Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Feb 17.
Published in final edited form as: J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2012 Feb 1;29(2):A182–A187. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A182

Uniform color spaces and natural image statistics

Kyle C McDermott 1, Michael A Webster 1
PMCID: PMC3281518  NIHMSID: NIHMS344805  PMID: 22330376

Abstract

Many aspects of visual coding have been successfully predicted by starting from the statistics of natural scenes and then asking how the stimulus could be efficiently represented. We started from the representation of color characterized by uniform color spaces, and then asked what type of color environment they implied. These spaces are designed to represent equal perceptual differences in color discrimination or appearance by equal distances in the space. The relative sensitivity to different axes within the space might therefore reflect the gamut of colors in natural scenes. To examine this, we projected perceptually uniform distributions within the Munsell, CIEL*u*v* or CIEL*a*b* spaces into cone-opponent space. All were elongated along a bluish-yellowish axis reflecting covarying signals along the L-M and S-L+M cardinal axes, a pattern typical (though not identical) to many natural environments. In turn, color distributions from environments were more uniform when projected into the CIEL*a*b* perceptual space than when represented in a normalized cone-opponent space. These analyses suggest the bluish-yellowish bias in environmental colors might be an important factor shaping chromatic sensitivity, and also suggest that perceptually uniform color metrics could be derived from natural scene statistics and potentially tailored to specific environments.

Introduction

Uniform color spaces are designed with the aim that equal distances in the space correspond to color differences of equal perceptual magnitude. Color differences do not vary linearly with changes in the stimulus spectrum, and thus it is well known that linear color spaces, for example based on the spectral sensitivities of the cones or cone-opponent channels, are not perceptually uniform. Uniform spaces must therefore apply distortions of the cone excitations in order to compensate for the variations in visual sensitivity and appearance for different stimuli and viewing conditions.

A number of uniform color spaces have been developed and are in wide use14. The structure of these spaces is based on empirical measurements of color discrimination and appearance. For example, the Munsell and Natural Color systems are color order systems and were derived from scaling experiments measuring the suprathreshold appearance differences between samples differing along defined dimensions corresponding to lightness, value, or chroma or to the Hering primaries5, 6. Other spaces such as CIE L*u*v* and L*a*b* were based on transformations of the CIE 1931 tristimulus values so that threshold color differences were equated in different regions of color space7, 8, and are designed to provide a uniform metric for representing color differences. Different perceptual color spaces or order systems thus approximately embody the characteristics of human color vision at threshold and/or suprathreshold levels. But what gives rise to these characteristics?

A powerful approach to understanding visual coding has been to characterize the properties of the visual environment and then ask what these properties predict about visual representations911. Many previous studies have adopted this strategy to specifically examine the visual encoding of color. For example, the distribution of intensity levels or chromaticities in natural scenes is not flat but instead is strongly peaked, so that most points in the scene have lower contrast. The most efficient representation of contrast should give equal importance to all possible levels of the channel’s output, and this predicts the sigmoidal response of the neural contrast response (so that the response changes rapidly at stimulus levels that are common while saturating at levels that are rare)12, 13. The probability distributions of colors in scenes have also been used to infer the number and organization of semantic categories required to represent color14, 15 and lightness16. Similar arguments have been used to predict the relative sensitivity to different color directions based on the relative range of the stimulus distribution along different axes (e.g. so that sensitivity to the cone contrasts signaling chromatic differences is much higher than for luminance differences because the available chromatic contrasts are much more restricted)13. Moreover, redundancy reduction has provided a functional account of the transformation of the cone responses into an opponent representation1723, while independent components analysis of scene statistics has been used to predict the color and spatial characteristics of cortical cells24. Analyses based on natural scene statistics have also been used to derive the spatiochromatic properties of receptive fields2528 and spatial color contrast29, as well as to infer the interdependence of different perceptual attributes defining color30.

From this perspective, uniform color spaces are of potential interest because they should reflect visual representations of color that are matched to properties of the color environment. For example, the relative scaling along different axes should reflect the gamut of color signals along these axes. The spaces might therefore provide clues about the structure of the environment that the visual system is calibrated for. We were specifically interested in what these spaces might suggest about the dominant axis of color variation in the environment. In many natural scenes colors tend to vary primarily along bluish-yellowish axes, which correspond to variations in the longwave-sensitive (L) cones opposed by signals in both the medium- (M) and shortwave-sensitive (S) cones22, 31, 32. If the perceptual scaling of color were matched to this variation, then we might expect these spaces to reveal reduced sensitivity for such axes. To test this, we projected distributions that were perceptually uniform by the metrics of different spaces, into cone-opponent space, and then compared these predicted distributions to the color variations observed in natural images. Conversely, we also started with empirically defined natural color distributions and asked how evenly sampled they became when projected into perceptually uniform color spaces.

Methods

Color spaces

We analyzed three common uniform color spaces: the Munsell color system, CIE 1976 L*u*v* and CIE 1976 L*a*b*. For each we generated a set of colors based on a uniform sampling within a sphere defined by the lightness and chromatic dimensions of the space. For the Munsell space points were sampled in cylindrical coordinates and were based on tabled values of Munsell coordinates. For L*u*v* and L*a*b* we instead sampled in much finer steps along Cartesian coordinates within a sphere. Differences in sampling density between the spaces were thus arbitrary but do not affect the analyses. The sphere was centered on a presumed adapting background corresponding to (0.33, 0.329), and had a radius of 4 in value and chroma (Munsell) or 25 in delta E (for CIE Luv and Lab). Coordinates within each space were converted into the corresponding values in a scaled version of the linear cone-opponent space of MacLeod and Boynton33 and Derrington, Krauskopf and Lennie34. The latter represents color in terms of contrast (relative to the adapting background color) along the three cardinal axes of early post-receptoral color coding: an achromatic axis (L+M+S) and two chromatic axes defined by the opposing signals in long- and medium-wavelength sensitive cones (L−M), or the opposing signals in the short-wavelength cones vs. the L and M cones [S-(L+M)]35. The relative scaling of the three axes is arbitrary or task-specific. For our analysis we used the scaling from our previous measurements of natural color distributions22, 31, which in turn was chosen to roughly equate sensitivity and adaptation along the different axes21. Coordinates in the space are defined by:

LM=1953(rmb.6568)SL+M=5533(bmb.01825)

where rmb and bmb are the coordinates in the MacLeod-Boynton diagram and (0.6568, 0.01825) the MacLeod-Boynton chromaticity of Illuminant C.

After projection into the cone-opponent space, the set of contrasts were analyzed to estimate the principal components of the distribution as well as the relative variance along the principal axes.

Results

Figure 1 shows the projections into the cone-opponent space of color distributions that are uniform within the Munsell, CIE L*u*v* or CIE L*a*b* spaces. The three columns plot the coordinates along the three different pairs of cardinal axes. To examine the bias in the distributions, we estimated the angles of the three orthogonal principal axes of the contrast variations. These are shown by the solid lines within each distribution. For all 3 spaces, the color distributions become strongly elongated along the second and fourth quadrants of the cone-opponent space. We refer to variations between these quadrants in general as a “bluish-yellowish” variation, since unique blue falls in the second quadrant and unique yellow in the fourth quadrant of our cone opponent space36. For the uniform spaces the principal axis is at −29 deg (Table 1). This is not a pure blue-yellow axis but instead corresponds to a color variation roughly from bluish-green to orange, though again to variations in the L cones opposed by both the S and M cones. Thus all 3 spaces predict a weaker sensitivity to this color direction, insofar as larger differences in cone excitation along this axis are required for the same perceptual difference.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Spherical distributions in the Munsell (left), CIE L*u*v* (middle) or CIE L*a*b* (right) spaces projected into the cone opponent space. Each row plots the distributions along different pairs of the cardinal axes; top: the S vs. LM isoluminant plane; middle: luminance and LM plane; bottom: luminance vs. S plane. Lines show the axes of the first (solid) or second (dashed) principal components of the distributions in each of the projected planes.

Table 1.

Principal axes and cone-opponent contrasts of spherical distributions in the uniform color spaces.

Color Space RMS (s.d.) Primary Secondary Primary RMS (s.d.) Secondary RMS (s.d.)
L vs. M (Rel. Cont.) S vs. LM (Rel. Cont.) Luminance (Rel. Cont.) Theta (degrees) Phi (degrees) Psi (degrees)
CIELAB Sphere 41.95 27.42 38.73 −25 −0.5 89 50.56 47.10
CIELuv Sphere 28.32 20.77 38.73 −26 −0.8 89 39.45 39.56
Munsell Sphere 32.74 25.21 43.96 −29 −1.2 89 58.08 54.93

Consistent with this shallow angle, the variance in color signals along the L−M axis is roughly 1.4 times greater than contrasts along the S-L+M axis. Note that the scaling within the cone-opponent space was based on estimates of the relative sensitivity to the two axes21, and thus this relative sensitivity is at least approximately preserved in each of the uniform spaces. That is, the spaces give roughly equal perceptual weight to the independent signals along the cardinal chromatic axes. This is very different from the relative weights in terms of metrics such as cone contrasts, for which the relative sensitivity to signals along the cardinal axes is markedly different37, 38.

The perceptually uniform distributions share similar characteristics - and important differences - to the distributions of colors in natural environments. For example, Figure 2 plots the distribution of chromaticities (i.e. the S vs. LM plane) within the same cone-opponent space for collections of outdoor scenes sampled by Webster, Mizokami, and Webster31. The four distributions were measured for two locations (Western Ghats in India or the Sierra Nevadas in the US), and at two different times of the year (corresponding to “wet” and “dry” seasons). Details of these images and analyses are given in Webster et al31. All four of the natural color distributions again have a strong orientation along the negative diagonal of the space, in part reflecting the variation from sky to earth, (though this bias persists in the color distributions restricted to earth)31. There is thus a rough qualitative agreement between the color bias in the scenes and the bias predicted by uniform color metrics. However, chromatic contrasts in the natural distributions are more strongly tilted toward the vertical axis, with angles ranging from −45 deg or more. Thus the scenes had a higher relative variance in S-L+M contrasts than predicted by the spaces.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Natural color distributions measured from two outdoor locations (top: Sierra Nevadas, USA; bottom: Western Ghats, India) and during two seasons (lush and arid)31. Plots show the pooled distributions of the chromaticities from individual scenes sampled in each environment. Lines show the axes of the first (solid) or second (dashed) principal components of the distributions in each of the projected planes.

If the “bluish-yellowish” bias in uniform spaces reflected scaling that (albeit roughly) matches the perceptual gamut of color to the range of colors in natural scenes, then the color distributions taken from scenes should become more uniform when projected into the perceptual spaces. To test this, we converted the color distributions for the four environments into CIE L*a*b*, and then compared the shapes of the distributions within the uniform vs. cone-opponent space. To ensure that this analysis was not affected by the arbitrary choice of scaling along the cone-opponent axes, we first converted each of the four raw distributions shown in Figure 2 by weighting the relative contribution of the separate sky and earth color distributions for each environment so that the mean chromaticity was as close as possible to white39. Next, we rescaled the cone-opponent axes so that the RMS contrast along each axis was equated. This allowed the distributions to be as close as possible to spherical in the linear cone-opponent space (since they were now forced to have equal radii along the three cardinal axes). Finally, we measured how uniform the resulting distributions were from the ratio of the variances along the principal and orthogonal axes in the chromatic plane. A ratio of 1 corresponded to an effectively circular distribution, while values greater than 1 corresponded to distributions that were instead elliptical and thus less uniform These ratios are shown in Table 2 and compared to the ratios taken when the same distributions were projected into CIE L*a*b*. For all four environments the chromatic variance along the principal and orthogonal axes is more similar within the uniform color space. That is, the natural color distributions are themselves more uniform when represented by a perceptually uniform color space such as CIE L*a*b*.

Table 2.

Ratio of variance in signals along the principal and orthogonal chromatic axes of the natural distributions from the four environments. Values closer to one represent more circular or unbiased distributions. Cone-opponent ratio gives a measure of the bias when the color distributions are represented in the cone-opponent space after rescaling the space so that the variance along each cardinal axis is equated. L*a*b* ratio gives a measure of the bias when the chromaticities are instead represented by their coordinates in the uniform color space.

Natural Distribution Cone-Opponent Ratio CIE L*a*b*
Dog Valley Lush 2.80 2.05
Dog Valley Arid 4.50 3.61
India Lush 1.96 1.46
India Arid 4.23 1.62

Mean 3.37 2.19

Discussion

In this study we started with the premise that uniform color spaces embody the transformations of the cone signals required to efficiently represent the statistics of color distributions in the observer’s environment, and then asked what kind of color environment the spaces implied. The general structure of these spaces shows important parallels with the structure of color in natural scenes. This suggests that at least some of the general characteristics of common uniform color spaces can be qualitatively accounted for by the characteristics of the color environment, and in turn, could point to which environmental characteristics have been important in shaping the perceptual representation.

As we showed, one of these general characteristics is for chromatic sensitivity to be lowest for “bluish-yellowish” color directions, in which signals from the S and M cones covary. This bias is not surprising, because color spaces like CIE L*u*v* and L*a*b* were derived from the McAdam ellipses describing just noticeable color differences40, 41, and Boynton et al.42 showed that these ellipses are oriented along a bluish-yellowish axis. Heightened discrimination along the orthogonal, reddish-greenish direction has also been found by Krauskopf and Gegenfurtner43 and Danilova and Mollon44. Moreover, this sensitivity bias has been observed in a number of other contexts. For example, achromatic settings show more variation both within and between observers along the blue-yellow dimension45, 46. At suprathreshold, McDermott et al.47 found in a visual search task that blue-yellow backgrounds were less effective distractors for reddish or greenish targets than vice versa, while Juricevic, Wilkins and Webster48 found that ratings of visual discomfort (potentially related to effective stimulus contrast49) were lower for blue-yellow patterns than reddish-greenish patterns defined by the orthogonal axis. Weaker sensitivity to bluish-yellowish contrasts has also been reported in adaptation effects such as the McCollough effect50. Finally, a recent study by Goddard et al.51 found that BOLD responses in different visual cortical areas were weaker for bluish-yellowish patterns than for reddish-greenish patterns matched for the same cardinal axis components. Thus the bias seen in perceptually uniform spaces is consistent with a general sensitivity bias against bluish-yellowish color directions seen in many visual tasks.

However, this bias is nevertheless of interest because it reflects an interaction between the L−M and S-(L+M) cardinal axes that have been thought to be central to the early postreceptoral encoding of chromatic signals34, 35, and thus raises the question of how these axes are matched to color in the environment. Color signals along these axes have been found to vary roughly (though not completely) independently for some natural scenes (of dense vegetation), and this had led to suggestions that the cardinal axes are the optimal opponent transformations for representing natural color signals20. However, as noted this independence is not characteristic of many natural environments. Webster and Mollon22 and Webster et al.31 found that the dominant axes varied from close to the S-(L+M) axis for scenes composed primarily of lush vegetation, but tended toward a pure blue-yellow variation for more panoramic and arid scenes. The present results suggest that more bluish-yellowish scenes may have played a more dominant role in shaping chromatic sensitivity. A related and possibly mutual factor is that the blue-yellow axis is also the principal axis of variation in natural daylight5254. In either case, our results are consistent with the idea that the greater variance in the natural world along the bluish-yellowish dimension is matched by a weaker relative sensitivity to this dimension in color vision, as reflected in the scaling of uniform color spaces. This is further suggested by our finding that natural color distributions themselves become less biased when represented in perceptually uniform color spaces.

Our results also highlight discrepancies between the stimulus distributions and the perceptual scaling of color. Quantitatively, there was a clear difference between the actual axis of minimum sensitivity implied by the perceptual spaces and the axis of maximum contrast in the color distributions. Specifically, the perceptual minima are along axes that are more orange-cyan than a pure blue-yellow, and fall outside the range of any of the outdoor scenes sampled by Webster et al, which instead varied from axes close to unique blue-yellow to purple-yellowish green axes close to the S axis31. This raises the possibility that color sensitivity has been shaped in the short term by environments with different color distributions. In fact, natural scenes may be uncharacteristic of the colors most individuals are exposed to in the more carpentered environments of modern societies. The color distributions of such environments are not well characterized, in part because of the interest in understanding the visual environments that shaped adaptations of the visual system over evolutionary timescales20, 22, 24, 31, 5558. Thus it remains to be seen whether largely indoor and constructed environments might provide a more accurate prediction of the ontogeny of color sensitivity.

A second clear discrepancy is that these color spaces do not reflect the characteristics of color contrast discrimination. While the CIE spaces were purportedly intended to reflect color differences for an observer adapted to daylight59, under this adaptation sensitivity is best near the adapting point and falls with increasing saturation or contrast from white13, 43, 60. In the CIE spaces sensitivity instead decreases monotonically with increasing S cone activity while remaining relatively constant with variations along the L−M axis, a pattern which is more typical of adaptation to the chromaticity that is being judged43, 61. This scaling is consistent with the transformations of cone signals that would be required to adjust to changes in the illuminant to maintain color constancy62, but does not capture contrast sensitivity, which does roughly follow the non-uniform distribution of contrasts in scenes12, 13. Uniform spaces that were constructed based on empirical measurements of contrast discrimination or on characteristic contrast distributions might therefore provide a better perceptual metric for some viewing contexts.

Finally, we have discussed uniform color spaces as if they were in fact perceptually uniform. Yet it is well known that they only approximate human judgments, and the fact that there is more than one “uniform” space highlights that there can be important discrepancies between distances within the space and observers’ perceptions of those differences. For instance, the metrics for spaces like CIE L*u*v* and L*a*b* were in part constrained by the desire to provide a relatively simple and general transformation from CIE 1931 chromaticities. Because they were based on threshold discrimination they do not accurately predict large color differences and this has prompted exploration of alternative measures, for example based on reaction times for suprathreshold stimuli63, 64. Because they were based on uniform fields they also fail to predict color differences at high spatial frequencies or in complex images, and this has led to elaborated spaces that include weightings for spatial contrast sensitivity6567. Our analyses suggest that another potential measure to exploit for predicting the perceptual impact of large color differences would be of the color statistics of the environment. Such considerations also suggest the possibility of tailoring perceptual color spaces to specific environments or contexts. To the extent that individuals are immersed in particular color worlds, for example because of their culture or vocation, their sensitivity may be adapted to the idiosyncratic color distributions of their environment68. The principles we explored could be used to guide the construction of spaces that better capture the experience of color for observers in these contexts, and thus could in theory better predict perceptual judgments and performance for these observers.

Acknowledgments

Supported by EY-10834.

Footnotes

OISS codes: 330.1690. 330.1720, 330.5020, 330.7310

References

  • 1.Derefeldt G. Colour appearance systems. In: Gouras P, editor. Vision and Visual Dysfunction 6: The Perception fo Colour. McMillan Press; London: 1991. pp. 218–261. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fairchild MD. Color Appearance Models. Addison-Wesley; Reading MA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hunt RGW. Measuring Colour. Ellis Horwood Ltd; Chichester: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Brainard DH. Color appearance and color difference specification. In: Shevell SK, editor. The Science of Color. 2. Optical Society of America; Washington D.C.: 2003. pp. 191–216. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Berns RS, Billmeyer FWJ. Development of the 1929 Munsell Book of Color: A historical review. Color Research and Application. 1985;10:246–250. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hard A, Sivik L. NCS-Natural color System: A Swedish standard for color notation. Color Research and Application. 1981;6:129–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pointer MR. A comparison of the CIE 1976 colour spaces. Color Research and Application. 1981;6:108–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Roberston AR. The CIE 1976 color-difference fomulae. Color Research and Application. 1977;2:7–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Simoncelli EP, Olshausen BA. Natural image statistics and neural representation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:1193–1216. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Geisler WS. Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:167–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Atick JJ. Could information-theory provide an ecological theory of sensory processing. Network - Computation in Neural Systems. 1990;3:213–251. doi: 10.3109/0954898X.2011.638888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Laughlin S. A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron’s information capacity. Z Naturforsch C. 1981;36(9–10):910–912. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.MacLeod DIA. Colour discrimination, colour constancy, and natural scene statistics (The Verriest Lecture) In: Mollon JD, Pokorny J, Knoblauch K, editors. Normal and Defective Colour Vision. Oxford University Press; London: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yendrikhovskij SN. Computing color categories from the statistics of natural images. Journal of Imaging Science and Technology. 2001;45:409–417. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lotto RB, Purves D. The empirical basis of color perception. Conscious Cogn. 2002;11(4):609–629. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8100(02)00014-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Baddeley R, Attewell D. The relationship between language and the environment: information theory shows why we have only three lightness terms. Psychol Sci. 2009;20(9):1100–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02412.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Atick JJ, Li Z, Redlich AN. What does post-adaptation color appearance reveal about cortical color representation? Vision Research. 1993;33(1):123–129. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90065-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Barlow HB. A theory about the functional role and synaptic mechanism of visual aftereffects. In: Blakemore C, editor. Visual Coding and Efficiency. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1990. pp. 363–375. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Buchsbaum G, Gottschalk A. Trichromacy, opponent colours coding and optimum colour information transmission in the retina. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1983;220(1218):89–113. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1983.0090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ruderman DL, Cronin TW, Chiao CC. Statistics of cone responses to natural images: implications for visual coding. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 1998;15:2036–2045. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Webster MA, Mollon JD. The influence of contrast adaptation on color appearance. Vision Research. 1994;34(15):1993–2020. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90028-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Webster MA, Mollon JD. Adaptation and the color statistics of natural images. Vision Research. 1997;37(23):3283–3298. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00125-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zaidi Q, Shapiro AG. Adaptive orthogonalization of opponent-color signals. Biological Cybernetics. 1993;69(5–6):415–428. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wachtler T, Lee TW, Sejnowski TJ. Chromatic structure of natural scenes. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2001;18(1):65–77. doi: 10.1364/josaa.18.000065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Atick JJ, Li Z, Redlich AN. Understanding retinal color coding from first principles. Neural Computation. 1992;4:559–572. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Parraga CA, Troscianko T, Tolhurst DJ. Spatiochromatic properties of natural images and human vision. Curr Biol. 2002;12(6):483–487. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00718-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.van Hateren JH. Spatial, temporal and spectral pre-processing for colour vision. Proc Biol Sci. 1993;251(1330):61–68. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1993.0009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wachtler T, Doi E, Lee T, Sejnowski TJ. Cone selectivity derived from the responses of the retinal cone mosaic to natural scenes. J Vis. 2007;7(8):6. doi: 10.1167/7.8.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Long F, Purves D. Natural scene statistics as the universal basis of color context effects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(25):15190–15193. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2036361100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Long F, Yang Z, Purves D. Spectral statistics in natural scenes predict hue, saturation, and brightness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(15):6013–6018. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600890103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Webster MA, Mizokami Y, Webster SM. Seasonal variations in the color statistics of natural images. Network. 2007;18(3):213–233. doi: 10.1080/09548980701654405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Shevell SK, Kingdom FA. Color in complex scenes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59:143–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.MacLeod DI, Boynton RM. Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance. J Opt Soc Am. 1979;69(8):1183–1186. doi: 10.1364/josa.69.001183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Derrington AM, Krauskopf J, Lennie P. Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. Journal of Physiology. 1984;357:241–265. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Krauskopf J, Williams DR, Heeley DW. Cardinal directions of color space. Vision Research. 1982;22(9):1123–1131. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90077-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Webster MA, Miyahara E, Malkoc G, Raker VE. Variations in normal color vision. II. Unique hues. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000;17(9):1545–1555. doi: 10.1364/josaa.17.001545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cole GR, Hine T, McIlhagga W. Detection mechanisms in L-, M-, and S-cone contrast space. J Opt Soc Am A. 1993;10(1):38–51. doi: 10.1364/josaa.10.000038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chaparro A, Stromeyer CF, 3rd, Huang EP, Kronauer RE, Eskew RT., Jr Colour is what the eye sees best. Nature. 1993;361(6410):348–350. doi: 10.1038/361348a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Juricevic I, Webster MA. Variations in normal color vision. V. Simulations of adaptation to natural color environments. Vis Neurosci. 2009;26(1):133–145. doi: 10.1017/S0952523808080942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.MacAdam DL. Visual sensitivities to color differences in daylight. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 1942;32:247–274. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brown WRJ, MacAdam DL. Visual sensitivities to combined chromaticity and luminance differences. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 1949;39:808–834. doi: 10.1364/josa.39.000808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Boynton RM, Nagy AL, Eskew RT., Jr Similarity of normalized discrimination ellipses in the constant-luminance chromaticity plane. Perception. 1986;15(6):755–763. doi: 10.1068/p150755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Krauskopf J, Gegenfurtner K. Color discrimination and adaptation. Vision Res. 1992;32(11):2165–2175. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90077-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Danilova MV, Mollon JD. Parafoveal color discrimination: a chromaticity locus of enhanced discrimination. J Vis. 2011;10(1) doi: 10.1167/10.1.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Webster MA, Leonard D. Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 2008;25(11):2817–2825. doi: 10.1364/josaa.25.002817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Werner JS, Schefrin BE. Loci of achromatic points throughout the life span. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 1993;10(7):1509–1516. doi: 10.1364/josaa.10.001509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.McDermott KC, Malkoc G, Mulligan JB, Webster MA. Adaptation and visual salience. J Vis. 2010;10(13):17. doi: 10.1167/10.13.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Juricevic I, Land L, Wilkins A, Webster MA. Visual discomfort and natural image statistics. Perception. 2010;39(7):884–899. doi: 10.1068/p6656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Fernandez D, Wilkins AJ. Uncomfortable images in art and nature. Perception. 2008;37(7):1098–1113. doi: 10.1068/p5814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.McCollough-Howard C, Webster MA. McCollough effect. Scholarpedia. 2011;6(2):8175. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Goddard E, Mannion DJ, McDonald JS, Solomon SG, Clifford CWG. Combination of subcortical color channels in human visual cortex. Journal of Vision. 2010;10:1–17. doi: 10.1167/10.5.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lee H-C. A computational model for opponent color encoding. Advanced Printing of Conference Summaries, SPSE’s 43rd Annual Conference; Rochester, NY. May 1990; 1990. pp. 178–181. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Mollon JD. Monge (The Verriest Lecture) Visual Neuroscience. 2006;23:297–309. doi: 10.1017/S0952523806233479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sherpard RN. The perceptual organization of colors: an adaptation to regularities of the terrestrial world? In: Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The Adapted Mind. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1992. pp. 495–532. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Tkacik G, Garrigan P, Ratliff C, Milcinski G, Klein JM, Seyfarth LH, Sterling P, Brainard DH, Balasubramanian V. Natural images from the birthplace of the human eye. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20409. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Regan BC, Julliot C, Simmen B, Vienot F, Charles-Dominique P, Mollon JD. Fruits, foliage and the evolution of primate colour vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356(1407):229–283. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Osorio D, Vorobyev M. Colour vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates. Proc Biol Sci. 1996;263(1370):593–599. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Nascimento SM, Ferreira FP, Foster DH. Statistics of spatial cone-excitation ratios in natural scenes. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2002;19(8):1484–1490. doi: 10.1364/josaa.19.001484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.CIE. Colorimetry (Commision Internationale de l’Eclairage. Central Bureau of the CIE; Vienna: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Switkes E, Bradley A, De Valois KK. Contrast dependence and mechanisms of masking interactions among chromatic and luminance gratings. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5(7):1149–1162. doi: 10.1364/josaa.5.001149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Boynton RM, Kambe N. Chromatic difference steps of moderate size measured along theoretically critical axes. Color Research and Application. 1980;5(1):13–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Zaidi Q. Identification of illuminant and object colors: heuristic-based algorithms. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1998;15(7):1767–1776. doi: 10.1364/josaa.15.001767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nagy AL, Sanchez RR. Critical color differences determined with a visual search task. J Opt Soc Am A. 1990;7(7):1209–1217. doi: 10.1364/josaa.7.001209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cavonius CR, Mollon JD. Reaction time as a measure of the discriminability of large colour differences. In: Gibson CP, editor. Colour Coded vs Monochrome Electronic Displays. HMSO; London: 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kolpatzik BW, Bouman CA. Optimized universal color palette design for error diffusion. Journal of Electronic Imaging. 1995;4:131–143. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Aldaba MA, Linhares JM, Pinto PD, Nascimento SM, Amano K, Foster DH. Visual sensitivity to color errors in images of natural scenes. Vis Neurosci. 2006;23(3–4):555–559. doi: 10.1017/S0952523806233467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Zhang X, Wandell BA. A spatial extension of CIELAB for digital color image reproduction. Society for Information Display Journal. 1996 [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Webster MA. Adaptation and visual coding. J Vis. 2011;113(5):1–23. doi: 10.1167/11.5.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES