Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011 Sep 19;17(6):1067–1079. doi: 10.1017/S135561771100107X

Cognitive Outcomes for Extremely Preterm/Extremely Low Birth Weight Children in Kindergarten

Leah J Orchinik 1, H Gerry Taylor 2, Kimberly Andrews Espy 3, Nori Minich 2, Nancy Klein 4, Tiffany Sheffield 3, Maureen Hack 2
PMCID: PMC3282051  NIHMSID: NIHMS355415  PMID: 21923973

Abstract

Our objectives were to examine cognitive outcomes for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight (EPT/ELBW, gestational age <28 weeks and/or birth weight <1000 g) children in kindergarten and the associations of these outcomes with neonatal factors, early childhood neurodevelopmental impairment, and socioeconomic status (SES). The sample comprised a hospital-based 2001-2003 birth cohort of 148 EPT/ELBW children (mean birth weight 818 g; mean gestational age 26 weeks) and a comparison group of 111 term-born normal birth weight (NBW) classmate controls. Controlling for background factors, the EPT/ELBW group had pervasive deficits relative to the NBW group on a comprehensive test battery, with rates of cognitive deficits that were 3 to 6 times higher in the EPT/ELBW group. Deficits on a measure of response inhibition were found in 48% versus 10%, OR (95% CI) = 7.32 (3.32, 16.16), p <.001. Deficits on measures of executive function and motor and perceptual-motor abilities were found even when controlling for acquired verbal knowledge. Neonatal risk factors, early neurodevelopmental impairment, and lower SES were associated with higher rates of deficits within the EPT/ELBW group. The findings document both global and selective cognitive deficits in EPT/ELBW children at school entry and justify efforts at early identification and intervention.

Keywords: PREMATURE BIRTH, NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS, RISK FACTORS, COGNITION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTION, CHILD

INTRODUCTION

Advances in neonatal care during the past few decades resulted in increased survival of infants born preterm or with low birth weight (Fanaroff et al., 2007; Stoll et al., 2010). These increases have been the most dramatic for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight (EPT/ELBW) children—defined as those with gestational age (GA) <28 weeks and/or birth weight <1000 g. Unfortunately, survival comes at a cost as these children are at high risk for a variety of developmental problems including cognitive deficits, poor academic achievement, and behavior disorders (Anderson, Doyle et al., 2003; Stjernqvist & Svenningsen, 1999; Taylor, 2010). Studies of EPT/ELBW children recruited at middle to later school age that controlled for IQ or vocabulary ability have also demonstrated specific deficits in memory, visual-spatial and perceptual-motor skills, attention, and executive function (EF) (Bayless & Stevenson, 2007; Kulseng et al., 2006; Taylor, Minich, Bangert, Filipek, & Hack, 2004).

Research on cognitive outcomes in EPT/ELBW children at 6 years of age or younger reveal deficits in IQ, expressive and receptive language skills, spatial reasoning, visual motor integration, and EF (Baron, Erickson, Ahronovich, Baker, & Litman, 2011; Baron, Erickson, Ahronovich, Litman, & Brandt, 2010; Dewey et al., 2011; Gidley Larson et al., 2011; Marlow, Hennessy, Bracewell, & Wolke; 2007; Mikkola et al., 2005; Woodward et al., 2009). Studies of very preterm (GA ≤32 weeks) children at this age level reveal similar findings (Aarnoudse-Moens, Smidts, Oosterlaan, Duivenvoorden, & Weisglas-Kuperus, 2009; Lind et al., 2011). Several of these studies document selective cognitive deficits. For example, Marlow et al. (2007) found that 6-year-olds with GA ≤25 weeks had lower scores than term-born normal birth weight (NBW) classmate controls on measures of visuospatial skills, motor and sensorimotor abilities, and attention and EF, even when controlling for global cognitive ability. Similarly, Lind et al. (2011) and Mikkola et al. (2005) found more adverse effects on Verbal IQ than on Performance IQ. However, other studies document language delays in young preterm children, suggesting that this cognitive domain may not be selectively spared and raising the possibility of more generalized deficits in younger than in older children (Foster-Cohen, Friesen, Champion, & Woodward, 2010; Luoma, Herrgard, Martikainen, & Ahonen, 1998; Ment et al., 2003; Mikkola et al., 2005; Wolke, Samara, Bracewell, & Marlow, 2008; Woodward et al., 2009).

Further studies using comprehensive assessments of neuropsychological skills are needed to establish the pattern of cognitive deficits exhibited by EPT/ELBW children at school entry. Assessments targeted specifically to children enrolled in kindergarten would help specify the nature of their difficulties as they begin to take on the challenges of formal education and are in a setting in which special interventions may be available for the first time, at least for children from less advantaged backgrounds. No studies of which we are aware have selected EPT/ELBW children for assessment based on enrollment in kindergarten and few have conducted in-depth evaluations at this age level of skill domains that are especially vulnerable to extreme degrees of preterm birth and low birth weight, such as EF. Recognition of types of cognitive weaknesses to which EPT/ELBW children are prone at school entry would also useful determining how best to identify those needing special educational assistance (Mulder, Pitchford, & Marlow, 2010). Early identification is especially critical in view of evidence that learning problems frequently go unrecognized for several years after school entry (Avchen, Scott, & Mason, 2001). Determining a child’s need for special education at or soon after school entry, followed by appropriate interventions, has the potential to diminish long-term educational and social problems that occur in EPT/ELBW adolescents and young adults (Belsky & Mackinnon, 1994; Espy, Fang, Charak, Minich, & Taylor, 2009; Johnson et al., 2009; Rimm-Kaufmann et al., 2000).

Another reason to focus on kindergarten outcomes is to determine which children are most in need of neuropsychological evaluations at the time of school entry. Previous research indicates that risks for more adverse cognitive outcomes include lower birth weight, lower GA, and neonatal complications such as brain abnormalities evident on neonatal cranial ultrasound, bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), and infection (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009; Bohm & Katz-Salaman, 2003; Bohm, Katz-Salamon, Smedler, & Forssberg, 2002; Bohm, Smedler, & Forssberg, 2004; Larroque et al., 2008; Leonard et al., 1990; Ment et al., 2003; Patrianakos-Hoobler, Msall, Marks, Huo, & Schreiber, 2009b; Taylor, Klein, Drotar, Schluchter, & Hack, 2006). Neurodevelopmental impairment (NDI) in early childhood is also related to cognitive deficits at school age (Dewey, Crawford, Creighton, & Sauve, 1999; Marlow, 2004; Roberts, Anderson, & Doyle, 2010; Patrianakos-Hoobler et al., 2009a; Sullivan & McGrath, 2003).

The objective of this study was to assess the neuropsychological skills of a recent birth cohort of EPT/ELBW children in kindergarten relative to NBW controls and to identify factors related to poorer cognitive outcomes. Our primary hypothesis was that EPT/ELBW children would have deficits across multiple ability domains relative to term-born NBW controls but that impairment in EF and spatial and perceptual-motor skills would be evident even when controlling for a measure of crystallized verbal intelligence. A secondary hypothesis was that cognitive deficits in the EPT/ELBW group would be more prevalent in children with extreme degrees of preterm birth or low birth weight, neonatal complications, early childhood NDI, and lower socioeconomic status (SES).

METHODS

Participants and Recruitment

The EPT/ELBW sample included 148 children with GA <28 weeks and/or birth weight <1000 g admitted to the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit at Rainbow Babies and Children’s Hospital between 2001 and 2003, comprising 75% of the 198 surviving infants treated during this period. Children with congenital infections and malformations were excluded. Ten of these children (7%) were born in other hospitals (i.e., outborn) and transferred to the neonatal center for treatment. Reasons for non-participation included failure to locate families (n = 21), moves out of the area (n = 16), and refusals to participate or failure to keep appointments (n = 5). Eight other children who were non-English speakers or for whom custody could not be established were also excluded. Comparison of the participants with the 50 non-participating survivors failed to reveal differences in sex, race, GA, birth weight, cranial ultrasound abnormality, BPD, infection, or retinopathy of prematurity.

A term-born NBW control group of 111 children with GA >36 weeks and birth weight >2500 g was recruited by enrolling classmates of the EPT/ELBW children attending regular kindergarten classrooms. Parents of NBW controls were not asked if their children were born post-term. However, as obstetrical practice dictates delivery prior to 42 weeks, few if any of the controls were likely to have been born at a GA >41 weeks. We recruited the closest available NBW classmate match based on age at assessment, sex, and race from among those children whose parents granted permission for us to contact them. If we were unable to enroll a match from the EPT/ELBW child’s classroom, we recruited one from another classroom at the same school or a demographically similar one. As suitable controls for some EPT/ELBW children were unavailable and some schools refused participation, we were unable to recruit controls for 18 of the 129 EPT/ELBW children in regular classrooms.

The birth status and demographic characteristics of the two groups are summarized in Table 1. Comparisons of the EPT/ELBW and NBW groups failed to reveal significant differences in sex, race, or socioeconomic status (SES) as measured by a composite of the sample z scores for maternal education, caregiver occupation as assessed by a socioeconomic index and averaged for two-parent households (Hauser, 1997), and census-based median family income for the neighborhood in which the family resided (Taylor et al., 2006).

Table 1.

Birth status and demographic characteristics of EPT/ELBW and NBW groups.

EPT/ELBW Group
(n=148)
NBW Group
(n=111)
Birth Status and Neonatal Risk Factors:
Gestational age (GA) in weeks, mean (SD) and
range
25.9 (1.6), range: 23-32 weeks >36
Birth weight in grams, mean (SD) and range 818.4 (174.0), range: 485-1281 g 3382.3 (445.9), range: 2501-
4508 g
GA <25 weeks, n (%) 32 (22%) 0 (0%)
Birth weight <750 g, n (%) 56 (38%) 0 (0%)
Small for gestational agea 37 (25%)
Severely abnormal cranial ultrasound,b n (%) 15 (10%) 0 (0%)
Bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD),c n (%) 77 (52%) 0 (0%)
Infection or necrotizing enterocolitis,d n (%) 60 (41%) 0 (0%)
Severe retinopathy of prematurity,e n (%) 27 (18%) 0 (0%)
Demographic Characteristics:
Age in years at testing, mean (SD) 5.96 (.37) 5.96 (.31)
Months in school at testing, mean (SD)* 4.25 (2.34) 6.38 (1.72)
Male sex 68 (46%) 51(46%)
African Americanf 91 (61%) 61 (55%)
Maternal education:
 < high school 20 (14%) 14 (13%)
 high school or GED 33 (22%) 16 (14%)
 > high school 95 (64%) 81 (73%)
Median family income,g mean (SD) 59.10 (32.37) 56.20 (23.67)
SES z-score composite,h mean (SD) −0.05 (1.02) 0.07 (0.98)

Note: The majority of the sample (112, 76%) had GA <28 weeks and birth weight <1000 g, whereas 21 (14%) had GA <28 weeks with birth weight ≥1000 g and 15 (10%) had birth weight <1000 g birth weight with GA ≥28 weeks.

Abbreviations: EPT/ELBW= extremely preterm; NBW = term-born normal birth weight; GED = General Education Diploma; SES = socioeconomic status, with z-score defined as mean of sample z-scores for maternal education, caregiver occupation, and census-based median family income.

a

Small for GA as defined by birth weight <-2 SDs below expectation for GA based on standard reported by Yudkin et al. (1987). The high rate reported here partially reflects selection of children who were ≥28 weeks GA but who had birth weight <1000 g, all of whom were small for GA. The rate was lower for children with GA <28 weeks (17%) and consistent with rates reported in other studies (Fanaroff et al., 2007; Stoll et al., 2010).

b

Severely abnormal ultrasound defined as clinician-identified Grade III/IV intraventricular hemorrhage, periventricular leukomalacia, or ventricular dilatation at discharge.

c

BPD defined as supplemental oxygen at 36 weeks corrected age.

d

Includes 9 cases of necrotizing enterocolitis (6% of sample), 56 of septicemia (38%), and 2 of meningitis (1%).

e

Severe retinopathy defined as Stage 4 or 5 retinopathy or treatment with cryotherapy or laser therapy (Schmidt et al., 2003).

e

All others Caucasian except for 4 Asian (2 in each group).

f

Based on data on the neighborhood tract of the family residence from the 2000 US Census (Federal Financial Institutions Examinations Council Geocoding System, 2010). Family income is in thousands of dollars.

g

SES was measured by a composite of the sample z scores for maternal education, caregiver occupation.

*

Significant group difference, p<.01; group differences in gestation and birth weight by definition. All other differences non-significant.

Procedure and Measures

Each child was assessed by a research assistant in a single half-day session while a second assistant interviewed the child’s caregiver regarding family characteristics. Children were assessed during their initial year in kindergarten. Tests were administered by examiners who were blind to the child’s birth status in a fixed order with breaks provided to reduce fatigue. Although child assessments also included measures of academic achievement and behavior, the present study focused on results from cognitive testing. Measures of neonatal medical status for the EPT/ELBW children were extracted from neonatal hospital records at the time of neonatal discharge. The children were prospectively followed and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd Edition (BSID, Bayley, 1993) and a neurological examination were administered at 20 months corrected age. Findings revealed a Mental Development Index (MDI) <70 in 57 (39%), cerebral palsy in 13 (9%), blindness in 4 (3%), and deafness in 2 (1%). The study was approved by the University Hospitals Case Medical Center Institutional Review Board and signed parental informed consent was obtained prior to participation.

Table 2 describes the test domains and measures included in the neuropsychological battery. Global cognitive functioning was assessed using the Brief Intellectual Ability (BIA) from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd Edition (WJ-III-COG, Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). The BIA is a composite of scores from the WJ-III-COG subtests Verbal Comprehension, Concept Formation, and Visual Matching. Verbal Comprehension was also used to assess language and as a measure of crystallized verbal ability, and Visual Matching as a measure of visual-motor skill. Other standardized tests included subtests from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP, Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999), the Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration (VMI, Beery & Beery, 2004), and the short-form of the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd edition (BOT-2, Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005).

Table 2.

Neuropsychological test battery.

Domain/Test Description Primary Ability Assessed Score for Analysis
Global Cognitive Ability:
WJ-III COG Brief
Intellectual Ability (BIA)
Composite of subtests Verbal
Comprehension, Concept
Formation, and Visual Matching
Global cognitive ability Standard score
Language:
WJ-III COG Verbal
Comprehension
Naming pictures and identifying
synonyms, antonyms, and analogies
Word knowledge Standard score
CTOPP Elision and Blending
Words
Manipulation of phonemes in a word
and combine separate sounds to
form a word
Phonological awareness Scaled score
Spatial and Non-verbal Reasoning :
WJ-III COG Concept
Formation
Identifying principles of similarity
and difference in arrays colored
forms
Non-verbal reasoning Standard score
WJ-III COG Spatial
Relations
Identifying component parts of
larger form
Spatial problem solving Standard score
Memory:
Verbal Paired Associates Recalling second member of word
pairs immediately and after a delay
Learning and short-term
recall memory
Number correct
CTOPP Memory for Digits Recalling orally presented numbers Verbal working memory Scaled score
Motor/Visual Motor:
WJ-III COG Visual Matching Finding and circling matching
numbers within number arrays
Visual search and
psychomotor speed
Standard score
VMI Copying of geometric designs Visual-motor coordination Standard score
BOT-2 Performing tests of balance,
coordination, and dexterity
Gross and fine motor
skills
T-score
Executive Functioning:
Shape School Naming storybook characters
according to different sets of rules
Inhibition, task switching Efficiency score
[accuracy/time]
Preschool Trails Finding and stamping a series of
targets in order of increasing size
Inhibition, task switching Efficiency score:
[(1/time)/(square
root(errors+1))]*100
Nebraska Barnyard Touching positions on computer
screen to reproduce named sequence
of farm animals
Spatial working memory Number of correct
sequences
Test of Inhibition and
Attention
Pressing bar to “catch” fish targets
while inhibiting presses to “no-go”
shark stimuli
Inhibition d’: Z(hit rate)-Z(false
alarm rate)

Note: WJ-III=Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement, 3rd ed. (Woodcock et al., 2001); CTOPP= Comprehensive Tests of Phonological Processing; VMI= Developmental Tests of Visual Motor Integration; BOT-2= Bruininks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd ed. Z(p) in computing d’ for Test of Inhibition and Attention is the inverse of the cumulative Gaussian distribution.

Memory was assessed using Verbal Paired Associates from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (Wechsler, 1987) as modified for children by Gonzalez, Anderson, Wood, Mitchell, and Harvey (2007). The test consists of 4 easy word pairs and 4 difficult word pairs. After the pairs are read to the child, the examiner presents each stimulus word and the child is asked to provide the other member of the word pair. After completion of three immediate recall trials and following a delay of approximately 30 minutes during which the child is given tests of non-verbal skills, the stimulus words are presented again to assess delayed memory. The scores for this test were the total correct for both immediate and delayed recall.

EF was assessed using four experimental tasks developed to assess the constructs of inhibition, cognitive set shifting, and working memory described by Wiebe et al. (2011). The Shape School task provided measures of inhibitory control and set shifting (Espy, Bull, Martin, & Stroup, 2006). This task follows a storybook format with drawings of cartoon-faced, colored shapes arranged on the page in rows. The child names the color or shape of the cartoon figures as fast as possible for each of several conditions. Administered to familiarize the child with task demands, the control conditions include naming of colors (Color Naming) and shapes (Shape Naming). The test conditions include the naming of the color of the figures with happy faces while inhibiting responses to those with sad faces (Inhibition); and naming of either the figure’s color or shape depending on whether it is wearing a hat (Switching). An efficiency score is computed for each task to take into account both accuracy and naming speed (see Table 2).

The Trails-Preschool Test--Revised (adapted from Espy & Cwik, 2004) provided an additional measure of inhibitory control and set switching. This task follows a storybook format in which the child is presented with a story book showing a family of dogs and their bones. The child is required to push down on the stimuli according to different rules using a non-permanent ink stamp. The control condition for this task requires stamping the dogs in order of increasing size. The “inhibit” condition requires the child to ignore the previously salient dogs and stamp only their bones in order of increasing size. The “switch” condition involves the alternative stamping the dogs and then their bones in order of increasing size. An efficiency score is computed for each task using a formula to take into account both accuracy and naming speed (see Table 2).

Working memory was measured by the Nebraska Barnyard task (adapted from the Noisy Book task, Hughes, Dunn & White, 1998), a computerized span task requiring the child to remember a sequence of animal names and press corresponding buttons on a touch screen in the correct order. The task begins with sequences of two animal names and progresses to increasingly longer sequences. The score is the total number of sequences correctly reproduced.

Inhibition was further assessed using a computerized Test of Inhibition and Attention (adapted from the Cat-Mouse task, Simpson & Riggs, 2006). In this task, the child observes a series of pictures of colored fish presented for 1500 ms each with a 1000 ms inter-stimulus interval. The child is asked to “catch” these fish by pressing a button on the keyboard. On ‘no-go’ trials, an image of a shark appears, signaling the child to inhibit responding. In the first condition, Go No-Go (GNG), 75% of stimuli are “go” fish trials and 25% are “no-go” shark trials, placing special demands on response inhibition. In the second condition, the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), 75% of stimuli are “no go” shark trials and 25% are “go” fish trials, placing lesser demands on inhibition and more on selective attention. The measure of performance on these tasks was d’ (signal detectability in signal detection theory) for each of the two conditions (see Table 2).

Past research supports the use of these experimental tasks as measures of executive function in young children. Espy et al. (2006) found moderate reliability levels for the Shape School conditions and documented their associations with other tests of EF. Simpson and Riggs (2006) found that performance on the original version of the Test of Inhibition and Attention and Inhibition was associated with other measures of this test construct. Espy and Cwik (2004) and Wiebe et al. (2011) found similar support for earlier versions of the Trails-Preschool Test— Revised and Nebraska Barnyard task.

Data Analysis

So that results would reflect outcomes for as many of the children as possible and thus more accurately represent outcomes for the EPT/ELBW cohort as a whole, children who were too low functioning to understand or follow basic test demands were assigned either the lowest possible raw score (for unstandarized tests) or a standard score that corresponded to the lowest possible raw score (for age-normed tests). Children who were too low functioning to comply with test demands on WJ-III-COG tests were assigned a standard score of 40 (Mather & Jaffe, 2002). The number of children receiving estimated scores ranged from 3 to 13 for all measures except CTOPP Elision on which 48 children received estimated scores. For many tests, estimated scores were assigned to significantly more children in the EPT/ELBW group than in the NBW group, documenting the greater difficulty that EPT/ELBW group had in understanding and complying with test demands. A smaller number of additional test scores were missing due to child noncompliance, equipment failure, or lack of opportunity to complete the assessments. Scores that were missing for these reasons were most common for the Verbal Paired Associates test, BOT-2, Shape School, Trails-Preschool Test—Revised, and Nebraska Barnyard. Standard or raw scores were normally distributed for most measures and thus suitable for parametric analysis. Because of the skewed distributions of scores on the Inhibition and Switching conditions of the Trails-Preschool Test—Revised, these measures were log transformed prior to analysis.

Group differences on the cognitive measures were assessed using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for SES, sex, and race. Standard scores for chronological age were used in analysis of standardized measures. Unstandardized measures of memory and EF were examined using raw scores covarying for age at assessment. Because each NBW control was recruited after the EPT/ELBW child had been assessed, length of schooling was significantly longer for the NBW group than for the EPT/ELBW group. For this reason and given past evidence for effects of length of early schooling on cognitive skills (Cahan & Cohen, 1989), the number of weeks the child had been in kindergarten was also included as a covariate. Inclusion of the covariates was justified by lack of evidence for group x covariate interactions for any of the measures.

A parallel set of logistic regressions was conducted to determine if the EPT/ELBW group had more cognitive deficits than the NBW controls. Deficits were defined by scores below the 10th percentile relative to either norms (standardized measures) or the performance of the NBW controls (unstandardized measures). Use of the 10th percentile as a cut-off for clinically significant deficits is justified by previous research and clinical practice (Fay et al., 2009).

To investigate the possibility of selective cognitive impairments, group comparisons were repeated in secondary analyses that controlled for WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension as a measure of crystallized intelligence (Woodcock et al., 2001). The BIA was not considered as Verbal Comprehension is a component of this score. Group comparisons were also repeated in further secondary analyses that: (a) excluded the scores of children with a BIA <70 and/or neurosensory impairment as defined by cerebral palsy, blindness, or deafness requiring hearing aids at 20 months corrected age (39 EPT/ELBW children and 2 NBW controls); (b) excluded estimated scores for children who were untestable due to inability to understand and follow basic test demands; and (c) based scores on corrected rather than chronological age. The aims of these analyses were to determine if group differences were evident even among children with less severe impairments and if findings would be similar when missing scores were not estimated or when performance was evaluated relative to the post-conceptual age. To determine if increased rates of deficits in some skills would be evident even among the subset of EPT/ELBW children without handicapping conditions and with at least broadly average global cognitive ability, logistic regressions were repeated after excluding the 61 EPT/ELBW children and 12 NBW controls with either neurosensory impairment or BIA <85.

Associations of cognitive deficits within the EPT/ELBW group with the neonatal risk factors listed in Table 1, early childhood NDI, and SES were examined using logistic regression. NDI was defined as a neurosensory disorder and/or Mental Development Index <70 on the BSID at 20 months corrected age (n=60, 41%). These risk factors predicted developmental outcomes in previous studies of children with low birth weight (Schmidt et al., 2003; Taylor et al., 2006; Wilson-Costello et al., 2007). The NDI and each neonatal predictor were examined separately, with SES, sex, race, and time in school included as covariates. SES was examined after adjusting for time in school. Age at assessment was included as an additional covariate in analyses of unstandardized measures.

An alpha level of .01 was used to adjust for multiple comparisons in all analyses. Effect sizes were computed using Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1992) for continuous measures and odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals for binary outcomes.

RESULTS

Group Differences in Neuropsychological Performance

ANCOVAs revealed that the EPT/ELBW group scored more poorly than NBW controls on nearly all measures (Table 3), with large effect sizes (Cohen’s d’s ≥.80) for the BIA, BOT-2, WJ-III-COG Visual Matching, and CPT condition of the Test of Inhibition and Attention. The findings were similar when the 10 outborn EPT/ELBW children were excluded from analysis, suggesting that results are representative of infants born at the treating medical center. Lower SES and minority race were associated with lower scores on most of the measures; boys scored more poorly than girls on the BOT-2, F (1, 237) = 17.97, p < .001; and less time in school was associated with lower scores on CTOPP Elision, F (1, 147) = 7.39, p = .007.

Table 3.

Comparison of EPT/ELBW and NBW groups in mean scores on cognitive measures.

Domain/Measure EPT/ELBW Group
n Adj M (SE)
NBW Group
n Adj M (SE)
F (df) from
ANCOVA
ES
Global Cognitive Ability:
WJ-III COG Brief Intellectual Ability
(BIA)
142 87.69 (1.57) 110 103.91 (1.81) 40.58*bc 0.87
Language:
WJ-III COG Verbal Comprehension 144 89.09 (1.46) 111 100.83 (1.69) 24.43*bc 0.67
CTOPP Elision 142 8.05 (0.22) 111 9.56 (0.26) 17.21* 0.56
CTOPP Blending Words 142 9.22 (0.19) 111 10.49 (0.22) 16.52*bc 0.55
Spatial and Non-verbal Reasoning :
WJ-III COG Concept Formation 144 92.59 (1.56) 110 104.44 (1.81) 21.88*bc 0.63
WJ-III COG Spatial Relations 143 94.68 (1.49) 110 102.96 (1.72) 11.73*c 0.46
Memory:
Verbal Paired Associates Immediate
Recall
136 9.49 (0.46) 109 12.27 (0.52) 14.08*c 0.52
Verbal Paired Associates Delayed Recall 135 3.73 (0.20) 109 5.01 (0.22) 16.62*c 0.56
CTOPP Memory for Digits 141 8.03 (0.27) 110 9.60 (0.31) 13.17*c 0.49
Motor/Visual Motor:
WJ-III COG Visual Matching 143 87.33 (1.56) 110 102.35 (1.81) 35.00*abc 0.80
VMI 147 88.88 (1.32) 111 97.15 (1.55) 14.68*c 0.52
BOT-2 133 41.19 (0.86) 110 49.58 (0.96) 37.37*abc 0.84
Executive Functioning:
Shape School Inhibition 136 0.85 (0.03) 109 1.05 (0.04) 15.73*bc 0.55
Shape School Switching 134 0.35 (0.01) 110 0.43 (0.02) 10.24* 0.44
Preschool Trails Inhibition 133 1.20 (0.07) 106 1.69 (0.08) 21.85*abc 0.65
Preschool Trails Switching 134 0.94 (0.05) 108 1.26 (0.06) 14.23*c 0.52
Nebraska Barnyard 130 4.67 (0.25) 108 6.29 (0.28) 16.53*c 0.57
Test of Inhibition and Attention GNG 143 1.46 (0.10) 111 2.32 (0.11) 29.72*abc 0.74
Test of Inhibition and Attention CPT 143 2.27 (0.11) 111 3.41 (0.13) 39.22*abc 0.85

Abbreviations: EPT/ELBW= extremely preterm; NBW = term-born normal birth weight; Adj M (se) = adjusted mean (standard error); ANCOVA = analyses of covariance, controlling for socioeconomic status, race, sex, time in school, and (for unstandardized measures only) age at assessment; ES = effect size as defined by Cohen’s d = mean difference/pooled standard deviation; WJ-III-COG = Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd Edition; CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; VMI = Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration; BOT-2 = Bruininks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd Edition; GNG = Test of Inhibition and Attention condition; CPT = Continuous Performance Test condition.

a

Significant controlling for WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension.

b

Significant excluding children with BIA < 70 and/or neurosensory impairment.

c

Significant excluding estimated scores for children who were not testable.

*

Adjusted p<.01.

Group differences for all measures remained significant or marginally significant (p < .05) when scores were adjusted for corrected rather than chronological age. Differences on WJ-III-COG Visual Matching, BOT-2, Trails-Preschool Test—Revised Inhibition, and CPT and GNG conditions of the Test of Inhibition and Attention remained significant when controlling for WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension. Many of the differences also remained significant when excluding children with BIA <70 and/or neurosensory impairment or when excluding estimated scores for children who were untestable due to low functioning (see Table 3 notations).

As shown in Table 4, the EPT/ELBW group had significantly higher rates of cognitive deficits on most of the measures, with rates for the EPT/ELBW group 3 to 6 times higher than those for the NBW group. As further documentation of pervasive deficits, 94 EPT/ELBW children (65%) versus 21 NBW controls (19%) had 3 or more deficits on the test battery, χ2 (1, N=256) = 53.56, p < .01. All group differences remained significant when scores were based on corrected rather than chronological age. The EPT/ELBW group had higher rates of deficits in WJ-III Visual Matching, BOT-2, and the GNG and CPT conditions of the Test of Inhibition and Attention even when controlling for WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension. The EPT/ELBW group also had significantly higher rates of some deficits when excluding children with BIA <70 and/or neurosensory impairment and when excluding estimated scores for children too low functioning to be tested (see Table 4 notations). When children with neurosensory disorders and BIA <85 were excluded, group differences remained significant for Verbal Paired Associates Immediate Recall, Trails-Preschool Test—Revised Inhibition, and GNG and CPT conditions of the Test of Inhibition and Attention.

Table 4.

Comparison of EPT/ELBW and NBW group in rates of cognitive deficits.

Domain/Measure of Deficit# EPT/ELBW
Group
No. (%)
NBW Group
No. (%)
Adjusted OR
(95% CI)
Global Cognitive Ability:
WJ-III COG Brief Intellectual Ability
(BIA)
50 (35.2) 8 (7.3) 4.82 (2.00, 11.64)*c
Language:
WJ-III COG Verbal Comprehension 48 (33.3) 14 (12.6) 2.79 (1.29, 6.04)*
CTOPP Elision 61 (43.0) 13 (11.7) 4.29 (2.00, 9.19)*
CTOPP Blending Words 26 (18.3) 6 (5.4) 3.66 (1.31, 10.22)
Spatial and Non-Verbal Reasoning :
WJ-III COG Concept Formation 49 (34.0) 18 (16.4) 2.49 (1.21, 5.12)
WJ-III COG Spatial Relations 21 (14.7) 3 (2.7) 4.34 (1.14, 16.54)
Memory:
Verbal Paired Associates Immediate
Recall
39 (28.7) 7 (6.4) 6.49 (2.47, 17.04)*bc
Verbal Paired Associates Delayed Recall 34 (25.2) 6 (5.5) 5.59 (2.05, 15.21)*c
CTOPP Memory for Digits 48 (34.0) 10 (9.1) 4.34 (1.91, 9.84)*c
Motor/Visual Motor:
WJ-III COG Visual Matching 54 (37.8) 7 (6.4) 7.89 (3.19, 19.55)*ac
VMI 43 (29.3) 7 (6.3) 4.47 (1.77, 11.28)*c
BOT-2 52 (39.1) 7 (6.4) 7.43 (2.97, 18.62)*ac
Executive Functioning:
Shape School Inhibition 47 (34.6) 10 (9.2) 3.72 (1.61, 8.56)*
Shape School Switching 28 (20.9) 10 (9.1) 2.65 (1.09, 6.46)
Preschool Trails Inhibition 44 (33.1) 10 (9.4) 5.26 (2.21, 12.53)*c
Preschool Trails Switching 36 (26.9) 10 (9.3) 3.26 (1.37, 7.75)*
Nebraska Barnyard 41 (31.5) 6 (5.6) 5.10 (1.89, 13.76)*
Test of Inhibition and Attention GNG 68 (47.6) 11 (9.9) 7.32 (3.32, 16.16)*abc
Test of Inhibition and Attention CPT 71 (49.7) 11 (9.9) 8.12 (3.71, 17.78)*abc

Abbreviations: EPT/ELBW= extremely preterm; NBW = term-born normal birth weight; OR (95% CI) = odds ratio (95% confidence interval), adjusting for socioeconomic status, race, sex, time in school, and (for unstandardized measures only) age at assessment and reflecting an increase in the odds of the deficit in the EPT/ELBW versus NBW group; WJ-III-COG = Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd Edition; CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; VMI = Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration; BOT-2 = Bruininks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd Edition; GNG = Test of Inhibition and Attention condition; CPT = Continuous Performance Test condition.

#

Deficit is defined as score at <10th percentile for age on standardized tests or <10th percentile relative to performance of the term-born normal birth weight group for unstandardized tests.

a

Significant controlling for WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension.

b

Significant excluding children with BIA <70 and/or neurosensory impairment.

c

Significant excluding estimated scores for children who were not testable.

*

Adjusted p<.01.

Factors Associated with Outcomes in EPT/ELBW children

Neonatal risk factors and NDI at 20 months corrected age were associated with higher rates of cognitive deficits within the EPT/ELBW group (Table 5). These factors were related to a wide range of cognitive deficits in kindergarten. Lower SES was also significantly associated with higher rates of deficits on the BIA, WJ-III-COG Verbal Comprehension, CTOPP Elision, Shape School Inhibition, and Test of Inhibition and Attention GNG and CPT conditions. The most consistent predictors of cognitive deficits were NDI and ultrasound abnormalities. However, even when excluding children with NDI, lower birth weight (<750 g) and GA (<25 weeks) were significantly associated with higher rates of deficits on Shape School Inhibition, OR (CI) = 7.69 (1.81, 32.68), p = .006, and OR (CI) = 8.61 (1.97, 37.57), p = .004, respectively; and GA <25 weeks with higher rates of deficits on CTOPP Elision, OR (CI) = 6.62 (1.65, 26.53), p = .008.

Table 5.

Neonatal and early neurodevelopmental risk factors significantly (p<0.01) associated with cognitive deficits within the EPT/ELBW group.

Domain/Deficit# Risk Factor Risk Present
No. (%)
Risk Absent
No. (%)
Adjusted OR
(95% CI)
Global Cognitive Ability:
WJ-III COG Brief Intellectual
Ability (BIA)
NDI 33 (60) 14 (17) 7.58 (3.19, 18.00)
GA <25 wks 18 (60) 32 (29) 3.66 (1.48, 9.06)
AbnUS 10 (67) 40 (31) 9.37 (2.09, 42.07)
Infection 28 (48) 22 (26) 2.78 (1.29, 6.01)
Language:
WJ-III COG Verbal Comprehension NDI 31 (55) 16 (19) 4.89 (2.14, 11.18)
AbnUS 9 (60) 39 (30) 8.61 (1.96, 37.84)
CTOPP Elision NDI 37 (65) 22 (27) 4.76 (2.10, 10.81)
GA <25 wks 22 (71) 39 (35) 4.74 (1.78, 12.59)
BPD 44 (59) 17 (26) 4.15 (1.88, 9.16)
CTOPP Blending Words ROP 10 (37) 16 (14) 3.92 (1.44, 10.69)
Spatial and Non-verbal Reasoning :
WJ-III COG Concept Formation NDI 36 (64) 13 (15) 10.38 (4.21, 25.55)
AbnUS 11 (73) 38 (29) 26.30 (4.59, 150.79)
WJ-III COG Spatial Relations NDI 17 (31) 3 (4) 14.23 (3.57, 56.65)
AbnUS 6 (40) 15 (12) 6.55 (1.73, 24.80)
Memory:
Verbal Paired Associates Delayed
Recall
NDI 23 (44) 11 (14) 5.24 (2.08, 13.22)
AbnUS 8 (53) 26 (22) 10.19 (2.41, 43.16)
CTOPP Memory for Digits NDI 29 (51) 18 (22) 3.42 (1.56, 7.46)
Motor/Visual Motor:
WJ-III COG Visual Matching NDI 34 (61) 17 (20) 5.61 (2.54, 12.40)
GA <25 wks 19 (63) 35 (31) 3.72 (1.55, 8.94)
AbnUS 11 (73) 43 (34) 11.08 (2.57, 47.78)
VMI NDI 31 (53) 11 (13) 7.76 (3.17, 18.96)
BW <750 g 26 (47) 17 (18) 3.20 (1.45, 7.04)
AbnUS 10 (67) 33 (25) 11.98 (2.76, 52.10)
BOT-2 NDI 36 (69) 16 (20) 20.63 (6.86, 62.05)
Executive Functioning:
Shape School Inhibition NDI 34 (64) 13 (16) 8.41 (3.39, 20.89)
Shape School Switching NDI 22 (42) 6 (8) 7.68 (2.62, 22.51)
AbnUS 7 (47) 21 (18) 10.81 (2.24, 52.07)
Preschool Trails Inhibition NDI 27 (53) 17 (21) 3.64 (1.46, 9.05)
Preschool Trails Switching NDI 28 (54) 8 (10) 9.03 (3.36, 24.29)
AbnUS 10 (67) 26 (22) 20.13 (3.97, 102.11)
AbnUS 8 (53) 21 (18) 8.11 (1.93, 34.00)
Nebraska Barnyard NDI 26 (52) 15 (19) 4.25 (1.76, 10.26)
Test of Inhibition and Attention
GNG
NDI 44 (76) 24 (29) 8.76 (3.56, 21.56)
Test of Inhibition and Attention
CPT
NDI 43 (74) 28 (34) 4.75 (2.14, 10.53)
GA <25 wks 23 (77) 48 (42) 4.79 (1.74, 13.14)

Note that the “risk present” column lists the number of children with a given deficit who had the specified risk factor out of the total number of children with that risk factor, whereas the “risk absent” column lists the number of children with a deficit who did not have the specified risk factor out of the total number of children without that risk factor. The odds ratio reflects the increase in the odds of EPT/ELBW children having a deficit given that the specified risk factor is present compared to when that risk factor is absent.

Abbreviations: EPT/ELBW= extremely preterm; OR (95% CI) = odds ratio (95% confidence interval), adjusting for socioeconomic status, race, sex, time in school, and (for unstandardized measures only) age at assessment; WJ-III-COG = Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd Edition; NDI = neurodevelopmental impairment as defined by a neurosensory disorder and/or Mental Development Index <70 on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd Edition at 20 months; GA = gestational age; AbnUS = abnormal cranial ultrasound; Infection = neonatal infection as defined by sepsis, necrotizing entercolitis, or meningitis; SES = socioeconomic status; CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; BPD = bronchopulmonary dysplasia; ROP = retinopathy of prematurity; VMI = Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration; BOT-2 = Bruininks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd Edition; GNG = Test of Inhibition and Attention condition; CPT = Continuous Performance Test condition.

#

Deficit is defined as score at <10th percentile for age on standardized tests or <10th percentile relative to performance of the term-born normal birth weight group for unstandardized tests.

DISCUSSION

The findings of this study demonstrate cognitive deficits in EPT/ELBW children at school entry in a broad range of domains, including language, spatial and non-verbal reasoning, motor and perceptual-motor skills, memory, and EF. Group differences were largely unchanged when scores were based on corrected rather than chronological age, indicating that the differences were not artifacts of the lowered post-conceptual ages of the EPT/ELBW group. Despite these pervasive deficits and evidence that the EPT/ELBW had higher rates of multiple deficits than their NBW classmates, results also revealed that some cognitive skills were more adversely affected than others. The EPT/ELBW group’s weaknesses in motor skills, visual-motor proficiency, inhibition, and selective attention remained even when controlling for acquired verbal knowledge. Similarly, the EPT/ELBW group had higher rates of deficits in verbal memory, inhibition, and selective attention even when excluding children with neurosensory disorders or lower global cognitive ability (BIA <85).

To examine deficits in EF in EPT/ELBW children in detail, we administered tests of inhibitory control and selective attention, mental set shifting, and working memory. The EPT/ELBW group performed at lower levels and had higher rates of deficits in each of these areas compared to NBW controls. However, specific impairments in EF were found only for inhibitory control and selective attention. These and other specific cognitive deficits help to explain the high rates of learning disabilities in the EPT/ELBW population and provide a rationale for including measures of these abilities as part of early cognitive assessments (Bull, Espy, & Wiebe, 2008; Clark, Pritchard, & Woodward, 2010; Johnson et al., 2009; Litt, Taylor, Klein, & Hack, 2005; Mulder et al., 2010; Pritchard et al., 2009; Taylor et al., in press; Taylor et al., 2006). Our results also suggest that measures of inhibition and selective attention are more useful than tests of set switching and working memory in identifying selective impairments in EF in young EPT/ELBW children. One interpretation of this finding is that the set shifting conditions of Shape School and Trails-Preschool Test—Revised were more difficult for both groups than the inhibition conditions or the Test of Inhibition and Attention, and that the greater difficulty of these tasks diminished their sensitivity to the effects of birth status (Pritchard & Woodward, 2011). A possible explanation for the failure of the Nebraska Barnyard task to detect selective deficits is the demand this task places on multiple skills in addition to working memory, including knowledge of animal names and sounds and the ability to associate these stimuli with corresponding locations and colors on the computer screen.

The results agree with those of past studies of very preterm or EPT/ELBW samples showing generalized cognitive weaknesses with more pronounced deficits in visual-motor proficiency, memory, and EF than in measures of IQ or verbal skills (Bohm et al., 2004; Marlow et al., 2007; Mikkola et al., 2005; Mulder et al., 2010; Lind et al., 2011; Taylor et al., 2004, 2006). Our observations of pervasive effects on EF for the sample as a whole are also in accord with other recent findings (Aaronoudse-Moens et al., 2009; Anderson et al., 2011; Woodward, Clark, Pritchard, Anderson, & Inder, 2011).

The types of brain insults sustained by EPT/ELBW children help to explain the pattern of cognitive deficits observed in this study. Diffuse white matter abnormalities and the possibility of widespread effects of periventricular insults on brain development may account for vulnerability of EPT/ELBW children to pervasive cognitive deficits, whereas regionally specific insults to periventricular white matter, frontal-striatal circuits, temporal and parietal regions, and the cerebellum may account for their selective impairments in perceptual-motor skills, memory, and EF (Lawrence et al., 2009; Narberhaus et al., 2008; Nosarti et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2011; Woodward et al., 2009, 2011; Volpe, 2009). Evidence for deficits in response inhibition and attention allocation is also consistent with disrupted frontostriatal connectivity (Casey, Galvan, & Hare, 2005). These disruptions may be accompanied by compensatory neural reorganization at later ages (Lawrence et al., 2009; Nosarti et al., 2006) but we know little about the extent of such plasticity, the biological and environmental circumstances that support it, or when it occurs.

Because the EPT/ELBW and NBW groups were well matched for age, sex, race, and SES, results suggest that the group differences were more related to biological than to environmental factors. As further evidence for the biological basis of group differences, poorer cognitive outcomes for children in the EPT/ELBW group were associated with early childhood NDI and several neonatal risk factors, including GA <25 weeks, birth weight <750 g, cranial ultrasound abnormality, BPD, infection, and retinopathy of prematurity. Previous studies have shown similar relations of these neonatal risk factors with cognitive outcomes in older school-age children (Bohm & Katz-Salamon, 2003; Bohm et al., 2002, 2004; Dewey et al., 1999; Lind et al., 2011; Patrianakos-Hoobler et al., 2009b; Roberts et al., 2010; Sullivan & McGrath, 2003; Taylor et al., 2006). The likely basis for these associations is the greater probability of neuropathology in children born at higher neonatal risk (Taylor et al., 2011). Lower SES was also associated with higher rates of deficits in the EPT/ELBW group, confirming the importance of taking this fact into account in predicting cognitive outcomes at school entry (Aylward, 1992). The association of increased time in school with higher scores on a test of phonological processing highlights the role of learning opportunities as an additional contributor to cognitive development during this period of transition to formal schooling (Cahan & Cohen, 1989).

To our knowledge this study is the first to target children for assessment based on their enrollment as first-time kindergarten students. Assessing the sample during the first year in school provided at least limited control over children’s experience with formal education. An additional benefit of this procedure is that findings are applicable to children at this stage in their schooling, which is the first time that many children with more subtle cognitive deficits may have access to formal cognitive assessments and special education assistance. A related virtue of the study design is that NBW controls were from the same classrooms and neighborhoods as the EPT/ELBW children, which insured that the two groups had similar educational experiences during their first year in school. Administration of a comprehensive test battery also allowed us to investigate cognitive impairment in EPT/ELBW children across multiple ability domains, establish the relative risks for different types of deficits, and examine EF in greater depth than most previous studies of this age group.

An important clinical implication of the findings is that, while EPT/ELBW children are vulnerable to a wide range of cognitive impairments in kindergarten, some have more selective deficits. The nature of these deficits suggests that even children with more subtle weaknesses may benefit from extra time to complete tasks, repeated exposures to new information, and highly structured approaches to learning (Litt et al., 2005; Taylor et al., 2006). The findings also support recommendations to facilitate the early development of EF in this population (Woodward et al., 2011) and to arrange for especially close monitoring of cognitive development at early school age in children who had earlier NDI or extreme degrees of preterm birth or low birth weight.

A limitation of the study is that some children were unable to comply with test demands due to child non-compliance, incomplete testing, or low cognitive functioning. Inclusion of estimated scores for children who were untestable due to limited cognitive functioning allowed us to take these children into account in our primary analyses of group differences. Results were similar when these estimated scores were excluded from consideration, indicating that our procedures for assigning missing values did not alter the pattern of results. Nevertheless, scores that were missing for other reasons, such as child noncompliance or lack of opportunity to complete the testing, could not be estimated and the magnitude of group differences must be interpreted with caution. A further limitation is that NBW controls were selected from among classmates of EPT/ELBW children whose parents granted permission for study contact, which limited our ability to find the closest possible match and may have biased selection of the control group. Questions can also be raised with regard to the representativeness of our sample as we were unable to recruit 25% of the original EPT/ELBW birth cohort and the children were recruited from a single perinatal center in a large urban area. The levels of cognitive ability and rates of deficits may differ from those for EPT/ELBW children born in other regions of the country. However, our cohort of EPT/ELBW children did not differ significantly in sociodemographic characteristics from either non-participating EPT/ELBW children or from the NBW group, suggesting that our group differences have internal validity. Additionally, the findings may provide information relevant to other urban centers, where many participating children are from lower SES backgrounds and where risks of premature births are high relative to other regions (Kaufman, Dole, Savitz, & Herring, 2003; Stoll et al., 2010).

Longitudinal follow-up of our sample will be useful in investigating age-related changes in the cognitive deficits revealed in this study. For example, follow-up of the children into later grades in school may reveal selective deficits in aspects of EF that were obscured in the present study by young children’s difficulties in comprehending more complex instructional sets. Efforts to elucidate the neuropathology of selective deficits in EF and other cognitive skills are also needed to understand why some skills are more affected than others and help account the considerable variability in outcomes within samples of EPT/ELBW children. Study of associations of cognitive assessments in kindergarten with children’s subsequent learning progress and behavior adjustment will be of additional value in determining the precursors of longer-term achievement and behavior problems and in identifying children most in need of early interventions.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was funded by grant HD050309 from the National Institutes of Health. We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Anne Birnbaum, Dr. Elizabeth Roth, Dan Maier, Andrea Barkoukis Gefteas, Michelle R. Jacobs, Alice Costiuc, and Ketrin Lengu in recruitment, data collection and coding. The findings presented in this paper have not been published previously either in print or electronically. No conflicts of interests exist for any of the authors.

REFERENCES

  1. Aaronoudse-Moens CSH, Smidts DP, Oosterlaan J, Duivenvoorden HJ, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:981–993. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9327-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Spencer-Smith MM, Roberts G, Doyle LW. Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;36:57–73. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson PJ, Doyle LW, the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:3264–3272. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.24.3264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avchen RN, Scott KG, Mason CA. Birth weight and school-age disabilities: A population-based study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;154:895–901. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.10.895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aylward GP. The relationship between environmental risk and developmental outcome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1992;13:222–229. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baron IS, Erickson K, Ahronovich MD, Baker R, Litman FR. Neuropsychological and behavioral outcomes of extremely low birth weight at age three. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;36:5–21. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baron IS, Erickson K, Ahronovich MD, Litman FR, Brandt J. Spatial location memory discriminates children born at extremely low birth weight and late-preterm at age three. Neuropsychology. 2010 doi: 10.1037/a0020382. doi: 10.1037/a0020382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bayless S, Stevenson J. Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development. 2007;83:247–254. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  10. Beery KE, Beery NA. The Beery-Buktenica Development Test of Visual-Motor Integration: Beery VMI, administration, scoring, and teaching manual. 5th Ed NCS Pearson; Minneapolis, MN: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  11. Belsky J, Mackinnon C. Transition to school: Developmental trajectories and school experiences. Early Education and Development. 1994;5:106–119. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bohm B, Katz-Salamon M. Cognitive development at 5.5 years of children with chronic lung disease of prematurity. Archives of Disease in Childhood -- Fetal & Neonatal Edition. 2003;88:F101–105. doi: 10.1136/fn.88.2.F101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bohm B, Katz-Salamon M, Smedler AC, Forssberg H. Developmental risks and protective factors for influencing cognitive outcome at 5½ years of age in very-low-birthweight children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2002;44(8):508–516. doi: 10.1017/s001216220100247x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bohm B, Smedler A-C, Forssberg H. Impulse control, working memory and other executive functions in preterm children when starting school. Acta Paediatrica. 2004;93:1363–1371. doi: 10.1080/08035250410021379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bruininks RH, Bruininks BD. BOT-2: Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. 2nd Ed American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33:205–228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cahan S, Cohen N. Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development. 1989;60(5):1239–1249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Casey BJ, Galvan A, Hare TA. Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15:239–244. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clark CAC, Pritchard VE, Woodward LJ. Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1176–1191. doi: 10.1037/a0019672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dewey D, Crawford SG, Creighton DE, Sauve RS. Long-term neuropsychological outcomes in very low birth weight children free of sensorineural impairments. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1999;21:851–865. doi: 10.1076/jcen.21.6.851.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dewey D, Creighton DE, Heath JA, Wilson BN, Anseeuw-Deeks D, Crawford SG. Assessment of developmental coordination disorder in children born with extremely low birth weights. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;16:42–56. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Espy KA, Bull R, Martin J, Stroup W. Measuring the development of executive control with the shape school. Psychological Assessment. 2006;18:373–381. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Espy K, Cwik M. The development of a trail making test in young children: The TRAILS-P. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2004;18:411–422. doi: 10.1080/138540409052416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Espy KA, Fang H, Charak D, Minich N, Taylor HG. Growth mixture modeling of academic achievement in children of varying birth weight risk. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:460–474. doi: 10.1037/a0015676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fanaroff AA, Stoll BJ, Wright LL, Carlo WA, Ehrenkranz RA, Stark AR, et al. Trends in neonatal morbidity and mortality for very low birthweight infants. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2007;196:147.e1–147.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2006.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fay TB, Yeates KO, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, Taylor HG. Predicting longitudinal patterns of functional deficits in children with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:271–282. doi: 10.1037/a0014936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Federal Financial Institutions Examinations Council Geocoding System [Accessed January 1, 2010-November 1, 2010]; http://www.ffiec.gov/Geocode/default.htm.
  29. Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2010;31:658–667. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gidley Larson G, Baron IS, Erickson K, Ahronovich MD, Baker R, Litman FR. Neuromotor outcomes at school age after extremely low birth weight: Early detection of subtle signs. Neuropsychology. 2011;25:66–75. doi: 10.1037/a0020478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gonzalez LM, Anderson VA, Wood SJ, Mitchell A, Harvey AS. The localization and lateralization of memory deficits in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2007;48:124–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00907.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hauser RM, Warren JR. Socioeconomic indexes for occupation: A review, update, and critique. Sociological Methodology. 1997;27:177–298. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hughes C, Dunn J, White A. Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in “Hard-to-manage” preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39:981–994. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Johnson S, Hennessy E, Smith R, Trikic R, Wolke D, Marlow N. Academic attainment and special educational needs in extremely preterm children at 11 years of age: The EPICure study. Archives of Disease in Childhood -- Fetal & Neonatal Edition. 2009;94:F283–289. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.152793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kaufman JS, Dole N, Savitz DA, Herring AH. Modeling community-level effects on preterm birth. Annals of Epidemiology. 2003;13:377–384. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(02)00480-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kulseng S, Jennekens-Schinkel A, Naess P, Romundstad P, Indredavik M, Vik T, Brubakk A-M. Very-low-birthweight and term small-for-gestational-age adolescents: Attention revisited. Acta Paediatrica. 2006;95:224–230. doi: 10.1080/08035250500421568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Larroque B, Ancel P-Y, Marret S, Marchand L, Andre M, Arnaud C, Kaminski M, for the EPIPAGE Study group Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): A longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet. 2008;371:813–820. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60380-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lawrence EJ, Rubia K, Murray RM, McGuire PK, Walshe M, Allin M, Nosarti C. The neural basis of response inhibition and attention allocation as mediated by gestational age. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:1038–1050. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Leonard CH, Clyman RI, Piecuch RE, Juster RP, Ballard RA, Behle MB. Effect of medical and social risk factors on outcome of prematurity and very low birth weight. Journal of Pediatrics. 1990;116:620–626. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(05)81616-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lind A, Korkman M, Lehtonen L, Lapinleimu H, Parkkola R, Matomake J, Haataja L, the Pipari Study Group Cognitive and neuropsychological outcomes at 5 years of age in preterm children born in the 2000s. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2011;53:256–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03828.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Litt J, Taylor HG, Klein N, Hack M. Learning disabilities in children with very low birth weig ht: Prevalence, neuropsychological correlates, and educational interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38:130–141. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380020301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Luoma L, Herrgard E, Martikainen A, Ahonen T. Speech and language development of children born at ≤32 weeks’ gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1998;40:380–387. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D. Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2007;120:793–804. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mather N, Jaffe LE. Woodcock-Johnson III: Reports, guidelines and recommendations. Wiley; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  45. Ment L, Vohr B, Allan W, Katz K, Schneider K, Westerveld M, Makuch R. Change in Cognitive Function Over Time in Very Low-Birth-Weight Infants. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:705–711. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.6.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mikkola K, Ritari N, Tommiska V, Salokorpi T, Lehtonen L, Tammela O, Fellman, for the Finnish ELBW Cohort Study Group Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born in 1996-1997. Pediatrics. 2005;116:1391–1400. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Marlow N. Processing speed and working memory underlie academic attainment in very preterm children. Archives of Disease in Childhood -- Fetal & Neonatal Edition. 2010 doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.167965. doi:10.1136/adc.209.167965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Narberhaus A, Segarra D, Caldu X, Gimenez M, Pueyo R, Botet F, Junque C. Corpus callosum and prefrontal functions in adolescents with history of very preterm birth. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:111–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nosarti C, Giouroukou E, Healy E, Rifkin L, Walshe M, Reichenberg A, Murray RM. Grey and white matter distribution in very preterm adolescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome. Brain. 2008;131:205–217. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nosarti C, Rubia K, Smith AB, Frearson S, Williams SC, Rifkin L, Murray RM. Altered functional neuroanatomy of response inhibition in adolescent males who were born very preterm. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2006;48:265–271. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206000582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Patra K, Wilson-Costello D, Taylor HG, Mercuri-Minich N, Hack M. Grades I-II intraventricular hemorrhage in extremely low birth weight infants: Effects on neurodevelopment. Pediatrics. 2006;149:169–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2006.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Patrianakos-Hoobler A, Msall ME, Huo D, Marks JD, Plesha-Troyke S, Schreiber MD. Predicting school readiness from neurodevelopmental assessments at age 2 years after respiratory distress syndrome in infants born preterm. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2009a;52:379–385. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03343.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Patrianakos-Hoobler A, Msall ME, Marks JD, Huo D, Schreiber MD. Risk factors affecting school readiness in premature infant with respiratory distress syndrome. Pediatrics. 2009b;124:258–267. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pritchard VE, Clark CAC, Liberty K, Champion PR, Wilson K, Woodward LJ. Early school-based learning difficulties in children born very preterm. Early Human Development. 2009;85:215–224. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pritchard VE, Woodward LJ. Preschool executive control on the Shape School task: Measurement considerations and utility. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23:31–43. doi: 10.1037/a0021095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rimm-Kaufmann SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2000;15:147–166. [Google Scholar]
  57. Roberts G, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW, for the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group The stability of the diagnosis of developmental disability between ages 2 and 9 in a geographic cohort of very preterm children born in 1997. Archives of Disease in Children. 2010;95:786–790. doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.160283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schmidt B, Asztalos EV, Roberts RS, Robertson CMT, Sauve RS, Whitfield MF. Impact of bronchopulmonary dysplasia, brain injury, and severe retinopathy on the outcome of extremely low-birth-weight infants at 18 months: Results from the Trial of Indomethacin Prophylaxis in Preterms. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:1124–1129. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.9.1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Simpson A, Riggs KJ. Conditions under which children experience inhibitory difficulty with a “button-press” Go/No-go task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2006;94:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stjernqvist K, Svenningsen NW. Ten-year follow-up of children born before 29 gestational weeks: health, cognitive development, behaviour and school achievement. Acta Paediatrica. 1999;88:557–562. doi: 10.1080/08035259950169594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Bell EF, Shankaran S, Laptook AR, Walsh MC, Higgins RD. Neonatal outcomes of extremely preterm infants from the NICHD Neonatal Research Network. Pediatrics. 2010;126:443–456. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sullivan MC, McGrath MM. Perinatal morbidity, mild motor delay, and later school outcomes. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2003;45:104–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Taylor HG. Academic performance and learning disabilities. In: Nosarti C, Murray RM, Hack M, editors. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm birth: from childhood to adult life. University Press; Cambridge: 2010. pp. 195–218. [Google Scholar]
  64. Taylor HG, Klein N, Anselmo MG, Espy KA, Minich N, Hack M. Learning problems in kindergarten children with extremely preterm birth. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.137. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Taylor HG, Klein N, Drotar D, Schluchter M, Hack M. Consequences and risks of <1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, achievement, and adaptive functioning. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2006;27:459–469. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200612000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Taylor HG, Filipek PA, Juranek, Bangert JB, Minich N, Hack M. Brain volumes in adolescents with very low birth weight: Effects on brain structures and adolescents with neuropsychological outcomes. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;36:96–117. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Taylor HG, Minich N, Bangert B, Filipek PA, Hack M. Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: Associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10:987–1004. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704107078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: A complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurology. 2009;8:110–124. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70294-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wagner RK, Torgesen CA, Rashotte C. Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Pro-ed; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  70. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  71. Weibe SA, Sheffield T, Nelson JM, Clark CAC, Chevalier N, Espy KA. The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108:436–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wilson-Costello D, Friedman H, Minich N, Siner B, Taylor G, Schluchter M, Hack M. Improved neurodevelopmental outcomes for extremely low birth weight infants in 2000-2002. Pediatrics. 2007;110:37–45. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wolke D, Samara M, Bracewell M, Marlow N, for the EPICure Study Group Specific language difficulties and school achievement in children born at 25 weeks of gestation or less. Journal of Pediatrics. 2008;152:256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.06.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Woodcock RC, McGrew KS, Mather S. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Riverside Publishing; Itasca, IL: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  75. Woodward LJ, Clark CAC, Pritchard VE, Anderson PJ, Inder TE. Neonatal white matter abnormalities predict global executive function impairment in children born very preterm. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;26:22–41. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Woodward LJ, Moor S, Hood KM, Champion PR, Foster-Cohen S, Inder TE, Austin NC. Very preterm children show impairments across multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age 4 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood -- Fetal & Neonatal Edition. 2009;94:F339–F334. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.146282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Yudkin PL, Aboualfa M, Eyre JA, Redman CWG, Wilkinson AR. New birthweight and head circumference centiles for gestational ages 24 to 42 weeks. Early Human Development. 1987;15:45–52. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(87)90099-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES