Abstract
Background
Knowledge of depression-related disturbances in cognitive functioning is advancing, but little is known about the cognitive response to treatment for major depression, especially in younger adults. This study investigated the deficits in multiple cognitive domains in middle-aged patients with major depressive disorder (MDD), using a prospective follow-up study design.
Methods
The sample consisted of 13 medication-free MDD patients and 13 education- and age-matched healthy controls. All subjects were administered clinical measures as well as a comprehensive neurocognitive test battery aimed at assessing multiple cognitive domains at the time of recruitment. Patients remitted after 6 months following treatment repeated the neurocognitive assessment.
Results
There were significant differences between the depressed subjects and controls at baseline. MDD patients with remitted symptoms still showed significant deficits in executive function and motor function, but not in memory or attention domains. Patients had significant improvement in memory and attention domains only, once their depressive symptoms had subsided; while executive functioning as well as motor functioning remained unchanged.
Conclusions
Executive functioning and motor functioning deficits might be stable vulnerability indicators for MDD, and memory and attention impairment might serve as state-dependent indicators for MDD.
Keywords
Major depressive disorder; Remission; Residual; Cognitive deficits; Follow-up
Introduction
Depression-related disturbances in cognitive function have been demonstrated in a range of domains [1,2]. However, it is still unclear if the cognitive impairment is a state characteristic of major depressive disorder (MDD), or whether the cognitive deficit persists upon recovery from depressive episodes. The findings across previous studies have varied somewhat, but suggest that a substantial proportion of patients with a history of depression exhibit persistent cognitive impairment, particularly in memory [3], attention [4], and executive functioning [5]. The use of testing before and after recovery is a potentially more powerful method of identifying and distinguishing state- from trait-related cognitive deficits [6]. Using this design, a small number of studies have reported improvement in cognitive functioning, such as memory [7-11], attention [10], executive function [9,11], and psychomotor function [9], after treatment. In contrast, some authors found that residual cognitive deficits involving memory [12-14], executive function [10,13], attention [13,15], and psychomotor function [10,12], persisted upon clinical recovery.
The controversial results may due to some methodological limitations in these longitudinal studies. For example, using inadequate definitions of recovery [7,8,15], failing to show that task performance was within the normal range at recovery [9,11], not controlling for the potential effects of age and educational level [9,11], and not controlling for the potential effects of medication and electroconvulsive therapy [9,10,14,15]. In addition, the majority of these studies focused on elderly individuals [10-13]. Although knowledge of residual cognitive dysfunction in elderly MDD patients is increasing, significant gaps remain in our understanding of the cognitive response to treatment in younger adult depression.
The present study investigated the deficits in multiple cognitive domains of adults with depression with a prospective follow-up, case-control design. The main objective was to examine whether cognitive deficits in MDD patients improve with successful treatment of depression, as well as to determine stable versus state-dependent markers of MDD.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and design
This was a prospective follow-up, case-control study. Thirteen patients (aged 18-50 years) with a diagnosis of non-psychotic, unipolar MDD from the DSM-IV, were recruited. In addition, they had scored at least 16 on the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D-17) [16] and were entirely psychotropic medication-free currently and for at least six weeks before recruitment. Subjects were excluded if they had a history of any neurological or major medical illness, or significant alcohol or substance abuse. Thirteen education- and age-matched healthy controls were recruited from the community by advertisement. These volunteers were interviewed by a senior psychiatrist using the Chinese version of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [17] to exclude individuals with mental illnesses.
Severity of depression was rated using the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D-17) for all subjects. A comprehensive neurocognitive test battery, including the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) [18], Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) [19], Continuous Performance Test (CPT) [20], and Finger Tapping Test (FTT) [21], was used to assess memory, attention, executive functioning, and motor functioning domains, respectively. The battery was administered at the time of recruitment as a baseline measure (assessment I). The same battery was used with the group of patients whose depressive symptoms had remitted (scoring less than 7 on the HAM-D) after six months of treatment (assessment II). During the follow-up period, patients received a standard treatment protocol with SSRI only. All subjects signed an informed consent statement. The Ethical Committee for Human Research of local hospital had approved the study protocol.
Data analyses
Rates and proportion with a two-tailed 95% CI were calculated for the variables of interest. Cross-table chi-square analysis was used to compare sex, education level, and marital status of the MDD patients and healthy controls. The significance of differences in cognitive performance between the two groups was calculated with the Mann-Whitney Test. The significance of differences in the cognitive measures of the MDD subjects between assessment I and assessment II was tested using a Wilcoxon signed ranks test. All data were analyzed using the SPSS for Windows package, Release 10.0.
Results
The demographic profiles and HAM-D scores for the MDD patients and controls are shown in Table 1. The average age of the patients was 37.2 years. There was no significant difference in gender, education level, or age between the patients and control groups.
Table 1. Demographic data and HAM-D scores for MDD patients and controls.
MDD patients |
Normal controls |
‡Z | p value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean ± SD | N | Mean ± SD | N | |||
Sex | 0.69 | |||||
Male | 4 | 6 | ||||
Female | 9 | 7 | ||||
Education level | 0.22 | |||||
Below junior high school | 3 | 0 | ||||
Above junior high school | 10 | 13 | ||||
Age | 37.19 ± 12.62 | 13 | 38.92 ± 12.61 | 13 | -0.51 | 0.61 |
†HAM-D-17 | 23.23 ± 6.10 | 13 | 0.38 ± 0.51 | 13 | -4.41 | < 0.01 |
†HAM-D-17:17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
‡Mann-Whitney Test was used to compare the ages and HAM-D scores in both groups
Cross table chi-square was used to compare the sex and education level
The HAM-D scores and neurocognitive measures for the MDD patients and controls are shown in Table 2. Compared to the controls, the depressed patients were significantly impaired in several cognitive domains, including memory (as measured by the WMS-R), executive function (as measured by the WCST), motor function (as measured by the FTT), and attention domains (as measured by the CPT) at assessment I. MDD patients with remitted symptoms still showed significant deficits in executive function and motor function, but not in the memory or attention domains at assessment II.
Table 2. HAM-D scores and neurocognitive measures for MDD patients and controls.
MDD Assessment I |
MDD Assessment II |
Normal Controls |
‡Assessment I | ‡Assessment II | § Assessment I | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
vs. | vs. | vs. | ||||||||||
Normal Controls |
Normal Controls |
Assessment II |
||||||||||
Mean ± SD | N | Mean ± SD | N | Mean ± SD | N | Z | p value | Z | Sig. | Z | p value | |
†HAM-D-17 | 23.23 ± 6.10 | 13 | 2.38 ± 2.84 | 13 | 0.38 ± 0.51 | 13 | -4.41 | < 0.01 | -1.91 | 0.06 | -3.18 | < 0.01 |
†WMS-R | ||||||||||||
Verbal MEM. | 81.33 ± 16.90 | 13 | 90.38 ± 15.23 | 13 | 96.18 ± 9.26 | 13 | -2.53 | < 0.05 | -1.54 | 0.13 | -2.33 | < 0.05 |
Visual MEM. | 78.92 ± 22.63 | 13 | 106.13 ± 20.99 | 13 | 111.18 ± 16.56 | 13 | -2.93 | < 0.01 | -0.74 | 0.46 | -2.52 | < 0.05 |
General MEM. | 76.08 ± 22.27 | 13 | 93.75 ± 14.92 | 13 | 100.55 ± 11.03 | 13 | -2.90 | < 0.01 | -1.49 | 0.14 | -2.38 | < 0.05 |
Attention/Concentration | 91.75 ± 18.22 | 13 | 99.70 ± 16.79 | 13 | 104.00 ± 14.13 | 13 | -2.34 | < 0.05 | -1.14 | 0.25 | -2.02 | < 0.05 |
Delayed Recall | 82.42 ± 22.67 | 13 | 102.25 ± 17.69 | 13 | 104.00 ± 15.40 | 13 | -2.40 | < 0.05 | -0.37 | 0.71 | -2.38 | < 0.05 |
†FTT | ||||||||||||
Dominant hand | 30.69 ± 15.03 | 13 | 31.99 ± 16.46 | 13 | 59.24 ± 10.85 | 13 | -3.59 | < 0.01 | -3.3.2 | <0.01 | -1.61 | 0.11 |
†WCST | ||||||||||||
Perseverative Errors | 10.50 ± 4.36 | 13 | 9.42 ± 3.40 | 13 | 4.50 ± 7.12 | 13 | -2.37 | < 0.05 | -2.10 | <0.05 | -1.53 | 0.13 |
†CPT | ||||||||||||
d’ Masked | 3.01 ± 0.94 | 13 | 3.47 ± 0.93 | 13 | 3.88 ± 0.64 | 13 | -2.32 | < 0.05 | -1.00 | 0.32 | -2.51 | < 0.05 |
†HAM-D-17:17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; WMS-R: Wechsler Memory ScaleRevised; CPT: Continuous Performance Test; FTT: Finger Tapping Test; WCST: Wisconsin Card-Sorting test
‡Mann-Whitney Test
§Wilcoxon signed ranks test
Discussion
Our study demonstrated that remitted depressed patients with baseline cognitive impairment had experienced improvement in specific cognitive domains, i.e., memory as well as attention, and could reach the same performance levels as the controls. This is consistent with a number of recent studies of major depression [7,8]. This result echoed the findings of studies of elderly patients by Butters et al [11] and Beats et al [10]. In contrast, Neu et al assessed middle-aged depressed patients with a melancholic subtype and reported that after sustained remission [14], the patient group still performed significantly worse in verbal memory and verbal fluency compared to the controls. Other studies found persistent memory or attention dysfunction more often, and found these phenomena in older patients, mostly with endogenous depression [2].
The other finding in our study was that cognitive impairment in the executive functioning and motor functioning domains persisted even after the depressed symptoms had recovered. This result adds to the body of evidence that MDD may yield some residual cognitive deficits even with successful treatment [5,15]. Beats et al investigated elderly depressed patients on tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction [10], and found that measures of simple and choice reaction times, perseveration on the set-shifting task, and verbal fluency did not fully recover. Nebes et al also found that decreased working memory and processing speed mediated cognitive impairment in elderly patients [12]. The processing resource decrement persisted after remission of the depression, and thus may be a trait marker of geriatric depression. Some authors suggest that working memory deficits in depression are due to persistent deficits in selective attention (state-independence) have been related to persistent abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex [4,15]. Furthermore, the findings across studies support the hypothesis that major depression is frequently associated with cognitive dysfunction and that the underlying defect is in the subcortical/frontal lobe neural circuitry, i.e., the frontostriatal dysfunction [10,22,23]. However, studies that did find an association between lesions and cognitive function more often found this phenomenon in depressives, mostly with a late-onset of the illness [23-26]. Although microvascular pathology may account for the persistent cognitive impairment seen in elderly patients with depression [27], the contribution of such a process to the residual cognitive impairment in younger depressives remains uncertain.
In sum, our study implies that there might be a specific profile of cognitive deficits as stable vulnerability indicators for MDD, and some other cognitive impairment may serve as state-dependent indicators for MDD. A generalization of our results must be viewed with caution, considering the relatively small sample size. Clearly, further researches need to be undertaken with a larger sample size and more parameters to clarify the specific cognitive deficit of remitted MDD patients.
References
- 1.Austin MP, Ross M, Murray C, O'Carroll RE, Ebmeier KP, Goodwin GM. Cognitive function in major depression. J Affect Disord. 1992;25(1):21–29. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(92)90089-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Porter RJ, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Young AH. Neurocognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2003;182:214–220. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.3.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.MacQueen GM, Galway TM, Hay J, Young LT, Joffe RT. Recollection memory deficits in patients with major depressive disorder predicted by past depressions but not current mood state or treatment status. Psychol Med. 2002;32(2):251–258. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701004834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Blumberg HP, Leung HC, Skudlarski P, Lacadie CM, Fredericks CA, Harris BC, Charney DS. et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of bipolar disorder: state- and trait-related dysfunction in ventral prefrontal cortices. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(6):601–609. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Roiser JP, Rubinsztein JS, Sahakian BJ. Abingdon. Hamilton (ON): Medicine Publishing Company; [Google Scholar]
- 6.Austin MP, Mitchell P, Goodwin GM. Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178:200–206. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.3.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Peselow ED, Corwin J, Fieve RR, Rotrosen J, Cooper TB. Disappearance of memory deficits in outpatient depressives responding to imipramine. J Affect Disord. 1991;21(3):173–183. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(91)90038-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bazin N, Perruchet P, De Bonis M, Feline A. The dissociation of explicit and implicit memory in depressed patients. Psychol Med. 1994;24(1):239–245. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700027008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tarbuck AF, Paykel ES. Effects of major depression on the cognitive function of younger and older subjects. Psychol Med. 1995;25(2):285–295. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700036187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Beats BC, Sahakian BJ, Levy R. Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol Med. 1996;26(3):591–603. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Butters MA, Becker JT, Nebes RD, Zmuda MD, Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, Reynolds CF, 3rd. Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(12):1949–1954. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.12.1949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nebes RD, Butters MA, Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, Zmuda MD, Houck PR, Reynolds CF, 3rd. Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Psychol Med. 2000;30(3):679–691. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Portella MJ, Marcos T, Rami L, Navarro V, Gasto C, Salamero M. Residual cognitive impairment in late-life depression after a 12-month period follow-up. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;18(7):571–576. doi: 10.1002/gps.895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Neu P, Bajbouj M, Schilling A, Godemann F, Berman RM, Schlattmann P. Cognitive function over the treatment course of depression in middle-aged patients: correlation with brain MRI signal hyperintensities. J Psychiatr Res. 2005;39(2):129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Trichard C, Martinot JL, Alagille M, Masure MC, Hardy P, Ginestet D, Feline A. Time course of prefrontal lobe dysfunction in severely depressed in-patients: a longitudinal neuropsychological study. Psychol Med. 1995;25(1):79–85. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700028105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6(4):278–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T. et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 20):22–33. quiz 34-57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wechsler DA. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psychological Corp, New York 1987. 1987 [Google Scholar]
- 19.Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc, Odessa, FL. 1993 [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gordon M. Microprocessor-based assessment of attention deficit disorders (ADD) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1986;22(1):288–290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: theory and clinical interpretation. 2nd ed. Tuscon, AZ: Neuropsychology Press; 1993. Chapter 24, Regression and correlation methods; pp. 447–486. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kiosses DN, Alexopoulos GS, Murphy C. Symptoms of striatofrontal dysfunction contribute to disability in geriatric depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15(11):992–999. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<992::aid-gps248>3.0.co;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Aizenstein HJ, Butters MA, Figurski JL, Stenger VA, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Carter CS. Prefrontal and striatal activation during sequence learning in geriatric depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(4):290–296. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kramer-Ginsberg E, Greenwald BS, Krishnan KR, Christiansen B, Hu J, Ashtari M, Patel M. et al. Neuropsychological functioning and MRI signal hyperintensities in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(3):438–444. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.3.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Naismith S, Hickie I, Ward PB, Turner K, Scott E, Little C, Mitchell P. et al. Caudate nucleus volumes and genetic determinants of homocysteine metabolism in the prediction of psychomotor speed in older persons with depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(12):2096–2098. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tupler LA, Krishnan KR, McDonald WM, Dombeck CB, D'Souza S, Steffens DC. Anatomic location and laterality of MRI signal hyperintensities in late-life depression. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(2):665–676. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00425-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kales HC, Maixner DF, Mellow AM. Cerebrovascular disease and late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13(2):88–98. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.2.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]