Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2012 Feb 13;136(1-3):51–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.030

Cortical Structural Abnormalities in Deficit Versus Nondeficit Schizophrenia

Bernard A Fischer 1,2, William R Keller 2, Celso Arango 3, Godfrey Pearlson 4, Robert P McMahon 2, Walter A Meyer 2, Alan Francis 5, Brian Kirkpatrick 6, William T Carpenter 1,2, Robert W Buchanan 2,*
PMCID: PMC3298625  NIHMSID: NIHMS353796  PMID: 22336954

Abstract

Objective

To examine the structural integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal-basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuit in people with the deficit form of schizophrenia.

Method

A three-dimensional structural MRI sequence was used to conduct morphometric assessments of cortical and subcortical regions in deficit and nondeficit outpatients with schizophrenia and healthy controls.

Results

The superior prefrontal and superior and middle temporal gyral gray matter volumes were significantly smaller in the deficit versus the nondeficit group and normal control groups. There were no significant group differences in examined subcortical structures.

Conclusion

People with deficit schizophrenia are characterized by selective reductions in the prefrontal and temporal cortex.

Keywords: schizophrenia, deficit syndrome, frontal lobe, temporal lobe, structural MRI

1. Introduction

Primary, enduring negative symptoms define a subtype of schizophrenia with a characteristic epidemiology, illness course, and clinical correlates (Kirkpatrick et al, 2001). Behavioral and functional neuroimaging studies (Buchanan et al, 1990, 1994, 1997; Lahti et al, 2001; Tamminga et al, 1992) have led us to hypothesize that the dorsolateral prefrontal-basal ganglia-thalamocortical (DLPFC) circuit (Alexander et al, 1990) is involved in deficit schizophrenia pathology.

We previously examined DLPFC circuit integrity by comparing prefrontal cortex and caudate volumes in deficit, nondeficit, and healthy comparison groups (Buchanan et al, 1993). We found that the nondeficit group had smaller prefrontal volumes compared to deficit and control groups, with the difference due to decreased white matter. There were no other significant schizophrenia group differences. A limitation of this earlier study was the inability to focus on specific cortical components of the circuit. Since then, we have developed a reliable procedure for segmenting cortical regions into component gyri (Buchanan et al, 2004; Rademacher et al, 1993).

The present study examined the structural integrity of cortical and subcortical DLPFC circuit components in deficit and nondeficit people with schizophrenia and a healthy comparison group. We hypothesized that we would find smaller volume of one or more components of the circuit specific to the deficit group, while there would be no deficit-specific volume differences in non-circuit regions.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

Participants were outpatients, aged 21–51 years old, with DSMIV-TR schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome (Kirkpatrick et al, 1989) was used to subtype participants into deficit and nondeficit groups. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) total score and positive symptom items and the modified Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) total score were used to assess global psychopathology and positive and negative symptoms, respectively.

Schizophrenia groups were matched on age, gender, proportion on each type of antipsychotic (FGA, second generation (SGA), or clozapine), and duration of illness. Healthy comparison participants were recruited from the community and matched to schizophrenia participants on age and gender. Potential participants were excluded for a history of neurological disorder, mental retardation, head injury with loss of consciousness > 30 minutes, or diagnosis of DSM-IV substance abuse or dependence (except nicotine) within the last 12 months. Healthy comparison participants were also excluded if they had a history of any DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis.

The University of Maryland School of Medicine IRB approved the protocol and informed consent procedures. Written documentation of informed consent and capacity to provide consent was obtained from all participants prior to study participation.

2.2 MRI Procedures

MRIs were performed on a 1.5-Tesla Signa GE Scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI). Brains were evaluated in the coronal plane using a spoiled gradient recall acquisition in the steady-state three-dimensional imaging sequence (TR=35msec; TE=5msec; flip angle=45 degrees; number of excitations=1; field of view=24cm; matrix size: 256 by 256; slice thickness=1.5mm without skips). The image processing and procedures for measuring the cortical regions of interest are described in detail elsewhere (Barta et al, 1997; Buchanan et al, 1998; Buchanan et al, 2004). The cortical regions of interest are illustrated in Buchanan et al (2004).

We measured the gray matter volumes of the following cortical and subcortical DLPFC circuit components: the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (middle frontal gyrus), inferior parietal cortex (angular and supramarginal gyri), thalamus, and caudate. Non-circuit control regions were other prefrontal cortex (orbital frontal, inferior frontal, superior frontal gyri), temporal cortex (superior and middle temporal gyri), and the amygdala-hippocampuc complex (AHC). Inter-rater reliability for volume calculations ranged from 0.86–0.99.

2.3 Statistical Analyses

A mixed analysis of covariance model for repeated measures, with volume as the within subject repeated measure, was used to examine group differences in circuit and non-circuit brain regions: Volume = diagnostic group + age + hemisphere + sex + (diagnosis × hemisphere) + (sex × hemisphere). In view of the limited number of female subjects, three- and four-way interaction terms were not included. Groups were not different on either TCV or TBV (see Table 1), so these measures were not included in the model. Separate models were run for each gray matter ROI in the volumes included or not in the circuit we hypothesized to distinguish deficit and non-deficit patients. Within the two sets of circuit and non-circuit ROIs, the Benjamini-Hochberg method was used to control for multiple testing of the main effect of group (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Post hoc pairwise tests for differences among the three groups were conducted in ROIs in which the main effect of group was significant after correction for multiple testing.

Table 1.

Sample Characteristics.

Deficit
Schizophrenia
(N=20)
Nondeficit
Schizophrenia
(N=36)
Healthy
Comparison
Group
(N=28)
Age at MRI (Years) [Mean (SD)] 40.1 (7.2) 38.4 (7.9) 36.0 (6.6)
Age Range (Years) 23–51 21–50 26–48
Male (%) 17 (85.0) 31 (86.1) 23 (82.1)
Non-White (%) 4 (20.0) 8 (22.2) 6 (21.4)
Education (Years)* 11.4 (2.5) 13.9 (1.9)+ 14.2 (1.7)
Schizoaffective Diagnosis (%) 0 3 (8) --
Duration of Illness (Years)
[Mean (SE)]
18.0 (1.9) 19.3 (1.4) --
BPRS Total Score [Mean (SE)] 34.1 (1.8) 33.4 (1.6) --
BPRS Positive Symptom Item
Score [Mean (SE)]
9.8 (1.1) 9.5 (0.7) --
SANS Total Score [Mean
(SE)]**
38.7 (2.8) 23.8 (4.4) --
Medication Status
FGAs (%) 9 (45.0) 15 (41.7) --
Non-Clozapine
SGAs (%)
4 (20.0) 10 (27.8) --
Clozapine (%) 7 (35.0) 11 (30.6) --
Right Handed (%) 20 (100.0) 32 (88.9) 27 (96.4)
Volume Comparisons (mL) [Mean (SE)]***
Total Cranial Volume (TCV) 1430.3 (47.0) 1455.4 (36.0) 1460.7 (36.4)
Total Brain Volume (TBV) 1299.2 (43.9) 1331.1 (33.6) 1339.2 (34.0)
Total Ventricular Volume 16.5 (2.8) 14.2 (2.1) 10.8 (2.1)
Total CSF Volume 114.6 (10.4) 110.1 (7.9) 110.6 (8.0)

BPRS=Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; SANS=Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms;

FGA: First Generation Antipsychotic; SGA: Second Generation Antipsychotic

SD: Standard Deviation; SE: Standard Error

*

: The deficit group had significantly less education than the nondeficit (t(52) = 4.33, p<0.0001) and healthy comparison (t(46) = 4.75, p=0.0001) groups.

**

: Significant group difference for SANS; t50=2.46, p=0.02.

***

: The main effect of diagnosis was not significant for TCV (F2,77 = 0.14, p=0.87), TBV (F2,77=0.28, p=0.76), total ventricular volume (F2,77=1.45, p=0.24), or total CSF volume (F2,77=0.07, p=0.94).

3. Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1.

3.1 Deficit versus Nondeficit versus Healthy Comparison Group

3.1.1 Circuit Volumes (see Table 2)

Table 2.

Comparison of DLPFC Circuit versus Non-circuit Volumes by Diagnosis.*

Region Deficit Schizophrenia Nondeficit
Schizophrenia
Healthy Comparison
Group
Group
Effect
(p-value)
False
Discovery
Rate Critical
Value**
n Volume SE n Volume SE n Volume SE
DLPFC Circuit Regions
Middle Frontal Gyrus
Gray Matter
20 11.40 1.15 35 13.55 0.92 28 13.42 0.95 0.20 0.01
Supramarginal Gyrus
Gray Matter (Inferior
Parietal)
20 5.34 0.37 36 5.99 0.29 28 5.54 0.30 0.23 0.02
Angular Gyrus Gray
Matter (Inferior
Parietal)
19 4.50 0.32 36 4.72 0.25 28 4.45 0.26 0.65 0.05
Thalamus 20 4.13 0.27 36 4.45 0.22 28 4.57 0.23 0.38 0.04
Caudate 13 4.37 0.27 32 4.78 0.18 24 4.83 0.19 0.25 0.03
Non-DLPFC Circuit Regions
Superior Frontal
Gyrus Gray Matter
20 19.47 1.01 35 22.04 0.80 28 22.72 0.83 0.019 0.025
Inferior Frontal Gray
Matter
20 7.28 0.61 35 8.28 0.49 28 8.85 0.51 0.09 0.04
Orbital Frontal Gray
Matter
20 11.91 0.45 35 12.37 0.36 28 12.72 0.37 0.31 0.05
Superior Temporal
Gyrus Gray Matter
20 13.28 0.64 36 14.91 0.51 28 15.12 0.53 0.0336 0.0333
Amygdala-
Hippocampal
Complex
20 6.04 0.18 35 6.29 0.15 28 6.77 0.15 0.002 0.017
Middle Temporal
Gyrus Gray Matter
20 7.42 0.45 36 9.33 0.36 28 9.28 0.38 0.0005 0.008

Dorsolateral Prefrontal-Basal Ganglia-Thalamocortical: DLPFC

SE: Standard Error

*

All volumes in mL.

**

Benjamini-Hochberg stepwise testing procedure used to keep overall false discovery rate for circuit and non-circuit regions at p<0.05.

Although the deficit group generally had smaller gray matter volumes, there were no significant effects of diagnosis for any cortical or subcortical circuit component.

3.1.2 Non-circuit Volumes (see Tables 2 and 3)

Table 3.

Comparison of Region Volumes by Deficit, Nondeficit, and Healthy Comparison Group.*

Brain Region Deficit versus Healthy Comparison Deficit versus Nondeficit Nondeficit versus Healthy
Comparison
Difference SE t df p Difference SE t df p Difference SE t df p
Superior
Frontal Gyrus
Gray Matter
−3.25 1.17 2.77 78.1 0.007 −2.57 1.10 2.34 78 0.02 −0.68 1.0 0.68 78 0.50
Superior
Temporal
Gyrus Gray
Matter
−1.84 0.75 2.44 79.0 0.02 −1.63 0.70 2.33 79.0 0.02 −0.21 0.64 0.32 79.0 0.75
Middle
Temporal
Gyrus Gray
Matter
−1.86 0.53 3.50 79.1 0.0008 −1.90 0.49 3.86 78.6 0.0002 0.05 0.45 −0.11 78.9 0.91
Amygdala-
Hippocampal
Complex
−0.73 0.21 3.54 78.4 0.0007 −0.24 0.19 1.27 78.1 0.21 −0.48 0.18 2.73 78.3 0.008

SE: Standard Error

df: Degrees of Freedom (calculated by Kenward-Rogers method).

*

All volumes in mL.

There was a significant main effect of diagnostic group for the superior frontal gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus, and the AHC; and a trend for the superior temporal gyrus.

The superior frontal and superior and middle temporal gyral gray matter volume findings were unique to the deficit group. All volumes were smaller in the deficit group compared to the nondeficit and healthy comparison groups. There was no difference between the nondeficit and healthy comparison groups in these volumes. The effect on AHC volume was a general schizophrenia finding; both patient group volumes were smaller than the healthy comparison group, but not different from each other.

4. Discussion

In contrast to our a priori hypothesis, we found little evidence for altered DLPFC circuit component volumes in people with deficit schizophrenia. Instead, we found that people with deficit schizophrenia were differentiated from nondeficit schizophrenia and healthy comparison groups by smaller superior frontal and superior and middle temporal gyral volumes. These smaller volumes were observed in the absence of significant group differences in TBV or TCV, which suggests these abnormalities are not merely a function of overall decreased brain volume, but are specific to these cortical regions.

The finding of smaller temporal gyral volumes in people with deficit schizophrenia is counter to our a priori hypothesis. However, several neuropsychological studies have found that people with deficit schizophrenia do as poorly (or worse) on tests involving temporal cortex as they do on those tests sensitive to frontal or parietal lobe function (Cohen et al, 2007). Functional neuroimaging studies have also shown diminished blood flow and metabolism in the temporal region of people with deficit schizophrenia compared to both healthy controls and nondeficit participants (Gonul et al, 2003; Heckers et al, 1999).

Moreover, other structural MRI studies have reported temporal lobe abnormalities in deficit schizophrenia. In an ROI study, in contrast to the nondeficit and healthy comparison groups, the deficit group had increased left temporal lobe CSF volume (Turetsky et al, 1995). There were no deficit/nondeficit frontal lobe volume differences. Using VBM, Sigmundsson et al (2001) found decreased gray and white matter signal intensity in the left superior temporal gyrus and medial temporal lobe in deficit schizophrenia versus a healthy comparison group. However, they did not include a nondeficit group for comparison. In a second VBM study, Galderisi et al (2008) found that temporal lobe signal intensity was decreased in both schizophrenia groups compared to controls, with greater decrease in right temporal lobe signal intensity in the deficit versus the nondeficit group. The schizophrenia groups did not differ on frontal lobe signal intensity. Finally, Cascella et al (2010) recently reported a VBM study in which bilaterally decreased superior frontal and superior temporal gyri signal intensity distinguished deficit from nondeficit schizophrenia.

There are several limitations to this study. First, in light of selecting the temporal cortex as a comparison region, we did not anticipate the need to examine the planum temporale; which would have been interesting given our temporal cortical findings. In addition, although our method of assessing cortical volume is generally sound, the potential for slight inaccuracies increases with atrophy (Buchanan et al, 2004). Finally, although the proportion of participants exposed to FGAs, SGAs, and clozapine were not different between schizophrenia groups, we did not control for lifetime antipsychotic dose exposure. In primate models, exposure to clinical levels of antipsychotics results in decreased cortical brain weights (Dorph-Petersen et al, 2005), which may also be the case in humans for some volumetric measurements (Ho et al, 2011). While unlikely, it is possible the deficit group was exposed to higher lifetime antipsychotic dosing, accounting for observed volume differences.

In summary, we found deficit symptoms to be uniquely associated with smaller superior frontal and middle temporal gray matter volumes. In light of the hypothesized functions of these cortical regions, the results suggest that future studies should examine whether there is selective involvement of the neural circuits that underlie decision-making (Paulus et al, 2001), language (Martin, 2003), and theory of mind (Carrington and Bailey, 2009) in people with the deficit form of schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank James M. Gold, Ph.D. and James A. Waltz, Ph.D. for comments on early versions of the manuscript. We would also like to thank the participants of this study for their time and effort.

Role of Funding Source

Funding for this study was provided by an NIMH FIRST Award, MH48225, to Dr. Buchanan. The NIMH had no further role in the study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Dr. Arango has consulted for or received research support from: Astra-Zeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Janssen, Lundbeck, Merck, Pfizer, Roche, Otsuka, Servier. Dr. McMahon has consulted for Amgen. Dr. Kirkpatrick has consulted for or received research support from: Astra-Zeneca, Abbott, Boehringer Ingelheim, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Eli Lilly, Pfizer, Organon, Solvay, Wyeth. Dr. Carpenter has consulted for or received research support from: Astra-Zeneca, Bristol Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Merck (Schering-Plough), and Shire. Dr. Buchanan has served as a DSMB member for Cephalon, Otsuka, and Pfizer, has consulted with Abbott, Amgen, Cypress Bioscience, Glaxo-Smith-Kline, Merck, Sanofi-Aventis, Solvay, Takeda, and Wyeth, and has served on the following Advisory Boards: Abbott; Amgen; Astellas; Astra-Zeneca; Merck; Pfizer; Roche; Solvay Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Wyeth.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflict of Interest

Drs. Fischer, Keller, and Pearlson, Mr. Meyer, and Mr. Francis report no conflicts.

Contributors

Drs. Buchanan, Arango, Kirkpatrick, Carpenter, and Pearlson designed the study and wrote the protocol. Dr. Buchanan was responsible for supervising the MRI morphometric assessments and Mr. Francis had primary responsibility for conducting these assessments. Dr. McMahon and Mr. Meyer managed the statistical analysis. Drs. Buchanan, Fischer, Keller, and McMahon interpreted the data. Drs. Fischer and Keller managed the literature search and Dr. Fischer wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

References

  1. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, "prefrontal" and "limbic" functions. Prog Brain Res. 1990;85:119–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arango C, McMahon RP, Lefkowitz DM, Pearlson G, Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW. Patterns of cranial, brain and sulcal CSF volumes in male and female deficit and nondeficit patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2008;162(2):91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barta PE, Dhingra L, Royall R, Schwartz E. Improving sterological estimates for the volume of structures identified in three-dimensional arrays of spatial data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1997;75(2):111–118. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(97)00049-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  5. Buchanan RW, Kirkpatrick B, Heinrichs DW, Carpenter WT., Jr Clinical correlates of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147(3):290–294. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.3.290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Buchanan RW, Breier A, Kirkpatrick B, Elkashef A, Munson RC, Gellad F, et al. Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150(1):59–65. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buchanan RW, Strauss ME, Kirkpatrick B, Holstein C, Breier A, Carpenter WT., Jr Neuropsychological Impairments in Deficit vs Nondeficit Forms of Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(10):804–811. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950100052005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buchanan RW, Strauss M, Breier A, Kirkpatrick B, Carpenter W., Jr Attentional impairments in deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(3):363–370. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.3.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buchanan RW, Vladar K, Barta PE, Pearlson GD. A structural evaluation of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155(8):1049–1055. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buchanan RW, Francis A, Arango C, Miller K, Lefkowitz DM, McMahon RP, et al. Morphometric assessment of the heteromodal association cortex in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(2):322–331. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ. Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(8):2313–2335. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cascella NG, Fieldstone SC, Rao VA, Pearlson GD, Sawa A, Schretlen DJ. Gray-matter abnormalities in deficit schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2010;120(1–3):63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen AS, Saperstein AM, Gold JM, Kirkpatrick B, Carpenter WT, Jr, Buchanan RW. Neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome: new data and meta-analysis of findings to date. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2007;33(5):1201–1212. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dorph-Petersen K-A, Pierri JN, Perel JM, Sun Z, Sampson AR, Lewis DA. The Influence of Chronic Exposure to Antipsychotic Medications on Brain Size before and after Tissue Fixation: A Comparison of Haloperidol and Olanzapine in Macaque Monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(9):1649–1661. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Galderisi S, Quarantelli M, Volpe U, Mucci A, Cassano GB, Invernizzi G, et al. Patterns of structural MRI abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(2):393–401. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gonul AS, Kula M, Esel E, Tutus A, Sofuoglu S. A Tc-99m HMPAO SPECT study of regional cerebral blood flow in drug-free schizophrenic patients with deficit and non-deficit syndrome. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2003;123(3):199–205. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00067-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Heckers S, Goff D, Schacter DL, Savage CR, Fischman AJ, Alpert NM, et al. Functional Imaging of Memory Retrieval in Deficit vs Nondeficit Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1117–1123. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ho B-C, Andreasen NC, Ziebell S, Pierson R, Magnotta V. Long-term Antipsychotic Treatment and Brain Volumes: A Longitudinal Study of First-Episode Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(2):128–137. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, McKenney PD, Alphs LD, Carpenter WT., Jr The Schedule for the Deficit syndrome: an instrument for research in schizophrenia. Psychiatry research. 1989;30(2):119–123. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90153-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, Ross DE, Carpenter WT., Jr A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58(2):165–171. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lahti AC, Holcomb HH, Medoff DR, Weiler MA, Tamminga CA, Carpenter WT., Jr Abnormal patterns of regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia with primary negative symptoms during an effortful auditory recognition task. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1797–1808. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Martin RC. Language Processing: Functional Organization and Neuroanatomical Basis. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54(1):55. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Paulus MP, Hozack N, Zauscher B, McDowell JE, Frank L, Brown GG, et al. Prefrontal, Parietal, and Temporal Cortex Networks Underlie Decision-Making in the Presence of Uncertainty. NeuroImage. 2001;13(1):91–100. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rademacher J, Caviness VS, Jr, Steinmetz H, Galaburda AM. Topographical variation of the human primary cortices: implications for neuroimaging, brain mapping, and neurobiology. Cereb Cortex. 1993;3(4):313–329. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.4.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Sigmundsson T, Suckling J, Maier M, Williams S, Bullmore E, Greenwood K, et al. Structural abnormalities in frontal, temporal, and limbic regions and interconnecting white matter tracts in schizophrenic patients with prominent negative symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(2):234–243. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Tamminga CA, Thaker GK, Buchanan R, Kirkpatrick B, Alphs LD, Chase TN, et al. Limbic system abnormalities identified in schizophrenia using positron emission tomography with fluorodeoxyglucose and neocortical alterations with deficit syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992;49(7):522–530. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820070016003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Turetsky B, Cowell PE, Gur RC, Grossman RI, Shtasel DL, Gur RE. Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia. Relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1061–1070. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240079013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES