lastly the HA+PT group (mean 12.47, SD 6.48 minutes). HA+PT spent significantly more time online than the PT group (95% CI, 2.73–6.53) or the CG (95% CI, 6.39–10.18). No significant differences were found at week 2 between the HA+PT and the HA-only condition (95% CI, –0.34 to 3.46). At week 5, there were no further significant differences (P = .67) between the four conditions in time spent filling out the follow-up questionnaire (CG: mean 9.54, SD 3.62 minutes; PT: mean 11.38, SD 9.84 minutes, HA: mean 10.44, SD 5.47 minutes; HA+PT: mean 9.64, SD 6.32 minutes).
Primary Outcomes
Saturated Fat Intake
Participants in all four conditions reported a significant increase in consumption of foods low in saturated fat (the mean of the two self-perceived intake items) between baseline and follow-up, apart from the CG (Table 2). The generalized linear model analysis showed a significant difference between the conditions (c2 3 = 13.1, P = .005) with respect to perceived saturated fat intake changes. Multiple comparisons of the conditions (with Bonferroni adjustment to allow for the six comparisons) indicated this was due to participants in the PT group reporting a higher perceived increase in low saturated fat foods than those in the CG (odds ratio, 11.40; 95% CI, 1.86–69.68).
Table 2.
Condition | Self-perceived items (baseline mean 4.73) | Index of food questionnaire (baseline mean 15.37%) | ||||
Week 5 | Week 5 – week 1a | Pr > |t| | Week 5 | Week 5 – week 1a | Pr > |t| | |
Control group | 4.857 | 0.125 (0.100) | .21 | 14.67 | –0.717 (0.198) | .0003 |
Planning tool | 5.087 | 0.355 (0.102) | .001 | 14.51 | –0.875 (0.198) | <.0001 |
Heart-Age | 4.943 | 0.212 (0.101) | .04 | 14.63 | –0.748 (0.197) | .0002 |
Heart-Age + planning tool | 4.977 | 0.245 (0.102) | .02 | 14.49 | –0.893 (0.200) | <.0001 |
a Mean and standard error after adjusting for baseline and other covariates. Note that the standard error for week 5 is the same as the standard error for weeks 5 – 1, due to the use of a baseline covariate in the analysis.
With regard to the index of food, participants in all conditions reported a significant reduction in percentage saturated fat intake between baseline and follow-up (Table 2 with no significant differences found between the four conditions (P = .89).
Secondary Outcomes
Risk Perceptions
The generalized linear model found no significant differences between the four experimental conditions in terms of their CVD risk perceptions, both for individual (Q1) (P = .88) and comparative risk (Q2) (P = .93). In order to test whether perceived risk was more related to actual risk, we further compared the change in perceived risk between week 2 and week 1 for all participants who received the HA risk message (HA, HA+PT) with those who did not (PT, CG) using a further generalized linear model. Figure 4 shows risk perception changes for Q1 and Q2 split by the different HA risk levels (low: 0–5, moderate: 5–10, and high: 10–15). HA level is the difference between an individual’s actual age and his or her risk-adjusted age. For example, the 0–5 HA level includes people whose HA is up to 5 years older than their actual age. For both Q1 and Q2, the regression slopes for those in the HA conditions moved to be more in line with participants’ HA risk levels, whereas this was not found for those in the non-HA conditions.
Specifically, for participants in the HA risk message conditions, there was a significant increase in the regression slope of the individual risk perception question (Q1) against the risk-adjusted age (change in slope 0.075, SE 0.029, P = .01) after participants were shown the risk message. There was no significant change (change in slope –0.027, SE 0.031, P = .38) for those participants in the non-HA risk message conditions. A similar pattern was found for the comparative risk question (Q2), with a statistically significant change in the regression slope against the risk-adjusted age for the HA risk message groups (change in slope 0.100, SE 0.030, P = .001), but no significant change for the non-HA risk message groups (change in slope –0.029, SE 0.030, P = .34).
Intentions to Change
Generalized linear modeling showed that change in intention to reduce saturated fat intake at week 2 compared with week 1 was significantly influenced by condition (c2 3 = 18.8, P < .001). Multiple comparisons between conditions (with a Bonferroni adjustment to allow for the six comparisons) showed that participants in the PT condition had a much higher intention than those allocated to the CG (odds ratio, 21.20; 95% CI, 2.6–172.4) or the HA risk message condition (odds ratio, 0.04; 95% CI, 0.0054–0.42).
There were no significant differences between the conditions for intention to take a cholesterol (P = .38) or blood pressure test within the next month (P = .90). There was a significant gender-by-group interaction (c2 3 = 13.6, P = .004). Comparisons within the interaction effect (with a Bonferroni multiplicity adjustment) indicated that women who received the HA risk message were more motivated than the women in the CG to get their cholesterol tested within the next month (odds ratio, 2.46; 95% CI, 1.14–5.28). The same was true for women when the HA+PT condition was compared against the CG (odds ratio, 2.60; 95% CI, 1.18–5.76). There was no significant effect of condition on intention to test blood pressure (c2 3 = 0.8, P = .85) and no interaction with gender.
Self-Efficacy
The generalized linear model showed that action self-efficacy measured at week 2 differed significantly between the conditions (c2 3 = 16.6, P < .001). This was due to participants in the PT group being more confident than those in the CG (odds ratio, 3.06; 95% CI, 1.40–6.66). This difference was not statistically significant at week 5 (c2 3 = 7.1, P = .07). Maintenance self-efficacy measured at week 5 was not significantly different between the four conditions (P = .45).
Planning and Outcome Expectancies
At week 5, there was no significant difference in the “how” (P = .87) or “when” (P = .60) to reduce saturated fat intake between the four conditions. There were no significant effects of conditions for any of the outcome expectancy items.
Feedback on the Intervention
At week 2, there was a significant difference in perceived trustworthiness (c2 3 = 8.9, P = .03), with those receiving the HA+PT reporting the intervention to be less trustworthy than those receiving the PT alone (mean 5.6 vs 5.9). There was also a difference between conditions for “informative” (c2 3 = 14.3, P = .003) with HA+PT being perceived as less informative than the HA alone or the CG (mean 5.8 vs 6.1 vs 6.04). There was an overall difference in “worried” scores (c2 3 = 4.8, P = .03). The HA+PT (mean 4.6) and the HA risk message participants (mean 4.7) were more worried than the PT participants (mean 4.0). All other feedback items were not significant. At week 5, there was still a significant difference between the conditions for “interesting” (c2 3 = 8.6, P = .04), with the HA+PT participants still reporting the experience as less interesting than those receiving HA alone (mean 5.4 vs 5.7).
Discussion
Principal Results
In this study, a fully automated planning tool was successfully used by participants to form a set of health plans. The planning tool boosted self-efficacy and intention and reduced perceived saturated fat intake for one of the measures (TIS) but not the other (FFQ). A CVD risk message improved people’s awareness of their risk relative to their age. Contrary to our expectations, combining a CVD risk message with the planning tool did not lead to bigger reductions in saturated fat intake than when they were presented on their own.
In line with theories of behavior change [36-38], the planning tool was better than the control group at increasing self-perceived consumption of low saturated fat foods (TIS). The same finding was not true for our second measure of saturated fat intake (FFQ). Also, participants in all conditions reported a change in percentage saturated fat intake measured by the FFQ, whereas participants in all conditions apart from the control group reported a change in their TIS score. Similar findings in terms of discrepancies between the FFQ and self-perceived items have been reported before [17]. This implies that the two self-perceived saturated fat intake items were better able than the FFQ measure to differentiate between the conditions. However, both come with limitations, which we discuss in the next section.
In line with our hypothesis, the planning tool was also better than the control group at boosting participants’ intentions to reduce saturated fat [18] and action self-efficacy in the short term [19,20]. However, maintenance self-efficacy did not differ between the conditions at follow-up. This might be because participants who formed plans and encountered difficulties needed further support (eg, coping plans) to maintain their healthy eating. A previous study found that action plans are more effective at the early stages of change, while coping plans are instrumental at later stages [37].
In support of previous studies, receiving the heart-age risk message led to more appropriate risk perceptions [4,5,44], linked to participants’ risk relative to their age group. Presentation of risk information also increased women’s intentions to test their cholesterol. The latter finding is important because people who are aware of their cholesterol levels can receive more precise risk estimates.
Contrary to our expectations, combining the heart-age risk message with the planning tool (HA+PT) did not lead to a bigger reduction in saturated fat intake. A mismatch might have been created between the global CVD risk message and the specific target plan, confusing smokers with a high heart-age, who saw smoking cessation as the primary route to better health rather than diet. Alternatively, cognitive overload might have confounded the impact of HA+PT on saturated fat intake [45,46]. The length of time spent interacting online may also have been a factor, with the HA+PT taking the longest (12.47 minutes vs 10.91 for HA and 7.84 for PT). Future research could explore whether there is a benefit from reducing cognitive load through the use of a delay between presenting risk information and forming plans.
Limitations, Advantages, and Future Studies
The impact of conditions on our two measures of saturated fat intake changes was inconsistent, and this could be due to the limitations present in the FFQ and the TIS. Underreporting of food consumption is a recurrent challenge for FFQs and is most pronounced among overweight and obese people [47]. Also, FFQs were initially designed to estimate individual intake relative to a population rather than to detect small changes in individual dietary intake [33,48], for which they might not be sufficiently sensitive. The present FFQ did not account for individual variation in portion sizes but instead assumed the average portion of the UK population [33], which might differ from portions consumed by our obese participants.
On the other hand, self-perceived items like the TIS have been designed to detect differences between conditions in experimental studies [49]. However, some have claimed that reported changes are influenced by demand characteristics [50], with participants in more active conditions being more aware of study aims and so responding differently. Two previous studies counter the argument of demand characteristics by showing no difference between conditions for awareness of the study’s hypothesis or feelings of obligation to comply [17,21]. Further research is needed to improve our ability to measure change in dietary intake (eg, through more objective measures).
To our knowledge, our planning tool is the first fully automated system to test online if–then plans in the format of an interactive volitional help sheet. An advantage of our approach was that participants could choose more personally relevant situations [30] from the list, promoting a sense of autonomy [51]. However, a disadvantage is that the list did not include highly idiosyncratic situations that a participant might have entered through a free-text entry approach. Future studies could evaluate the relative impact of guiding participants to appropriate cues versus giving them complete autonomy.
As this was the first evaluation of a fully automated PT, we used a completers, per-protocol analysis, which, although limiting interpretation of the application of our results, allowed us to focus on the impact of the tool when used appropriately. Further research is needed to test the effectiveness of an implementation intention-based automated PT at a population level (via intention-to-treat analysis) over longer periods of time and to evaluate the impact of reminders [52-55].
Another advantage of the current study was that we assessed risk perceptions at two time points, giving us the opportunity to measure change in risk perceptions. Also, whereas previous research has used fictitious illnesses and hypothetical scenarios to communicate risk [23,41], our study risk corresponded to participants’ personal characteristics, making it more relevant. To the best of our knowledge this is the first study to combine implementation intentions with personally relevant health risk information through a Web-based medium.
Conclusions
Web-based tools provide a good opportunity to present risk information and plan behavior change. In the present study, the HA risk message helped improve obese people’s awareness of risk relative to their age, and the PT reduced levels of perceived saturated fat intake. Future research is required to identify ways of matching motivational and volitional strategies to change behavior.
Acknowledgments
Andrew Hinton and Steve Baker from Tessella PLC had full access to all of the data and take responsibility for the integrity of the data. Virtual Surveys PLC was in charge of the online recruitment of participants.
Abbreviations
- CG
control group
- CVD
cardiovascular disease
- FFQ
food frequency questionnaire
- HA
Heart-Age
- PT
planning tool
- Q1
individual risk question
- Q2
comparative risk question
- TIS
2-item scale
Multimedia Appendix 1
Research protocol.
Multimedia Appendix 2
Ethics committee approval document.
Footnotes
This exploratory study was funded by Unilever, in which several authors are employees. Heart-Age is a commercially available product developed by Unilever and used as the basis of risk communication, while the planning tool is a non-commercially available research vehicle.
References
- 1.US Department of Health and Human Services . Health, United States 2003: With Chartbook on Trends in the Health of Americans (Health United States) Washington, DC: Congress; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Janz NK, Becker MH. The Health Belief Model: a decade later. Health Educ Q. 1984;11(1):1–47. doi: 10.1177/109019818401100101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schwarzer R. Modelling health behaviour change: how to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviours. Appl Psychol Int Rev. 2008;57(1):1–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Soureti A, Hurling R, Murray P, van Mechelen W, Cobain M. Evaluation of a cardiovascular disease risk assessment tool for the promotion of healthier lifestyles. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2010 Oct;17(5):519–23. doi: 10.1097/HJR.0b013e328337ccd3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Parkes G, Greenhalgh T, Griffin M, Dent R. Effect on smoking quit rate of telling patients their lung age: the Step2quit randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2008 Mar 15;336(7644):598–600. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39503.582396.25. http://www.bmj.com/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=18326503.bmj.39503.582396.25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006 Mar;132(2):249–68. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249.2006-03023-004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Milne S, Orbell S, Sheeran P. Combining motivational and volitional interventions to promote exercise participation: protection motivation theory and implementation intentions. Br J Health Psychol. 2002 May;7(Pt 2):163–84. doi: 10.1348/135910702169420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;38:69–111. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000 Oct;27(5):591–615. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Orbell S, Sheeran P. Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: a field study of the role of implementation intentions. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2000;30(4):780–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02823.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Webb TL, Sheeran P. Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: the role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. Br J Soc Psychol. 2008 Sep;47(Pt 3):373–95. doi: 10.1348/014466607X267010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Webb TL, Sheeran P. How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2007;43(2):295–302. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gollwitzer PM. Goal achievement: the role of intentions. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 1993;4(1):141–85. doi: 10.1080/14792779343000059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A, Midden C. To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviours. Eur J Social Psychol. 1999;29(8):971–9. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A. Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000 Jan;78(1):53–63. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Oettingen G, Honig G, Gollwitzer PM. Effective self-regulation of goal attainment. Int J Educ Res. 2000;33(7-8):705–32. doi: 10.1016/S0883-0355(00)00046-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Chapman J, Armitage CJ, Norman P. Comparing implementation intention interventions in relation to young adults' intake of fruit and vegetables. Psychol Health. 2009 Mar;24(3):317–32. doi: 10.1080/08870440701864538.791416506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Armitage CJ. Evidence that implementation intentions reduce dietary fat intake: a randomized trial. Health Psychol. 2004 May;23(3):319–23. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.3.319.2004-13299-013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Murray TC, Rodgers WM, Shawn NF. Examining implementation intentions in an exercise intervention: the effects on adherence and self-efficacy in a naturalistic setting. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2009;39:2303–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00527.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rodgers WM, Hall CR, Blanchard CM, McAuley E, Munroe KJ. Task and scheduling self-efficacy as predictors of exercise behavior. Psychol Health. 2002;17(4):405–16. doi: 10.1080/0887044022000004902. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Budden JS, Sagarin BJ. Implementation intentions, occupational stress, and the exercise intention-behavior relationship. J Occup Health Psychol. 2007 Oct;12(4):391–401. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.391.2007-15130-006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Soureti A, Murray P, Cobain M, Chinapaw M, van Mechelen W, Hurling R. A study exploring the impact of on an online planning tool and mobile text reminder service on healthier dietary behaviours. J Med Internet Res. 2011 doi: 10.2196/jmir.1773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Prestwich A, Ayres K, Lawton R. Crossing two types of implementation intentions with a protection motivation intervention for the reduction of saturated fat intake: a randomized trial. Soc Sci Med. 2008 Nov;67(10):1550–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.07.019.S0277-9536(08)00385-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Renner B, Knoll N, Schwarzer R. Age and body make a difference in optimistic health beliefs and nutrition behaviours. Int J Behav Med. 2000;7(2):143–59. doi: 10.1207/S15327558IJBM0702_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Varnhagen CK, Gushta M, Daniels J, Peters TC, Parmar N, Law D, Hirsch R, Takach BS, Johnson T. How informed is online informed consent? Ethics Behav. 2005;15(1):37–48. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb1501_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.D'Agostino RB, Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, Wolf PA, Cobain M, Massaro JM, Kannel WB. General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care: the Framingham Heart Study. Circulation. 2008 Feb 12;117(6):743–53. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.699579. http://circ.ahajournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=18212285.CIRCULATIONAHA.107.699579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Armitage CJ. A volitional help sheet to encourage smoking cessation: a randomized exploratory trial. Health Psychol. 2008 Sep;27(5):557–66. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.557.2008-13168-007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Prochaska JO. Systems of Psychotherapy: A Transtheoretical Analysis. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 29.UK Food Standards Agency. [2010-02-01]. Saturated fat http://www.eatwell.gov.uk/healthydiet/fss/fats/satfat/
- 30.Adriaanse MA, de Ridder DT, de Wit JB. Finding the critical cue: implementation intentions to change one's diet work best when tailored to personally relevant reasons for unhealthy eating. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009 Jan;35(1):60–71. doi: 10.1177/0146167208325612.35/1/60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rossi SR, Greene GW, Rossi JS, Plummer BA, Benisovich SV, Keller S, Velicer WF, Redding CA, Prochaska JO, Pallonen UE, Meier KS. Validation of decisional balance and situational temptations measures for dietary fat reduction in a large school-based population of adolescents. Eat Behav. 2001;2(1):1–18. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(00)00019-2.S1471-0153(00)00019-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Velicer WF, Diclemente CC, Rossi JS, Prochaska JO. Relapse situations and self-efficacy: an integrative model. Addict Behav. 1990;15(3):271–83. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90070-e.0306-4603(90)90070-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Margetts BM, Cade JE, Osmond C. Comparison of a food frequency questionnaire with a diet record. Int J Epidemiol. 1989 Dec;18(4):868–73. doi: 10.1093/ije/18.4.868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Armitage CJ, Conner M. The theory of planned behaviour: assessment of predictive validity and 'perceived control'. Br J Soc Psychol. 1999;38(1):35–54. doi: 10.1348/014466699164022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Thompson RL, Margetts BM. Comparison of a food frequency questionnaire with a 10-day weighed record in cigarette smokers. Int J Epidemiol. 1993 Oct;22(5):824–33. doi: 10.1093/ije/22.5.824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Renner B, Schwarzer R. International University Bremen and Freie Universität Berlin. [2011-06-27]. Risk and Health Behaviors: Documentation of the Scales of the Research Project: "Risk Appraisal Consequences in Korea" (RACK). 2nd edition http://www.gesundheitsrisiko.de/docs/RACKEnglish.pdf.
- 37.Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R. Bridging the intention-behaviour gap: planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychol Health. 2005;20(2):143–60. doi: 10.1080/08870440512331317670. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schwarzer R, Sniehotta FF, Lippke S, Luszczynska A, Scholz U, Schüz B, Wegner M, Ziegelman J. Freie Universität Berlin. 2003. Nov 1, [2011-06-27]. On the Assessment and Analysis of Variables in the Health Action Process Approach: Conducting an Investigation http://web.fu-berlin.de/gesund/hapa_web.pdf.
- 39.De Bourdeaudhuij I, Brug J. Tailoring dietary feedback to reduce fat intake: an intervention at the family level. Health Educ Res. 2000 Aug;15(4):449–62. doi: 10.1093/her/15.4.449. http://her.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=11066462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Brug J, Glanz K, Van Assema P, Kok G, van Breukelen GJ. The impact of computer-tailored feedback and iterative feedback on fat, fruit, and vegetable intake. Health Educ Behav. 1998 Aug;25(4):517–31. doi: 10.1177/109019819802500409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Fair AK, Murray PG, Thomas A, Cobain MR. Using hypothetical data to assess the effect of numerical format and context on the perception of coronary heart disease risk. Am J Health Promot. 2008;22(4):291–6. doi: 10.4278/061030140R2.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Anonymous World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA. 2000 Dec 20;284(23):3043–5.jsc00472 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.BBC Health. 2010. Jan, [2010-03-11]. Nutrition: Healthy Eating: Fats and Sugars http://www.bbc.co.uk/health/healthy_living/nutrition/healthy_fatsugar.shtml.
- 44.Grover SA, Lowensteyn I, Joseph L, Kaouache M, Marchand S, Coupal L, Boudreau G, Cardiovascular Health Evaluation to Improve Compliance and Knowledge Among Uninformed Patients (CHECK-UP) Study Group Patient knowledge of coronary risk profile improves the effectiveness of dyslipidemia therapy: the CHECK-UP study: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. 2007 Nov 26;167(21):2296–303. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.21.2296. http://archinte.ama-assn.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=18039987.167/21/2296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dewitte S, Pandalaere M, Briers B, Warlog L. Social Science Research Network. 2005. Oct, [2011-10-31]. Cognitive Load Has Negative After Effects on Consumer Decision Making http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=813684.
- 46.Ward A, Mann T. Don't mind if I do: disinhibited eating under cognitive load. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000 Apr;78(4):753–63. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.4.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Garriguet D. Under-reporting of energy intake in the Canadian Community Health Survey. Health Rep. 2008 Dec;19(4):37–45. http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/82-003-x/2008004/article/10703-eng.pdf. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cade JE, Burley VJ, Warm DL, Thompson RL, Margetts BM. Food-frequency questionnaires: a review of their design, validation and utilisation. Nutr Res Rev. 2004 Jun;17(1):5–22. doi: 10.1079/NRR200370.S0954422404000022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Chapman J, Armitage CJ. Evidence that boosters augment the long-term impact of implementation intentions on fruit and vegetable intake. Psychol Health. 2010 Mar;25(3):365–81. doi: 10.1080/08870440802642148.908219487 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Faith MS, Wong FY, Allison DB. Demand characteristics of the research setting can influence indexes of negative affect-induced eating in obese individuals. Obes Res. 1998 Mar;6(2):134–6. doi: 10.1002/j.1550-8528.1998.tb00327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Koestner R, Horberg EJ, Gaudreau P, Powers T, Di Dio P, Bryan C, Jochum R, Salter N. Bolstering implementation plans for the long haul: the benefits of simultaneously boosting self-concordance or self-efficacy. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2006 Nov;32(11):1547–58. doi: 10.1177/0146167206291782.32/11/1547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Patrick K, Raab F, Adams MA, Dillon L, Zabinski M, Rock CL, Griswold WG, Norman GJ. A text message-based intervention for weight loss: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2009;11(1):e1. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1100. http://www.jmir.org/2009/1/e1/v11i1e1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hurling R, Catt M, Boni MD, Fairley BW, Hurst T, Murray P, Richardson A, Sodhi JS. Using internet and mobile phone technology to deliver an automated physical activity program: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2007 Apr;9(2):e7. doi: 10.2196/jmir.9.2.e7. http://www.jmir.org/2007/2/e7/v9i2e7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ware LJ, Hurling R, Bataveljic O, Fairley BW, Hurst TL, Murray P, Rennie KL, Tomkins CE, Finn A, Cobain MR, Pearson DA, Foreyt JP. Rates and determinants of uptake and use of an internet physical activity and weight management program in office and manufacturing work sites in England: cohort study. J Med Internet Res. 2008;10(4):e56. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1108. http://www.jmir.org/2008/4/e56/v10i4e56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Prestwich A, Perugini M, Hurling R. Can implementation intentions and text messages promote brisk walking? A randomized trial. Health Psychol. 2010 Jan;29(1):40–9. doi: 10.1037/a0016993.2010-00152-006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]