Abstract
The glutamatergic system has been implicated in the pathophysiology of depression and the mechanism of action of antidepressants. Leptin, an adipocyte-derived hormone, has antidepressant-like properties. However, the functional role of leptin receptor (Lepr) signaling in glutamatergic neurons remains to be elucidated. In this study, we generated conditional knockout mice in which the long form of Lepr was ablated selectively in glutamatergic neurons located in the forebrain structures, including the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (Lepr cKO). Lepr cKO mice exhibit normal growth and body weight. Behavioral characterization of Lepr cKO mice reveals depression-like behavioral deficits, including anhedonia, behavioral despair, enhanced learned helplessness and social withdrawal, with no evident signs of anxiety. In addition, loss of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons facilitates N-methyl--aspartate (NMDA)-induced hippocampal long-term synaptic depression (LTD), whereas conventional LTD or long-term potentiation (LTP) was not affected. The facilitated LTD induction requires activation of the NMDA receptor GluN2B (NR2B) subunit as it was completely blocked by a selective GluN2B antagonist. Moreover, Lepr cKO mice are highly sensitive to the antidepressant-like behavioral effects of the GluN2B antagonist but resistant to leptin. These results support important roles for Lepr signaling in glutamatergic neurons in regulating depression-related behaviors and modulating excitatory synaptic strength, suggesting a possible association between synaptic depression and behavioral manifestation of behavioral depression.
Keywords: behavioral depression, GluN2B subunit, glutamatergic neurons, leptin receptor, NMDA receptors, synaptic depression
Introduction
Depression is a chronic and debilitating mental illness with a 17% lifetime prevalence and is a major cause of morbidity, disability and mortality.1, 2, 3 Research in this field has been dominated by the monoamine theory for over 50 years. Currently available pharmacologic treatments for depression primarily target monoamine systems. However, the monoamine-based antidepressants have significant limitations in efficacy, safety and onset of action. Recently, there is increasing preclinical and clinical evidence supporting a role for the glutamatergic system in depression and antidepressant activity.4, 5, 6, 7, 8 Among glutamate receptor subtypes, the N-methyl--aspartate (NMDA) receptor has received the most attention as drugs that modulate NMDA receptor functional activity have demonstrated antidepressant-like properties in animal models9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16 and in patients suffering from major depressive disorder.17, 18, 19, 20
Leptin, a hormone secreted from adipocytes, circulates in the blood and enters the brain where it exerts physiological actions beyond the control of food intake and metabolism. Indeed, systemic or intracranial administration of leptin produces antidepressant-like effects in both rats and mice.21, 22, 23 Circulating leptin levels are reduced in animals subjected to chronic social defeat or chronic unpredictable stress, two animal models of depression.22, 24 Although results from clinical investigations have been inconsistent, several studies have found an association between low levels of leptin and major depressive disorders.25, 26, 27 Of note, a recent study by Lawson et al.28 reported an inverse correlation between leptin levels and the severity of depression symptoms. These correlational studies suggest that leptin signaling may be involved in the pathophysiology of major depressive disorders. Leptin exerts its effects by activating the long form of the leptin receptor (Lepr), which is widely but selectively distributed in various brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus,29, 30, 31 two limbic brain areas implicated in depression.32, 33, 34 However, whether defects in leptin receptor function in limbic neural circuits contributes to the pathogenesis of depression and whether specific neuronal populations mediate leptin's antidepressant-like activity are not yet known.
In this study, we produced mice in which the long form of the leptin receptor was ablated specifically in glutamatergic neurons in the dorsal forebrain, including the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, using Cre-mediated recombination driven by the promoter of Emx1, a homeobox gene, principally expressed in forebrain glutamatergic neurons.35, 36, 37 We characterized the behavioral, neuroendocrine and electrophysiological phenotypes of these mice. Moreover, we examined the responses of these mice to the antidepressant-like behavioral effects of a GluN2B antagonist and leptin. Our results suggest that Lepr signaling in forebrain glutamatergic neurons is critical for regulating depression-like behaviors and induction of hippocampal long-term synaptic depression.
Materials and methods
Animals
Emx1 is expressed principally in glutamatergic neurons in the forebrain including cerebral cortex and hippocampal formation.35, 36, 37 To generate mice with ablation of the long form of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons, Leprflox/flox mice, in which loxP sites flank exon 17 that contains the Box 1 motif crucial for leptin signal transduction,38 were mated with Emx1-Cre mice, in which Cre recombinase was driven by the promoter of the Emx1 gene.39 The Leprflox/+, Emx1-Cre offsprings were then bred with Leprflox/flox mice for 4–5 generations. Genomic DNA was extracted and used as the template for PCR-based genotyping. For verification of the Leprflox/flox alleles, the PCR products were amplified using the primers 5′-ATGCTATCGACAAGCAGCAGAATGA-3′ and 5′-CAGGCTTGAGAACATGAACACAACAAC-3′. The presence of the LoxP sites was verified by digesting the PCR product with HindIII. The Emx1-Cre allele was verified using the primers 5′-CACTCATGGAAAATAGCGATC-3′ and 5′-ATCTCCGGTATTGAAACTCCAGCGC-3′. All mice were housed in groups of four or five on a 14 h light/10 h dark cycle (lights on at 0700 h) with ad libitum access to food and water. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio and carried out in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide.
Cre expression
The Emx1-Cre knock-in construct contains a lacZ gene coding for nuclear-localized β-galactosidase that is linked to an internal ribosomal entry site followed by a nuclear-localized Cre-coding region.40 This allows for coexpression of Cre and β-galactosidase. To confirm the expression pattern of Cre in the Leprflox/flox mouse brain, lacZ expression was used as readout for cell-specific Cre expression. Immunoreactivity of β-galactosidase was performed as described in detail below.
Reverse transcription-PCR
Cortex, hippocampus and hypothalamus of Leprflox/flox (fWT) and Leprflox/flox, Emx1-Cre/+ (Lepr conditional knockout (Lepr cKO)) mice were dissected and total RNA was extracted using Trizol reagent (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). SuperScript II reverse transcriptase (Invitrogen) was used to generate complementary DNA (cDNA) using the oligo(dT)25. The reaction mixture consisted of 4 μg of total RNA, 500 ng oligo(dT)25, 4 μl of 5 × First-Strand buffer, 10 m dithiothreitol, 40 units of RNaseOUT (Invitrogen) and 200 units of SuperScript II reverse transcriptase (Invitrogen). The resulting cDNA was used for PCR amplification of Lepr exon 17 or β-actin with Accuprime pfx Supermix (Invitrogen). The PCR mix consisted of 22 μl of the Accuprime pfx Supermix with 1 μl of each primer (10 μ stock) and 1 μl cDNA. The conditions for PCR were 94 °C for 5 min, followed by 35 cycles of 94 °C for 1 min, 60 °C for 1 min and 72 °C for 1 min, followed by a final incubation at 72 °C for 10 min. The primer sequences used to amplify each product are as follows: Lepr exon 17, forward: 5′-GGGACGATGTTCCAAACCCCA-3′ and reverse: 5′-AGGCTCCAGAAGAAGAGGACC-3′ β-actin, forward: 5′-AGCCATGTACGTAGCCATCC-3′ and reverse: 5′-TGTGGTGGTGAAGCTGTAGC-3′. The PCR products were analyzed on a 1% agarose gel with ethidium bromide.
Real-time reverse transcription-PCR
Primers specific for exon 17 of Lepr or β-actin were used to amplify a single PCR product from each cDNA sample. Real-time PCR was performed on a Realplex2 Mastercycler (Eppendorf, Westburg, NY, USA) and analyzed using Mastercycler EP Realplex, version 1.5. The amplification mixture contained 25 μl of the Power SYBRGreen PCR Master Mix (Applied Biosystems, Carlsbad, CA, USA), 1 μl of each primer (10 μ stock), 1 μl of cDNA template and 22 μl of PCR-grade water. The cycling conditions began with a hot start at 95 °C for 5 min followed by 40 cycles of 95, 60 and 72 °C for 1 min each. The reactions were performed in duplicate. Control reactions consisting of master mix and primers but excluding cDNA template were run in parallel. The Ct values for each duplicate were averaged and used for quantification. The relative amount of mRNA for Lepr exon 17 for each sample was normalized to β-actin mRNA using the following formula: 2(CTexon 17 – CTβ-actin).
Measurement of body weight and body composition
For Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate controls, body weight was recorded once per week from 4 to 12 weeks of age. Body composition in 14-week-old mice was measured by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (GE Medical Systems, Madison, WI, USA). The instrument was calibrated using the phantom mouse model accompanied with the machine, and quality control was performed before the detection of animals.
Behavioral procedures
All behavioral tests used adult mice (9–12 weeks old) and were performed during the late light phase except sucrose preference that was measured every 24 h. On the test day, animals were singly housed in a new cage with some home cage bedding to avoid the stressful effect of removal from their home cage. Animals were transferred to the testing room and habituated to the room conditions for 3 to 4 h before the beginning of the experiments. After each individual test session, the apparatus was thoroughly cleaned with 20% alcohol to eliminate the odor and trace of the previously tested animals. All behaviors were scored by experimenters who were blind to the treatments and genotypes.
Locomotor activity
On the testing day, the home cage lid was replaced by clear Plexiglas to enable recording of animal's activity using a CCD (charge-coupled device) camera mounted above the cage. Locomotor activity was monitored and recorded for 30 min. The total distance traveled in 30 min was measured in 2 min bins by using the Noldus EthoVision 3.0 system (Noldus Information Technology, Leesburg, VA, USA).
Rotarod test
Mice were trained for 2 days on the rotarod at fixed speeds of 5, 15 and 30 r.p.m. for 5 min each. On the third day, mice were tested on the rotarod at accelerating speeds from 4 to 40 r.p.m. over 5 min. The latency for each mouse to fall from the rod was recorded.
Behavioral despair tests
Mice were assessed in the tail suspension test and the forced swim test. The tail suspension apparatus was a wooden box (30 × 30 × 30 cm). The front of the box was open, and a horizontal bar was placed 1 cm from the top with an attached vertical bar hanging down in the center. Mice were individually suspended by the tail to the vertical bar with adhesive tape affixed 2 cm from the tip of the tail. A CCD camera was positioned in front of the tail suspension test box and the animal's behavior was recorded for 6 min. Immobility is defined as the absence of any limb or body movements, except those caused by respiration. For the forced swim test, mice were placed in a clear Plexiglas cylinder (25 cm high; 10 cm in diameter) filled with 24 °C water to a depth of 15 cm. Fresh water was used for each animal. A CCD camera positioned directly above the cylinder recorded the swim session. In a 6-min test session, the first 2 min were designated as a habituation period and the duration of immobility during last 4 min was measured using Noldus EthoVision 3.0 system (Noldus Information Technology).21
For evaluating the antidepressant-like behavioral effects of leptin (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) or Ro25-6981 (Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA), the drugs were dissolved in saline immediately before use and administered intraperitoneally to mice 30 min before the tail suspension test or the forced swim test. The testing procedures and scoring methods were the same as described above.
Sweet solution preference
Mice were habituated to drinking from two bottles of water for 1 week before testing. To measure the preference for sweet solutions, the animals were singly housed and tested with one bottle of water and one bottle of either 1% sucrose or 0.01% saccharin for 4 consecutive days. Water and sucrose or saccharin intake were measured daily and the position of two bottles was switched every day to avoid side preference. Sucrose or saccharin preference was calculated as the mass of sucrose or saccharin solution consumed over the total mass of fluid intake.
Learned helplessness test
This test was performed in a shuttle cage that was divided equally into two chambers with an auto-controlled guillotine door. Learned helplessness was induced in mice by administering 200 scrambled, inescapable foot shock (0.3 mA shock amplitude, 2-s duration, 16-s average interval) over a 1-h session. Control animals were exposed to the apparatus for the same period of time without receiving foot shocks. Escape performance was tested 24 h later in the same shuttle chamber. Each mouse was given 30 shuttle escape trials with 25 s maximum duration and 30-s intervals. On the first five trials, a sound cue and the shock took place at the same time as the door to the safe compartment opened. For the remaining trials, the door opened 2 s after the shock was delivered. Each trial was terminated when the mouse crossed into the non-shock compartment. Latency to escape and the number of escape failure were recorded automatically by the Graphic State software (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA).
Hot-plate test
Pain sensitivity was assessed by detecting the reflexes in the hot-plate test. The surface of the hot plate was heated to a constant temperature of 55 °C. Mice were placed on the hot plate with surrounded wall. The duration of the test session was a maximum of 30 s. The latency to respond with hindpaw lick, hindpaw flick or jump was recorded. The mouse was immediately removed from the hot plate and returned to its home cage. If the mouse did not respond, the test was terminated after 30 s.
Chronic social defeat
This was conducted using a resident–intruder paradigm as previously reported.41, 42, 43, 44 Briefly, each male fWT and Lepr cKO mouse was individually introduced to the home cage of an unfamiliar aggressive CD1 resident mouse for 10 min and physically defeated. After the defeat, the resident CD1 mouse and the intruder mouse were housed together but separated by a perforated plastic divider to allow visual, olfactory and auditory contact for the remainder of the 24-h period. Mice were exposed to a new resident CD1 mouse and subjected to social defeat each day for 10 consecutive days. Non-defeated control mice were housed two per cage in cages identical to those used for the socially defeated mice. At 24 h after the last social defeat session, mice were subjected to the social interaction/avoidance test, which was performed in a 40 × 40 cm arena with infrared light illumination. The approach–avoidance behavior of mice was recorded with a video tracking system. Each test consisted of two 2.5-min sessions. In the first 2.5-min ‘no target' session, the mouse was introduced to the open arena with an empty wire mesh cage (10 × 6.5 × 5 cm) as indicated in Figure 2g. For the second 2.5-min session with target, an unfamiliar CD1 male mouse was placed into the wire mesh cage. The wire mesh cage allowed visual and olfactory interactions between the test mouse and the target CD1 mouse and prevented direct physical contact. The time spent in the ‘interaction zone' (25 × 14 cm) located around where the wire mesh cage was located and in the ‘avoidance zone' (on the side opposite to the interaction zone, 40 × 10 cm) were measured using EthoVision 3.0 software. Social preference ratio was calculated as time in the interaction zone/(time in the interaction zone+time in the avoidance zone).
Elevated plus maze (EPM)
The EPM was made of black acrylic, with four arms (30 cm long and 5 cm wide) arranged in the shape of a ‘plus' sign and elevated to a height of 70 cm from the floor.45 Two arms have no side or end walls (open arms). The other two arms have side walls and end walls (12 cm high) but are open on top (closed arms). The open and closed arms intersect, at a central 5 × 5 cm square platform giving access to all arms. The mice were placed in the central square facing the corner between a closed arm and an open arm, and their exploration was videotaped. The numbers of entries and the time spent on each arm were measured. Entry was defined as all four paws being positioned within one arm. The degree of anxiety was assessed by calculating the percentage of open arm entries (entries into the open arms/total entries into all arms) and percentage of open arm time (time spent in the open arms/total time spent in all arms).
Open-field test
The apparatus was made of plastic coated wood and consisted of a 60 × 60 cm open arena with 40 cm high walls. The open field arena was divided into nine equal squares. The center square was defined as the central zone, in which the animal's activity is usually regarded as a measure of anxiety. The entire test arena was adjusted to even illumination. Mice were placed in the center of the arena, and their activity in the arena was recorded for 5 min using a CCD camera. The percent time mice spent in the central zone and the total distance traveled in the open arena were quantified using the Noldus EthoVision 3.0 system. The overall motor activity during the open field test was assessed as the total distance traveled.
Light/dark choice
This test is based on a natural conflict of a mouse between the innate aversion to brightly illuminated areas and the exploration of a novel environment. The apparatus consisted of two equally sized compartments (17.8 × 17.8 × 30.5 cm) divided by a wall with an opening at the floor level. The light compartment was brightly illuminated with light intensity of 700 lux; and the dark compartment was black walled. Mice were placed individually in the center of the light compartment facing away from the opening. The number of transitions between two compartments and time spent in the light compartment were recorded over 5 min.
Visual cliff test
This test measures the ability to avoid a visual cliff.45 The apparatus is used to evaluate the ability of the animal to see the drop-off at the edge of a horizontal surface. A box with the inner surface covered with checkerboard paper was connected to a vertical drop of 0.7 m. A sheet of clear Plexiglas provided a solid horizontal surface in spite of the visual appearance of a cliff. Each mouse was subjected to 10 consecutive trials. Normal mice stop at the ‘edge'. The visually impaired animals cannot detect the ‘cliff' and will move forward across the ‘cliff' without pause.
Mice that were naïve to behavioral testing were used for assessing locomotor activity, tail suspension, forced swim, sweet solution preference, learned helplessness, chronic social defeat and EPM. The rotarod and visual cliff tests were performed on the mice used for the locomotion test; the hot-plate test was performed on the mice following the learned helplessness test; the open-field test and light/dark choice were performed using the same cohort of mice.
Plasma corticosterone, insulin and leptin level analysis
Plasma corticosterone was assayed using the radioimmunoassay method. Briefly, 10-μl duplicate samples of plasma were heated at 70 °C for 30 min to denature corticosterone-binding protein, and incubated overnight with corticosterone antibody (Chemicon International, Temecula, CA, USA) and [3H]corticosterone (PerkinElmer, Boston, MA, USA). Free and bound corticosterone were separated by incubation with charcoal for 15 min. The levels of plasma insulin and leptin were measured using mouse insulin and leptin ELISA kits, respectively (Alpco Diagnostics, Salem, NH, USA). Plasma was diluted 1:5 in a dilution buffer and immobilized in 96-well plates coated with specific antibodies conjugated to horseradish peroxidase and quantified by a chromogenic substrate.
Immunohistochemistry
Immunohistochemistry was performed as described previously.46 Mouse brains were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde and cut into 40-μm coronal sections. For the detection of β-galactosidase in Leprflox/flox, Emx1-Cre/+ mice, the brain sections were first incubated with rabbit anti-β-galactosidase (1:100, Abcam, Cambridge, MA, USA), followed by goat anti-rabbit conjugated to Alexa-Fluor 488 (1:400, Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR, USA).
In situ hybridization
Antisense cRNA probes directed against the sequence of the mouse leptin receptor C-terminus glucocorticoid receptor and corticotropin-releasing hormone mRNA were labeled with S35-UTP (uridine triphosphate) and S35-CTP (cytidine triphosphate) (PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA, USA) using the standard transcription system as described in our previous studies.47, 48 Briefly, coronal brain sections were cut and mounted onto polylysine-coated slides. The brain sections were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 1 h, rinsed in 2 × SSC (300 m NaCl, 30 m Na citrate, pH 7.2) three times (5 min each). The slides were then acetylated in 0.1 triethanolamine (pH 8.0) with 0.25% acetic anhydride for 10 min and dehydrated through a graded series of alcohol (50–100%). Hybridization was conducted by the addition of 70 μl of the diluted radiolabeled probe to the slide and incubation at 55 °C overnight. The slides were rinsed in 2 × SSC and incubated in RNase A buffer (200 mg ml–1) for 1 h at 37 °C, followed by a series of washes of increasing stringency (2 × , 1 × , 0.5 × SSC). Finally, the slides were placed in 0.1 × SSC at 70 °C for 1 h, rinsed in distilled water, dehydrated in a graded series of alcohol and were exposed to X-ray film.
Electrophysiology
Coronal hippocampal slices (400 μm) were prepared from 8- to 12-week-old male mice. Briefly, hippocampal slices were cut using a vibrating microtome (NVSLM1, WPI, Sarasota, FL, USA) in ice-cold slicing buffer (in m: 127 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 KH2PO4, 1.9 KCl, 1.1 CaCl2, 2 MgSO4 and 10 -Glucose) bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. Slices were then transferred to a holding chamber containing oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF; in m: 127 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 KH2PO4, 1.9 KCl, 2.2 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4 and 10 -Glucose) for 30 min at 34 °C and for another 30 min at 22 °C for recovery, and then transferred to a submersion recording chamber while continually perfused with 32 °C oxygenated ACSF (rate: 2 ml min–1). Slices were equilibrated for at least 15 min before each recording.
ACSF-filled glass electrodes (resistance <1 MΩ) were positioned in the stratum radiatum of area CA1 for extracellular recording. Synaptic responses were evoked by stimulating Schaffer collaterals with 0.2 ms pulses with a bipolar tungsten electrode (WPI) once every 15 s. The stimulation intensity was systematically increased to determine the maximal field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) slope and then adjusted to yield 40–60% of the maximal (fEPSP) slope. Experiments with maximal fEPSPs of <0.5 mV or with substantial changes in the fiber volley were rejected. After recording of a stable baseline for 15 min, long-term potentiation (LTP) was induced by one 1 s per 100 Hz stimulus train, and long-term depression (LTD) was induced by 900 pulses delivered with a frequency of 1 Hz in the absence or presence of L-trans-pyrrolidine-2,4-dicarboxylic acid (PDC; Sigma), a glutamate transporter inhibitor, and ifenprodil (Sigma), a GluN2B selective antagonist.
Field EPSPs were recorded (AxoClamp 2B amplifier, Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA, USA), filtered at 1 kHz, digitized at 10 kHz (Axon Digidata 1322A) and stored for off-line analysis (Clampfit 9). Whole-cell recordings from CA1 pyramidal neurons were made using an Axopatch 200B amplifier (Axon Instruments), sampled at 10 kHz, digitized by a DigiData 1322A and later analyzed off-line by ClampFit (Axon software). Whole-cell patch pipettes with resistances ranging between 3 and 7 MΩ were pulled using standard borosilicate capillaries by an upright PB-7 electrode puller (Narishige, East Meadow, NY, USA) and were filled with a Cs-gluconate-based patch solution (in m: 125 Cs-gluconate, 20 CsCl, 10 NaCl, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 2.5 QX314, 10 PIPES, 0.2 EGTA, pH 7.3 adjusted with CsOH). Synaptic currents were evoked by monopolar stimulation at a rate of 0.05 Hz via a silver wire housed in an ACSF-filled glass electrode (1–2 MΩ) placed in the Schaeffer collaterals. Magnitude and rate of extracellular stimulation were controlled with a Master-8 stimulator (AMPI, Jerusalem, Israel).
To determine the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptor (AMPAR)/NMDA receptor (NMDAR) current ratio, whole-cell recordings were performed at a holding potential of +40 mV in the presence of a γ-Aminobutyric acid A receptor antagonist, picrotoxin (50 μ, Sigma). Baseline responses were recorded in the absence of APV (2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid) to determine the combined AMPAR- and NMDAR-evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (eEPSCs, at least 10 traces). Subsequently, evoked responses were recorded in the presence of 100 μ APV to determine AMPAR eEPSCs (at least 10 traces). The average NMDAR eEPSC amplitude was calculated by subtracting the averaged AMPAR eEPSCs from the averaged combined AMPAR/NMDAR eEPSCs.
Statistical analysis
Results are expressed as mean±s.e.m. Statistical analyses were performed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures on the effect of genotype on body weight gain and locomotor activity; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on escape latencies with genotype and foot shock as main factors and two-way ANOVA without repeated measures on number of failures to escape in the learned helplessness test; two-way ANOVA on corticosterone levels with genotype and restraint stress as main factors and immobility time in the tail suspension and forced swim tests with genotype and drug treatment as main factors; three-way ANOVA on time spent in the social interaction and avoidance zones and social preference ratio in the chronic social defeat paradigm with genotype, defeat and target as main factors. Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc comparisons followed ANOVAs. Two-tailed Student's t-test was used for statistical analysis of the rest of experimental results. P<0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Generation of mice with ablation of the long form of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons
The ablation of the long form of Lepr was induced by crossing Leprflox/flox with mice bearing the Emx1-Cre transgene,39 which is expressed principally in glutamatergic neurons of the hippocampal formation and dorsal cerebral cortex.35, 36, 37 Immunohistochemical detection of β-galactosidase, a readout of cell-specific Emx1-Cre expression,39, 40 confirmed that Cre expression was restricted to neurons located in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex in Leprflox/flox, Emx1-Cre/+ (Lepr cKO) (Figure 1a). Reverse transcription-PCR analysis showed that exon 17 of Lepr mRNA was almost abolished in the hippocampus and cortex, but unaltered in the hypothalamus, indicating that Cre-mediated deletion of Lepr in Lepr cKO mice was specific to the hippocampus and cerebral cortex (Figure 1b). Real-time PCR results confirmed the deletion of >90% of Lepr in the hippocampus and cortex in Lepr cKO mice (Figure 1c).
General physical and hormonal assessments of Lepr cKO mice
Body weight of Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate controls were monitored from 4 to 12 weeks of age. Mice exhibited similar body weight gain and showed no genotype difference (F(1, 288)=0.063, P>0.5 for males; F(1, 280)=1.100, P>0.1 for females; Figure 1d). Body composition was analyzed using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry at 14 weeks of age. Fat mass and lean mass were similar between the two genotypes (males: fat mass t(14)=0.056, P>0.5; lean mass t(14)=0.361, P>0.5. females: fat mass t(14)=0.413, P>0.5; lean mass t(14)=0.427, P>0.5; Figure 1e). These results indicate that growth rate and fat deposition are not altered by deletion of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons. In consistence with normal body weight and adiposity, plasma levels of leptin and insulin were indistinguishable between the two genotypes (t(8)=1.068, P>0.1 for leptin; t(8)=0.933, P>0.1 for insulin; Figures 1f and g).
Many tests for depression- and anxiety-related behaviors depend upon proper motor and visual functions. We examined locomotor activity in Lepr cKO mice. No differences in the distance traveled within 30 min were observed between Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate controls (males: F(1, 266)=0.587, P>0.1 for the time course of the distance traveled every 2 min; t(19)=0.766, P>0.1 for the total distance traveled within 30 min; Figure 2a, females: F(1, 224)=0.034, P>0.5 for the time course of distance traveled every 2 min; t(16)=0.185, P>0.5 for the total distance traveled within 30 min; Figure 2b). To examine motor coordination, we performed the rotarod test. Lepr cKO and fWT control mice showed similar latencies to fall (males: fWT 164±23 s, Lepr cKO 172±27 s; t(18)=0.224, P>0.5; females: fWT 152±18 s, Lepr cKO 130±16 s; t(36)=0.883, P>0.1). These results suggest that Lepr cKO mice have normal locomotor activity and motor coordination. To evaluate visual acuity, we performed the visual cliff test in mice at the age of 13 weeks. The fWT control mice showed 73±6.2% safe responses and Lepr cKO mice exhibited 73±4.1% safe responses, suggesting that both genotypes are capable of pattern discrimination and have no gross vision impairments at the age tested.
Behavioral phenotypes of Lepr cKO mice
Depression-like behaviors
To determine whether inactivation of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons affects mood-related behaviors, we performed a series of behavioral tests. The tail suspension test and forced swim test have been widely used to assess ‘despair behavior',49, 50 in which animals are subjected to an inescapable stressful situation and subsequently adopt an immobile posture. The duration of immobility, referred to as ‘despair', is attenuated by a variety of antidepressants.51, 52 Both male and female Lepr cKO mice showed a significantly longer immobility time than fWT littermate controls in the tail suspension test (males: t(17)=3.282, P<0.01; females: t(15)=3.129, P<0.01; Figure 2c) and forced swim test (males: t(19)=2.18, P<0.05; females: t(15)=6.836, P<0.001; Figure 2d). The increased immobility in Lepr cKO mice is unlikely due to hypolocomotion because the locomotor activity and motor coordination were normal in Lepr cKO mice.
A core symptom of human depression is anhedonia, or the inability to experience pleasure. The preference for sweet solutions over plain water has been used as a paradigm to measure hedonic response to natural reward in rodents.53 Mice were habituated to two drinking bottles in their home cage and then allowed free access to a choice of water and a sweet solution for 4 days. Lepr cKO mice showed significantly less preference for 1% sucrose solution than littermate fWT controls (t(34)=2.198, P<0.05; Figure 2e). To rule out the potential confounding effect of caloric intake in the sucrose preference test, we subsequently tested the mice with 0.01% saccharin, a noncaloric sweetener. A significant reduction in saccharin preference was observed in Lepr cKO mice compared with fWT littermate controls (t(17)=2.480, P<0.05; Figure 2e). These results indicate a hedonic-like phenotype in Lepr cKO mice.
To determine if loss of Lepr in glutamatergic neurons increases the vulnerability to stress-induced depression-like behaviors, Lepr cKO mice were tested using the learned helplessness paradigm. Animals were exposed to inescapable foot shock stress and subsequently tested for a coping deficit in avoidance-escape performance. Escape deficits in this test are sensitive to reversal by antidepressant treatments.54 Lepr cKO mice exhibited significant increase in escape latency (F(1, 215)=5.020, P<0.05) and number of escape failure (F(1, 43)=9.031, P<0.01; Figure 2f). To ensure that the escape deficit of Lepr cKO was not due to alteration in pain sensitivity, we performed the hot-plate test to measure the pain reflex threshold of the footpad contacting a heated surface. No difference in hot-plate response latencies was observed between Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate controls (fWT 24.4±3.5 s, Lepr cKO 24.3±5.4 s; t(16)=0.052, P>0.5), suggesting that the behavioral changes in the learned helplessness test represent true coping deficits.
Chronic social defeat stress using the resident–intruder paradigm has been validated as an animal model of depression.41, 44 To determine whether Lepr deletion in glutamatergic neurons confers a vulnerability to stress-induced social avoidance, male Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate control mice were subjected to social defeat in the home cage of an unfamiliar aggressive CD1 mouse each day for 10 days. Social interaction was examined for two 2.5-min sessions in the absence or presence of a ‘target' CD1 mouse, respectively, at 24 h after the last session of social defeat (Figure 2g). ANOVA analyses showed significant effects of social defeat on the time spent in the interaction zone (F(1, 64)=43.301, P<0.001) and the avoidance zone (F(1, 64)=25.738, P<0.001) and on social preference ratio (F(1, 64)=30.388, P<0.001) and significant effects of genotype on the time spent in the avoidance zone (F(1, 64)=5.772, P<0.05) and social preference ratio (F(1, 64)=4.605, P<0.05). There were significant defeat × genotype interactions observed on time spent in the avoidance zone (F(1, 64)=5.228, P<0.05) and social preference ratio (F(1, 64)=4.172, P<0.05). Post hoc comparisons indicated that time spent in the avoidance zone was increased and social preference ratio was decreased in Lepr cKO mice compared with fWT littermate controls during the presence of a ‘target' CD1 mouse (Figure 2g). This suggests that loss of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons enhances stress-induced social aversion.
Anxiety-like behaviors
To assess whether depression-like behaviors in Lepr cKO mice are accompanied by anxiety-like behaviors, we performed three behavioral tests to evaluate anxiety levels: the EPM, the open-field test and the light/dark choice. In the EPM, reduced time spent in the open arms and numbers of entries made into the open arms within the 5 min test are taken as indices of anxiety. There were no differences in time spent in the open arms (males: t(16)=0.270, P>0.5; females: t(14)=0.260, P>0.5) or entries into the open arms (males: t(16)=0.371, P>0.5; females: t(14)=0.182, P>0.5) between Lepr cKO and fWT mice (Supplementary Figure S1A). When placed in an open field arena, anxious mice tend to spend less time in the center and more time in the corners. Lepr cKO and fWT mice spent similar amounts of time in the central zone (males: t(18)=0.182, P>0.5; females: t(15)=0.870, P>0.1) and traveled similar total distances within the arena (males: t(18)=0.003, P>0.5; females: t(15)=1.936; P>0.05; Supplementary Figure S1B). In the light/dark choice test, there were no differences in the amount of time spent in the light box (males: t(15)=0.338, P>0.5; females: t(23)=1.007, P>0.1) or the total number of transitions between light and dark boxes (males: t(15)=0.682, P>0.5; females: t(23)=0.796, P>0.1; Supplementary Figure S1C). Collectively, these tests demonstrate that anxiety-like behaviors are not affected by loss of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons.
Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis activity in Lepr cKO mice
The HPA axis hyperactivity is an important mechanism in the pathogenesis of depression.55, 56 The HPA axis activity is under the negative control of hippocampus.57, 58 Therefore, we determined whether deletion of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons would affect HPA axis activity. First, we measured glucocorticoid receptor mRNA in the hippocampus using semiquantitative in situ hybridization. The levels of glucocorticoid receptor mRNA in CA1, CA3 and dentate gyrus of the hippocampus were indistinguishable between Lepr cKO mice and fWT littermate controls (CA1 t(6)=0.007, P>0.5; CA3 t(6)=0.881, P>0.1; dentate gyrus t(6)=1.151, P>0.1; Figure 3a). Second, corticotropin-releasing hormone mRNA expression in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus was examined and showed no difference between the two genotypes (t(6)=1.723, P>0.1; Figure 3b). Moreover, plasma levels of corticosterone were determined under basal and stressed conditions. Lepr cKO mice displayed a normal diurnal rhythm in plasma corticosterone concentrations as compared with fWT mice (t(8)=0.357; P>0.5 for the morning and t(10)=0.267; P>0.5 for the afternoon; Figure 3c). The responsiveness of corticosterone to stress was determined by subjecting animals to 30 min restraint stress. ANOVA showed significant effect of stress (F(2, 26)=275.728; P<0.001) but not genotype (F(1, 26)=2.785; P>0.1) or stress × genotype interaction (F(2, 26)=1.472; P>0.1) on corticosterone levels. Restraint stress-induced elevation of corticosterone levels were similar between the two genotypes (Figure 3d). These results indicate that Lepr cKO mice have normal HPA axis function.
Hippocampal synaptic plasticity in Lepr cKO mice
Neuroplasticity has been proposed as a mechanism underlying the development of major depressive disorder.59, 60 Abnormal hippocampal synaptic plasticity has been observed in chronic mild stress, an animal model of depression, and antidepressants can reverse this effect.61, 62, 63 To determine whether hippocampal synaptic plasticity is affected in Lepr cKO mice, we performed systematic analysis of synaptic functions in the hippocampus. The fEPSPs were recorded at CA3–CA1 Schaeffer collateral synapses in hippocampal slices derived from adult Lepr cKO and fWT mice (8–12 weeks). Initial characterization of Lepr cKO slices revealed no obvious deficits in the basic properties of CA1 synapses. A high-frequency, tetanic stimulation train (100 Hz, 1 s) was used to induce LTP. The high-frequency stimulation produced almost identical LTP in Lepr cKO and fWT mice (fWT: 152±8% Lepr cKO: 152±9%, measured 55–60 min after tetanus; Figure 4a). Induction of LTD by low-frequency stimulation (LFS; 15 min, 1 Hz) was examined in adult hippocampal slices. LFS failed to induce LTD in fWT control mice (95±2% at 70–75 min, Figure 4b). This is not surprising because LFS-induced LTD is age dependent and is observed in juvenile but not adult wild-type mice.64, 65, 66 LFS also did not induce LTD in Lepr cKO mice (Figure 4b). The fEPSP slope recorded at 70–75 min after the onset of LFS was 99±5% of the baseline recorded before the application of LFS. The input–output curves in Lepr cKO and fWT were virtually indistinguishable (Figure 4c), suggesting that deletion of Lepr in glutamatergic neurons does not affect basal synaptic transmission. Similarly, there was no difference in paired-pulse facilitation, a measure of presynaptic release probability, over the whole range of interstimulus intervals between the two groups (Figure 4d).
The standard LFS-induced plasticity only accesses a fraction of available synapses. LTD can also be induced chemically by a brief application of NMDA to the hippocampal slices,67, 68 which activates synaptic as well as extrasynaptic NMDA receptors.69 Application of NMDA for 7 min resulted in a transient depression of synaptic efficacy that almost returned to baseline within <30 min in fWT control slices (Figure 5a). Surprisingly, the same NMDA perfusion induced a robust LTD in Lepr cKO slices (Figure 5a). At least two possible mechanisms may underlie the enhancement of NMDA-induced LTD. First, a deficit in the developmental switch from GluN2B to GluN2A (NR2A) may result in CA1 synapses containing more GluN2B, which has much slower decay time and is implicated in LTD. However, whole-cell recording of evoked synaptic currents indicated that the ratio of NMDAR/AMPAR currents was unaltered in Lepr cKO synapses (Supplementary Figure S2A). This, together with the finding that basal synaptic transmission is normal in Lepr cKO slices (Figure 4c), indicates that synaptic NMDAR activity is not grossly elevated in the synapses of Lepr cKO to cause enhanced LTD. Furthermore, the decay time of NMDA currents at fWT and Lepr cKO synapses was quite similar, suggesting that GluN2B/GluN2A ratio is not dramatically increased at the synapses of Lepr cKO mice (Supplementary Figure S2B). Thus, a deficit in the developmental switch from GluN2A to GluN2B is less likely. The second possibility is that Lepr deletion results in an increase in extrasynaptic NMDAR activity. To test this, LTD-inducing protocol was applied in the presence of PDC (a glutamate transporter inhibitor), which reduces glutamate uptake at the synaptic cleft and therefore increases glutamate spillover to extrasynaptic sites to activate extrasynaptic NMDAR. If NMDA-induced LTD is truly because of extrasynaptic NMDAR, LFS should now be able to induce LTD in adult CA1 synapses. Indeed, a mild LTD was observed 75 min after 1 Hz stimulation in the fWT control slices. Remarkably, Lepr cKO exhibited a much greater LFS-induced LTD (Figure 5b). Thus, the facilitation of NMDA-induced LTD in Lepr cKO mice is likely mediated by an increase in extrasynaptic NMDAR activity. Because extrasynaptic NMDA receptors contain mostly GluN2B subunits,70 we therefore examined the role of GluN2B in LFS-induced LTD. The application of the GluN2B antagonist ifenprodil completely blocked LTD in Lepr cKO mice induced by LFS in the presence of PDC (Figure 5c).
Behavioral responses of Lepr cKO mice to leptin and an GluN2B antagonist
To determine whether Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons mediates the antidepressant-like effects of leptin, we tested the behavioral responses of Lepr cKO mice to leptin. As described previously,21 mice received injection of leptin (1.0 mg kg–1, intraperitoneal) or saline and subjected to the tail suspension test or forced swim test at 30 min after intraperitoneal injection. ANOVA showed significant effects of genotype (F(1, 44)=18.000; P<0.001), drug treatment (F(2, 44)=28.811; P<0.001) and interaction between genotype and drug treatment (F(2, 44)=12.653; P<0.001) on tail suspension test. Similar results were found in forced swim test as significant effects of genotype (F(1, 51)=8.181; P<0.01), drug treatment (F(2, 51)=18.277; P<0.001) and interaction between genotype and drug treatment (F(2, 51)=3.383; P<0.05). Post hoc analysis demonstrated that Lepr cKO mice treated with saline exhibited increased immobility compared with fWT littermate control mice treated with saline in both behavioral tests (Figures 6a and b). Although leptin significantly decreased ‘behavioral despair' in fWT littermate controls, this effect was abolished by deletion of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons in Lepr cKO mice (Figures 6a and b).
Selective GluN2B antagonists produce antidepressant actions similar to ketamine in both animal models and humans.14, 20 We examined whether Lepr cKO mice respond to antidepressant effects induced by blocking GluN2B. Mice were administered the selective GluN2B antagonist Ro25-6981 (3.0 mg kg–1, intraperitoneal) and tested as described above. Post hoc analysis demonstrated that Ro25-6981 treatment induced a greater reduction of ‘behavioral despair' in Lepr cKO mice than fWT littermate controls in the tail suspension test (71% for Lepr cKO mice versus 46% for fWT littermate controls), eliminating the difference in ‘behavioral despair' between the two genotypes (Figure 6a). Similar findings were obtained from the forced swim test (63% decrease in immobility for Lepr cKO mice versus 42% for fWT littermate controls; Figure 6b). These results suggest that Ro25-6981 can reverse the increased ‘behavioral despair' in depressive Lepr cKO mice. Together, these findings suggest that the leptin receptor on forebrain glutamatergic neurons is required for the antidepressant-like behavioral effect of leptin, and the GluN2B antagonist has antidepressant efficacy in mice resistant to leptin.
Discussion
There are several interesting findings in this study. First, selective ablation of the long form of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons located in the hippocampus and dorsal cerebral cortex leads to depression-related behaviors. Second, NMDA-induced hippocampal LTD is facilitated in mice lacking Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons, which is mediated by GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors. Third, these mice are sensitive to the antidepressant-like behavioral effects of the GluN2B antagonist but resistant to leptin. These findings provide strong evidence for a critical role of Lepr signaling in forebrain glutamatergic neurons in regulating depression-related behaviors and suggest a functional relationship between Lepr signaling and NMDA receptors in modulating depression-related behaviors and induction of long-term synaptic depression.
Depression is a complex disorder characterized by complex patterns of symptoms. We assessed behavioral phenotypes of Lepr cKO mice using a battery of tests that detect different aspects of depression, including anhedonia, behavioral despair, learned helplessness and social withdrawal. Under basal conditions, loss of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons caused an anhedonia-like phenotype in the sucrose/saccharin preference tests. Also, Lepr cKO mice displayed behavioral despair in the tail suspension and forced swim tests. Stress is a key environmental factor that can trigger the onset of depression in genetically vulnerable individuals. Increased susceptibility to stress-induced depressive behaviors was observed in Lepr cKO mice as evidenced by increased escape latency and number of failures to escape following exposure to inescapable foot shock and enhanced social avoidance after exposure to chronic social defeat. These findings suggest that defects in Lepr signaling in forebrain glutamatergic neurons may be a predisposition that interacts with stress to increase vulnerability for depression. Locomotor activity and visual functions were not affected in Lepr cKO mice, suggesting that the depression-like behavioral deficits in these mice cannot be attributed to motor or sensory impairments. We have previously shown that leptin produces antidepressant-like effects in rats and mice,21, 22 and this finding has been recently confirmed by another research group.23 However, it remained unclear what neuronal populations mediate leptin's antidepressant-like effects. In the present study, we demonstrated that loss of Lepr in glutamatergic neurons in the hippocampus and cortex abolished the antidepressant-like behavioral effect of leptin in both the forced swim test and the tail suspension test. These results suggest that glutamatergic neurons in the dorsal forebrain regions are targets for leptin's action on depressive-like behaviors. Although depression often coexists with anxiety, there were no evident signs of anxiety observed in Lepr cKO mice as assessed in multiple behavioral tests including the EPM, light/dark choice and open-field tests. Our recent studies have shown that mice with selective ablation of Lepr in midbrain dopamine neurons display anxiogenic-like behaviors without affecting depression-like behaviors.45 Taken together, these findings suggest that leptin action on depression- and anxiety-related behaviors are mediated by distinct neuronal circuits.
Increasing evidence points to a link between impairments of plasticity at glutamatergic synapses and depression. LTP and LTD are two major forms of persistent synaptic plasticity in the central nervous system characterized by a long-lasting increase or decrease in synaptic strength, respectively. Stress, including chronic unpredictable stress, an animal model of depression, has been shown to impair LTP and facilitate LTD in the adult hippocampus,61, 71, 72, 73 and the effects of stress on synaptic plasticity can be blocked by treatment with antidepressants.61, 74 Previous pharmacological studies have reported that leptin regulates hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. However, the results from these studies are inconsistent. Both enhancement and inhibition of hippocampal LTP in response to leptin treatment have been reported,75, 76, 77 often depending on the dosage of leptin used or the age of animals. In mice and rats with naturally occurring loss-of-function mutations in Lepr (db/db, fa/fa), both LTP and LTD are impaired.78, 79, 80 However, these animals are morbidly obese and diabetic with hyperinsulinemia and hypercorticosteronemia.81, 82, 83 Both insulin and corticosterone have been shown to modulate LTP and LTD.84, 85, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90 Thus, one cannot rule out the possibility that abnormally high levels of insulin and corticosterone contribute to the disrupted LTP and LTD induction observed in db/db mice and fa/fa rats. In this study, mice with ablation of Lepr selectively in forebrain glutamatergic neurons had normal body weight and normal insulin and corticosterone levels. Standard electric stimulus-induced LTP or LTD was not affected in these mice, but they displayed a robust enhancement of chemically (NMDA)-induced LTD in adult hippocampus. This could result from a deficit in the developmental switch from GluN2B- to GluN2A-containing receptors at synapses. However, this possibility is not supported by the data presented here as synaptic NMDAR activity and GluN2B/GluN2A ratio were not significantly elevated at the synapses of Lepr cKO mice. Another possibility is that increased extrasynaptic NMDAR activity causes LTD in Lepr cKO mice. Inhibiting the synaptic glutamate transporter facilitates spillover of glutamate to extrasynaptic sites. We found that the treatment with PDC, a glutamate transporter inhibitor, dramatically increased low-frequency electrical stimulation-induced LTD in Lepr cKO mice. Thus, activation of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors by glutamate spillover in Lepr cKO mice is likely responsible for the induction of LTD. NMDA receptor GluN2B subunits are localized predominantly at extrasynaptic sites.70 We found that selective blockade of NMDA receptors containing the GluN2B subunit by ifenprodil prevented the induction of LTD, suggesting that facilitation of LTD induction in Lepr cKO requires GluN2B, possibly by elevating expression or signaling of extrasynaptic GluN2B.
NMDA receptor antagonists, such as ketamine, exert rapid and robust antidepressant effects in some patients with major depression.6, 17, 18 Selective GluN2B antagonists have been used to treat patients with treatment-resistant depression.20 In animal studies, the nonselective NMDA antagonist ketamine and the GluN2B-specific antagonist Ro25-6981 have been shown to reverse behavioral despair and learned helplessness.12, 14 In this study, we demonstrated that Lepr cKO mice were highly sensitive to the antidepressant-like behavioral effects of the GluN2B antagonist Ro25-6981 in both the tail suspension test and the forced swim test, whereas leptin failed to show the antidepressant-like behavioral effects in these mutant mice. These findings suggest that glutamatergic neurons in the forebrain region are target neurons mediating the antidepressant-like effect of leptin and that GluN2B signaling lies downstream of Lepr signaling in regulating depression-related behaviors. The reversal of hippocampal LTD induction and behavioral despair by GluN2B antagonists in Lepr cKO mice imply a link between induction of persistent synaptic depression and behavioral manifestation of depression. However, further investigations are required to better understand this association. Whether a blockade of a specific form of synaptic plasticity could prevent or reverse behavioral depression remains to be determined. It is noteworthy to point out that although LTD has been implicated in hippocampus-dependent learning and memory,91, 92, 93, 94, 95 Lepr cKO mice exhibited normal performance in hippocampus-dependent learning and memory tests including spatial and reversal learning in the Morris water maze, T-maze alternation and contextual fear conditioning (data not shown).
Recent epidemiological studies indicate that obesity, a state reflecting leptin resistance in most obese subjects, increases the incidence of depression.96, 97, 98, 99, 100 Diet-induced obese mice display depression-like behaviors23, 101 and are resistant to antidepressant-like behavioral effects of leptin.23 However, the question remains whether depression in obese individuals occurs secondary to metabolic changes or as a direct consequence of leptin resistance in the limbic circuitry. Although global loss or neuron-specific loss of leptin signaling (defects in leptin or its receptor) in mice and humans leads to severe obesity,83, 102, 103, 104, 105, 106, 107, 108, 109, 110 ablation of Lepr in forebrain glutamatergic neurons had no effect on body weight, adiposity and hormone levels under basal chow-fed conditions. Thus, the development of depression-like behaviors observed in Lepr cKO mice reflects a direct effect of resistance to leptin's action. Recent studies have suggested a U-shaped trend in the association between depression and body mass; both underweight and obesity are associated with depression.111, 112, 113 We speculate that low leptin levels in underweight individuals and leptin resistance in obese people may contribute to the high prevalence of depression among these populations.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the NIH Grants NIMH 076929 and NIMH 073844 (to X-YL), the NRSA predoctoral fellowship award NIMH 83442 (to JCG) and funds from intramural research programs of the NICHD and NIMH (to BL).
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Translational Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/tp)
Supplementary Material
References
- Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Koretz D, Merikangas KR, et al. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) JAMA. 2003;289:3095–3105. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell AJ, Chan M, Bhatti H, Halton M, Grassi L, Johansen C, et al. Prevalence of depression, anxiety, and adjustment disorder in oncological, haematological, and palliative-care settings: a meta-analysis of 94 interview-based studies. Lancet Oncol. 2011;12:160–174. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(11)70002-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong ML, Licinio J. Research and treatment approaches to depression. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:343–351. doi: 10.1038/35072566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimoto K. Emerging role of glutamate in the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder. Brain Res Rev. 2009;61:105–123. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maeng S, Zarate CA., Jr The role of glutamate in mood disorders: results from the ketamine in major depression study and the presumed cellular mechanism underlying its antidepressant effects. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2007;9:467–474. doi: 10.1007/s11920-007-0063-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanacora G, Zarate CA, Krystal JH, Manji HK. Targeting the glutamatergic system to develop novel, improved therapeutics for mood disorders. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2008;7:426–437. doi: 10.1038/nrd2462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skolnick P, Popik P, Trullas R. Glutamate-based antidepressants: 20 years on. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2009;30:563–569. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machado-Vieira R, Salvadore G, Luckenbaugh DA, Manji HK, Zarate CA., Jr Rapid onset of antidepressant action: a new paradigm in the research and treatment of major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69:946–958. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaturvedi HK, Chandra D, Bapna JS. Interaction between N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists and imipramine in shock-induced depression. Indian J Exp Biol. 1999;37:952–958. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yilmaz A, Schulz D, Aksoy A, Canbeyli R. Prolonged effect of an anesthetic dose of ketamine on behavioral despair. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;71:341–344. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00693-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Autry AE, Adachi M, Nosyreva E, Na ES, Los MF, Cheng PF, et al. NMDA receptor blockade at rest triggers rapid behavioural antidepressant responses. Nature. 2011;475:91–95. doi: 10.1038/nature10130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li N, Lee B, Liu RJ, Banasr M, Dwyer JM, Iwata M, et al. mTOR-dependent synapse formation underlies the rapid antidepressant effects of NMDA antagonists. Science. 2010;329:959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1190287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li N, Liu RJ, Dwyer JM, Banasr M, Lee B, Son H, et al. Glutamate N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists rapidly reverse behavioral and synaptic deficits caused by chronic stress exposure. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:754–761. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maeng S, Zarate CA, Jr, Du J, Schloesser RJ, McCammon J, Chen G, et al. Cellular mechanisms underlying the antidepressant effects of ketamine: role of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionic acid receptors. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:349–352. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trullas R, Skolnick P. Functional antagonists at the NMDA receptor complex exhibit antidepressant actions. Eur J Pharmacol. 1990;185:1–10. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(90)90204-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul IA, Skolnick P. Glutamate and depression: clinical and preclinical studies. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2003;1003:250–272. doi: 10.1196/annals.1300.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman RM, Cappiello A, Anand A, Oren DA, Heninger GR, Charney DS, et al. Antidepressant effects of ketamine in depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:351–354. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00230-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zarate CA, Jr, Singh JB, Carlson PJ, Brutsche NE, Ameli R, Luckenbaugh DA, et al. A randomized trial of an N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist in treatment-resistant major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:856–864. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price RB, Nock MK, Charney DS, Mathew SJ. Effects of intravenous ketamine on explicit and implicit measures of suicidality in treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:522–526. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preskorn SH, Baker B, Kolluri S, Menniti FS, Krams M, Landen JW. An innovative design to establish proof of concept of the antidepressant effects of the GluN2B subunit selective N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist, CP-101,606, in patients with treatment-refractory major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008;28:631–637. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31818a6cea. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Garza JC, Bronner J, Kim CS, Zhang W, Lu XY. Acute administration of leptin produces anxiolytic-like effects: a comparison with fluoxetine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;207:535–545. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1684-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu XY, Kim CS, Frazer A, Zhang W. Leptin: a potential novel antidepressant. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:1593–1598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508901103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamada N, Katsuura G, Ochi Y, Ebihara K, Kusakabe T, Hosoda K, et al. Impaired CNS leptin action is implicated in depression associated with obesity. Endocrinology. 2011;152:2634–2643. doi: 10.1210/en.2011-0004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finger BC, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. High-fat diet selectively protects against the effects of chronic social stress in the mouse. Neuroscience. 2011;192:351–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.06.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jow GM, Yang TT, Chen CL. Leptin and cholesterol levels are low in major depressive disorder, but high in schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. 2006;90:21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraus T, Haack M, Schuld A, Hinze-Selch D, Pollmacher T. Low leptin levels but normal body mass indices in patients with depression or schizophrenia. Neuroendocrinology. 2001;73:243–247. doi: 10.1159/000054641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang K, Xie G, Zhang Z, Wang C, Li W, Zhou W, et al. Levels of serum interleukin (IL)-6, IL-1beta, tumour necrosis factor-alpha and leptin and their correlation in depression. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2007;41:266–273. doi: 10.1080/00048670601057759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawson EA, Miller KK, Blum JI, Meenaghan E, Misra M, Eddy KT, et al. Leptin levels are associated with decreased depressive symptoms in women across the weight spectrum, independent of body fat Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2011. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2011.04182.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Scott MM, Lachey JL, Sternson SM, Lee CE, Elias CF, Friedman JM, et al. Leptin targets in the mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. 2009;514:518–532. doi: 10.1002/cne.22025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caron E, Sachot C, Prevot V, Bouret SG. Distribution of leptin-sensitive cells in the postnatal and adult mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:459–476. doi: 10.1002/cne.22219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson CM, Leshan RL, Jones JC, Myers MG., Jr Molecular mapping of mouse brain regions innervated by leptin receptor-expressing cells. Brain Res. 2011;1378:18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell S, Macqueen G. The role of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2004;29:417–426. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenigs M, Grafman J. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2009;201:239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soares JC, Mann JJ. The anatomy of mood disorders--review of structural neuroimaging studies. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41:86–106. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00006-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan CH, Godinho LN, Thomaidou D, Tan SS, Gulisano M, Parnavelas JG. Emx1 is a marker for pyramidal neurons of the cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1191–1198. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.12.1191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorski JA, Talley T, Qiu M, Puelles L, Rubenstein JL, Jones KR. Cortical excitatory neurons and glia, but not GABAergic neurons, are produced in the Emx1-expressing lineage. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6309–6314. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06309.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasato T, Nomura R, Ando R, Ikeda T, Tanaka M, Itohara S. Dorsal telencephalon-specific expression of Cre recombinase in PAC transgenic mice. Genesis. 2004;38:130–138. doi: 10.1002/gene.20009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMinn JE, Liu SM, Dragatsis I, Dietrich P, Ludwig T, Eiden S, et al. An allelic series for the leptin receptor gene generated by CRE and FLP recombinase. Mamm Genome. 2004;15:677–685. doi: 10.1007/s00335-004-2340-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo H, Hong S, Jin XL, Chen RS, Avasthi PP, Tu YT, et al. Specificity and efficiency of Cre-mediated recombination in Emx1-Cre knock-in mice. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2000;273:661–665. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.2000.2870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin XL, Guo H, Mao C, Atkins N, Wang H, Avasthi PP, et al. Emx1-specific expression of foreign genes using ‘knock-in' approach. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2000;270:978–982. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.2000.2532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsankova NM, Berton O, Renthal W, Kumar A, Neve RL, Nestler EJ. Sustained hippocampal chromatin regulation in a mouse model of depression and antidepressant action. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:519–525. doi: 10.1038/nn1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolhaas JM, De Boer SF, De Rutter AJ, Meerlo P, Sgoifo A. Social stress in rats and mice. Acta Physiol Scand Suppl. 1997;640:69–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez M, Calvo-Torrent A, Pico-Alfonso M. Social defeat and subordination as models of social stress in laboratory rodents: a review. Aggress Behav. 1998;24:241–256. [Google Scholar]
- Yu T, Guo M, Garza J, Rendon S, Sun XL, Zhang W, et al. Cognitive and neural correlates of depression-like behaviour in socially defeated mice: an animal model of depression with cognitive dysfunction. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;14:303–317. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710000945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Perez SM, Zhang W, Lodge DJ, Lu XY. Selective deletion of the leptin receptor in dopamine neurons produces anxiogenic-like behavior and increases dopaminergic activity in amygdala. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:1024–1038. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garza JC, Guo M, Zhang W, Lu XY. Leptin increases adult hippocampal neurogenesis in vivo and in vitro. J Biol Chem. 2008;283:18238–18247. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M800053200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Garza JC, Truong HV, Henschel J, Zhang W, Lu XY. The melanocortinergic pathway is rapidly recruited by emotional stress and contributes to stress-induced anorexia and anxiety-like behavior. Endocrinology. 2007;148:5531–5540. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-0745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garza JC, Kim CS, Liu J, Zhang W, Lu XY. Adeno-associated virus-mediated knockdown of melanocortin-4 receptor in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus promotes high-fat diet-induced hyperphagia and obesity. J Endocrinol. 2008;197:471–482. doi: 10.1677/JOE-08-0009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porsolt RD, Bertin A, Jalfre M. Behavioral despair in mice: a primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacodyn Ther. 1977;229:327–336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steru L, Chermat R, Thierry B, Simon P. The tail suspension test: a new method for screening antidepressants in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985;85:367–370. doi: 10.1007/BF00428203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cryan JF, Mombereau C, Vassout A. The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:571–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petit-Demouliere B, Chenu F, Bourin M. Forced swimming test in mice: a review of antidepressant activity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;177:245–255. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2048-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willner P, Muscat R, Papp M. Chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia: a realistic animal model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1992;16:525–534. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80194-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shanks N, Anisman H. Strain-specific effects of antidepressants on escape deficits induced by inescapable shock. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;99:122–128. doi: 10.1007/BF00634465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB. Psychoneuroendocrinology of depression. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1998;21:293–307. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(05)70006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holsboer F. The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:477–501. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00159-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman JP, Schafer MK, Young EA, Thompson R, Douglass J, Akil H, et al. Evidence for hippocampal regulation of neuroendocrine neurons of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. J Neurosci. 1989;9:3072–3082. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-09-03072.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson L, Sapolsky R. The role of the hippocampus in feedback regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Endocr Rev. 1991;12:118–134. doi: 10.1210/edrv-12-2-118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Stress- and allostasis-induced brain plasticity. Annu Rev Med. 2011;62:431–445. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-052209-100430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pittenger C, Duman RS. Stress, depression, and neuroplasticity: a convergence of mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:88–109. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holderbach R, Clark K, Moreau JL, Bischofberger J, Normann C. Enhanced long-term synaptic depression in an animal model of depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang M, Yang Y, Dong Z, Cao J, Xu L. GluN2B-containing N-methyl-D-aspartate subtype glutamate receptors regulate the acute stress effect on hippocampal long-term potentiation/long-term depression in vivo. NeuroReport. 2006;17:1343–1346. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000227994.07799.6c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niehusmann P, Seifert G, Clark K, Atas HC, Herpfer I, Fiebich B, et al. Coincidence detection and stress modulation of spike time-dependent long-term depression in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2010;30:6225–6235. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6411-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudek SM, Bear MF. Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in the adult and immature hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1993;13:2910–2918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-07-02910.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulkey RM, Malenka RC. Mechanisms underlying induction of homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron. 1992;9:967–975. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90248-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner JJ, Alger BE. GABAergic and developmental influences on homosynaptic LTD and depotentiation in rat hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1577–1586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01577.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamal A, Ramakers GM, Urban IJ, De Graan PN, Gispen WH. Chemical LTD in the CA1 field of the hippocampus from young and mature rats. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:3512–3516. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee HK, Kameyama K, Huganir RL, Bear MF. NMDA induces long-term synaptic depression and dephosphorylation of the GluR1 subunit of AMPA receptors in hippocampus. Neuron. 1998;21:1151–1162. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80632-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiels E, Xie X, Yeckel MF, Barrionuevo G, Berger TW. NMDA receptor-dependent LTD in different subfields of hippocampus in vivo and in vitro. Hippocampus. 1996;6:43–51. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:1<43::AID-HIPO8>3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tovar KR, Westbrook GL. The incorporation of NMDA receptors with a distinct subunit composition at nascent hippocampal synapses in vitro. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4180–4188. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04180.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JJ, Foy MR, Thompson RF. Behavioral stress modifies hippocampal plasticity through N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:4750–4753. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.10.4750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu L, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Behavioural stress facilitates the induction of long-term depression in the hippocampus. Nature. 1997;387:497–500. doi: 10.1038/387497a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong TP, Howland JG, Robillard JM, Ge Y, Yu W, Titterness AK, et al. Hippocampal long-term depression mediates acute stress-induced spatial memory retrieval impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:11471–11476. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702308104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Frijtag JC, Kamal A, Reijmers LG, Schrama LH, van den Bos R, Spruijt BM. Chronic imipramine treatment partially reverses the long-term changes of hippocampal synaptic plasticity in socially stressed rats. Neurosci Lett. 2001;309:153–156. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02062-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oomura Y, Hori N, Shiraishi T, Fukunaga K, Takeda H, Tsuji M, et al. Leptin facilitates learning and memory performance and enhances hippocampal CA1 long-term potentiation and CaMK II phosphorylation in rats. Peptides. 2006;27:2738–2749. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2006.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wayner MJ, Armstrong DL, Phelix CF, Oomura Y. Orexin-A (Hypocretin-1) and leptin enhance LTP in the dentate gyrus of rats in vivo. Peptides. 2004;25:991–996. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2004.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker CD, Long H, Williams S, Richard D. Long-lasting effects of elevated neonatal leptin on rat hippocampal function, synaptic proteins and NMDA receptor subunits. J Neurosci Res. 2007;85:816–828. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li XL, Aou S, Oomura Y, Hori N, Fukunaga K, Hori T. Impairment of long-term potentiation and spatial memory in leptin receptor-deficient rodents. Neuroscience. 2002;113:607–615. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00162-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerges NZ, Aleisa AM, Alkadhi KA. Impaired long-term potentiation in obese zucker rats: possible involvement of presynaptic mechanism. Neuroscience. 2003;120:535–539. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00297-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stranahan AM, Arumugam TV, Cutler RG, Lee K, Egan JM, Mattson MP. Diabetes impairs hippocampal function through glucocorticoid-mediated effects on new and mature neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:309–317. doi: 10.1038/nn2055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen H, Charlat O, Tartaglia LA, Woolf EA, Weng X, Ellis SJ, et al. Evidence that the diabetes gene encodes the leptin receptor: identification of a mutation in the leptin receptor gene in db/db mice. Cell. 1996;84:491–495. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81294-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee GH, Proenca R, Montez JM, Carroll KM, Darvishzadeh JG, Lee JI, et al. Abnormal splicing of the leptin receptor in diabetic mice. Nature. 1996;379:632–635. doi: 10.1038/379632a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tartaglia LA, Dembski M, Weng X, Deng N, Culpepper J, Devos R, et al. Identification and expression cloning of a leptin receptor, OB-R. Cell. 1995;83:1263–1271. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90151-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alfarez DN, Wiegert O, Joels M, Krugers HJ. Corticosterone and stress reduce synaptic potentiation in mouse hippocampal slices with mild stimulation. Neuroscience. 2002;115:1119–1126. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00483-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biessels GJ, Kamal A, Ramakers GM, Urban IJ, Spruijt BM, Erkelens DW, et al. Place learning and hippocampal synaptic plasticity in streptozotocin-induced diabetic rats. Diabetes. 1996;45:1259–1266. doi: 10.2337/diab.45.9.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM. Inverted-U relationship between the level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus. 1992;2:421–430. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang CC, Lee CC, Hsu KS. An investigation into signal transduction mechanisms involved in insulin-induced long-term depression in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. J Neurochem. 2004;89:217–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2003.02307.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Man HY, Lin JW, Ju WH, Ahmadian G, Liu L, Becker LE, et al. Regulation of AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission by clathrin-dependent receptor internalization. Neuron. 2000;25:649–662. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81067-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlides C, Kimura A, Magarinos AM, McEwen BS. Hippocampal homosynaptic long-term depression/depotentiation induced by adrenal steroids. Neuroscience. 1995;68:379–385. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)94332-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Heide LP, Kamal A, Artola A, Gispen WH, Ramakers GM. Insulin modulates hippocampal activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in a N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor and phosphatidyl-inositol-3-kinase-dependent manner. J Neurochem. 2005;94:1158–1166. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03269.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brigman JL, Wright T, Talani G, Prasad-Mulcare S, Jinde S, Seabold GK, et al. Loss of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in CA1 hippocampus and cortex impairs long-term depression, reduces dendritic spine density, and disrupts learning. J Neurosci. 2010;30:4590–4600. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0640-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collingridge GL, Peineau S, Howland JG, Wang YT. Long-term depression in the CNS. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:459–473. doi: 10.1038/nrn2867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp A, Manahan-Vaughan D. Hippocampal long-term depression: master or minion in declarative memory processes. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massey PV, Bashir ZI. Long-term depression: multiple forms and implications for brain function. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholls RE, Alarcon JM, Malleret G, Carroll RC, Grody M, Vronskaya S, et al. Transgenic mice lacking NMDAR-dependent LTD exhibit deficits in behavioral flexibility. Neuron. 2008;58:104–117. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon GE, Von Korff M, Saunders K, Miglioretti DL, Crane PK, van Belle G, et al. Association between obesity and psychiatric disorders in the US adult population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:824–830. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kivimaki M, Lawlor DA, Singh-Manoux A, Batty GD, Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, et al. Common mental disorder and obesity: insight from four repeat measures over 19 years: prospective Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. 2009;339:b3765. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b3765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowitz S, Friedman MA, Arent SM. Understanding the relation between obesity and depression: causal mechanisms and implications for treatment. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2008;15:1–20. [Google Scholar]
- Luppino FS, de Wit LM, Bouvy PF, Stijnen T, Cuijpers P, Penninx BW, et al. Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:220–229. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McElroy SL, Kotwal R, Malhotra S, Nelson EB, Keck PE, Nemeroff CB.Are mood disorders and obesity related? A review for the mental health professional J Clin Psychiatry 200465634–651.quiz 730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vucetic Z, Kimmel J, Reyes TM. Chronic high-fat diet drives postnatal epigenetic regulation of mu-opioid receptor in the brain. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:1199–1206. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clement K, Vaisse C, Lahlou N, Cabrol S, Pelloux V, Cassuto D, et al. A mutation in the human leptin receptor gene causes obesity and pituitary dysfunction. Nature. 1998;392:398–401. doi: 10.1038/32911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen P, Zhao C, Cai X, Montez JM, Rohani SC, Feinstein P, et al. Selective deletion of leptin receptor in neurons leads to obesity. J Clin Invest. 2001;108:1113–1121. doi: 10.1172/JCI13914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Luca C, Kowalski TJ, Zhang Y, Elmquist JK, Lee C, Kilimann MW, et al. Complete rescue of obesity, diabetes, and infertility in db/db mice by neuron-specific LEPR-B transgenes. J Clin Invest. 2005;115:3484–3493. doi: 10.1172/JCI24059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooqi IS, Wangensteen T, Collins S, Kimber W, Matarese G, Keogh JM, et al. Clinical and molecular genetic spectrum of congenital deficiency of the leptin receptor. N Engl J Med. 2007;356:237–247. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa063988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson WT, Farooqi IS, Moreau M, DePaoli AM, Lawrence E, O'Rahilly S, et al. Congenital leptin deficiency due to homozygosity for the Delta133G mutation: report of another case and evaluation of response to four years of leptin therapy. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:4821–4826. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-0376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague CT, Farooqi IS, Whitehead JP, Soos MA, Rau H, Wareham NJ, et al. Congenital leptin deficiency is associated with severe early-onset obesity in humans. Nature. 1997;387:903–908. doi: 10.1038/43185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozata M, Ozdemir IC, Licinio J. Human leptin deficiency caused by a missense mutation: multiple endocrine defects, decreased sympathetic tone, and immune system dysfunction indicate new targets for leptin action, greater central than peripheral resistance to the effects of leptin, and spontaneous correction of leptin-mediated defects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1999;84:3686–3695. doi: 10.1210/jcem.84.10.5999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strobel A, Issad T, Camoin L, Ozata M, Strosberg AD. A leptin missense mutation associated with hypogonadism and morbid obesity. Nat Genet. 1998;18:213–215. doi: 10.1038/ng0398-213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, Friedman JM. Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue. Nature. 1994;372:425–432. doi: 10.1038/372425a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y, Jiang Y, Mao Y. Association between obesity and depression in Canadians. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 2009;18:1687–1692. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2008.1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit LM, van Straten A, van Herten M, Penninx BW, Cuijpers P. Depression and body mass index, a u-shaped association. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrea RL, Berger YG, King MB.Body mass index and common mental disorders: exploring the shape of the association and its moderation by age, gender and education Int J Obes (Lond) 2011. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.65 [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.