Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2012 Feb 21;7(2):e31441. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031441

Cardiac Signatures of Personality

Stefan Koelsch 1,*, Juliane Enge 1, Sebastian Jentschke 1
Editor: Martin Gerbert Frasch2
PMCID: PMC3283631  PMID: 22363649

Abstract

Background

There are well-established relations between personality and the heart, as evidenced by associations between negative emotions on the one hand, and coronary heart disease or chronic heart failure on the other. However, there are substantial gaps in our knowledge about relations between the heart and personality in healthy individuals. Here, we investigated whether amplitude patterns of the electrocardiogram (ECG) correlate with neurotisicm, extraversion, agreeableness, warmth, positive emotion, and tender-mindedness as measured with the Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness (NEO) personality inventory. Specifically, we investigated (a) whether a cardiac amplitude measure that was previously reported to be related to flattened affectivity (referred to as Inline graphic values) would explain variance of NEO scores, and (b) whether correlations can be found between NEO scores and amplitudes of the ECG.

Methodology/Principal Findings

NEO scores and rest ECGs were obtained from 425 healthy individuals. Neuroticism and positive emotion significantly differed between individuals with high and low Inline graphic values. In addition, stepwise cross-validated regressions indicated correlations between ECG amplitudes and (a) agreeableness, as well as (b) positive emotion.

Conclusions/Significance

These results are the first to demonstrate that ECG amplitude patterns provide information about the personality of an individual as measured with NEO personality scales and facets. These findings open new perspectives for a more efficient personality assessment using cardiac measures, as well as for more efficient risk-stratification and pre-clinical diagnosis of individuals at risk for cardiac, affective and psychosomatic disorders.

Introduction

The personality of an individual has profound effects on the peripheral physiology. Such effects appear to be due to the modulatory influence of brain structures implicated in personality (such as the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, insular cortex, and hippocampal formation) [1], [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7] on peripheral organs and tissues through the autonomic, the endocrine, and the immune system [2], [8], [9], [5], [10], [11], [7], [12], [13]. These modulatory influences are relevant for the understanding of a wide range of somatic diseases, such as cardiovascular disease [14], [15], inflammatory disorders [16], or autoimmune diseases [17].

The present study uses electrocardiography to investigate such modulatory influences of personality traits on peripheral physiology in healthy individuals. Usually, electrocardiograms (ECGs) are recorded with several leads and a temporal resolution in the millisecond range, thus providing multidimensional information about cardiac function (see also Figure 1a,b). Regional cardiac function (such as conduction of excitation, conduction velocity, contractile force, coronary circulation, as well as aspects of cardiac valve function) is modulated by neurons in intrathoracic extracardiac and intrinsic cardiac ganglia [18]. These ganglia probably represent the final common pathway through which the diverse, extrinsic neural signals to the heart are modified before being transmitted to the effector tissues [19]. The neurons of these ganglia form the cardiac nerve plexus, a system of nerve cells within and around the heart which integrates and modifies sensory input and cardiac output [19].

Figure 1. Illustration of ECG waves and Inline graphic values.

Figure 1

(a) Illustration of standard ECG leads: The six extremity leads (I, II, III, aVL, aVR, aVF) record voltage differences by means of electrodes placed on the limbs (left panel). The triangle shows the spatial relationships of the extremity leads, which record electrical voltages onto the frontal plane of the body. The six chest leads (V1–V6) record voltage differences by means of electrodes placed on the chest wall (right panel). The oval indicates spatial relationships of the six chest leads, which record electrical voltages transmitted onto the horizontal plane. (b) Illustration of ECG amplitude parameters used in the present study (P-, R-, RS-, and T-waves, each wave is measured from each of the twelve ECG leads in each individual). Due to the absence of a Q-wave in some individuals at some leads, the R-wave is measured with regard to the iso-electric line. (c) Illustration of ECG amplitude parameters used to calculate Inline graphic values (indicated in red): R-wave of III (measured from the baseline preceding the P-wave), RS-complex of aVL, T-wave of aVL, and RS-complex of the chest lead with the maximal RS amplitude (usually V3 or V2). The upper panel shows averaged ECG cycles measured from a male subject with a high Inline graphic value (Inline graphic90th percentile of our study population), the lower panel shows averaged ECG cycles from a male subject with a low Inline graphic value (Inline graphic10th percentile of our study population). Both subjects had similar body height (184 vs. 185 cm), body weigt (70 vs. 72 kg), and normal QRS axis orientation (96.23Inline graphic vs. 75.24Inline graphic). Note the smaller T-wave in aVL in relation to the RS-wave in aVL, and the smaller R-wave in III in relation to the RS-wave in V3, in the subject with low Inline graphic value (lower panel), compared to the subject with high Inline graphic value (upper panel). (d) Equation for computation of Inline graphic values. Computation of the ECG amplitude parameters shown in (c) leads to an Inline graphic value of 1.61 for the ECG shown in the upper, and an Inline graphic value of 0.17 for the ECG shown in the lower panel of (c) (for a better readability, values are scaled by a factor Inline graphic). Abbrevations: Inline graphic: R-wave of lead III (measured from the baseline preceding the P-wave); Inline graphic: RS-complex of aVL; Inline graphic: RS-complex of the chest lead with the maximal RS amplitude; Inline graphic: T- wave of aVL.

Other than traditionally believed, intrathoracic ganglia are not merely relay stations for autonomic efferent neuronal control of the heart, but integrate the spatial and temporal summation of varied sensory inputs, as well as inputs from central sympathetic/parasympathetic nerve fibres [19]. Therefore, activity of neurons within the cardiac nerve plexus (and thus regional heart activity) is directly and indirectly modulated by several psychological factors: (1) Both parasympathetic and sympathetic nerve fibres project into the cardiac nerve plexus [20]. Such autonomic activity is an integral component of emotion and affective traits [14], [21]. (2) Efferent autonomic neuronal activity is modulated by limbic/paralimbic forebrain structures such as hypothalamus, amygdala, insular cortex, and (medial) orbitofrontal cortex. The orbitofrontal cortex, e.g., hosts neurons projecting directly to the the parabrachial nucleus, the nucleus of the solitary tract, and to sympathetic preganglionic neurons in the interomedial lateral nucleus of the spinal cord [20]. This is relevant because patients with orbitofrontal lesions show marked personality changes [3], lending plausibility to the assumption that personality has peripheral physiological effects that become apparent in regional activity of the heart. (3) The left and right hemispheres of the human forebrain are assumed to have different associations with particular emotions and affective traits [21], probably due to an asymmetrical representation of homeostatic activity that originates from asymmetries in the peripheral autonomic nervous system. Lesion studies indicate a right hemisphere dominance for sympathetic efferent neuronal effects, and stimulation of the right anterior insular cortex elicits increases in heart rate and blood pressure, while left anterior insular stimulation decreases heart rate [22], [23], [20]. Correspondingly, different emotional states and affective traits exert asymmetric autonomic outflow that can modulate regional activity of the heart. For example, the right cardiac nerve innervates the anterior surface of the heart (including the sinoatrial node, the atrioventricular node, and the anterior surfaces of the right and left ventricles), whereas the left cardiac nerve innervates the posterolateral surface of the heart (including the atrioventricular node and the posterior surfaces of the right and left ventricles) [24], [25]. (4) Activity of neurons within intrathoracic autonomic ganglia is modulated by circulating hormones (in particular circulating adrenalin and angiotensin II), as well as by other sensory information (such as vascular information, information about blood pressure, electrolytic balance, and blood gases). Due to the effects of particular emotions and affective traits on circulating hormones (including adrenalin), blood pressure and blood gases, such emotions and affective traits indirectly also modulate regional activity of the heart.

In clinical populations, the diverse effects of affective traits on the heart have been demonstrated by a plethora of clinical and experimental evidence implicating anger, hostility, depression and anxiety in the occurrence of arteriosclerosis, coronary artery disease, hypertension, myocardial ischemia and infarction, cardiac arrythmia formation, and sudden cardiac death [26]. Our approach is to find correlations between regional heart activity (as reflected in the various waves of different ECG leads) and affective traits in non-clinical populations.

The present study builds upon a previous study that used high-resolution ECGs to investigate whether amplitude patterns of ECG waves correlate with flattened affectivity [6]. In that study [6], flattened affectivity was assessed based on interviews, and characterized by emotional coldness and a limited capacity to express warm, tender feelings (as indicated by reduced facial and eye expression of emotion, reduced prosodic and gestural expression of emotion, and sparse use of emotional language) [27], [28]. Using discriminant analysis, four ECG waves were found as predictors for flattened activity (Figure 1c). The computation of these values according to the equation shown in Figure 1d results in a single value (henceforth referred to as Inline graphic value) for each individual. Inline graphic values were lower in individuals with flattened affectivity than in normal controls, and differences in Inline graphic values were related to autonomic (sympatho-vagal) balance: Individuals with low Inline graphic values showed a lower heart rate variability (HRV, in terms of a lower variability of beat-to-beat intervals), as well as lower high-frequency (HF) and higher low-frequency (LF) spectral power (and a higher LF/HF ratio) than subjects with high Inline graphic values [6]. Moreover, two functional neuroimaging experiments showed functional differences in the amygdala and the hippocampal formation between individuals with high and low Inline graphic values [6], supporting the notion of differences in emotionality between groups.

The first aim of the present study was to validate the relation between Inline graphic values and emotional personality using standardized personality measures. Therefore, we investigated whether scores of neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness (as measured with the Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Five Factor Inventory, NEO-FFI) [29], as well as scores of positive emotion, warmth, and tender-mindedness (as measured with the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, NEO-PI-R) [29] differed between individuals with higher and lower Inline graphic values. The facets positive emotion (i.e., the tendency to experience positive emotions), warmth (i.e., interest in and friendliness towards others), and tender-mindedness (i.e., attitude of sympathy for others) [29] were chosen because they are related to the concept of flattened affectivity [6]. These three facets are part of the personality factors extraversion and agreeableness, of which we obtained the entire scales, along with the neuroticism scale (which is also related to the experience of emotional states), using the NEO-FFI [29]. In specific, we hypothesized that individuals with low Inline graphic values would show higher neuroticism scores, and lower scores of extraversion, agreeableness, warmth, tender-mindedness, and positive emotion. This would represent further evidence for a relation between ECG amplitude ratios (as measured with the Inline graphic values) and personality traits. In addition, based on previous results [6], individuals with low Inline graphic values were expected to show a lower HRV (in terms of beat-to-beat interval variability), as well as lower HF and higher LF spectral power (and a higher LF/HF ratio) than subjects with high Inline graphic values.

Furthermore, the present study also takes a new approach: We investigated whether the scores of the NEO scales (neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness) and NEO facets (positive emotion, warmth, and tender-mindedness) can be used to identify cardiac amplitude signatures related to an individual's personality. Therefore, we obtained the amplitude values of the ECG waves shown in Figure 1b from each subject, separately for each of the twelve ECG leads (leading to an array of 48 values per subject). Using stepwise regressions, we then investigated whether an individual's personality (as measured with the NEO) can be predicted based on the ECG amplitude pattern of that individual.

The rationale for our approach is that personality traits have peripheral-physiological effects which are reflected in the activity of the heart. Previous work with non-clinical adult populations suggests relations between temporal measures of heart activity, showing lower HRV in individuals with higher neuroticism [30], consistent with findings of lower HRV in depressive patients (without myocardial infarction) [31]. However, reports on relations between personality and HRV in non-clinical populations are extremely sparse, and the relatively moderate test-retest reliability of HRV measures renders HRV measures suboptimal for the assessment of personality aspects (this is partly due to HRV being influenced, for example, by the breathing rate and the circadian rhythm) [32]. The test-retest reliability of Inline graphic values, by contrast, appears to be relatively high (Inline graphic in our previous study) [6]. Moreover, compared to personality questionnaires, physiological measures do not face the problem of potential subjective bias such as socially desirable responding, inaccuracies in self-perception, self-favouring tendencies, self-deception, and moralistic bias [33], [34], [35], [36], [37], and may thus be better suited to assess long-term emotional dispositions related to personality. Therefore, the discovery of ECG signatures reflecting personality traits would not only broaden our knowledge about cardiac effects of personality, but also represent a step towards the diagnostics of emotional personality aspects with cardiac measures.

Methods

Subjects

425 participants (212 females), aged between 18 and 33 years, were measured. All participants were university students. Exlusion criteria were previous diagnosis of any cardio-vascular disease, or any mental or psychiatric disorder (no clinical diagnostic interviews were conducted in the course of this study). Written informed consent was obtained, the study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Leipzig, and conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Questionnaires

We obtained scores of the personality scales Extraversion, Agreeableness and Neuroticism (each with 12 Items) from the German translation of the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) [29], [38], as well as scores of the personality facets warmth, positive emotion and tender-mindedness (each with 8 Items) from the German translation of the revised version of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) [29], [39]. These facets were used because they are more closely related to the construct of flattened affectivity than other NEO-PI-R facets (positive emotion is related to a tendency to experience positive emotions, warmth is related to interest in and friendliness towards others, and tender-mindedness is related to an attitude of sympathy for others) [29]; thus, these facets appeared to be well suited to test whether Inline graphic values are related to personality facets. The number of items amounted to 54 Items; note that the NEO-FFI is a short version of the NEO-PI-R and that, hence, twelve items of the NEO-FFI scales are identical to those of the NEO-PI-R facets (warmth and positive emotion are facets of the extraversion scale, and tendermindedness is a facet of the agreeableness scale). Each item was answered on a five-steps Likert scale (“strongly disagree”, “disagree”, “neutral”, “agree”, “strongly agree”). These response categories were re-coded into values from 0 to 4 (with 4 corresponding to a high score of the respective scale of the facet); then for each subject the summed values for each facet or scale were divided by the number of items (thus a score of 0 indicates the lowest possible score for a facet or scale, and a score of 4 indicates the highest possible score for a scale or facet). For each individual, all scores were then standardized according to the manuals of the German version of the NEO-FFI [38] and the NEO-PI-R [39].

ECG Measurements and Data analysis

From each participant, a 12 leads resting ECG (2 min duration) was obtained in supine position under standard conditions according to the guidelines of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology [32]. ECGs were recorded (with disposable electrodes) with high resolution (sampling rate: 1000 Hz, resolution: 22 bit, without on-line filtering) using a Refa-system (Twente Medical Systems, Enschede, NL).

All ECG analyses were carried out blinded, that is, without any knowledge about the NEO-scores of any participant. ECG-wave detection and measurement of ECG amplitude values was performed electronically (using the in-house software package Kardionoon 1.0) [6] and visually controlled by the first author. The amplitude measurements are described in detail elsewhere [6]; in short, for each lead of each participant, all P-, R-, S-, and T-wave peaks were identified in the raw ECG, and artifact-free waves were then averaged separately for each lead to obtain ECG waves representative for each participant (i.e., for an artifact-free ECG in a subject with a heart rate of 60 beats per minute, 120 P-, 120 R-, 120 R-S-, and 120 T-waves were averaged, separately for each wave, and for each of the twelve ECG leads). From these averaged ECG waves, absolute amplitude values of P-, R-, R-S-, and T-waves were measured electronically; R-wave amplitudes were measured with respect to the baseline of the averaged ECG cycle (see also Figure 1b for illustration), T-wave amplitudes were measured with respect to the first plateau-like wave shape preceding the T-wave peak [6]. Moreover, to reduce potential bias introduced by slightly different placement of chest electrodes, the maximal value of each of the four ECG waves (P-, R-, R-S-, and T-wave) measured at any of the chest leads was entered into further analysis. Thus, an array of 52 ECG amplitude values was obtained from each individual (4 ECG amplitude values×12 ECG leads, plus the 4 maximal ECG wave values measured at the chest leads).

With regard to the HRV, the following temporal and spectral measures of the HRV were electronically computed and analyzed for each subject (using the BIOSIG toolbox) [40]: mNN (mean time of successive heart beat intervals), SDNN (SD of successive heart beat intervals), RMSSD (root mean square of the SD of successive heart beat intervals), SD1, SD2, low frequency power (LF, 0.04–0.15 Hz), high frequency power (HF 0.15–0.4 Hz), and the LF/HF ratio (both LF and HF were calculated in normalized units, n.u.). QRS detection was visually controlled by the first author.

The Inline graphic value was computed for each subject according to the equation shown in Figure 1d. To test whether NEO personality scores, as well as HRV values, differ between individuals with higher and lower Inline graphic values, subjects were divided into two groups based on a median split of the Inline graphic values. Subsequently, scores of NEO-scales and NEO-facets, as well as HRV measures (mNN, SDNN, RMSSD, SD1, SD2, LF n.u., HF n.u., LF/HF) were compared between groups using two-sided independent samples Inline graphictests. Test were carried out according to the directed hypotheses based on our previous study [6], therefore the significance level was set to Inline graphic.

To investigate correlations between ECG amplitude values and body height, body weight, and body mass index (BMI), stepwise linear regressions were performed with height, weight, and BMI as dependent variables, and all ECG amplitude values (52 per subject, see above) as independent variables. 32 participants were excluded from this analysis, because not all amplitude values could be obtained (e.g., no clear P- or T-wave could be measured at aVL), resulting in a pool of 393 participants (194 males) used for the regression analyses. To investigate correlations between personality scores of the NEO scales and ECG amplitude values, stepwise linear regressions were performed for each scale and facet separately, with the standardized scores of each score, and the T-scores of each facet, as dependent variable, and all ECG amplitude values as independent variables; in addition, body height, body size, and BMI were included as independent variables (to estimate a possible influence of these variables). Stepwise regressions were calculated using using SPSS 19 (IBM, NY). To ensure the validity of the results obtained from the stepwise regression analyses, cross-validated (leave-one-out) regression coefficients were computed using matlab (Nattick, MA): For each participant a predicted value was calculated using the b-weights from the stepwise regression of all remaining participants. That is, 393 stepwise regressions were performed, each without the data set of one individual (‘leave-one-out’), in order to use the regression model obtained with the remaining 392 individuals to predict the score of the ‘left-out’ individual). Then, the correlation between the predicted scores and the actual scores was calculated. This cross-validated regression was performed to ensure that any statistically significant regression would not be due to the large number of subjects, or the large number of variables used for the regressions. Bonferroni-corrected significance level was Inline graphic.

Results

Analysis of Inline graphic values

Based on the median of the Inline graphic values (Inline graphic, range: Inline graphic), participants were split into two groups (higher and lower Inline graphic values) to compare NEO scores and HRV parameters between groups. Two-sided independent samples Inline graphictests indicated that individuals with lower Inline graphic values showed significantly higher neuroticism (Inline graphic, Cohen's Inline graphic, Figure 2a) and lower positive emotion (Inline graphic, Cohen's Inline graphic, Figure 2b), and there was a trend for lower extraversion in individuals with lower Inline graphic values (see Table 1a for complete statistics). Moreover, with regard to the HRV values, analogous Inline graphictests indicated that individuals of the group with lower Inline graphic values had lower HF (normalized units) power (Inline graphic, Cohen's Inline graphic), higher LF (normalized units) power (Inline graphic, Cohen's Inline graphic), and a higher LF/HF ratio (Inline graphic, Cohen's Inline graphic, see also Table 1b, Figure 2c), and there was a trend for a lower mean heart rate in the group with lower Inline graphic values.

Figure 2. Group comparisons between individuals with Inline graphic values above and below the median of Inline graphic values.

Figure 2

Individuals with higher Inline graphic values had lower neuroticism (a), and higher positive emotion scores (b). Moreover, individuals with higher Inline graphic values had lower normalized LF power, higher normalized HF power, and a lower LF/HF ratio (c). Abbrevations: LF (n.u.): low frequency power (0.04–0.15 Hz) of the HRV in normalized units; HF (n.u.): high frequency power (0.15–0.4 Hz) of the HRV in normalized units; LF/HF: ratio of low frequency power to high frequency power.

Table 1. NEO-scores, heart rate variability values, and body characteristics (means, with SD in parentheses), separately for the group with Inline graphic values below and above the median of Inline graphic values.

lower Inline graphic higher Inline graphic Inline graphic (Inline graphic) values Cohen's Inline graphic
(a) NEO-parameters
Neuroticism 101.88 (1.20) 97.11 (1.13) −2.88 (.004) −.29
Extraversion 100.85 (1.31) 104.36 (1.23) 1.95 (.052) .19
Agreeableness 103.81 (1.14) 106.00 (1.11) 1.38 (.168) .14
Warmth 50.30 (.71) 51.90 (0.75) 1.54 (.123) .15
Positive  Emotion 49.48 (.66) 51.51 (0.69) 2.13 (.034) .21
Tender- mindedness 48.49 (.72) 49.22 (0.69) 0.723 (.470) .07
(b) HRV-parameters
mNN 921.35 (9.46) 950.98 (11.97) 1.95 (.052) .19
RMSSD 70.23 (4.07) 71.26 (4.35) .173 (.863) .02
LF (n.u.) .38 (.01) .34 (.01) −2.53 (.012) −.25
HF (n.u.) .39 (.01) .42 (.01) 2.33 (.020) .23
LF/HF 1.33 (.08) 1.11 (.08) −1.97 (.050) −.19
(c) Body characteristics
Age (yr) 24.62 (.21) 24.47 (.17) 0.55 (.581) −.05
Height (m) 1.76 (.65) 1.75 (.68) 1.02 (.310) −.09
Weight (kg) 69.73 (.85) 68.58 (.81) .98 (.326) −.09
BMI 22.52 (.20) 22.40 (.18) .43 (.670) −.04

Inline graphic and Inline graphicvalues indicate results of independent samples Inline graphic tests, significant test results are indicated by bold font. The outermost right column provides effect sizes as indicated by Cohen's Inline graphic (Hedges' bias correction, values >.2 indicate moderate effect sizes). Abbrevations: BMI: body mass index (body weight/body heightInline graphic); HF (n.u.): high frequency power (0.15–0.4 Hz) of the HRV in normalized units; HRV: heart rate variability; LF (n.u.): low frequency power (0.04–0.15 Hz) of the HRV in normalized units; RMSSD: root mean square of the SD of successive heart beat intervals.

Body height, body weight, and BMI was virtually identical in both groups (see Table 1c for statistics). Likewise, QRS axis orientation was similar in both groups: Median of QRS axis orientation was 67.01Inline graphic in the group with higher, and 64.43Inline graphic in the group with lower Inline graphic values. A Mann-Whitney Inline graphictest did not indicate a significant difference of QRS axis orientation between the group with higher and lower Inline graphic values (Inline graphic; a non-parametric test was used because values of QRS axis orientation were not normally distributed according to a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, Inline graphic).

We also compared the scores of each NEO item between groups to further characterize personality differences between individuals with low and high Inline graphic values. Six items were identified (see Table S4) for which the scores differed between groups with effect sizes of Cohen's Inline graphic.2 (Hedges' bias correction; when comparing the scores of these items between groups using two-samples Inline graphictests, Inline graphicvalues were Inline graphic in each of the tests). These items were characterized by words such as ‘light-hearted’, ‘cheerful optimism’, ‘tense and jittery’, happiness', ‘lonely or blue’, and ‘high-spirited’.

Regressions between NEO personality parameters and ECG amplitudes

Whereas the last section investigated relations between a specific ECG amplitude measure (the Inline graphic values) and personality parameters (as measured with the NEO inventory), the following section investigates whether ECG amplitude values can be used to predict NEO personality scores. To validate our computational method, we first computed stepwise regressions with body height, body weight, as well as body mass index (BMI) as dependent variables, and ECG amplitude values as independent variables (Bonferroni-corrected significance level was Inline graphic, as for all regressions reported in the following section, see Methods). These computations indicated significant correlations for height (Inline graphic, Figure 3a), weight (Inline graphic), and BMI (Inline graphic; see Table S1 for regression models). Cross-validated regression coefficients (see Methods for cross-validation) were Inline graphic for body height, Inline graphic for body weight, and Inline graphic for BMI (all Inline graphic). Thus, as expected, ECG amplitude values well predicted body height, weight, and BMI.

Figure 3. Stepswise regressions.

Figure 3

(a) Result of the stepwise regression with body height as dependent variable, and ECG amplitude values as independent (predictor) variables. (b) shows the result of the stepwise regression (corrected for body height) with agreeableness as dependent variable, and ECG amplitude values, as well as body height, body weight, and body mass index as independent variables. The analogous regression for positive emotion is shown in (c).

Subsequently, analogous stepwise regressions were performed with the standardized scores of the NEO scales as dependent variables, and ECG amplitudes (as well as height, weight, and BMI) as independent variables. The regression for agreeableness showed both a significant regression model (Inline graphic), as well as a cross-validated regression coefficient of Inline graphic (Inline graphic, see Methods for cross-validation). This regression model indicated body height as one out of six predictor variables (in addition to five ECG waves, see Table S2a), due to a significant (and unexpected) correlation between agreeableness and body height in our sample (linear regression: Inline graphic). To remove this contribution of body height to the explanation of agreeableness with ECG waves, the residuals of the linear regression of agreeableness and body height were used as dependent variable in a stepwise regression, with the ECG amplitude values (as well as height, weight, and BMI) as independent variables. This analysis indicated a significant correlation between ECG amplitudes and agreeableness (Inline graphic, cross-validated correlation coefficient Inline graphic), with the contribution of body height removed (Figure 3b, see Table S2b for regression model). Stepwise regressions with neuroticism or extraversion as dependent variables did not yield significiant cross-validated correlations.

Likewise, stepwise regressions were also performed with the standardized scores of the NEO facets (positive emotion, warmth, and tendermindedness) as dependent variables, and ECG amplitudes (as well as height, weight, and BMI) as independent variables. Here, the regression for positive emotion showed a significant regression model (Inline graphic, Figure 3c, see Table S2c for regression model), as well as a cross-validated regression coefficient of Inline graphic (Inline graphic).

The analogous stepwise regressions performed with the standardized scores of the NEO scales and facets as dependent variables, and the HRV parameters as independent variables (instead of ECG amplitudes) indicated a marginally significant positive correlation for positive emotion and high frequency power of the HRV (Inline graphic, cross-validated regression coefficient: Inline graphic). The analogous regressions with tendermindedness or warmth as dependent variables did not yield significiant cross-validated correlations.

To substantiate the relations between ECG amplitudes and agreeableness as well as positive emotion, we also performed discriminant function analyses (DFAs): Participants were split into quartiles based on the standardized NEO scores, separately for agreeableness or positive emotion (balanced for gender). Then, separately for agreeableness and positive emotion, DFAs were computed with the two groups (upper and lower quartile of agreeableness, or of positive emotion scores, respectively) as dependent variable, and the absolute ECG amplitude values, as well as body height, weight, and BMI, as predictor variables (see also Methods). The DFAs indicated significant classifications, for both agreeableness (Inline graphic, 66% cross-validated cases) and positive emotion (Inline graphic, 60% cross-validated cases; see Table S3 for standardized canonical discrimination coefficients).

Discussion

Our results reveal that ECG amplitude patterns correlate with the personality of an individual (as measured with a NEO questionnaire): Firstly, individuals with lower Inline graphic values showed higher neuroticism, and lower positive emotion, as well as a tendency towards lower extraversion (compared to individuals with higher Inline graphic values). Note that the six items that showed the largest differences between groups (in terms of effect size) were characterized by words such as ‘light-hearted’, ‘cheerful’ and ‘happiness’. This supports the notion that that individuals with low Inline graphic values have fewer experiences of positive feelings (such as cheerfulness and happiness), corroborating that low Inline graphic values correlate with flattened affectivity [6]. Secondly, cross-validated stepwise regressions, and cross-validated discriminant function analyses (DFAs) indicated that ECG amplitude values can be used to predict agreeableness and positive emotion.

With regard to the heart rate variability (HRV) measures, individuals with lower Inline graphic values showed lower high frequency (HF) power, higher low frequency (LF) power, a higher LF/HF ratio, and nominally (though statistically not significant) lower RMSSD. This pattern of HRV parameters replicates our previous observations [6] (see also Introduction), and resembles the HRV pattern previously reported in studies investigating patients with major depression [15] or pathologic anxiety [41], [42], that is, in individuals with disorders that are characterized by higher neuroticism, and frequent phases of absence of positive emotions. For example, a recent meta analysis [15] reported that depressed patients exhibited reduced HRV, reduced HF spectral power of the HRV, and an increased LF/HF ratio.

LF and HF components of the HRV provide information about the autonomic tone of an individual (i.e. about the strength of sympathetic and parasympathetic activation): The LF component is taken to reflect sympathetic or perhaps both sympathetic and parasympathetic effects, and the HF component is taken to reflect mainly parasympathetic effects [32], [15]. The LF/HF ratio is taken to reflect autonomic balance (i.e., the balance or predominance of the sympathetic/parasympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system) [32]. Thus, the present results support previous observations that autonomic tone and autonomic balance differ between individuals with high and low Inline graphic values [6]. Notably, the autonomic balance of an individual has effects on regional heart activity: Autonomic imbalance is associated with lateralization of central autonomic drive [21], and such lateralized autonomic drive leads to a modulation of signal transduction mechanisms on the anterior and posterior heart surface, as well as to a modulation of atrioventricular (AV) and sinoatrial (SA) node conductivity: As mentioned in the Introduction, the right cardiac nerve innervates the anterior surface of the heart (including the SA node, the AV node, and the anterior surfaces of the right and left ventricles), whereas the left cardiac nerve innervates the posterolateral surface of the heart (including the AV node and the posterior surfaces of the right and left ventricles [42], [24], [25]. Thus, it is conceivable that the autonomic imbalance of individuals with low Inline graphic values results in a left-right imbalance in autonomic drive across the surface of the heart, which modulates the electrophysiological homogeneity of ventricular de- and repolarization [43].

Previous research also suggested that negative feelings, such as discontent, anger and competitiveness, are associated with autonomic functions involved in signal transduction mechanisms that might lead to left ventricular hypertrophy [44], and it is possible that such a morphological factor accounts partly for the ECG pattern described in the present study. However, we [6] have previously reported that serum concentrations of N-terminal-pro brain natriuretic peptide (NT-pro BNP) [45] do not correlate with Inline graphic values. Because NT-pro BNP is associated with left ventricular hypertrophy, and correlates with left ventricular mass [46], this suggests that gross morphological differences between the hearts of individuals with higher and lower Inline graphic values are unlikely in our non-clinical population, at least with respect to left ventricular mass. This assumption is corroborated by the observation that, in the present study, neither QRS axis orientation, nor body height, body weight, or BMI differed between individuals with higher and lower Inline graphic values. This strongly suggests that the Inline graphic values are not simply determined by the anatomy of an individual, but rather reflect aspects of regional cardiac activity. In this regard, it is also important to note that the Inline graphic values do not simply reflect that ECG amplitudes are generally “larger” or “smaller”, but that the Inline graphic values are the result of a computation of ECG amplitude ratios (see Figure 1): For example, a larger amplitude of the R-wave in lead III results in a higher Inline graphic value, whereas a larger amplitude of the RS-wave in aVL results in a lower Inline graphic value.

Similarly, with regard to the prediction of agreeableness and positive emotion (computed based on all measured ECG amplitude values), the final regression models did not include body weight, body height, nor body-mass index as predictor variables (see Tables S2b, c). That is, the prediction of agreeableness and positive emotion based on ECG amplitude values was not simply determined by body weight, body height, or body-mass index.

As mentioned in the Introduction, regional cardiac activity is modulated by the endocrine and neurological communication between the central nervous system and the cardiac nerve plexus. Neural mechanisms underlying modulations of regional cardiac activity include personality-characteristic autonomic innervation (including forebrain asymmetries in autonomic innervation) [21], as well as personality-characteristic endocrine activity [12]. Note that the neurological axis modulating heart activity reaches up to the forebrain, including the amygdala, the insular cortex and obitofrontal cortex [20]. These structures have been implicated in emotion, empathy, and personality, and both functional and structural differences in these structures have been shown as a function of an individual's personality [2], [4], [1], [3], [6], [47], [48]. Such personality-characteristic modulations of the neurological axis between the brain and the heart are consistent with HRV studies showing relations between HRV and depression [31], [14], [15]. Our data suggest that such personality-characteristic dispositions modulate the input into the cardiac nerve plexus, with effects on regional cardiac activity and thus on ECG amplitudes. For example, it has been shown that lateralized autonomic drive leads to alterations of signal transduction mechanisms on the anterior and posterior heart surface [43] that result in electrophysiological inhomogeneities of ventricular (re)polarization [43]. Such characteristics of regional cardiac activity are likely to be reflected in amplitude patterns of high-resolution ECGs. The exact mechanisms that lead to personality-characteristic modulations of regional heart activity, however, remain to be specified.

The present results are important for two reasons: Firstly, due to the above-mentioned relations between personality and cardiovascular diseases [49], [50], [51], [52], further research is likely to identify cardiac signatures characteristic for individuals at risk for such diseases. For example, using the methodological and statistical approach of the present study, future studies could compare ECG amplitude values between healthy individuals with Type A or Type D personality, and normal controls, to identify pre-clinical ECG amplitude signatures of individuals at risk for coronary heart disease or chronic heart failure. It is even conceivable that cardiac signatures can be identified that are characteristic for other chronic somatic diseases involving an affective personality component. For example, we are currently investigating ECG amplitude signatures of patients with autoimmune diseases such as Morbus Crohn or type 1 diabetes, with the aim to identify ECG signatures that can potentially be used for pre-clinical diagnosis of such diseases. A similar approach could be taken with regard to depression, particulary due to the finding that individuals with lower Inline graphic values showed increased neurotisicm, and reduced positive emotion.

Secondly, research on relations between ECG amplitude patterns and personality might provide cardiac indices that are potentially suited to assess personality. So far, empirical research on personality has relied predominantly on personality questionnaires. Such paper-pencil measures of personality have the advantage that they are capable of obtaining attitudes and behaviours of an individual. However, paper-pencil tests face the problem of several sources of potential subjective bias such as socially desirable responding, inaccuracies in self-perception, self-favouring tendencies, self-deception, and moralistic bias [33], [34], [35], [36], [37]. Notably, long-term emotional dispositions also have long-term peripheral-physiological effects [17], [2], [14], [8], [16], [11], [15], [12], [13]. Because such effects are more directly related to an individual's personality, identifying such effects can provide physiological indices of personality that are objective in the sense that they can hardly be influenced voluntarily by an individual (although they may be influenced by factors unrelated to personality).

Conclusions

Our results show that the cardiac amplitude signature captured by the Inline graphic values is related to neuroticism and positive emotion, thus corroborating the notion that low Inline graphic values reflect flattened affectivity. In addition, our results also reveal that agreeableness (as measured with the NEO-FFI) can be predicted to a moderate degree by ECG amplitude patterns. The findings give rise to future research investigating relations between ECG amplitude patterns and other personality measures (such as other NEO-PI-R scales and facets, or scales of other personality inventories) [53]. Our results also open perspectives for a more efficient assessment of personality using cardiac measures, with the advantage that such biological measures are less prone to several sources of subjective bias (although they can potentially be biased by other factors, this remains to be specified). The present results also give rise to identify ECG signatures characteristic for affective disorders and personality disorders characterized by increased neuroticism, and reduced positive emotion (such as depression, or schizoid personality disorder). Similarly, due to the relations between personality on the one side, and endocrine, autonomic, as well as immune system activity on the other [14], [16], [15], [12], [11], [10], [13], [8], [9], the present results also open perspectives for investigations on cardiac amplitude signatures characteristic for individuals at risk for cardiovascular disease, as well as for cardiac signatures characteristic for chronic somatic diseases involving a personality-specific affective component [14], [17]. Identification of such ECG signatures could lead to more efficient risk-stratification, risk-prevention models, and pre-clinical diagnostics.

Supporting Information

Table S1

Models resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods) as independent (predictor) variables, and body height (a), body weight (b), and body mass index (c) as dependent variables. Inline graphic: P-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: R-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: RS-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: T-wave amplitude. Subscript indicates the ECG lead.

(PDF)

Table S2

Regression models. (a) Model resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods), as well as body height, weight, and BMI, as independent (predictor) variables, and agreeableness (standard scores) as dependent variable. Note that this regression model included body height as a predictor variable (due to a significant correlation between body height and agreeableness in our sample, see main text). To remove this contribution of body height to the explanation of agreeableness with ECG waves, the residuals of a linear regression with agreeableness as dependent variable, and body height as independent variable were used for a stepwise regression with the ECG amplitude values (as well as height, weight, and BMI) as independent variables. (b) shows the model of resulting from this regression (in which the explanation of agreeableness by means of ECG amplitude values is corrected for body height). (c) Model resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods), as well as body height, weight, and BMI, as independent (predictor) variables, and positive emotion (T-scores) as dependent variable. Abbrevations of ECG waves as in Table S1.

(PDF)

Table S3

Standardized canonical discrimination coefficients. The dependent variables of the discriminant analyses were upper and lower quartiles of standard agreeableness scores (a), or upper and lower quartiles of standard positive emotion scores (b), predictor variables were the absolute ECG amplitude values, as well as body height, weight, and BMI. Abbrevations of ECG waves as in Table S1.

(PDF)

Table S4

Items for which the scores between the two Inline graphic groups ( Inline graphic values above and below the median of Inline graphic values) differed with effect sizes of Cohen's Inline graphic .2. A plus sign indicates that scores were higher (“agree”) for individuals with lower Inline graphic values, the minus sign indicates that scores were lower (“disagree”) for individuals with lower Inline graphic values. For example, individuals with lower Inline graphic values had higher scores agreeing to the statement of not being a cheerful optimist. Cohen's Inline graphic was computed using Hedges' bias correction. When comparing the scores of these items between groups using two-samples Inline graphictests, Inline graphicvalues were Inline graphic in each of the tests.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Ronny Enk for his help in data acquisition.

Footnotes

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Funding: This work was supported by the Freie University Berlin and by the Max Planck Society and by a grant from the German Ministry of Education and Research (Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung, BMBF, grant-nr. 01EZ1024A, awareded to SK). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Bird G, Silani G, Brindley R, White S, Frith U, et al. Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain. 2010;133:1515. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Canli T. Biology of personality and individual differences. The Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Damasio A. A neural basis for sociopathy. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:128. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Deckersbach T, Miller K, Klibanski A, Fischman A, Dougherty D, et al. Regional cerebral brain metabolism correlates of neuroticism and extraversion. Depression and anxiety. 2006;23:133–138. doi: 10.1002/da.20152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Depue R, Collins P. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22:491–517. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Koelsch S, Remppis A, Sammler D, Jentschke S, Mietchen D, et al. A cardiac signature of emotionality. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;26:3328–3338. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05889.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Swerdlow N, Koob G. Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania, and depression: toward a unified hypothesis of cortico-striatopallido-thalamic function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1987;10:197–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cloninger C. Biology of personality dimensions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2000;13:611. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Davidson R. Toward a biology of personality and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;935:191–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Eysenck H. The biological basis of personality. Transaction Pub. 1967 doi: 10.1038/1991031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Irwin M. Human psychoneuroimmunology: 20 years of discovery. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2008;22:129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wolkowitz O, Rothschild A. Psychoneuroendocrinology: the scientific basis of clinical practice. American Psychiatric Pub 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zuckerman M. Psychobiology of personality. Cambridge Univ Pr 1991 [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Carney R, Freedland K, Veith R. Depression, the autonomic nervous system, and coronary heart disease. Psychosomatic medicine. 2005;67:S29. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000162254.61556.d5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kemp A, Quintana D, Gray M, Felmingham K, Brown K, et al. Impact of Depression and Antidepressant Treatment on Heart Rate Variability: A Review and Meta-Analysis. Biological psychiatry. 2010;67:1067–1074. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dantzer R, O'Connor J, Freund G, Johnson R, Kelley K. From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:46–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bruni R, Serino F, Galluzzo S, Coppolino G, Cacciapaglia F, et al. Alexithymia and Neuroendocrine-Immune Response in Patients with Autoimmune Diseases. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1069:208–211. doi: 10.1196/annals.1351.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Horackova M. Colocalization of Multiple Neurochemicals in Mammalian Intrathoracic Neurons. In: Armour J, Ardell J, editors. Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 61–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Adams D, Cuevas J. Electrophysiological properties of intrinsic cardiac neurons. In: Armour J, Ardell J, editors. Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 1–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chechetto D. Forebrain control of healthy and diseased hearts. In: Armour J, Ardell J, editors. Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 220–251. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Craig A. Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:566–571. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sander D, Klingelhöfer J. Changes of circadian blood pressure patterns and cardiovascular parameters indicate lateralization of sympathetic activation following hemispheric brain infarction. Journal of neurology. 1995;242:313–318. doi: 10.1007/BF00878874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zamrini E, Meador K, Loring D, Nichols F, Lee G, et al. Unilateral cerebral inactivation produces differential left/right heart rate responses. Neurology. 1990;40:1408–1408. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.9.1408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lane R, Schwartz G. Induction of lateralized sympathetic input to the heart by the cns during emotional arousal: A possible neurophysiologic trigger of sudden cardiac death. Psychosomatic medicine. 1987;49:274–284. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198705000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cardinal R, Pagé P. Neuronalmodulation of atrial and ventricular electrical properties. In: Armour J, Ardell J, editors. Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 315–339. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gagnon C, Ramachandruni S, Bragdon E, Sheps D. Psychological aspects of heart disease. In: Armour J, Ardell J, editors. Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 393–418. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Andreasen N. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia: definition and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1982;39:784. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290070020005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cooper J. Pocket guide to the ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: with glossary and diagnostic criteria for research. 1994. WB Saunders Company.
  • 29.Costa P, McCrea R. Revised neo personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and neo fivefactor inventory (NEO-FFI). 1992. Psychological Assessment Resources Odessa, FL.
  • 30.Riese H, Rosmalen J, Ormel J, Van Roon A, Oldehinkel A, et al. The genetic relationship between neuroticism and autonomic function in female twins. Psychological medicine. 2007;37:257–267. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Agelink M, Boz C, Ullrich H, Andrich J. Relationship between major depression and heart rate variability: Clinical consequences and implications for antidepressive treatment. Psychiatry Research. 2002;113:139–149. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00225-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Malik M, Bigger J, Camm A, Kleiger R, Malliani A, et al. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation. 1996;93:1043. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McCrae R, Costa P. Social desirability scales: More substance than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1983;51:882–888. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Paulhus D. Self-deception and impression management in test responses. Personality assessment via questionnaire. 1986:143–165. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Barrick MR, Mount MK. Effects of impression management and self-deception on the predictive validity of personality constructs. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1996;81:261–272. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.81.3.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Paulhus D, John O. Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of personality. 1998;66:1025–1060. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pauls C, Stemmler G. Substance and bias in social desirability responding. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;35:263–275. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Borkenau P, Ostendorf F. NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar:(NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae. 1993. Hogrefe Göttingen.
  • 39.Ostendorf F, Angleitner A. NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar (NEO-PI-R) nach Costa und McCrae. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schlögl A, Brunner C, Scherer R, Glatz A. Towards Brain-Computer Interfacing. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press; 2007. BioSig – an open source software library for BCI research. pp. 347–358. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Friedman B, Thayer J. Autonomic balance revisited: panic anxiety and heart rate variability. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1998;44:133–151. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00202-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Yeragani V, Pohl R, Jampala V, Balon R, Ramesh C, et al. Increased qt variability in patients with panic disorder and depression. Psychiatry research. 2000;93:225–235. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00119-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Critchley H, Taggart P, Sutton P, Holdright D, Batchvarov V, et al. Mental stress and sudden cardiac death: asymmetric midbrain activity as a linking mechanism. Brain. 2005;128:75. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Agewall S, Wikstrand J, Dahlöf C, Fagerberg B. Negative feelings (discontent) predict progress of intima-media thickness of the common carotid artery in treated hypertensive men at high cardiovascular risk. American journal of hypertension. 1996;9:545–550. doi: 10.1016/0895-7061(95)00355-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hall C. Essential biochemistry and physiology of (nt-pro) bnp. European Journal of Heart Failure. 2004;6:257. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2003.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Luchner A, Hengstenberg C, Löwel H, Trawinski J, Baumann M, et al. N-terminal pro-brain natriuretic peptide after myocardial infarction. Hypertension. 2002;39:99–104. doi: 10.1161/hy0102.100537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sterzer P, Stadler C, Poustka F, Kleinschmidt A. A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy. Neuroimage. 2007;37:335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Berlin H, Rolls E, Iversen S. Borderline personality disorder, impulsivity, and the orbitofrontal cortex. American journal of psychiatry. 2005;162:2360. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schiffer A, Pedersen S, Widdershoven J, Denollet J. Type d personality and depressive symptoms are independent predictors of impaired health status in chronic heart failure. European journal of heart failure. 2008;10:922. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2008.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Mols F, Martens E, Denollet J. Type d personality and depressive symptoms are independent predictors of impaired health status following acute myocardial infarction. Heart. 2010;96:30. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2009.170357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Denollet J, Schiffer A, Spek V. A general propensity to psychological distress affects cardiovascular outcomes. Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes. 2010;3:546. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.109.934406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chida Y, Steptoe A. The association of anger and hostility with future coronary heart disease:: A meta-analytic review of prospective evidence. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2009;53:936–946. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2008.11.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Finholt T. Collaboratories as a new form of scientific organization. Economics of Innovation and New Technology. 2003;12:5–25. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Table S1

Models resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods) as independent (predictor) variables, and body height (a), body weight (b), and body mass index (c) as dependent variables. Inline graphic: P-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: R-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: RS-wave amplitude, Inline graphic: T-wave amplitude. Subscript indicates the ECG lead.

(PDF)

Table S2

Regression models. (a) Model resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods), as well as body height, weight, and BMI, as independent (predictor) variables, and agreeableness (standard scores) as dependent variable. Note that this regression model included body height as a predictor variable (due to a significant correlation between body height and agreeableness in our sample, see main text). To remove this contribution of body height to the explanation of agreeableness with ECG waves, the residuals of a linear regression with agreeableness as dependent variable, and body height as independent variable were used for a stepwise regression with the ECG amplitude values (as well as height, weight, and BMI) as independent variables. (b) shows the model of resulting from this regression (in which the explanation of agreeableness by means of ECG amplitude values is corrected for body height). (c) Model resulting from stepwise linear regressions with absolute values of all ECG amplitude waves (see Methods), as well as body height, weight, and BMI, as independent (predictor) variables, and positive emotion (T-scores) as dependent variable. Abbrevations of ECG waves as in Table S1.

(PDF)

Table S3

Standardized canonical discrimination coefficients. The dependent variables of the discriminant analyses were upper and lower quartiles of standard agreeableness scores (a), or upper and lower quartiles of standard positive emotion scores (b), predictor variables were the absolute ECG amplitude values, as well as body height, weight, and BMI. Abbrevations of ECG waves as in Table S1.

(PDF)

Table S4

Items for which the scores between the two Inline graphic groups ( Inline graphic values above and below the median of Inline graphic values) differed with effect sizes of Cohen's Inline graphic .2. A plus sign indicates that scores were higher (“agree”) for individuals with lower Inline graphic values, the minus sign indicates that scores were lower (“disagree”) for individuals with lower Inline graphic values. For example, individuals with lower Inline graphic values had higher scores agreeing to the statement of not being a cheerful optimist. Cohen's Inline graphic was computed using Hedges' bias correction. When comparing the scores of these items between groups using two-samples Inline graphictests, Inline graphicvalues were Inline graphic in each of the tests.

(PDF)


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES