Abstract
In humans, heterogeneity in the decline of hippocampal-dependent episodic memory is observed during aging. Rodents have been employed as models of age-related cognitive decline and the spatial water maze has been used to show variability in the emergence and extent of impaired hippocampal-dependent memory. Impairment in the consolidation of intermediate-term memory for rapidly acquired and flexible spatial information emerges early, in middle-age. As aging proceeds, deficits may broaden to include impaired incremental learning of a spatial reference memory. The extent and time course of impairment has been be linked to senescence of calcium (Ca2+) regulation and Ca2+-dependent synaptic plasticity mechanisms in region CA1. Specifically, aging is associated with altered function of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs), voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels (VDCCs), and ryanodine receptors (RyRs) linked to intracellular Ca2+ stores (ICS). In young animals, NMDAR activation induces long-term potentiation of synaptic transmission (NMDAR-LTP), which is thought to mediate the rapid consolidation of intermediate-term memory. Oxidative stress, starting in middle-age, reduces NMDAR function. In addition, VDCCs and ICS can actively inhibit NMDAR-dependent LTP and oxidative stress enhances the role of VDCC and RyR-ICS in regulating synaptic plasticity. Blockade of L-type VDCCs promotes NMDAR-LTP and memory in older animals. Interestingly, pharmacological or genetic manipulations to reduce hippocampal NMDAR function readily impair memory consolidation or rapid learning, generally leaving incremental learning intact. Finally, evidence is mounting to indicate a role for VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity in associative learning and the consolidation of remote memories. Thus, VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity and extrahippocampal systems may contribute to incremental learning deficits observed with advanced aging.
Keywords: episodic memory, incremental learning, long-term potentiation, aging
1. Introduction
Animal models of age-related cognitive decline are employed to mimic features of human senescence in order to assist in identifying the molecular mechanisms and development of potential therapeutics. For each model, the appropriateness will depend on a number of factors including translational relevance, background genetics, availability of aged animals, and type of experimental manipulations involved. In order to enhance the translational relevance, it is important that the model has face validity, exhibiting similar symptoms relative to the human condition. Aging in humans is associated with a weakening of working memory, executive function, and processing speed; however, the most notable decline is observed as impaired episodic memory, including spatial memory (Kukolja et al., 2009; Plancher et al., 2010; Uttl and Graf, 1993). The impairment can be identified as a mild deficit in the rapid acquisition of flexible information and an increase in the rate of forgetting (Davis et al., 2003; Hogge et al., 2008; Huppert and Kopelman, 1989; Kral, 1962; Macdonald et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 1990; Park et al., 1988; Rajah et al., 2010).
A second important aspect of age-related cognitive decline is that chronological age alone does not predict cognitive health. There is enormous variability in cognitive function across the life span from “successful” aging with little memory decline, through memory deficits associated with “unsuccessful” aging, to cognitive changes linked to dementia and neurodegeneration (Foster, 2006; Glisky, 2007). Notably, the deterioration of memory function in humans is progressive (Christensen et al., 1999; Colsher and Wallace, 1991; Mungas et al., 2010; Schonknecht et al., 2005; Zelinski and Burnight, 1997) and an increase in forgetting may be an early sign of impaired synaptic transmission or an evolving neurodegeneration (Dierckx et al., 2010; Gagnon and Belleville, 2010). Indeed, episodic memory deficits are symptomatic of impaired hippocampal function and the progressive decline in episodic memory is associated with a decrease in hippocampal volume (Kramer et al., 2007; Mueller et al., 2007; Mungas et al., 2005; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2010; Sexton et al., 2010; Stoub et al., 2008). The question remains as to what mechanisms underlie different trajectories for memory decline and the heterogeneity in memory function in older individuals.
Rodents offer several benefits as models for investigation of the mechanisms and potential treatment of age-related cognitive decline. Similar to humans, information that requires hippocampal processing is particularly vulnerable to age. Furthermore, the memory deficit can be observed as a delay-dependent increase in the rate of forgetting (Dunnett et al., 1988; Eichenbaum et al., 2010; Forster and Lal, 1992; Lal et al., 1973; Mabry et al., 1996; Martinez et al., 1988; Winocur, 1988; Zornetzer et al., 1982). Importantly, rodents are not subject to neurodegenerative diseases, such that cognitive decline is thought to result from aging of physiological processes rather than cell death (Baxter and Gallagher, 1996; Foster, 2006; Rapp and Amaral, 1992). Finally, like humans, rodents exhibit wide-ranging heterogeneity in the extent of memory decline. This variability can be used to better define the process of cognitive senescence and investigate age-related changes in biological mechanisms critical to episodic memory.
In order to take full advantage of the translational relevance of animal models, consideration of the behavioral tasks employed is critical in characterizing the cognitive processes under consideration and limiting confounding influences or alternative hypotheses. In the absence of dementia, aged humans and rodents exhibit a preservation of non-hippocampal processes including incremental stimulus-response learning and procedural memory (Churchill et al., 2003), as well as intact remote memories (Winocur et al., 2008). Most studies in animals describe the probability of cognitive impairment with advancing age. However, it is unclear whether cognitive decline is progressive in rodents and there is a lack of longitudinal studies tracking the course of altered cognitive function. Nevertheless, the existing literature provides a rich source from which to draw information for understanding the trajectory and extent of deficits in episodic memory. This literature provides evidence to indicate that the onset of memory decline emerges in middle-age. Furthermore, cognitive decline appears to be progressive, such that impairment in delay-dependent memory for rapidly acquired and flexible spatial information may advance to more severe deficits observed as impaired short-term memory and an inability to acquire a spatial reference memory through incremental learning.
The extent of age-related cognitive decline is highly variable. Therefore, behavioral tasks should be sensitive enough to detect mild deficits, identify the emergence of memory impairments in middle-age, and distinguish the severity of impairment in order to examine biological mechanisms. The sensitivity of the tasks for identifying age-related memory impairments will depend on the parameters of the task. Several hippocampal-dependent tasks can be designed in a manner that will permit the identification of memory deficits in middle-aged animals. Furthermore, many of these tasks suggest sex differences in the extent or the onset of cognitive decline. A short list of tasks would include olfactory memory (Roman et al., 1996; Taylor et al., 1999), contextual memory during fear conditioning (Kaczorowski and Disterhoft, 2009; Moyer and Brown, 2006), spatial working memory examined on the radial arm maze (Dellu-Hagedorn et al., 2004; Granholm et al., 2008; Sabolek et al., 2004) however see (Dellu et al., 1997; Jacobson et al., 2008; Oler and Markus, 1998), performance on various spatial mazes (Fouquet et al., 2009; Ingram, 1988; Kametani et al., 1989), and retention of inhibitory/passive avoidance (Benice et al., 2006; Moretti et al., 2011; Paris et al., 2010; Samorajski et al., 1985). Tests that examine recognition of novel objects or object locations have reported mixed results (Benice et al., 2006; Blalock et al., 2003; Fahlstrom et al., 2009; Paris et al., 2010), possibly due to procedures that render the task more or less hippocampal-dependent (Broadbent et al., 2009; Hammond et al., 2004). By far the most widely used task is the spatial version of the water escape task. Importantly, this task can be designed to identify deficits that emerge in middle-age (~12–14 months) and increase with advancing age (Adams et al., 2008; Bizon et al., 2009; Blalock et al., 2003; Davis et al., 1993; Driscoll et al., 2006; Foster et al., 2003; Francia et al., 2006; Granholm et al., 2008; Lindner, 1997).
2. Sensitivity of the water maze task
2.1 Procedures
Several reviews have discussed the proper procedures for employing the Morris water maze (Brandeis et al., 1989; Vorhees and Williams, 2006) and factors that must be considered when examining aged animals including stress, fatigue, and sensory-motor deficits (Foster, 1999; van der Staay, 2002). Some measures, such as latency to find the platform, are poor indicators of cognitive function since latency can be influenced by an age-related decline in swim speed (Foster et al., 2001; Norris and Foster, 1999). Normally, a cue discrimination task is used in order to identify sensory-motor or motivational deficits which would impede acquisition of a spatial search strategy. In addition to identifying animals with sensory-motor deficits, training on the cue discrimination task can insure that animals learn the procedural aspects of the task, including how to swim and the fact that the pool wall is not a route of escape (Vorhees and Williams, 2006). Consequently, animals may perform better on the spatial version of the task when they are first trained on the cue task. Similarly, when animals are initially trained on the spatial version of the task, they may perform relatively poorly due to a number of factors, but exhibit superior performance on subsequent cue training (Gerlai, 2001). Accordingly, extensive training on the spatial swim maze may mask differences in procedural learning. Alternatively, a correspondence between performance on the spatial swim task and cue discrimination task may indicate more global impairments and correlations between the tasks have been used as an indication of the extent of pathology in mouse models of neurodegenerative disease (Arendash and King, 2002; Leighty et al., 2004).
Probe trials are employed to evaluate the acquisition and retention of a spatial search strategy. For the probe trial, the platform is removed from the pool and the animal is allowed to freely explore the maze for a set time (e.g. 60 sec). During the probe trial, the distance from the previous platform location is averaged or summed over the trial to obtain a proximity score (Carter et al., 2009; Gallagher et al., 1993; Maei et al., 2009). In some cases, the number of times the animal crosses the previous location of the platform (platform crossings) is reported. However, this measure may be flawed for comparing across groups. A decrease in the number of platform crossings can result from differences in motor ability and young animals make quick sharp turns, while aged animals make more sweeping turns resulting in a reduction in the number of crossings and reduced variability of the measure (Clayton et al., 2002; Devan et al., 1996; Foster et al., 2001).
Measures of the percent time searching the goal quadrant provide evidence for the use of a spatial search strategy. A probe trial delivered shortly after training is used to determine whether an animal has acquired information on the location of the escape platform, and a subsequent probe trial can be used to access retention (e.g. 24 hr after the acquisition probe trial) (Foster et al., 1991; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Foster et al., 2003; Foster et al., 2001; Norris and Foster, 1999). Animals are considered to have acquired or retained the spatial information if they spend greater than 25% (i.e. chance) of the time searching the goal quadrant. A discrimination index (DI score) can be calculated for probe trials using the time spent searching the goal and opposite quadrants according to the formula DI = (time in goal - time in opposite)/(time in goal + time in opposite). Since the DI score is dependent on discriminating two quadrants, it is less susceptible to influence of motor function. A larger DI score indicates better performance and a score near 0 indicates chance performance. Finally, it is possible that the animals change their search strategy within a probe trial. For example, after initially searching where the platform should be, the animals may then search other quadrants. Alternatively, during retention testing, it may take some time for the animal find their directional bearings and begin to search the correct quadrant. In this case, the probe trial can be broken down in to smaller time segments. Thus, one may want to separately examine the first and second 30 sec of a 60 sec probe trial.
2.2 Sensitivity of the water maze to hippocampal function
In considering the sensitivity of the spatial swim task in detecting impaired hippocampal function, it is important to note that the degree of impairment observed during aging is generally less severe than that observed following hippocampal lesions (Foster, 1999). Animals with damage to hippocampal circuits exhibit impairments in the retention of rapidly acquired spatial information (Martin and Clark, 2007; Morris et al., 1990b; Ordy et al., 1988; Steele and Morris, 1999). In contrast to hippocampal damage, most aged animals can acquire a spatial search strategy during one or a few training sessions and deficits surface as retention delays increase (Bizon et al., 2009; Blalock et al., 2003; Driscoll et al., 2006; Foster et al., 1991; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Foster et al., 2003; Norris and Foster, 1999). Thus, memory deficits probably result from more subtle changes in memory processes such as storage or maintenance mechanisms (Foster, 1999).
Deficits in incremental learning of a spatial reference memory and impaired short-term memory are more common in the oldest animals (Bizon et al., 2009; Frick et al., 1995; Haberman et al., 2009; Schulz et al., 2002). The impairment in incremental learning in older animals may relate to aging of other brain regions. Animals impaired in incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory are more reactive to novel stimuli (Collier et al., 2004; Gallagher and Burwell, 1989; Rowe et al., 1998) and exhibit impaired olfactory learning (Frick et al., 2000; LaSarge et al., 2007; Matzel et al., 2008; Zyzak et al., 1995) suggesting involvement of neocortical systems and neuromodulatory input from the locus coeruleus. Older animals exhibit a sever decrease in catecholamine release (Del Arco et al., 2001; Porras et al., 1997) and a decline in cholinergic responsiveness (Ayyagari et al., 1998; Narang et al., 1996; Schwarz et al., 1990). A decrease in neuromodulatory input, particularly to the prefrontal cortex, is associated with learning and memory deficits (Dunnett, 1990; Tanila et al., 1994; Zhang et al., 2007). However, age-related impairment of prefrontal function is linked to impaired skill or source information related to the task rather than increased forgetting of specific information (Eichenbaum et al., 2010; Winocur and Moscovitch, 1990).
The use of multiple tasks, including cue discrimination, can provide important clues to the possible contribution of extrahippocampal regions. In one study examining Wistar rats, no relationship was observed between incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory and other hippocampal-dependent cognitive tasks; however, rats impaired for incremental learning exhibited increased thigmotaxis on the cue discrimination swim task, indicating impaired acquisition of the procedural aspects of the task (Bergado et al., 2011). Similarly, for mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease, the incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory was correlated with performance on the cue discrimination task (Arendash and King, 2002; Leighty et al., 2004). Furthermore, performance was correlated with beta-amyloid deposition throughout the brain, indicating that deficits likely involved multiple systems.
It might be expected that individual differences would correlate across hippocampal-dependent tasks. However, there are several difficulties in relating performance across tasks including differential influences of sensory-motor processes or sensitivity of the task for cognitive processes linked to learning or memory. Even for tasks that appear to measure the same cognitive process, little relationship may be observed. For example, the ability to acquire a spatial reference memory is sensitive to age for both for the water maze and the Barnes maze; however, there is little predictability between tasks for individual animals (Arendash and King, 2002; Gallagher and Burwell, 1989; Leighty et al., 2004; Markowska et al., 1989).
The lack of correlation between hippocampal-dependent tasks can result from differences in the sensitivity of the tasks and the degree of impairment. In this case the problem of sensitivity is compounded since it requires that both tasks are similarly sensitive to the extent of cognitive decline. There is evidence for a correspondence across tasks, when both tasks focus on retention or memory consolidation (Benice et al., 2006; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Gower and Lamberty, 1993; Paris et al., 2010). Inhibitory avoidance can be designed to be sensitive to the emergence of memory decline during aging (Gold et al., 1981; Martinez et al., 1988). In Lewis (24 month) and Sprague-Dawley (22–24 month) rats, no relationship was observed between impaired retention of inhibitory avoidance and incremental learning on the water maze (Blokland and Raaijmakers, 1993; Markowska et al., 1989). In contrast, aged (26 month) Wistar rats exhibited deficits for retention of inhibitory avoidance and retention of a spatial reference memory, when retention was examined 6 days following the end of training (Miettinen et al., 1993). As detailed below, the sensitivity of the water maze task to retention deficits can be increased by providing a single training session. Retention of inhibitory avoidance was related to retention of spatial memory examined 24 hr following a single training session on the water maze in aged (18–23 month) Fisher 344 (F344) rats (Foster and Kumar, 2007). For this study, retention on the inhibitory avoidance task was not correlated with acquisition of a spatial search strategy or performance on the cue task. In middle-age (14 month) and aged (24 month) F344 rats, impaired retention 24 hr following a single day of water maze training was related to the 24 hr retention performance on a novel object recognition task (Blalock et al., 2003). Retention impairments may emerge earlier in females. In a study of middle-age (12 month) Long-Evans female rats, declining reproductive function was associated with poor retention for the water maze, inhibitory avoidance, and object recognition (Paris et al., 2010). In C57BL/6J mice, impairment on the water maze was related to age and gender, such that impairment for a probe trial delivered 1 hr after training, was greater for aged females (Benice et al., 2006). In addition, this group of aged female mice also exhibited poorer retention of inhibitory avoidance. Again, it must be emphasized that in these cases, the inhibitory avoidance task is sensitive to milder deficits in aging, namely delay-dependent forgetting, and if designed correctly, can detect the emergence of impaired retention in middle-aged animals. However, the utility of the inhibitory avoidance task in detecting individual differences is limited due to ceiling effects and it is not as amendable to repeated training and testing. To differentiate early deficits in intermediate memory from deficits that develop over the course of aging requires a certain level of sophistication in terms of behavioral testing on the water maze.
2.3 Sensitivity and training schedules
2.3.1 Distributed training
Training schedules are important in determining the sensitivity of the swim task for detection of acquisition and retention deficits (Table 1). In general, training is massed into a single session or distributed across several days in order to examine rapid or incremental acquisition of a spatial search strategy, respectively (Fig 1). In mice the incremental learning paradigm is generally employed to examine age effects. However, recently researchers have developed versions of the water maze that focus on rapid acquisition in order to characterize more subtle changes associated with aging, age-related disease, the function of hippocampal subregions, and synaptic plasticity mechanisms (Gulinello et al., 2009; Magnusson et al., 2003; Malleret et al., 2010; Nakashiba et al., 2008).
TABLE 1.
Method | Cognitive Processes | Advantage | Disadvantage | Age of onset (months) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Distributed Training | Incremental learning, spatial reference memory | Sensitive to incremental learning deficits, may better mimic global deficits of dementia | Lack of sensitivity for early detection of memory deficits, deficits may disappear with extended training, may result in chronic stress | 18–24 |
Massed training | Rapid acquisition and intermediate memory | Sensitive to early detection of acquisition and memory deficits | May induce an acute stress | Acquisition 12–18 Memory 12–14 |
Repeated acquisition | Rapid, flexible acquisition | Sensitive to early detection of acquisition deficits for rapidly acquired information | May result in a chronic stress with repeated training | 18–24 |
Delayed match-to- sample | Intermediate memory | Sensitive to early detection of memory deficits for rapidly acquired information | May result in a chronic stress with repeated training | Retention duration decreases with age |
Figure 1.
Performance differences during aging for distributed or massed training on the water maze. A) Schematic illustration of the pool, with an escape platform (grey circle) in the upper right hand quadrant. The line indicates the path length to find the platform, which decreases from the earlier to later training trials. B) During a probe trial, the platform is removed and the search pattern is focused on the area (i.e. goal quadrant) in which the platform was located. C) Hypothetical performance curves of age-related differences in performance for distributed training, modeled from Aitken and Meaney, 1989; Bizon et al., 2009; Clayton et al., 2002; Diana et al., 1995; Driscoll et al., 2006; Gage et al., 1984; Mabry et al., 1996; Miyagawa et al., 1998; Nyffeler et al., 2010; Rapp et al., 1987; and van Groen et al., 2002. Older animals (filled circles) exhibit an initial impairment compared to middle-aged (grey circles) and young (open circles). However, most aged animals can acquire a spatial reference memory following extended distributed training. The saw-tooth pattern of performance in aged animals may be due to forgetting across days (dashed lines). A subset of aged animals (filled triangles) may exhibit profound learning deficits such that they will not be able to acquire a spatial reference memory with repeated training to the same location across several days. D) A probe trial, delivered on alternate days, can be substituted for the last trial of that day. The average distance from the platform (proximity) is recorded and those animals that exhibit distances greater than young (dashed line) are classified as learning impaired. Most middle-aged animals would not be classified as learning impaired. E) Age-related differences in performance for massed training, adapted from Blalock et al., 2003; Foster et al., 1991; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Foster et al., 2003; Mabry et al., 1996; Norris and Foster, 1999. Aged animals exhibit a slower rate of learning when training is massed into a single training session. The acquisition probe trial is delivered near the end of training, followed by three refresher training trials, and a retention probe trial, delivered 24 hrs later. F) Measures of the percent time searching the goal quadrant for the initial probe trial (Acquisition) indicate that most animals have acquired a search strategy, focusing their search in the goal quadrant relative to chance (dashed line). The retention probe trial reveals a subgroup of memory impaired aged and middle aged animals (bracket) that exhibit goal search times that are less than that observed for young.
Training distributed across several days may not be as sensitive to the emergence of deficits in middle-aged rats (Bizon et al., 2009; Harati et al., 2009; Jacobson et al., 2008; Luparini et al., 2000; Oliveira et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2004). For example, little or no difference in acquisition was observed between 6 and 25 month old F344 Brown Norway crossed (F344BN) rats for training distributed across multiple days (Hebda-Bauer et al., 1999; Wu et al., 2004); however, training within a single day was sensitive, revealing learning deficits between 12 and 25 month old F344BN rats (Carter et al., 2009). One study employed distributed training for young, middle-aged, and aged F344 rats and probe trials, delivered at the end of training on alternate days, were used to determine the extent of learning (Bizon et al., 2009). While a significant difference was observed for probe trial measures between young and middle-aged animals, this difference was mainly due to a subgroup of young animals with highly superior performance. If a cut-off was set using the performance in young animals, the vast majority of middle-aged animals exhibited learning measures within the range of young animals and these animals could be considered unimpaired (Fig 1D).
The number of days of training is an important factor in determining the sensitivity of distributed training (Fig 1C). In many cases, older animals can acquire a spatial reference memory following distributed training (Clayton et al., 2002; Miyagawa et al., 1998; Nyffeler et al., 2010; Rapp et al., 1987; van Groen et al., 2002), such that acquisition deficits, that are apparent during the first couple of days of training, disappear after several days of repetitive training to the same location (Clayton et al., 2002; Jacobson et al., 2008; Luparini et al., 2000). For aged (~24 months) Wistar rats, subgroups of impaired and unimpaired rats could be differentiated over the initial two to three days of training (2–6 trials/day) (Fontana et al., 1995; Miyagawa et al., 1998). However, differences were diminished following further training. The diminished age differences with extended or repetitive training may be due to compensation, such that the animals may engage mechanisms involved in slower incremental acquisition of spatial information. Animals with hippocampal lesions can acquire a spatial reference memory following training distributed across several days, indicating extrahippocampal mechanisms involved in incremental learning of a spatial reference memory (Gerlai et al., 2002; Hannesson and Skelton, 1998; Martin and Clark, 2007; Morris et al., 1990b; Steele and Morris, 1999; Stoelzel et al., 2002; Whishaw et al., 1995). Furthermore, these other systems may be more resistant to aging.
The deficits observed early in distributed training may represent impaired memory consolidation processes mediated by the hippocampus (Fig 1C). We have noted that memory consolidation deficits could explain the characteristic saw-toothed pattern of behavior observed for aged rats (Foster, 1999). In this case, older rats exhibit improved performance across trials within a day and impairments are observed for the first trial on the following day (Aitken and Meaney, 1989; Diana et al., 1995; Driscoll et al., 2006; Gage et al., 1984; Mabry et al., 1996; Rapp et al., 1987). This pattern has also been reported to emerge in middle-age (12 month) Long-Evans rats (Aitken and Meaney, 1989), and a similar pattern may be observed for aged male mice (Benice et al., 2006). However, the saw-toothed pattern of memory deficits disappears with extended training, as aged animals incrementally acquire a spatial reference memory (Fig 1C).
Several labs have characterized a subgroup of aged rats that exhibit impaired incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory. The impairment is particularly evident in the oldest animals (28–30 month) or when training consists of one to two trials per day (Bergado et al., 2011; Bizon et al., 2009; Collier et al., 2004; Frick et al., 1995; Gallagher and Burwell, 1989; Ivy et al., 1994; Lindner, 1997; Rowe et al., 1998; Schulz et al., 2002; Yamazaki et al., 1995). The ability to detect memory deficits during aging is enhanced by using a single training event or minimal training massed into a single session (Lal et al., 1973; Vasquez et al., 1983). As such, the sensitivity of distributed training in detecting age differences can be increased by reducing the number of training trials per day. When the number of trials was reduced to one trial per day for 10 days, the majority of F344 rats, 12 months or older, exhibited impaired acquisition, relative to 2 month old animals (Lindner, 1997). In contrast, when the training consisted of 6 trials per day for 5 days, F344 rats 4 to 17 months of age performed similarly after 5 days of training and deficits were only readily apparent for 24 month old rats (Frick et al., 1995). Studies that have employed multiple training trials (usually 4–8 trials per day) have provided evidence that the age at which incremental learning deficits emerge is strain sensitive. For Sprague-Dawley and F344 rats, the proportion of animals classified as impaired increases with advancing age and group differences were readily apparent by ~22–24 months of age (Bizon et al., 2009; Fischer et al., 1992). In contrast, F344BN rats exhibit either minimal impairment or a subgroup exhibit impaired incremental learning at ~24 months and notable deficits are not observed until ~31 months (Barrientos et al., 2006; Hebda-Bauer et al., 1999; Markowska and Savonenko, 2002; Wu et al., 2004). The appearance of deficits at divergent ages across rat strains may be related to differences in life span, which varies from ~25 months for F344 and ~34 months for F344BN rats. Thus, impairment in the ability to gradually acquire spatial knowledge becomes prominent when animals reach ~90% of their average life span (LaSarge and Nicolle, 2009).
As noted above, animals that exhibit impaired incremental learning may exhibit other behavioral differences including reactivity to novel stimuli (Collier et al., 2004; Gallagher and Burwell, 1989; Rowe et al., 1998) and impaired associative learning (Bergado et al., 2011; Frick et al., 2000; LaSarge et al., 2007; Matzel et al., 2008; Schulz et al., 2002; Zyzak et al., 1995) suggesting involvement of extrahippocampal systems. Certainly, for mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease, impaired incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory is correlated with impairment in several behavioral domains as well as the extent of brain beta-amyloid deposition (Arendash and King, 2002; Leighty et al., 2004). The results suggest that the inability to acquire a spatial reference memory following extensive training may portend robust hippocampal dysfunction and/or more global changes which preclude incremental learning (Cho and Jaffard, 1995; Moffat et al., 2007). As such, impaired incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory following distributed training may be a good model of severe cognitive deficits associated with dementia.
2.3.2 Massed training
Due to the fact that learning and/or memory deficits on the water maze are more pronounced during the initial training period, some studies have massed training into a single session in order to increase the sensitivity of the task (Table 1). Memory following massed training is not as strong as memory acquired following distributed training (Commins et al., 2003; Dash et al., 2002; Spreng et al., 2002) and depends more on the hippocampus (Bouffard and Jarrard, 1988). Furthermore, massed training schedules make use of distinct molecular memory mechanisms that are sensitive to aging (Foster et al., 2001; Genoux et al., 2002; Malleret et al., 2001). Again, aged animals may exhibit slower learning during training (Fig 1E). Acquisition of a spatial search strategy following a single training session can be confirmed by a probe trial and calculating the discrimination index or measuring the portion of time spent searching in the quadrant that originally held the escape platform (Fig 1F). This probe trial can be used to detect age-related impairment in the acquisition of a spatial search strategy. Once it is clear that an animal has acquired a spatial search strategy, a subsequent probe trial may be delivered at more distant time points in order to evaluate retention (Fig 1F). Work from our lab and others indicates that a subset of middle-age animals exhibit memory deficits examined 24 hr after acquisition and the probability of memory impairment increases with advancing age (Aitken and Meaney, 1989; Blalock et al., 2003; Driscoll et al., 2006; Foster et al., 1991; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Foster et al., 2003; Mabry et al., 1996; Norris and Foster, 1999).
Virtual environments, including versions of the water maze, have been used to examine spatial learning and memory in amnesic and elderly humans. In general, training is massed into a single session and the research confirms that damage to the hippocampus results in impaired spatial navigation (Astur et al., 2002; Bartsch et al.,; Bohbot et al., 2004; Goodrich-Hunsaker et al.,; Skelton et al., 2000). Furthermore, elderly individuals exhibit deficits involving increased path length during acquisition and impaired performance on probe trials (Driscoll et al., 2003; Moffat et al., 2007; Moffat and Resnick, 2002; Moffat et al., 2001). While all age groups exhibit acquisition on a virtual environment maze, middle-aged and aged subjects were slower to learn and exhibited increased spatial memory errors (Driscoll et al., 2003; Driscoll et al., 2005; Jansen et al., 2010; Moffat et al., 2001; Thomas et al., 1999). Similar to animal studies, deficits were apparent following a single training session, and with continued training, older subjects performed in a manner that was not different from younger individuals (Jansen et al., 2010). Again, the initial learning and asymptotic level of performance in virtual environments may depend on different brain systems (Bohbot et al., 2007; Etchamendy and Bohbot, 2007; Etchamendy et al., 2011; Hartley and Burgess, 2005). The results suggest that training massed into a single session is sensitive in detecting the emergence of deficits in intermediate-term memory for rapidly acquired spatial information during middle-age in rodents and humans.
2.3.3 Repeated acquisition/matching-to-sample training
Repeated acquisition training provides another version of massed training, which is designed to specifically examine rapid flexible learning and can be adapted to examine retention or forgetting (Table 1). For this task, the escape location is changed across sessions such that the animal must repeatedly learn new escape locations (Fig 2A). This task usually involves only one or two days of training to each location. Deficits in the rapid acquisition of a spatial memory emerge by ~12 months of age in spontaneously hypertensive rats; however, most rat strains exhibit deficits by 18 months of age (Driscoll et al., 2006; Markowska and Savonenko, 2002; van der Staay and de Jonge, 1993; Wyss et al., 2000) (Fig 2B). Moreover, studies that directly compare incremental learning using distributed training and repeated acquisition in which the escape platform is changed each day indicate that repeated acquisition testing is more sensitive in detecting the emergence of age-related cognitive decline (Bizon et al., 2009; Markowska and Savonenko, 2002; Miyagawa et al., 1998; Nyffeler et al., 2010). In these cases, animals that exhibited impairments on the repeated acquisition version of the task could still acquire a spatial reference memory with training distributed across several days, consistent with the idea that impairments in rapid acquisition precede impaired incremental learning.
Figure 2.
Performance differences related to aging for repeated acquisition training. A) Schematic illustration of the pool, with an escape platform (grey circle) shifted to a new location each day such that the animal must repeatedly learn new escape locations. B) Hypothetical performance curves of the age-related decline in the rate of acquisition, modeled from Driscoll et al., 2006; Markowska and Savonenko, 2002; Miyagawa et al., 1998; Nyffeler et al., 2010; van der Staay and de Jonge, 1993; Wyss et al., 2000. The distance for each trial is averaged across days of training. The rate of learning decreases with age (aged: filled circles; middle-aged: grey circles; young: open circles). C) For delayed matching-to-sample, a variable delay (minutes to hours) is inserted between the first two trials. The savings (i.e. decrease in path length to find the platform on trial two compared to the first trial) is used as an indication of memory. An age by delay interaction is observed such that similar performance is observed for short delays and age-related memory impairments are seen with increasing delays.
Memory deficits can be characterized using the repeated acquisition training and delayed matching-to-sample testing to examining the savings from trial to trial. In this case, deficits are observed as a decrease in savings as the inter-trial interval is increased (Fig 2C). Interestingly, there is evidence to indicate that the extent of the memory impairment increases with advancing age. Compared to young rats, middle-aged animals exhibit a similar level of performance during acquisition training (8 trials/day) and impaired retention is observed on the first trial examined on day 2, 24 hr after the initial training (Driscoll et al., 2006). Furthermore, the extent of impairment increases with advancing age (Driscoll et al., 2006). Indeed, following a single training trial, young rats exhibit retention for up to 6 hr; middle-aged animals begin to exhibit retention deficits as the delay is increased to 2 hr and aged animals exhibit deficits for delays greater than ~1 hr (Bizon et al., 2009; Means and Kennard, 1991) (Fig 2C).
2.4 Reliability
One distinguishing feature of the swim task is that it is amendable to multiple testing sessions in order to examine the reliability of acquisition and retention deficits, as well as the success of treatments (Fontana et al., 1995; Markowska et al., 1994). However, it is important to recognize that there are carry-over effects of multiple testing sessions. Furthermore, testing on the water maze increases the release of stress hormones (Engelmann et al., 2006) and the stress of extended water maze training can influence hippocampal neurogenesis (Namestkova et al., 2005) and may have enduring affects on hippocampal function, particularly in learning impaired animals in which water maze training may be analogous to a being exposed to an uncontrollable swim stress (Foster and Kumar, 2007). On the other hand, training may act as cognitive stimulation and promote skills needed to solve the water maze. Animals that are trained on the maze during young adulthood or middle-age exhibit an advantage in re-learning when tested during aging (Hansalik et al., 2006; Vallee et al., 1999; van Groen et al., 2002; Vicens et al., 2002). As noted above, procedural memory is relatively intact during aging such that animals likely acquire a procedural strategy for how to find a hidden platform across multiple testing sessions. However, individual differences are observed such that heterogeneity in performance is consistently observed between individual subjects. Tests of repeated acquisition in the same environment indicate a slower rate of acquisition for aged animals, while performance of middle-aged animals is similar to young (Driscoll et al., 2006; Frick et al., 1995; Wyss et al., 2000). Nevertheless, middle-aged animals continue to exhibit retention deficits relative to young (Bizon et al., 2009; Driscoll et al., 2006; Means and Kennard, 1991). Similarly, for incremental learning across days of repetitive training, some aged animals consistently perform more poorly than young (Fontana et al., 1995; Lindner, 1997). When the acquisition of a spatial reference memory is retested using a new environment, carry-over effects are observed such that young and aged animals exhibited more rapid learning relative to the initial environment; however, aged animals that exhibited the poorest initial performance also exhibited poorer performance in the new environment (Colombo and Gallagher, 2002).
2.5 Aging of hippocampal subregions and the trajectory of cognitive decline
Little is known concerning the trajectory of cognitive decline during aging in rodents. The preceding review of the literature, focused on the water maze, supports the idea that cognitive decline is progressive and can be tracked using different water maze training procedures. Middle-age is associated with impairment of rapid spatial learning/memory and intact gradual acquisition of spatial knowledge. Tests involving a single training session or delayed matching-to-sample provide sensitive measures for deficits in intermediated-term memory as an early marker of cognitive decline. Repeated acquisition training can detect impairments in rapid, flexible spatial learning as animals move from middle-age to old age. Furthermore, the propensity for acquisition and memory deficits increases with advanced age. Finally, a subset of the oldest animals will exhibit profound learning deficits such that they will not be able to acquire a spatial reference memory, even with distributed training to the same location.
The cognitive changes observed with advancing age suggest a progression in the senescence of biological mechanisms of memory. Cognitive deficits are not associated with obvious hippocampal pathology involving cell death (Gallagher et al., 1996). Rather, memory deficits probably result from more subtle changes in memory processes for storage or maintenance mechanisms (Foster, 1999). All hippocampal subregions; dentate gyrus, CA3, and CA1, show some form of age-related change, which is likely to contribute to impaired cognition (Burke and Barnes, 2011; Rosenzweig and Barnes, 2003; Small et al., 2011; Wilson et al., 2006). Furthermore, these subregions are differentially susceptible to diseases of age including effects of stress, Alzheimer’s disease, and cell death due to ischemia.
Importantly, the subregions differentially contribute to different phases or aspects of episodic memory (Daumas et al., 2005; Kesner and Hunsaker, 2010; Kesner et al., 2004), suggesting that aging of subregions could uniquely modify performance on the water maze. As such, aging of different subregions or senescence of different mechanisms may account for early deficits in memory consolidation relative to later deficits in rapid acquisition, and finally impaired incremental learning. For example, the dentate gyrus is involved in spatial pattern separation (Gilbert et al., 2001; McHugh et al., 2007). Pattern separation declines during aging in humans and is associated with altered activity in the dentate gyrus (Small et al., 2011; Yassa et al., 2011). Neurogenesis is a notable form of neuroplasticity observed in the dentate gyrus, which declines during aging (Cameron and McKay, 1999; Kempermann et al., 1998; Kuhn et al., 1996; Nacher et al., 2003). The exact role of neurogenesis in memory function is currently a subject of intense research. However, it is interesting to note that variability in neurogenesis in aged animals is correlated with the rapid acquisition of spatial information observed during the early phases of spatial learning (Drapeau et al., 2003; Drapeau et al., 2007; Driscoll et al., 2006) and is not associated with impaired incremental learning (Bizon and Gallagher, 2003; Bizon et al., 2004; Merrill et al., 2003).
Region CA3 is thought to be involved in rapidly encoding and pattern completion for spatial information and the modifiability of CA3 cell activity is reduced with age (Wilson et al., 2005). Behavioral stress induces dendritic structural changes in region CA3 and susceptibility to stress effects increases with age (Christian et al., 2011; Cohen et al., 2011; Gartside et al., 2003; McEwen, 2001; Shoji and Mizoguchi, 2010). Gene changes in this region during aging indicate that preservation of CA3 function is maintained by neurotrophic mechanisms that permit adaptation to stress (Zeier et al., 2010) and failure to enlist these adaptation mechanisms is associated with impaired learning (Haberman et al., 2009). N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) contribute to CA3 function in rapid encoding and mutant mice with a deletion of NMDARs, specific to CA3, exhibit normal incremental learning and impaired performance on repeated acquisition training (McHugh et al., 2007; Nakazawa et al., 2002). In addition, the stress mediated dendrite changes require activation of CA3 NMDARs (Christian et al., 2011). Aging and learning impairment are associated with changes in CA3 synaptic markers, including NMDARs (Adams et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2000). However, the decline in CA3 NMDAR markers have been associated with learning deficits (Adams et al., 2001), no change in learning (Nicolle et al., 1996), or better performance (Le Jeune et al., 1996), suggesting that loss of NMDAR may mediate deficits or may act as a compensatory mechanism to protect cells from stress.
Region CA1 of aged animals exhibits gene changes related to increased susceptibility to inflammation, oxidative stress, Ca2+ dysregulation, and a decline in neurotrophic support (Blalock et al., 2003; Jackson and Foster, 2009; Jackson et al., 2010; Wang and Michaelis, 2010; Zeier et al., 2010). In turn, changes in these processes are thought to contribute to the propensity for region CA1 to exhibit increased susceptibility to Alzheimer’s disease and increased cell loss following ischemia, as well as impaired memory and synaptic plasticity during aging (Blalock et al., 2003; Zeier et al., 2010). Region CA1 is involved in consolidation of an intermediate or long-term form of memory (Dudai, 2004; Kesner and Hunsaker, 2010; Kesner et al., 2004; Lee and Kesner, 2002; Morris et al., 2003; Rawlins and Tsaltas, 1983), and as discussed above, disruption of memory consolidation is an early behavioral marker of cognitive aging. As such, the age-related disruption in Ca2+ regulation and synaptic plasticity is thought to contribute to the emergence of memory consolidation deficits (Foster, 2007; Foster and Norris, 1997; Kumar et al., 2009).
3. NMDAR-dependent and VDCC-dependent plasticity during aging
The general view is that synaptic plasticity mechanisms represent the biological substrate for memory and learning and the weight of evidence indicates a link between synaptic plasticity and spatial memory (McNaughton et al., 1986; Morris et al., 1986; Moser et al., 1998). Considering the prominence of synaptic plasticity as a model of memory storage, it is not surprising that a number of studies have examined age-related changes in two major forms of synaptic plasticity, long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD). LTP is an increase in synaptic transmission, induced by pattern stimulation of afferent fibers and is proposed as a cellular mechanism for the kind of rapid and flexible intermediate-term memory that is disrupted early in the course of cognitive senescence (Dudai, 2004; Morris et al., 2003). LTD is a decrease in synaptic strength, which may contribute to loss of synaptic contacts and increased forgetting during aging (Foster, 1999, 2007; Shinoda et al., 2005; Zhou et al., 2004). Age-related changes in LTP and LTD suggest the functional significance of altered synaptic plasticity for cognitive function (Foster, 1999, 2002; Foster and Norris, 1997).
While age-related changes in synaptic plasticity have been reported for all regions of the hippocampus, the majority of work has focused on perforant path to dentate gyrus and CA3 to CA1 synapses. For the dentate gyrus, the evidence indicates that impaired induction of LTP during aging results from neuroinflammatory changes, oxidative stress, and a decrease in NMDAR function (Lynch, 2009; Rosenzweig and Barnes, 2003). Undoubtedly, synaptic contacts between CA3 and CA1 pyramidal cells have received the most attention in terms of age-related changes in synaptic plasticity and this synapse is the focus of the current review. In general, no difference is observed in the expression mechanisms or the asymptotic amplitude of LTP and LTD, rather there is a shift in the mechanisms that regulate the threshold for induction of synaptic plasticity (for a review see Foster, 1999). Thus, age differences are likely to be observed for a given stimulation paradigm, particularly if the pattern of stimulation is close to the threshold for induction of synaptic plasticity. The threshold for induction of LTP increases with age such that higher stimulation frequencies or more induction sessions are required in older animals in order to achieve the same level of LTP. Similarly, the threshold for induction of LTD is lowered in aged animals, facilitating induction of LTD. Thus, an important question concerns differences in the mechanisms that regulate the induction of synaptic plasticity.
Induction of LTP involves released of glutamate from the presynaptic terminals. The glutamate binds to postsynaptic NMDARs and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors (Fig 3). The activation of AMPA receptors depolarizes of the postsynaptic membrane. The depolarization is critical in removing the Mg2+ block of the NMDAR channel and provoking a brief, large magnitude rise in postsynaptic Ca2+. The large rise in intracellular Ca2+ activates kinases including protein kinase C (PKC), Ca2+-calmodulin kinase II (CaMKII), and protein kinase A (PKA). The kinases phosphorylate proteins, increasing the function of AMPA receptors, resulting in an increase in synaptic transmission (Fig 3A). Induction of LTD is achieved by prolonged low frequency pattern stimulation to produce a modest and prolonged rise in intracellular Ca2+. A modest rise in Ca2+ activates phosphatase cascades, calcineurin (CaN) and protein phosphatae 1 (PP1), dephosphorylating proteins to decrease synaptic responses (Fig 3B). Thus, both LTP and LTD depend on a rise in intracellular Ca2+, and the degree and direction of altered synaptic function is determined by the level and duration of the Ca2+ rise.
Figure 3.
Synaptic plasticity induced by pattern stimulation. A) LTP is an increase in synaptic transmission, induced by HFS or TBS activation of presynaptic fibers to release glutamate, depolarizing the postsynaptic side to release the Mg2+ block of the NMDAR, and provoke a brief, large magnitude rise in postsynaptic Ca2+ from NMDARs, VDCCs and ICS. In addition, glutamate acting on mGluRs can increase the release of Ca2+ from ICS through IP3R activation. The large rise in Ca2+ activates kinases (CaMKII, PKC, PKA) to increase the AMPA receptor component of synaptic transmission. B) LTD is a decrease in synaptic strength induced by LFS or PP-LFS to produce a modest and prolonged rise in intracellular Ca2+, activating phosphatases (CaN, PP1) to decrease the AMPA receptor component of synaptic transmission.
The rise in intracellular Ca2+ levels is a function of the stimulation pattern, and the stimulation pattern influences the source of intracellular Ca2+. There are three main Ca2+ sources for induction of synaptic plasticity, NMDARs, voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels (VDCCs), and intracellular Ca2+ stores (ICS) (Fig 3). During aging, NMDAR function decreases and Ca2+ from VDCCs-ICS increases in region CA1. This shift in Ca2+ sources contributes to modifications in the threshold for induction of synaptic plasticity, leading to the proposed Ca2+ dysregulation hypothesis for age-related changes in synaptic plasticity (Foster, 1999; Foster and Norris, 1997).
3.1 LTP
Electrophysiological and pharmacological data indicate that at CA3-CA1 synapses, there are at least two forms of LTP that differ in terms of induction mechanisms. It should be emphasized that all Ca2+ sources normally contribute to LTP induction in the absence of specific antagonists. Nevertheless, depending on the stimulation pattern, LTP induction is mainly due to NMDAR activity (NMDAR-LTP) or can include NMDAR-independent and VDCC-dependent mechanisms (VDCC-LTP). In young animals, high frequency stimulation (HFS 25–100 Hz) or stimulation patterns near the threshold for LTP-induction, including brief episodes of theta burst stimulation (TBS) induce a rise in intracellular Ca2+ mainly through activation of NMDARs (Fig 3A). The shift in LTP mechanisms during aging can be observed as a decrease in the LTP magnitude for weak or threshold stimulation that largely activates NMDARs (Barnes, 1979; Foster, 1999, 2002, 2007; Landfield et al., 1978) and no age-related difference is observed when strong stimulation is used (Kumar et al., 2007). Higher frequency stimulation (200 Hz) can induce LTP independent of NMDARs, due to increased Ca2+ from VDCCs, particularly L-channels (Cavus and Teyler, 1996; Grover and Teyler, 1990). The Ca2+ from L-channels binds to ryanodine receptors (RyRs) to release Ca2+ from ICS, resulting in a further increase in intracellular Ca2+ (Fig 3). In addition, glutamate binding to metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) can increase release of Ca2+ through inositol trisphosphate receptor (IP3R) activation on ICS. Unlike NMDAR-LTP, VDCC-LTP tends to be greater in older animals (Boric et al., 2008; Robillard et al., 2011; Shankar et al., 1998).
There are two mechanisms that mediate the age-related decrease in NMDAR-LTP, one involves a reduction in NMDAR function and the other involves active inhibition of NMDARs through VDCCs and ICS (Fig 4). Electrophysiological studies consistently show a decrease in the NMDAR component of the synaptic transmission in region CA1 during aging (Barnes et al., 1997; Billard and Rouaud, 2007; Bodhinathan et al., 2010a; Tombaugh et al., 2002). The weight of evidence indicates that NMDAR binding declines in a number of brain regions, including the hippocampus (Magnusson et al., 2010). However, the level of receptor binding does not readily match the levels of protein or RNA expression, suggesting that changes in the functional state of the receptors or regulation of receptor activity contribute to the decrease in agonist/antagonist binding and the decrease in NMDAR function (Magnusson et al., 2010).
Figure 4.
The interaction of NMDARs, VDCCs, RyRs, and redox state regulates synaptic plasticity during aging. In young animals, the rise in intracellular Ca2+ for induction of synaptic plasticity (LTD/LTP) arises mainly from NMDARs. Activation of NMDARs requires glutamate binding and postsynaptic depolarization (+) due to AMPA glutamate receptor activity and backpropagating action potentials. The action potential will activate VDCCs to increase Ca2+ influx along the dendritic membrane. The influx of Ca2+ from VDCCs acts on RyRs to release Ca2+ from ICS. In turn, the rise in intracellular Ca2+ activates K+ channels along the membrane, resulting in an efflux of K+, which hyperpolarizes (−) the cell membrane. The K+ channels include SK channels near NMDARs and channels involved in the generation of the sAHP. With advanced age, an increase in oxidative stress (ROS) shifts the function of Ca2+ sources, decreasing and increasing Ca2+ from NMDARs and RyRs, respectively. The increased Ca2+ from ICS results in a larger hyperpolarizing response, which further inhibits NMDAR activation, raising the threshold stimulation needed to induce LTP.
Recent work demonstrates that the decrease in NMDAR function is related to oxidative stress and a shift in the intracellular oxidation-reduction (redox) state (Bodhinathan et al., 2010a). The nervous system is highly sensitive to oxidative stress (Halliwell, 1992). Irreversible damage to lipids, DNA, and proteins results from the production of the oxygen radical, superoxide. To protect tissue from irreversible damage, superoxide dismutase converts superoxide in to the nonradical hydrogen peroxide. Hydrogen peroxide per se does not induce irreversible oxidative damage (Catala, 2010; Leutner et al., 2001; Linden et al., 2008; Shacter, 2000). The relatively milder hydrogen peroxide induces the reversible formation of disulfide bonds between pairs of cysteine residues in proteins, shifting protein structure and function, and influencing multiple signaling cascades including Ca2+ signaling (Foster, 2007). The redox state of cysteine residues on the extracellular portion of the NMDAR have been implicated in regulating NMDAR function in cell cultures and neonatal animals (Aizenman et al., 1990; Aizenman et al., 1989; Bernard et al., 1997; Choi and Lipton, 2000), suggesting that altered redox state of these extracellular cysteine residues could mediate the decline in NMDAR function during aging. Indeed, application of the cysteine specific reducing agent, dithiothreitol, selectively increased the NMDAR component of the synaptic response and the magnitude of LTP in hippocampal slices from older rodents (Bodhinathan et al., 2010a). The membrane-impermeable reducing agent, L-glutathione, failed to increase the NMDAR response when applied to the slice; however, an increase in the NMDAR response was observed by intracellular delivery of L-glutathione through the intracellular recording pipette. The results indicate that intracellular redox state, rather than disulfide bonds of extracellular cysteine residues, mediates the suppression of NMDAR responses and impaired LTP. The decline in NMDAR function during aging, mediated by intracellular redox state, has recently been confirmed by other labs (Robillard et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2010). Finally, the dithiothreitol mediated growth of the NMDAR response was blocked by inhibition of CaMKII, but not by inhibition of PKC, PP1, or CaN. Furthermore, CaMKII activity of aged animals could be enhanced by dithiothreitol treatment, indicating that the effect was specific to CaMKII activity (Bodhinathan et al., 2010a).
The other mechanism for decreasing NMDAR-LTP in aged animals is mediated by VDCCs and ICS. VDCCs and ICS regulate NMDAR-dependent LTP by influencing the membrane potential (Fig 4). NMDAR activation requires depolarization of the postsynaptic membrane (Fig 3A). Release of Ca2+ from ICS or influx through VDCCs activates Ca2+-dependent SK potassium (K+) channels to hyperpolarize dendrites (Faber, 2010) and inhibition of Ca2+ release from ICS increases the NMDAR response in older animals (Kumar and Foster, 2004), suggesting a direct connection between ICS and NMDAR function. The membrane potential is also regulated by another well-characterized biomarker of age-related memory impairment; the Ca2+-dependent, K+ mediated slow afterhyperpolarization (sAHP). In this case, action potentials activate VDCCs, the influx of Ca2+ stimulates RyRs to release Ca2+ from ICS and the rise in intracellular Ca2+ opens K+ channels to hyperpolarize the membrane (Fig 4). In aged animals, increased Ca2+ from VDCCs and ICS enhance the magnitude and duration of the sAHP (Bodhinathan et al., 2010b; Kumar et al., 2009; Kumar and Foster, 2004; Oh et al., 2010; Thibault and Landfield, 1996). The sAHP represents a window of time after cell discharge activity, with a peak at ~200 ms (Fig 5A), in which the hyperpolarization disrupts the integration of depolarizing postsynaptic potentials. This disruption is evident for stimulation patterns near the theta frequency (i.e. 5 Hz with a 200 ms interval between stimulation episodes), including TBS (Diamond et al., 1988; Fuenzalida et al., 2007; Kramar et al., 2004; Moore et al., 1993).
Figure 5.
The threshold for LTP is regulated by the sAHP. A) Illustration of the sAHP. The resting membrane potential is indicated by the dashed line. Intracellular injection of depolarizing current induced a burst of five action potentials (spike burst), which is followed by the sAHP with a peak ~200 ms from the spike burst and lasting several 100 ms. The sAHP creates a window of hyperpolarization that disrupts the activation of NMDARs. The arrow indicates a reduction in the sAHP induced by pharmacological treatments to decrease Ca2+ from VDCC or ICS. B) Frequency-response functions for stimulation induced synaptic plasticity. The baseline synaptic response is 100%. Pattern stimulation (900 pulses) of increasing stimulation frequency (1–100) was delivered and the synaptic response was measured at least 30 min after the end of pattern stimulation. The curves indicate the changes in synaptic transmission (LTP > 100 percent of baseline or LTD < 100 percent of baseline). Normally, LTD is observed for LFS between 1–3 Hz and aged animals (solid curve) exhibit increased susceptibility, exhibiting greater LTD relative to younger animals (dashed curve). The threshold for LTP is ~ 5 Hz in young animals. In contrast, aged animals exhibit a plateau region of no synaptic modification starting at ~5 Hz, such that the threshold stimulation frequency for LTP induction is increased. The age-related shift in synaptic plasticity is due in part to the increase in the sAHP. Pharmacological treatments that decrease the sAHP amplitude in aged animals (Aged (−) AHP, solid curve with open circles) reduce the threshold stimulation required for induction of LTP.
The effect of the larger sAHP on the threshold for induction of LTP can be observed by plotting the change in synaptic strength as a result of different stimulation frequencies (Fig 5B). For young animals, the threshold frequency for induction of LTP is near 5 Hz and the level of LTP increases as stimulation frequency increases. The frequency response function for CA3-CA1 synapses in older animals is marked by a plateau region in which no LTP occurs for intermediate stimulation frequencies (~5–25 Hz), resulting in an increased threshold frequency required for induction of LTP (Foster, 1999; Hsu et al., 2002; Kumar and Foster, 2007b). An important observation is that treatments that reduce the sAHP drastically decreased the LTP threshold for aged animals; unmasking NMDAR-LTP for stimulation patterns as low as 5 Hz (Fig 5B). Treatments to reduce the sAHP and promote LTP include blockade of Ca2+ release from intracellular stores, blockade of SK channels, and L-channel antagonists (Bodhinathan et al., 2010a; Kumar and Foster, 2004; Norris et al., 1998). The results emphasize that VDCCs, RyRs, and ICS regulate synaptic plasticity not only by supplying intracellular Ca2+ for induction of VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity, but also by regulating the membrane potential to dynamically inhibit NMDAR activation. Thus, increased Ca2+ from VDCCs and ICS facilitates LTP for higher frequency stimulation (i.e. 200 Hz) and actively inhibits NMDAR-LTP in older animals.
Recent work has emphasized oxidation of RyRs and increased release of Ca2+ from ICS in mediating age-related changes in the sAHP and synaptic plasticity. Blockade of L-channels reduces the sAHP to a similar extent (~30%) in young and aged animals (Bodhinathan et al., 2010b; Disterhoft et al., 2004; Norris et al., 1998; Power et al., 2002). In contrast, treatments to reduce the release of Ca2+ from ICS has greater effect in aged animals, reducing the sAHP by ~50%, such that the amplitude approximates that normally observed in young animals (Bodhinathan et al., 2010b; Kumar and Foster, 2004). RyRs are redox sensitive such that release of Ca2+ from ICS is increased under conditions of mild oxidative stress (Bodhinathan et al., 2010b; Hidalgo et al., 2004). Application of cysteine specific reducing agents decreased the amplitude of the sAHP specifically in aged animals (Bodhinathan et al., 2010b). The antioxidant mediated reduction in the sAHP could be blocked by depletion of Ca2+ from ICS or blockade of RyRs, but not through blockade of VDCCs. The results indicate that redox state of RyRs is the major contributor to the age-related increase in the sAHP. In addition, RyR involvement in the age-related growth of the sAHP appears to emerge in middle-age (Gant et al., 2006). Finally, the shift in redox state and increased release of Ca2+ from ICS may underlie the age-related shift from NMDAR-LTP to VDCC-LTP. As noted above, Ca2+ release from ICS decreases the NMDAR response in older animals (Kumar and Foster, 2004), suggesting a direct connection between ICS and NMDAR function.
The disruption of Ca2+ regulation and impaired learning and memory are hypothesized to result from a shift in the level of hydrogen peroxide and reversible redox state of proteins, rather than irreversible oxidative damage associated with superoxide. Application of hydrogen peroxide to hippocampal slices from young animals decreases CaMKII activity (Shetty et al., 2008), decreases NMDAR responses (Bodhinathan et al., 2010a), and impairs LTP (Kamsler and Segal, 2003; Pellmar et al., 1991). Furthermore, hydrogen peroxide can enhance VDCC-LTP, possible through increased RyR activation (Huddleston et al., 2008; Kamsler and Segal, 2003). Virus mediated over expression of individual antioxidant enzymes in the hippocampus decreased the level of lipid and DNA oxidative damage without improving learning on the water maze (Lee et al., 2011). In fact, expression of superoxide dismutase-1 in older animals enhanced impairments of learning on the water maze. The effect of superoxide dismutase-1 is thought to result from an increase in the production of hydrogen peroxide. In support of this idea, deficits due to over expression of superoxide dismutase-1, as well as age-related deficits on the water maze, were ameliorated by co-expression of superoxide dismutase-1 and catalase. Together, the results establish a mechanism that links general theories of aging (i.e. oxidative stress and redox state) with altered Ca2+ homeostasis and senescent physiology, thought to under lie cognitive decline (Foster, 2007; Kumar et al., 2009).
3.2 LTD
Relative to LTP, induction of LTD requires a modest rise in Ca2+, which is usually induced by 1–3 Hz low frequency stimulation (LFS) or paired-pulse LFS (PP-LFS) using a 50 ms interval between pulse pairs (Fig 3B). In the hippocampal CA1 region, several mechanisms for induction of LTD have been characterized and these mechanisms involve different Ca2+ sources. Again it must be emphasized that in the absence of specific blockers, all sources are likely to contribute to the induction of LTD. The dysregulation of Ca2+ homeostasis during aging results in an increased susceptibility to LTD induction (Foster and Kumar, 2007; Foy et al., 2008; Hsu et al., 2002; Norris et al., 1996), in the absence of a change in the maximum LTD amplitude (Kumar et al., 2007). Due to the age-related shift in Ca2+ sources, NMDARs appear to contribute less to LTD with advanced age. Thus, NMDAR antagonists reduce but do not block induction of LTD in older animals (Ahmed et al., 2011; Norris et al., 1996) and there is an increased reliance on VDCCs (Norris et al., 1996) and ICS (Kumar and Foster, 2005) for LTD induction. Another form of LTD can be induced by activation of mGluRs (Oliet et al., 1997; Palmer et al., 1997) and the susceptibility to this form of LTD also increases with age (Kumar and Foster, 2007c).
The characterization of age-related modifications in Ca2+ homeostasis has provided a deeper level of understanding concerning the interaction of the cellular and molecular components that regulate LTP and LTD. The next section discusses the potential role of NMDARs and VDCCs in mediating deficits in consolidation of rapidly acquired memory in middle-age and impairments in incremental learning with advanced age.
4 Age-related deficits in consolidation of rapidly acquired memories: Relation to NMDAR function
Consolidation of memory from a short-term form, lasting seconds, to an intermediate-term form lasting hours is thought to depend on NMDAR activity (Jerusalinsky et al., 1992; Kim et al., 1992; Newcomer and Krystal, 2001; Packard and Teather, 1997; Roberts and Shapiro, 2002; Rossato et al., 2004; Shimizu et al., 2000; Steele and Morris, 1999). Early on, it was recognized that treatment with NMDAR antagonists impaired spatial memory (Butcher et al., 1990; Handelmann et al., 1987; McLamb et al., 1990; Mondadori et al., 1989; Morris, 1989; Morris et al., 1986; Morris et al., 1990a). Later research provided evidence that NMDAR blockade can result in an initial disruption in the acquisition of spatial information; however, this initial impairment may not be observed with continued training (Bannerman et al., 1995; Kesner et al., 2004; Steele and Morris, 1999). Rather, a certain level of NMDAR activity is required for the rapid consolidation of flexible spatial information (Jerusalinsky et al., 1992; Lee and Kesner, 2002; Steele and Morris, 1999). Furthermore, the role of NMDARs in acquisition and retention of spatial information appears to be region specific. Blockade of CA3 NMDARs contributes to the initial impairment in spatial working memory as the environment or task demands change (Kesner et al., 2004). Importantly, improved performance is observed with continued training suggesting non-NMDAR dependent plasticity mechanisms may compensate for loss of NMDAR function in CA3 to reorganize or store spatial information. In contrast, blockade of NMDARs in region CA1 results in impairment of intermediate-term spatial memory (Kesner et al., 2004). Unlike the recovery of the initial learning impairment, the intermediate memory deficit did not improve with continued training, indicating that other plasticity mechanism cannot compensate for a loss of CA1 NMDARs to rescue memory consolidation.
Comparison of water maze performance during NMDAR blockade and aging supports the idea that a decline in NMDAR function could mediate the memory consolidation deficits which are an early indication of cognitive decline. An effect of NMDAR blockade on retention can be demonstrated by measuring the savings in latency to find a hidden platform on the second (i.e. retention) trial during repeated acquisition training. When the inter-trial interval is short (15 sec), rats treated with the NMDAR blocker AP5 exhibit acquisition similar to saline treated animals, decreasing the latency to find a hidden platform on the second training trial. Similar to older animals (Bizon et al., 2009), this savings is reduced if the inter-trial interval is increased (Steele and Morris, 1999). It may be important to point out that, for these studies, the spatial representations were established by prior training. It appears that aging and NMDAR blockade do not influence the ability to use or reorganize established representations; rather there is impaired maintenance of newly acquired information.
Intact acquisition and impaired memory consolidation during NMDAR blockade is apparent when the initial training is massed into a single day. In this case, repeated trials with relatively short inter-trial intervals permit rats to acquire a spatial search strategy in a novel environment under NMDA receptor blockade. However, like aged animals (Blalock et al., 2003; Foster et al., 1991; Foster and Kumar, 2007; Norris and Foster, 1999), impaired retention was observed over a 24 hr period after training (Ge et al., 2010; Holahan et al., 2005; Kesner and Dakis, 1995; McDonald et al., 2005). Together, the results indicate that aged animals and animals under NMDAR blockade can acquire novel spatial information; however, consolidation deficits are evident if this information needs to be maintained.
The effect of NMDAR blockade on distributed training depends on the same factors that influence performance in aged animals, including sensory-motor function, prior training, and the number of training trials per day. Prior training facilitates incremental learning in aged animals (Colombo and Gallagher, 2002; Hansalik et al., 2006; van Groen et al., 2002) and during NMDAR blockade (Bannerman et al., 1995; Saucier et al., 1996). Similar to aged animals (Lindner, 1997), animals under NMDAR blockade fail to acquire a spatial search strategy when the number of training trials is reduced to one trial per day (Bannerman et al., 1995). This deficit may be overcome to a certain extent by increasing the number of trials per day. In studies using four to six trials per day, the antagonist AP5 resulted in a dose dependent impairment in acquisition performance (Davis et al., 1992; Uekita and Okaichi, 2009). The highest doses completely blocked learning; however, higher doses also resulted in sensory-motor disturbances and thigmotaxic behavior. Similar dose dependent effects on learning and sensory-motor function are observed for other NMDAR antagonists (Ahlander et al., 1999; Saucier et al., 1996). For doses within the range that normally block the induction of LTP, some impairment in learning was observed; nevertheless, a probe trial at the end of training indicated that search behavior was focused on the goal quadrant (Davis et al., 1992). Treatment with ifenprodil, which is a specific antagonist for the NR2B subunit of the NMDAR, did not block incremental acquisition of spatial learning over eight days of training (Ma et al., 2010). However, treatment for three days following training was associated with impaired memory consolidation (Ma et al., 2010). Thus, similar to aged animals that exhibit impaired incremental learning, is associated with sensory-motor impairment, suggesting the involvement of other brain regions. For lower doses that do not impair sensory-motor function, incremental learning can occur during NMDAR blockade; however, impaired memory consolidation is observed. Incremental spatial learning in the face of NMDAR antagonist treatment may result from incomplete receptor blockade which slows rather than blocks learning. Alternatively, NMDAR-independent mechanisms may compensate to permit acquisition of a spatial reference memory following multiple days of distributed training.
The NMDAR is composed of heteromeric subunits involving an essential NR1 subunit and four other possible subunits. Mutant mice have been developed to enable the selective inducible knockout of specific subunits and in some cases the knockout is specific to the hippocampus or hippocampal subregions. Mice with a selective knockout of the NR1 subunit in region CA3 or the dentate gyrus are able to acquire and recall a spatial reference memory during distributed training (McHugh et al., 2007; Nakazawa et al., 2002). However, deficits are observed for repeated acquisition or matching to sample training indicating impaired short-term memory for novel spatial information (Nakazawa et al., 2003). Knockout of CA1 NR1 prior to training severely impairs LTP induction and incremental learning (Shimizu et al., 2000). Furthermore, when CA1-NR1knockout was initiated at the end of incremental training a memory consolidation deficit was observed. The results are consistent with work examining region specific delivery of NMDAR antagonists (Kesner et al., 2004), pointing to CA3 NMDAR involvement in rapid acquisition and spatial working memory and CA1 NMDAR involvement in spatial information that is maintained for an intermediate period lasting several minutes or longer-term memory consolidation.
The results suggest that a reduction in NMDAR function with age could have differential effects on learning and memory depending on the regions and extent of NMDAR decline. In this regard, studies that knockout the other subunits may be enlightening since, like aging, NMDAR function is reduced, but not eliminated. For example, disruption of NR2A, limited to the hippocampus, impaired short-term spatial working memory and no deficit was observed for incremental learning on the water maze (Bannerman et al., 2008). Indeed, mice with disruption of the NR2A subunit throughout the brain exhibit only a mild impairments in LTP and the acquisition of a spatial reference memory when training is distributed across multiple days (Sakimura et al., 1995). Widespread disruption of NR2B throughout the forebrain resulted in impaired incremental spatial learning and impaired acquisition on the cue discrimination task (von Engelhardt et al., 2008). When disruption of NR2B was limited to the hippocampus, mice exhibited reduced LTP for a pairing protocol and impaired short-term spatial working memory; however, incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory was identical to that of controls (von Engelhardt et al., 2008). Interestingly, overexpression of NR2B improved the induction of LTP, learning, and memory in aged mice (Cao et al., 2007). Finally, a study employing antisense techniques in rats, found that decreased expression of hippocampal NR2B mimicked aging effects on the incremental learning task. Specifically, the impairment was observed early in training and differences diminished following further training (Clayton et al., 2002). The results suggest that an intermediate-term memory and memory consolidation are sensitive to reduced hippocampal NMDAR function and that incremental learning impairments are only observed following a substantial loss of NMDAR function in region CA1 or a wide spread decline in NMDAR function that impairs sensory-motor function. Together the behavioral data support the idea that the onset of age-related impairment in intermediate-term memory could result from a decrease in hippocampal NMDAR function, leaving incremental learning mechanisms intact.
Is impaired NMDAR-LTP related to memory deficits? Evidence is mounting to indicate a relationship between the appearance of memory deficits, a decline in NMDAR function, and altered synaptic plasticity. Impaired induction of LTP can be observed in middle-age and LTP magnitude is related to cognitive function of middle-aged animals (Brunson et al., 2005; Fouquet et al., 2009; Rex et al., 2005). We recently observed a decrease in NMDAR function, limited to those middle-aged animals that exhibit retention deficits on the water maze following a single training session (Kumar et al., 2011a). For studies that examine aged animals, the LTP magnitude following weak stimulation that would induce NMDAR-LTP (50 pulses at 100 Hz) correlated with retention examined 24 hr after 4 days of training (Deupree et al., 1991) and treatment to enhance NMDAR function improved TBS-induced LTP and performance on the repeated acquisition version of the water maze (Burgdorf et al., 2011). Thus, the weight of evidence supports the idea that the emergence of memory consolidation deficits is associated with a decline in NMDAR function and NMDAR-LTP.
The age-related decline in NMDAR function does not appear to correlate with impairment of incremental learning and acquisition of a spatial reference memory (Boric et al., 2008). This may not be surprising since the establishment of a spatial reference memory, acquired by incremental learning, is observed for many pharmacological or genetic conditions that decrease hippocampal NMDAR function (Bannerman et al., 2008; Bannerman et al., 2006). In one study, LTP induced by 5 Hz, but not 30 or 70 Hz stimulation correlated with learning across five days of training (Tombaugh et al., 2002). While these researchers found that NMDAR synaptic responses were decreased with age, impaired incremental learning was not related to the NMDAR synaptic responses. The authors suggest that the relationship between LTP for 5 Hz stimulation and incremental learning may be due to a larger sAHP in impaired animals. Importantly, 5 Hz stimulation would result in synaptic activation arriving within a window for the peak of the sAHP from the preceding stimulation induced action potential (Fig 5). In this case, the decrease in NMDAR activation may be enhanced for patterned stimulation that falls within the window of a larger sAHP. The sAHP amplitude is increased in aged animals that exhibit impaired incremental learning (Oh et al., 2010; Tombaugh et al., 2005) and reduction of the sAHP facilitates learning and LTP induction by 5 Hz stimulation in aged animals (Disterhoft et al., 1996; Kumar and Foster, 2004; Norris et al., 1998). Together, the results indicate that the initial memory deficits of senescence are associated with a decline in NMDAR function, including induction of NMDAR-LTP. However, incremental learning deficits observed in advance age are not directly related to the decline in NMDAR function.
5. VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity and incremental learning
The involvement of L-type Ca2+ channels and VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity in memory is complex and may depend on the brain region examined. It is important to note that L-channel antagonists have effects opposite that of NMDAR antagonists on hippocampal-dependent memory. In young animals, VDCC blockade facilitates retention of inhibitory avoidance and spatial memory, and facilitates the rate of acquisition of the radial arm maze (Batuecas et al., 1998; Kim et al., 2011; Levy et al., 1991; Quartermain et al., 2001; Quartermain et al., 1993; Quevedo et al., 1998). Furthermore, the improved retention following L-channel blockade appears to be specific for the hippocampus (Kim et al., 2011; Quevedo et al., 1998). Similar improvement in retention of hippocampal-dependent memory following L-channel blockade has also been observed for aged animals. In fact, L-channel antagonists ameliorate or prevent age-related memory decline across several species including humans (Ban et al., 1990; Deyo et al., 1989; Ingram et al., 1994; Levere and Walker, 1992; Riekkinen et al., 1997; Rose et al., 2007; Sandin et al., 1990; Solomon et al., 1995; Trompet et al., 2008; Veng et al., 2003; Woodruff-Pak et al., 1997). The results indicate that the beneficial effects of L-channel blockade on memory are specific for hippocampal-dependent memory, possibly mediated through a reduction in the sAHP (Disterhoft et al., 1996), facilitation of NMDAR-LTP, or inhibition of LTD (Kumar and Foster, 2004; Norris et al., 1998).
In contrast to facilitation of hippocampal-dependent memory, mounting evidence indicates that blockade of L-channels impairs incremental learning and the consolidation of remote memories in the neocortex. The L-channel antagonist nifedipine, but not the NMDAR antagonist AP5, blocked incremental associative learning on an olfactory discrimination task (Zhang et al., 2010). Comparison of NMDAR and VDCC antagonists on performance of the radial 8-arm maze confirmed that NMDAR blockade impairs rapidly acquired spatial memory. In contrast, animals under VDCC blockade acquired the task to the same extent as controls, but exhibited a deficit in spatial reference memory when retested 7–10 days later (Borroni et al., 2000; Woodside et al., 2004). Furthermore, mice with knockout of L-type VDCCs exhibit intact spatial learning on the water maze; however, retention deficits were observed when examined 30 days after training, indicating disruption of processes for remote memories (McKinney and Murphy, 2006; White et al., 2008), which are thought to be processed in the neocortex (Quinn et al., 2008; Teixeira et al., 2006; Wiltgen et al., 2004). Together with the fact that young rats with hippocampal lesions continue to exhibit incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory, the results suggest preserved incremental learning in aged rats may involve VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity in neocortical regions.
Before discussing the relationship between VDCC-plasticity and incremental learning during aging, it is important to point out that there are several parallels between the effects of behavioral stress and aging on memory and synaptic plasticity. As such, the level of stress associated with training and the time between training and examination of synaptic plasticity is important in determining the relationship between synaptic plasticity and memory. An acute stress, including a single day of water maze training, can result in impaired induction of LTP and enhanced induction of LTD lasting hours or days (Kavushansky et al., 2006; Li et al., 2005; Shors et al., 1997; Xu et al., 1997). Stress hormones have a direct effect on NMDAR function (Ooishi et al., 2011) and the sAHP amplitude is influenced by stress hormones, which in turn can influence the induction of synaptic plasticity (Diamond et al., 1992; Joels and de Kloet, 1991; Weiss et al., 2005). Treatments such as environmental enrichment will protect memory, the sAHP, and synaptic plasticity from mild stressors and aging (Kumar and Foster, 2007a; Kumar et al., 2011b; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2006). Acute stressor effects can be minimized by examining synaptic plasticity several days after a single training session and results indicate no relation between acute stress for a single day of water maze training and altered synaptic plasticity during aging (Foster and Kumar, 2007). In contrast, exposure to stress for several days can impair LTP and enhance LTD for months (Artola et al., 2006; Holderbach et al., 2007; Sterlemann et al., 2010). Furthermore, chronic stress modifies L-channel function and VDCC-synaptic plasticity, (Foster and Kumar, 2007; Liebmann et al., 2008; Mamczarz et al., 1999; Ryan et al., 2010; van Gemert and Joels, 2006). Thus, stress associated with training across multiple days could have enduring effects on synaptic plasticity, particularly in learning impaired animals in which training may be analogous to a being exposed to an uncontrollable swim stress.
A handful of studies have examined the relationship between VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity and behavior in aged animals. As detailed above, age-related changes in Ca2+ regulation include diminished NMDAR function and increased Ca2+ from VDCC-ICS. The increase VDCC-ICS component is thought to underlie the increase susceptibility to LTD induction in aged animals (Kumar and Foster, 2007c; Kumar et al., 2007; Norris et al., 1998). The increased susceptibility to LTD is correlated with forgetting examine 24 hrs after a single training session on the spatial version of the water maze (Foster and Kumar, 2007). Moreover, the level of LTD was not correlated with the level of stress measured as the amount of time swimming in the pool. The results suggest that the decline in intermediate-term memory is associated with a shift from NMDAR-dependent to VDCC-ICS-dependent regulation of synaptic plasticity. Other studies have examined VDCC-synaptic plasticity in animals characterized as impaired or unimpaired for incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory following 8–9 days of distributed training. In one study, researchers induced LTP using 200 Hz stimulation (Schulz et al., 2002), which should favor VDCC-LTP in aged animals (Shankar et al., 1998). No difference in LTP was observed between aged impaired and unimpaired animals; however, a correlation was observed between LTP and behavior for aged unimpaired animals (Schulz et al., 2002). The results suggest a possible relationship between VDCC-LTP and incremental learning in animals that are able to acquire the task. The authors suggest that the lack of correlation for impaired learners may be due to the severity of impairment (Schulz et al., 2002). Thus, aging of systems involved in incremental learning may have precipitated a complete absence of learning in impaired animals. Another series of studies used AP5 to block NMDARs in order to examine VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity (Boric et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2005). The researchers confirmed an age-related decrease LTP induced by TBS and TBS-induced LTP did not correlate with incremental learning in aged rats. In the presence of AP5, VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity was enhanced in aged animals that exhibited intact incremental acquisition of a spatial reference memory. The authors suggest that VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity may compensate for loss of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity in order to preserve acquisition of a spatial reference memory. Together, the results indicate that VDCC-synaptic plasticity is intact in aged animals that can acquire a spatial reference memory. Further investigations using treatments to inhibit or enhance VDCC function may be able to determine whether VDCC-synaptic plasticity contributes to preservation of incremental learning in aged animals or whether increased stress associated with an inability to escape from the water underlies the correlation of VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity and incremental learning.
As discussed above, in older animals, L-channel blockers improve the rapid acquisition and consolidation of intermediate-term memory that depends on hippocampal NMDARs. However, studies in younger animals indicate that L-channel blockers may impair the storage of remote memories. Thus, there may be a trade off for treatments designed to improve memory during aging through L-channel blockade. While not specifically examined, one study provides evidence for this idea. The study examined the influence of nimodipine treatment on water maze performance over 4 days of training (two trials per day) (Riekkinen et al., 1997). Untreated aged animals (22 months of age) were impaired relative to young (6 months). Nimodipine treatment facilitated acquisition in aged rats, consistent with the idea that L-channel blockade can improve learning and intermediate-term memory. Animals were then re-tested 30 days after drug washout. Young animals exhibited long-term memory observed as carry-over effects of previous training, with improved re-learning on the maze relative to original training. In contrast to young animals, aged animals, treated with nimodipine, which previously exhibited superior spatial learning, did not exhibit a carry-over advantage during retesting. The lack of a carry-over effect for aged animals is in contrast to other studies that indicate previous learning facilitates acquisition on subsequent retesting (Colombo and Gallagher, 2002; Hansalik et al., 2006; van Groen et al., 2002). Rather, the absence of carry-over effects is consistent with work indicating that VDCC-dependent synaptic plasticity is necessary for the storage of long-term or remote memories (Borroni et al., 2000; Lashgari et al., 2006; Seoane et al., 2009; White et al., 2008; Woodside et al., 2004). For example, delivery of Ca2+ channel blockers to young rats impaired long-term reference memory in the absence of learning deficits (Borroni et al., 2000; Woodside et al., 2004). Thus, blockade of VDCCs may improve hippocampal and NMDAR-dependent memory in aged animals at the expense of long-term memory. This idea remains to be tested. If true, then our ideas about treating age-related memory decline with L-channel antagonists may need to be reevaluated.
6. Conclusion
Several advances have been made which refine ideas concerning Ca2+ dysregulation in mediating age-related changes in synaptic plasticity and memory (Foster, 1999; Foster and Norris, 1997). NMDAR-dependent and VDCC-dependent mechanisms are likely to interact cooperatively in young animals. Hippocampal NMDAR-dependent mechanisms mediate intermediate-term memory for rapidly acquired information. In turn, intermediate-term memory may contribute to incremental learning and consolidation of spatial reference memories involving VDCC-dependent mechanisms and the neocortex. The decline in NMDAR function and increase in Ca2+ from ICS in the hippocampus during aging are thought to underlie the initiation of memory decline. Thus, memory consolidation deficits, increased sAHP, and impaired NMDAR function, including impaired induction of NMDAR-LTP, provide early markers of cognitive decline. The inability to acquire a spatial reference memory through incremental learning manifests later and suggests a decline in other processes or the involvement of other brain regions. It will be important to continue to develop more sophisticated paradigms for early detection of cognitive aging, as well as documenting the mechanisms for changes in synaptic plasticity. Future studies may want to determine if cognitive deficits are progressive. Do memory consolidation impairments in middle-age predict the extent of rapid acquisition or incremental learning deficits?
The translational relevance of animal models will depend on the development of treatments for age-related memory impairments that depend on proper hippocampal function. Several recent reviews have addressed possible mechanisms which increase the vulnerability of the hippocampus to aging and cognitive decline (Burger, 2010; Foster, 2007; Kumar et al., 2009; Lynch, 2009; Magnusson et al., 2010; Oh et al., 2010; Penner et al., 2010; Potier et al., 2010; Schimanski and Barnes, 2010; Schneider et al., 2010; Wang and Michaelis, 2010). Most hypotheses include Ca2+ dysregulation and increased oxidative stress as a component of aging. The results from studies examining the effect of redox-active agents on neural physiology indicate that an age-related shift in redox state underlies the pattern of altered Ca2+ homeostasis, reducing NMDAR function and increasing Ca2+ from ICS, decreasing cell excitability, leading to alter synaptic plasticity processes that are critical for memory. Due to the interaction of NMDARs, RyR-ICS, and redox state in regulating synaptic plasticity (Fig 4), treatments may want to focus on restoring intracellular redox state in order to reverse the age-related shift in Ca2+ sources. In this regard, it will be important to identify the source for the shift in redox state. Metabolic changes and increased inflammation during aging provide likely candidates for increased oxidative stress. Recent studies indicate that region CA1 is more susceptible to an age-related increase in inflammation and oxidative stress and exhibits reduced neurotrophic and pro-survival signaling (Jackson and Foster, 2009; Jackson et al., 2010; Zeier et al., 2010). Combined with studies demonstrating that NMDARs in region CA1 are involved in memory consolidation (Daumas et al., 2005; Kesner et al., 2004; Lee and Kesner, 2002), the increased susceptibility to oxidative stress in region CA1 and subsequent decrease in NMDAR function could explain the emergence of memory consolidation deficits in middle-age, as an early sign of age-related cognitive decline.
Highlights.
The water maze can detect a progressive decline in cognition during aging.
Dysregulation of Ca2+ homeostasis modifies the threshold for synaptic plasticity.
NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity declines with age.
VDCC and RyR-ICS regulation of synaptic plasticity increases during aging.
Aging of synaptic plasticity mechanisms contribute to variability in memory deficits.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by National Institutes of Aging Grant AG014979, AG037984, AG036800, and the Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Research Foundation.
Abbreviations
- AMPA
α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid
- CaN
Calcineurin
- Ca2+
Calcium
- CaMKII
Calmodulin kinase II
- IP3R
Inositol trisphosphate receptor
- ICS
Intracellular calcium stores
- F344
Fisher 344
- F344BN
Fisher 344 Brown Norway crossed
- HFS
High frequency stimulation
- LFS
Low frequency stimulation
- LTP
Long-term potentiation
- LTD
Long-term depression
- mGluR
Metabotropic glutamate receptor
- NMDAR
N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors
- PP-LFS
Paired-pulse low frequency stimulation
- K+
Potassium
- PKA
Protein kinase A
- PKC
Protein kinase C
- PP1
Protein phosphatae 1
- ROS
Reactive oxygen species
- RyR
Ryanodine receptor
- sAHP
Slow afterhyperpolarization
- TBS
Theta burst stimulation
- VDCC
Voltage-dependent calcium channel
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adams MM, Shi L, Linville MC, Forbes ME, Long AB, Bennett C, Newton IG, Carter CS, Sonntag WE, Riddle DR, Brunso-Bechtold JK. Caloric restriction and age affect synaptic proteins in hippocampal CA3 and spatial learning ability. Exp Neurol. 2008;211:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams MM, Smith TD, Moga D, Gallagher M, Wang Y, Wolfe BB, Rapp PR, Morrison JH. Hippocampal dependent learning ability correlates with N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor levels in CA3 neurons of young and aged rats. J Comp Neurol. 2001;432:230–243. doi: 10.1002/cne.1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahlander M, Misane I, Schott PA, Ogren SO. A behavioral analysis of the spatial learning deficit induced by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 (dizocilpine) in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:414–426. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00116-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed T, Sabanov V, D’Hooge R, Balschun D. An N-methyl-D-aspartate-receptor dependent, late-phase long-term depression in middle-aged mice identifies no GluN2-subunit bias. Neuroscience. 2011;185:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aitken DH, Meaney MJ. Temporally graded, age-related impairments in spatial memory in the rat. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:273–276. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(89)90062-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aizenman E, Hartnett KA, Reynolds IJ. Oxygen free radicals regulate NMDA receptor function via a redox modulatory site. Neuron. 1990;5:841–846. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90343-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aizenman E, Lipton SA, Loring RH. Selective modulation of NMDA responses by reduction and oxidation. Neuron. 1989;2:1257–1263. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(89)90310-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arendash GW, King DL. Intra- and intertask relationships in a behavioral test battery given to Tg2576 transgenic mice and controls. Physiol Behav. 2002;75:643–652. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00640-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Artola A, von Frijtag JC, Fermont PC, Gispen WH, Schrama LH, Kamal A, Spruijt BM. Long-lasting modulation of the induction of LTD and LTP in rat hippocampal CA1 by behavioural stress and environmental enrichment. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:261–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04552.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Astur RS, Taylor LB, Mamelak AN, Philpott L, Sutherland RJ. Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav Brain Res. 2002;132:77–84. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00399-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayyagari PV, Gerber M, Joseph JA, Crews FT. Uncoupling of muscarinic cholinergic phosphoinositide signals in senescent cerebral cortical and hippocampal membranes. Neurochem Int. 1998;32:107–115. doi: 10.1016/s0197-0186(97)00044-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ban TA, Morey L, Aguglia E, Azzarelli O, Balsano F, Marigliano V, Caglieris N, Sterlicchio M, Capurso A, Tomasi NA, et al. Nimodipine in the treatment of old age dementias. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1990;14:525–551. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(90)90005-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannerman DM, Good MA, Butcher SP, Ramsay M, Morris RG. Distinct components of spatial learning revealed by prior training and NMDA receptor blockade. Nature. 1995;378:182–186. doi: 10.1038/378182a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannerman DM, Niewoehner B, Lyon L, Romberg C, Schmitt WB, Taylor A, Sanderson DJ, Cottam J, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Kohr G, Rawlins JN. NMDA receptor subunit NR2A is required for rapidly acquired spatial working memory but not incremental spatial reference memory. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3623–3630. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3639-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannerman DM, Rawlins JN, Good MA. The drugs don’t work-or do they? Pharmacological and transgenic studies of the contribution of NMDA and GluR-A-containing AMPA receptors to hippocampal-dependent memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188:552–566. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0403-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes CA. Memory deficits associated with senescence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1979;93:74–104. doi: 10.1037/h0077579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes CA, Rao G, Shen J. Age-related decrease in the N-methyl-D-aspartateR-mediated excitatory postsynaptic potential in hippocampal region CA1. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:445–452. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(97)00044-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrientos RM, Higgins EA, Biedenkapp JC, Sprunger DB, Wright-Hardesty KJ, Watkins LR, Rudy JW, Maier SF. Peripheral infection and aging interact to impair hippocampal memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:723–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartsch T, Schonfeld R, Muller FJ, Alfke K, Leplow B, Aldenhoff J, Deuschl G, Koch JM. Focal lesions of human hippocampal CA1 neurons in transient global amnesia impair place memory. Science (New York, NY. 328:1412–1415. doi: 10.1126/science.1188160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batuecas A, Pereira R, Centeno C, Pulido JA, Hernandez M, Bollati A, Bogonez E, Satrustegui J. Effects of chronic nimodipine on working memory of old rats in relation to defects in synaptosomal calcium homeostasis. Eur J Pharmacol. 1998;350:141–150. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(98)00250-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter MG, Gallagher M. Neurobiological substrates of behavioral decline: models and data analytic strategies for individual differences in aging. Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17:491–495. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(96)00011-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benice TS, Rizk A, Kohama S, Pfankuch T, Raber J. Sex-differences in age-related cognitive decline in C57BL/6J mice associated with increased brain microtubule-associated protein 2 and synaptophysin immunoreactivity. Neuroscience. 2006;137:413–423. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergado JA, Almaguer W, Rojas Y, Capdevila V, Frey JU. Spatial and emotional memory in aged rats: a behavioral-statistical analysis. Neuroscience. 2011;172:256–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.10.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard CL, Hirsch JC, Khazipov R, Ben-Ari Y, Gozlan H. Redox modulation of synaptic responses and plasticity in rat CA1 hippocampal neurons. Exp Brain Res. 1997;113:343–352. doi: 10.1007/BF02450332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billard JM, Rouaud E. Deficit of NMDA receptor activation in CA1 hippocampal area of aged rats is rescued by D-cycloserine. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:2260–2268. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05488.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizon JL, Gallagher M. Production of new cells in the rat dentate gyrus over the lifespan: relation to cognitive decline. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:215–219. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizon JL, LaSarge CL, Montgomery KS, McDermott AN, Setlow B, Griffith WH. Spatial reference and working memory across the lifespan of male Fischer 344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:646–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizon JL, Lee HJ, Gallagher M. Neurogenesis in a rat model of age-related cognitive decline. Aging Cell. 2004;3:227–234. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9728.2004.00099.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blalock EM, Chen KC, Sharrow K, Herman JP, Porter NM, Foster TC, Landfield PW. Gene microarrays in hippocampal aging: statistical profiling identifies novel processes correlated with cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. 2003;23:3807–3819. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03807.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blokland A, Raaijmakers W. Age-related changes in correlation between behavioral and biochemical parameters in Lewis rats. Behav Neural Biol. 1993;60:52–61. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90716-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodhinathan K, Kumar A, Foster TC. Intracellular redox state alters NMDA receptor response during aging through Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. J Neurosci. 2010a;30:1914–1924. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5485-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodhinathan K, Kumar A, Foster TC. Redox sensitive calcium stores underlie enhanced after hyperpolarization of aged neurons: role for ryanodine receptor mediated calcium signaling. J Neurophysiol. 2010b;104:2586–2593. doi: 10.1152/jn.00577.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohbot VD, Iaria G, Petrides M. Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:418–425. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohbot VD, Lerch J, Thorndycraft B, Iaria G, Zijdenbos AP. Gray matter differences correlate with spontaneous strategies in a human virtual navigation task. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10078–10083. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1763-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boric K, Munoz P, Gallagher M, Kirkwood A. Potential adaptive function for altered long-term potentiation mechanisms in aging hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8034–8039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2036-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borroni AM, Fichtenholtz H, Woodside BL, Teyler TJ. Role of voltage-dependent calcium channel long-term potentiation (LTP) and NMDA LTP in spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2000;20:9272–9276. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09272.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouffard JP, Jarrard LE. Acquisition of a complex place task in rats with selective ibotenate lesions of hippocampal formation: combined lesions of subiculum and entorhinal cortex versus hippocampus. Behav Neurosci. 1988;102:828–834. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.6.828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandeis R, Brandys Y, Yehuda S. The use of the Morris Water Maze in the study of memory and learning. Int J Neurosci. 1989;48:29–69. doi: 10.3109/00207458909002151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent NJ, Gaskin S, Squire LR, Clark RE. Object recognition memory and the rodent hippocampus. Learn Mem. 2009;17:5–11. doi: 10.1101/lm.1650110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunson KL, Kramar E, Lin B, Chen Y, Colgin LL, Yanagihara TK, Lynch G, Baram TZ. Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. J Neurosci. 2005;25:9328–9338. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2281-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgdorf J, Zhang XL, Weiss C, Matthews E, Disterhoft JF, Stanton PK, Moskal JR. The N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor modulator GLYX-13 enhances learning and memory, in young adult and learning impaired aging rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32:698–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burger C. Region-specific genetic alterations in the aging hippocampus: implications for cognitive aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:140. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke SN, Barnes CA. Senescent synapses and hippocampal circuit dynamics. Trends Neurosci. 2011;33:153–161. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butcher SP, Davis S, Morris RG. A dose-related impairment of spatial learning by the NMDA receptor antagonist, 2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate (AP5) Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1990;1:15–20. doi: 10.1016/0924-977x(90)90005-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron HA, McKay RD. Restoring production of hippocampal neurons in old age. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:894–897. doi: 10.1038/13197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao X, Cui Z, Feng R, Tang YP, Qin Z, Mei B, Tsien JZ. Maintenance of superior learning and memory function in NR2B transgenic mice during ageing. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:1815–1822. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS, Leeuwenburgh C, Daniels M, Foster TC. Influence of calorie restriction on measures of age-related cognitive decline: role of increased physical activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:850–859. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catala A. A synopsis of the process of lipid peroxidation since the discovery of the essential fatty acids. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2010;399:318–323. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2010.07.087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavus I, Teyler T. Two forms of long-term potentiation in area CA1 activate different signal transduction cascades. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:3038–3047. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.5.3038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho YH, Jaffard R. Spatial location learning in mice with ibotenate lesions of entorhinal cortex or subiculum. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1995;64:285–290. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1995.0011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi YB, Lipton SA. Redox modulation of the NMDA receptor. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2000;57:1535–1541. doi: 10.1007/PL00000638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen H, Mackinnon AJ, Korten AE, Jorm AF, Henderson AS, Jacomb P, Rodgers B. An analysis of diversity in the cognitive performance of elderly community dwellers: individual differences in change scores as a function of age. Psychol Aging. 1999;14:365–379. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.3.365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christian KM, Miracle AD, Wellman CL, Nakazawa K. Chronic stress-induced hippocampal dendritic retraction requires CA3 NMDA receptors. Neuroscience. 2011;174:26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.11.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchill JD, Stanis JJ, Press C, Kushelev M, Greenough WT. Is procedural memory relatively spared from age effects? Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:883–892. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00194-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton DA, Mesches MH, Alvarez E, Bickford PC, Browning MD. A hippocampal NR2B deficit can mimic age-related changes in long-term potentiation and spatial learning in the Fischer 344 rat. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3628–3637. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03628.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JW, Louneva N, Han LY, Hodes GE, Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Lucki I, Arnold SE. Chronic corticosterone exposure alters postsynaptic protein levels of PSD-95, NR1, and synaptopodin in the mouse brain. Synapse. 2011;65:763–770. doi: 10.1002/syn.20900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collier TJ, Greene JG, Felten DL, Stevens SY, Collier KS. Reduced cortical noradrenergic neurotransmission is associated with increased neophobia and impaired spatial memory in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:209–221. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00042-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colombo PJ, Gallagher M. Individual differences in spatial memory among aged rats are related to hippocampal PKCgamma immunoreactivity. Hippocampus. 2002;12:285–289. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colsher PL, Wallace RB. Longitudinal application of cognitive function measures in a defined population of community-dwelling elders. Ann Epidemiol. 1991;1:215–230. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(91)90001-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Commins S, Cunningham L, Harvey D, Walsh D. Massed but not spaced training impairs spatial memory. Behav Brain Res. 2003;139:215–223. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00270-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dash PK, Mach SA, Blum S, Moore AN. Intrahippocampal wortmannin infusion enhances long-term spatial and contextual memories. Learn Mem. 2002;9:167–177. doi: 10.1101/lm.50002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daumas S, Halley H, Frances B, Lassalle JM. Encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of contextual memory: differential involvement of dorsal CA3 and CA1 hippocampal subregions. Learn Mem. 2005;12:375–382. doi: 10.1101/lm.81905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis HP, Small SA, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Feldstein SN, Keller FR. Acquisition, recall, and forgetting of verbal information in long-term memory by young, middle-aged, and elderly individuals. Cortex. 2003;39:1063–1091. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70878-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis S, Butcher SP, Morris RG. The NMDA receptor antagonist D-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate (D-AP5) impairs spatial learning and LTP in vivo at intracerebral concentrations comparable to those that block LTP in vitro. J Neurosci. 1992;12:21–34. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-01-00021.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis S, Markowska AL, Wenk GL, Barnes CA. Acetyl-L-carnitine: behavioral, electrophysiological, and neurochemical effects. Neurobiol Aging. 1993;14:107–115. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(93)90030-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Arco A, Segovia G, Mora F. Dopamine release during stress in the prefrontal cortex of the rat decreases with age. Neuroreport. 2001;12:4019–4022. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112210-00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dellu-Hagedorn F, Trunet S, Simon H. Impulsivity in youth predicts early age-related cognitive deficits in rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:525–537. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dellu F, Mayo W, Vallee M, Le Moal M, Simon H. Facilitation of cognitive performance in aged rats by past experience depends on the type of information processing involved: a combined cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;67:121–128. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deupree DL, Turner DA, Watters CL. Spatial performance correlates with in vitro potentiation in young and aged Fischer 344 rats. Brain Res. 1991;554:1–9. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90164-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devan BD, Goad EH, Petri HL. Dissociation of hippocampal and striatal contributions to spatial navigation in the water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1996;66:305–323. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deyo RA, Straube KT, Moyer JR, Jr, Disterhoft JF. Nimodipine ameliorates aging-related changes in open-field behaviors of the rabbit. Exp Aging Res. 1989;15:169–175. doi: 10.1080/03610738908259771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM. Inverted-U relationship between the level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus. 1992;2:421–430. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond DM, Dunwiddie TV, Rose GM. Characteristics of hippocampal primed burst potentiation in vitro and in the awake rat. J Neurosci. 1988;8:4079–4088. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-11-04079.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diana G, Domenici MR, Scotti de Carolis A, Loizzo A, Sagratella S. Reduced hippocampal CA1 Ca(2+)-induced long-term potentiation is associated with age-dependent impairment of spatial learning. Brain Res. 1995;686:107–110. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00440-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dierckx E, Engelborghs S, De Raedt R, De Deyn PP, D’Haenens E, Verte D, Ponjaert-Kristoffersen I. The 10-word learning task in the differential diagnosis of early Alzheimer’s disease and elderly depression: A cross-sectional pilot study. Aging Ment Health. 2010;15:113–121. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2010.505228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disterhoft JF, Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Jr, Mogul DJ. Calcium-dependent afterhyperpolarization and learning in young and aging hippocampus. Life Sci. 1996;59:413–420. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(96)00320-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disterhoft JF, Wu WW, Ohno M. Biophysical alterations of hippocampal pyramidal neurons in learning, ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res Rev. 2004;3:383–406. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drapeau E, Mayo W, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza PV, Abrous DN. Spatial memory performances of aged rats in the water maze predict levels of hippocampal neurogenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:14385–14390. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2334169100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drapeau E, Montaron MF, Aguerre S, Abrous DN. Learning-induced survival of new neurons depends on the cognitive status of aged rats. J Neurosci. 2007;27:6037–6044. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1031-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Baumgartner RN, Sutherland RJ. The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1344–1351. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ. Virtual navigation in humans: the impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning. Horm Behav. 2005;47:326–335. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driscoll I, Howard SR, Stone JC, Monfils MH, Tomanek B, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ. The aging hippocampus: a multi-level analysis in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;139:1173–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunnett SB. Role of prefrontal cortex and striatal output systems in short-term memory deficits associated with ageing, basal forebrain lesions, and cholinergic-rich grafts. Can J Psychol. 1990;44:210–232. doi: 10.1037/h0084240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunnett SB, Evenden JL, Iversen SD. Delay-dependent short-term memory deficits in aged rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988;96:174–180. doi: 10.1007/BF00177557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, Fortin N, Sauvage M, Robitsek RJ, Farovik A. An animal model of amnesia that uses Receiver Operating Characteristics (ROC) analysis to distinguish recollection from familiarity deficits in recognition memory. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2281–2289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann M, Ebner K, Landgraf R, Wotjak CT. Effects of Morris water maze testing on the neuroendocrine stress response and intrahypothalamic release of vasopressin and oxytocin in the rat. Horm Behav. 2006;50:496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etchamendy N, Bohbot VD. Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town. Hippocampus. 2007;17:595–599. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etchamendy N, Konishi K, Pike GB, Marighetto A, Bohbot VD. Evidence for a virtual human analog of a rodent relational memory task: A study of aging and fMRI in young adults. Hippocampus. 2011 doi: 10.1002/hipo.20948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faber ES. Functional interplay between NMDA receptors, SK channels and voltage-gated Ca2+ channels regulates synaptic excitability in the medial prefrontal cortex. J Physiol. 2010;588:1281–1292. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.185645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahlstrom A, Yu Q, Ulfhake B. Behavioral changes in aging female C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer W, Chen KS, Gage FH, Bjorklund A. Progressive decline in spatial learning and integrity of forebrain cholinergic neurons in rats during aging. Neurobiol Aging. 1992;13:9–23. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(92)90003-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontana DJ, Daniels SE, Henderson C, Eglen RM, Wong EH. Ondansetron improves cognitive performance in the Morris water maze spatial navigation task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;120:409–417. doi: 10.1007/BF02245812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forster MJ, Lal H. Within-subject behavioral analysis of recent memory in aging mice. Behav Pharmacol. 1992;3:337–349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC. Involvement of hippocampal synaptic plasticity in age-related memory decline. Brain Res Rev. 1999;30:236–249. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00017-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC. Regulation of synaptic plasticity in memory and memory decline with aging. Prog Brain Res. 2002;138:283–303. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38083-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC. Biological markers of age-related memory deficits: treatment of senescent physiology. CNS Drugs. 2006;20:153–166. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200620020-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC. Calcium homeostasis and modulation of synaptic plasticity in the aged brain. Aging Cell. 2007;6:319–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2007.00283.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, Barnes CA, Rao G, McNaughton BL. Increase in perforant path quantal size in aged F-344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1991;12:441–448. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(91)90071-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, Kumar A. Susceptibility to induction of long-term depression is associated with impaired memory in aged Fischer 344 rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:522–535. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, Norris CM. Age-associated changes in Ca(2+)-dependent processes: relation to hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Hippocampus. 1997;7:602–612. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:6<602::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, Sharrow KM, Kumar A, Masse J. Interaction of age and chronic estradiol replacement on memory and markers of brain aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:839–852. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00014-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, Sharrow KM, Masse JR, Norris CM, Kumar A. Calcineurin links Ca2+ dysregulation with brain aging. J Neurosci. 2001;21:4066–4073. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-04066.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fouquet C, Petit GH, Auffret A, Gaillard E, Rovira C, Mariani J, Rondi-Reig L. Early detection of age-related memory deficits in individual mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foy MR, Baudry M, Foy JG, Thompson RF. 17beta-estradiol modifies stress-induced and age-related changes in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122:301–309. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francia N, Cirulli F, Chiarotti F, Antonelli A, Aloe L, Alleva E. Spatial memory deficits in middle-aged mice correlate with lower exploratory activity and a subordinate status: role of hippocampal neurotrophins. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:711–728. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick KM, Baxter MG, Markowska AL, Olton DS, Price DL. Age-related spatial reference and working memory deficits assessed in the water maze. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16:149–160. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(94)00155-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick KM, Burlingame LA, Arters JA, Berger-Sweeney J. Reference memory, anxiety and estrous cyclicity in C57BL/6NIA mice are affected by age and sex. Neuroscience. 2000;95:293–307. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00418-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuenzalida M, Fernandez de Sevilla D, Buno W. Changes of the EPSP waveform regulate the temporal window for spike-timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11940–11948. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0900-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gage FH, Dunnett SB, Bjorklund A. Spatial learning and motor deficits in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1984;5:43–48. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(84)90084-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon LG, Belleville S. Working memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: contribution of forgetting and predictive value of complex span tasks. Neuropsychology. 2010;25:226–236. doi: 10.1037/a0020919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, Burwell R, Burchinal M. Severity of spatial learning impairment in aging: development of a learning index for performance in the Morris water maze. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:618–626. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.4.618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, Burwell RD. Relationship of age-related decline across several behavioral domains. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:691–708. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(89)90006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, Landfield PW, McEwen B, Meaney MJ, Rapp PR, Sapolsky R, West MJ. Hippocampal neurodegeneration in aging. Science (New York, NY. 1996;274:484–485. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5287.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gant JC, Sama MM, Landfield PW, Thibault O. Early and simultaneous emergence of multiple hippocampal biomarkers of aging is mediated by Ca2+-induced Ca2+ release. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3482–3490. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4171-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gartside SE, Leitch MM, McQuade R, Swarbrick DJ. Flattening the glucocorticoid rhythm causes changes in hippocampal expression of messenger RNAs coding structural and functional proteins: implications for aging and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:821–829. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge Y, Dong Z, Bagot RC, Howland JG, Phillips AG, Wong TP, Wang YT. Hippocampal long-term depression is required for the consolidation of spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:16697–16702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008200107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genoux D, Haditsch U, Knobloch M, Michalon A, Storm D, Mansuy IM. Protein phosphatase 1 is a molecular constraint on learning and memory. Nature. 2002;418:970–975. doi: 10.1038/nature00928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlai R. Behavioral tests of hippocampal function: simple paradigms complex problems. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125:269–277. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00296-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlai RT, McNamara A, Williams S, Phillips HS. Hippocampal dysfunction and behavioral deficit in the water maze in mice: an unresolved issue? Brain Res Bull. 2002;57:3–9. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00630-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert PE, Kesner RP, Lee I. Dissociating hippocampal subregions: double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus. 2001;11:626–636. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glisky EL. Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging. In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. CRC Press; Boca Raton (FL): 2007. pp. 3–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold PE, McGaugh JL, Hankins LL, Rose RP, Vasquez BJ. Age-dependent changes in retention in rats. Exp Aging Res. 1981;8:53–59. [Google Scholar]
- Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Livingstone SA, Skelton RW, Hopkins RO. Spatial deficits in a virtual water maze in amnesic participants with hippocampal damage. Hippocampus. 20:481–491. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gower AJ, Lamberty Y. The aged mouse as a model of cognitive decline with special emphasis on studies in NMRI mice. Behav Brain Res. 1993;57:163–173. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90132-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granholm AC, Bimonte-Nelson HA, Moore AB, Nelson ME, Freeman LR, Sambamurti K. Effects of a saturated fat and high cholesterol diet on memory and hippocampal morphology in the middle-aged rat. J Alzheimers Dis. 2008;14:133–145. doi: 10.3233/jad-2008-14202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grover LM, Teyler TJ. Differential effects of NMDA receptor antagonist APV on tetanic stimulation induced and calcium induced potentiation. Neurosci Lett. 1990;113:309–314. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(90)90603-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gulinello M, Gertner M, Mendoza G, Schoenfeld BP, Oddo S, LaFerla F, Choi CH, McBride SM, Faber DS. Validation of a 2-day water maze protocol in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2009;196:220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haberman RP, Colantuoni C, Stocker AM, Schmidt AC, Pedersen JT, Gallagher M. Prominent hippocampal CA3 gene expression profile in neurocognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halliwell B. Reactive oxygen species and the central nervous system. J Neurochem. 1992;59:1609–1623. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb10990.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammond RS, Tull LE, Stackman RW. On the delay-dependent involvement of the hippocampus in object recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handelmann GE, Contreras PC, O’Donohue TL. Selective memory impairment by phencyclidine in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1987;140:69–73. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(87)90635-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannesson DK, Skelton RW. Recovery of spatial performance in the Morris water maze following bilateral transection of the fimbria/fornix in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1998;90:35–56. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00081-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansalik M, Skalicky M, Viidik A. Impairment of water maze behaviour with ageing is counteracted by maze learning earlier in life but not by physical exercise, food restriction or housing conditions. Exp Gerontol. 2006;41:169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harati H, Majchrzak M, Cosquer B, Galani R, Kelche C, Cassel JC, Barbelivien A. Attention and memory in aged rats: Impact of lifelong environmental enrichment. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;32:718–736. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartley T, Burgess N. Complementary memory systems: competition, cooperation and compensation. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:169–170. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebda-Bauer EK, Morano MI, Therrien B. Aging and corticosterone injections affect spatial learning in Fischer-344 X Brown norway rats. Brain Res. 1999;827:93–103. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01310-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hidalgo C, Bull R, Behrens MI, Donoso P. Redox regulation of RyR-mediated Ca2+ release in muscle and neurons. Biol Res. 2004;37:539–552. doi: 10.4067/s0716-97602004000400007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogge M, Adam S, Collette F. Directed forgetting and aging: the role of retrieval processes, processing speed, and proactive interference. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2008;15:471–491. doi: 10.1080/13825580701878065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holahan MR, Taverna FA, Emrich SM, Louis M, Muller RU, Roder JC, McDonald RJ. Impairment in long-term retention but not short-term performance on a water maze reversal task following hippocampal or mediodorsal striatal N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:1563–1571. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holderbach R, Clark K, Moreau JL, Bischofberger J, Normann C. Enhanced long-term synaptic depression in an animal model of depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu KS, Huang CC, Liang YC, Wu HM, Chen YL, Lo SW, Ho WC. Alterations in the balance of protein kinase and phosphatase activities and age-related impairments of synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation. Hippocampus. 2002;12:787–802. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huddleston AT, Tang W, Takeshima H, Hamilton SL, Klann E. Superoxide-induced potentiation in the hippocampus requires activation of ryanodine receptor type 3 and ERK. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:1565–1571. doi: 10.1152/jn.00659.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert FA, Kopelman MD. Rates of forgetting in normal ageing: a comparison with dementia. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:849–860. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingram DK. Complex maze learning in rodents as a model of age-related memory impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:475–485. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80101-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingram DK, Joseph JA, Spangler EL, Roberts D, Hengemihle J, Fanelli RJ. Chronic nimodipine treatment in aged rats: analysis of motor and cognitive effects and muscarinic-induced striatal dopamine release. Neurobiol Aging. 1994;15:55–61. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(94)90144-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivy GO, Rick JT, Murphy MP, Head E, Reid C, Milgram NW. Effects of L-deprenyl on manifestations of aging in the rat and dog. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1994;717:45–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1994.tb12072.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson TC, Foster TC. Regional Health and Function in the hippocampus: Evolutionary compromises for a critical brain region. Biosci Hypotheses. 2009;2:245–251. doi: 10.1016/j.bihy.2009.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson TC, Verrier JD, Semple-Rowland S, Kumar A, Foster TC. PHLPP1 splice variants differentially regulate AKT and PKCalpha signaling in hippocampal neurons: characterization of PHLPP proteins in the adult hippocampus. J Neurochem. 2010;115:941–955. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06984.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson L, Zhang R, Elliffe D, Chen KF, Mathai S, McCarthy D, Waldvogel H, Guan J. Correlation of cellular changes and spatial memory during aging in rats. Exp Gerontol. 2008;43:929–938. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen P, Schmelter A, Heil M. Spatial knowledge acquisition in younger and elderly adults: a study in a virtual environment. Exp Psychol. 2010;57:54–60. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerusalinsky D, Ferreira MB, Walz R, Da Silva RC, Bianchin M, Ruschel AC, Zanatta MS, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Amnesia by post-training infusion of glutamate receptor antagonists into the amygdala, hippocampus, and entorhinal cortex. Behav Neural Biol. 1992;58:76–80. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(92)90982-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joels M, de Kloet ER. Effect of corticosteroid hormones on electrical activity in rat hippocampus. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 1991;40:83–86. doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(91)90170-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaczorowski CC, Disterhoft JF. Memory deficits are associated with impaired ability to modulate neuronal excitability in middle-aged mice. Learn Mem. 2009;16:362–366. doi: 10.1101/lm.1365609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kametani H, Bresnahan EL, Chachich ME, Spangler EL, Ingram DK. Comparison of retention performance between young rats with fimbria-fornix lesions and aged rats in a 14-unit T-maze. Behav Brain Res. 1989;35:253–263. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(89)80145-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamsler A, Segal M. Paradoxical actions of hydrogen peroxide on long-term potentiation in transgenic superoxide dismutase-1 mice. J Neurosci. 2003;23:10359–10367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-32-10359.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kavushansky A, Vouimba RM, Cohen H, Richter-Levin G. Activity and plasticity in the CA1, the dentate gyrus, and the amygdala following controllable vs. uncontrollable water stress. Hippocampus. 2006;16:35–42. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage FH. Experience-induced neurogenesis in the senescent dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3206–3212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03206.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Dakis M. Phencyclidine injections into the dorsal hippocampus disrupt long- but not short-term memory within a spatial learning task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;120:203–208. doi: 10.1007/BF02246194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Hunsaker MR. The temporal attributes of episodic memory. Behav Brain Res. 2010;215:299–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Lee I, Gilbert P. A behavioral assessment of hippocampal function based on a subregional analysis. Rev Neurosci. 2004;15:333–351. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2004.15.5.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JJ, Fanselow MS, DeCola JP, Landeira-Fernandez J. Selective impairment of long-term but not short-term conditional fear by the N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist APV. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:591–596. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.4.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim R, Moki R, Kida S. Molecular mechanisms for the destabilization and restabilization of reactivated spatial memory in the Morris water maze. Mol Brain. 2011;4:9. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-4-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kral VA. Senescent forgetfulness: benign and malignant. Can Med Assoc J. 1962;86:257–260. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramar EA, Lin B, Lin CY, Arai AC, Gall CM, Lynch G. A novel mechanism for the facilitation of theta-induced long-term potentiation by brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J Neurosci. 2004;24:5151–5161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0800-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer JH, Mungas D, Reed BR, Wetzel ME, Burnett MM, Miller BL, Weiner MW, Chui HC. Longitudinal MRI and cognitive change in healthy elderly. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:412–418. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn HG, Dickinson-Anson H, Gage FH. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat: age-related decrease of neuronal progenitor proliferation. J Neurosci. 1996;16:2027–2033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-06-02027.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kukolja J, Thiel CM, Wilms M, Mirzazade S, Fink GR. Ageing-related changes of neural activity associated with spatial contextual memory. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:630–645. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Bodhinathan K, Foster TC. Susceptibility to calcium dysregulation during brain aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2009;1 doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.002.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Foster T. Environmental enrichment decreases the afterhyperpolarization in senescent rats. Brain Res. 2007a;1130:103–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.10.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Foster TC. Enhanced long-term potentiation during aging is masked by processes involving intracellular calcium stores. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2437–2444. doi: 10.1152/jn.01148.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Foster TC. Intracellular calcium stores contribute to increased susceptibility to LTD induction during aging. Brain Res. 2005;1031:125–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Foster TC. Neurophysiology of Old Neurons and Synapses. In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. CRC Press; Boca Raton (FL): 2007b. pp. 229–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Foster TC. Shift in induction mechanisms underlies an age-dependent increase in DHPG-induced synaptic depression at CA3 CA1 synapses. J Neurophysiol. 2007c;98:2729–2736. doi: 10.1152/jn.00514.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Rani A, Foster TC. Memory consolidation deficit is associated with decreased NMDA receptor synaptic responses at CA3–CA1 hippocampal synapses. Soc Neurosci Abst 2011a [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Rani A, Tchigranova O, Lee WH, Foster TC. Influence of late-life exposure to environmental enrichment or exercise on hippocampal function and CA1 senescent physiology. Neurobiol Aging. 2011b doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Thinschmidt JS, Foster TC, King MA. Aging effects on the limits and stability of long-term synaptic potentiation and depression in rat hippocampal area CA1. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:594–601. doi: 10.1152/jn.00249.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lal H, Pogacar S, Daly PR, Puri SK. Behavioral and neuropathological manifestations of nutritionally induced central nervous system “aging” in the rat. Prog Brain Res. 1973;40:128–140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landfield PW, McGaugh JL, Lynch G. Impaired synaptic potentiation processes in the hippocampus of aged, memory-deficient rats. Brain Res. 1978;150:85–101. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90655-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaSarge CL, Montgomery KS, Tucker C, Slaton GS, Griffith WH, Setlow B, Bizon JL. Deficits across multiple cognitive domains in a subset of aged Fischer 344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:928–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaSarge CL, Nicolle MM. Comparison of different cognitive rat models of human aging. In: Bizon JL, Woods AG, editors. Animal models of human cognitive aging. Humana Press; 2009. pp. 73–102. [Google Scholar]
- Lashgari R, Motamedi F, Zahedi Asl S, Shahidi S, Komaki A. Behavioral and electrophysiological studies of chronic oral administration of L-type calcium channel blocker verapamil on learning and memory in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;171:324–328. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Jeune H, Cecyre D, Rowe W, Meaney MJ, Quirion R. Ionotropic glutamate receptor subtypes in the aged memory-impaired and unimpaired Long-Evans rat. Neuroscience. 1996;74:349–363. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(96)00213-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee HK, Min SS, Gallagher M, Kirkwood A. NMDA receptor-independent long-term depression correlates with successful aging in rats. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1657–1659. doi: 10.1038/nn1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee I, Kesner RP. Differential contribution of NMDA receptors in hippocampal subregions to spatial working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:162–168. doi: 10.1038/nn790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee WH, Kumar A, Rani A, Herrera J, Xu J, Someya S, Foster TC. Influence of viral vector-mediated delivery of superoxide dismutase and catalase to the hippocampus on spatial learning and memory during aging. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2011 doi: 10.1089/ars.2011.4054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leighty RE, Nilsson LN, Potter H, Costa DA, Low MA, Bales KR, Paul SM, Arendash GW. Use of multimetric statistical analysis to characterize and discriminate between the performance of four Alzheimer’s transgenic mouse lines differing in Abeta deposition. Behav Brain Res. 2004;153:107–121. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutner S, Eckert A, Muller WE. ROS generation, lipid peroxidation and antioxidant enzyme activities in the aging brain. J Neural Transm. 2001;108:955–967. doi: 10.1007/s007020170015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levere TE, Walker A. Old age and cognition: enhancement of recent memory in aged rats by the calcium channel blocker nimodipine. Neurobiol Aging. 1992;13:63–66. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(92)90010-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy A, Kong RM, Stillman MJ, Shukitt-Hale B, Kadar T, Rauch TM, Lieberman HR. Nimodipine improves spatial working memory and elevates hippocampal acetylcholine in young rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;39:781–786. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90164-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Z, Zhou Q, Li L, Mao R, Wang M, Peng W, Dong Z, Xu L, Cao J. Effects of unconditioned and conditioned aversive stimuli in an intense fear conditioning paradigm on synaptic plasticity in the hippocampal CA1 area in vivo. Hippocampus. 2005;15:815–824. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liebmann L, Karst H, Sidiropoulou K, van Gemert N, Meijer OC, Poirazi P, Joels M. Differential effects of corticosterone on the slow afterhyperpolarization in the basolateral amygdala and CA1 region: possible role of calcium channel subunits. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:958–968. doi: 10.1152/jn.01137.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linden A, Gulden M, Martin HJ, Maser E, Seibert H. Peroxide-induced cell death and lipid peroxidation in C6 glioma cells. Toxicol In Vitro. 2008;22:1371–1376. doi: 10.1016/j.tiv.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindner MD. Reliability, distribution, and validity of age-related cognitive deficits in the Morris water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68:203–220. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luparini MR, Del Vecchio A, Barillari G, Magnani M, Prosdocimi M. Cognitive impairment in old rats: a comparison of object displacement, object recognition and water maze. Aging (Milano) 2000;12:264–273. doi: 10.1007/BF03339846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch MA. Age-related neuroinflammatory changes negatively impact on neuronal function. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2009;1 doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.006.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma YY, Yu P, Guo CY, Cui CL. Effects of ifenprodil on morphine-induced conditioned place preference and spatial learning and memory in rats. Neurochem Res. 2010;36:383–391. doi: 10.1007/s11064-010-0342-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mabry TR, McCarty R, Gold PE, Foster TC. Age and stress history effects on spatial performance in a swim task in Fischer-344 rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1996;66:1–10. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macdonald SW, Stigsdotter-Neely A, Derwinger A, Backman L. Rate of acquisition, adult age, and basic cognitive abilities predict forgetting: new views on a classic problem. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2006;135:368–390. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maei HR, Zaslavsky K, Teixeira CM, Frankland PW. What is the Most Sensitive Measure of Water Maze Probe Test Performance? Front Integr Neurosci. 2009;3:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.004.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnusson KR, Brim BL, Das SR. Selective vulnerabilities of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors during brain aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:11. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnusson KR, Scruggs B, Aniya J, Wright KC, Ontl T, Xing Y, Bai L. Age-related deficits in mice performing working memory tasks in a water maze. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:485–495. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malleret G, Alarcon JM, Martel G, Takizawa S, Vronskaya S, Yin D, Chen IZ, Kandel ER, Shumyatsky GP. Bidirectional regulation of hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity and its influence on opposing forms of memory. J Neurosci. 2010;30:3813–3825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1330-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malleret G, Haditsch U, Genoux D, Jones MW, Bliss TV, Vanhoose AM, Weitlauf C, Kandel ER, Winder DG, Mansuy IM. Inducible and reversible enhancement of learning, memory, and long-term potentiation by genetic inhibition of calcineurin. Cell. 2001;104:675–686. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(01)00264-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mamczarz J, Budziszewska B, Antkiewicz-Michaluk L, Vetulani J. The Ca2+ channel blockade changes the behavioral and biochemical effects of immobilization stress. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:248–254. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00071-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowska AL, Koliatsos VE, Breckler SJ, Price DL, Olton DS. Human nerve growth factor improves spatial memory in aged but not in young rats. J Neurosci. 1994;14:4815–4824. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-08-04815.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowska AL, Savonenko A. Retardation of cognitive aging by lifelong diet restriction: implications for genetic variance. Neurobiol Aging. 2002;23:75–86. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00249-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowska AL, Stone WS, Ingram DK, Reynolds J, Gold PE, Conti LH, Pontecorvo MJ, Wenk GL, Olton DS. Individual differences in aging: behavioral and neurobiological correlates. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:31–43. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(89)80008-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ, Clark RE. The rodent hippocampus and spatial memory: from synapses to systems. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2007;64:401–431. doi: 10.1007/s00018-007-6336-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez JL, Jr, Schulteis G, Janak PH, Weinberger SB. Behavioral assessment of forgetting in aged rodents and its relationship to peripheral sympathetic function. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:697–708. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80135-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matzel LD, Grossman H, Light K, Townsend D, Kolata S. Age-related declines in general cognitive abilities of Balb/C mice are associated with disparities in working memory, body weight, and general activity. Learn Mem. 2008;15:733–746. doi: 10.1101/lm.954808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald RJ, Hong NS, Craig LA, Holahan MR, Louis M, Muller RU. NMDA-receptor blockade by CPP impairs post-training consolidation of a rapidly acquired spatial representation in rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:1201–1213. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04272.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Plasticity of the hippocampus: adaptation to chronic stress and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2001;933:265–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05830.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh TJ, Jones MW, Quinn JJ, Balthasar N, Coppari R, Elmquist JK, Lowell BB, Fanselow MS, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science (New York, NY. 2007;317:94–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1140263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKinney BC, Murphy GG. The L-Type voltage-gated calcium channel Cav1.3 mediates consolidation, but not extinction, of contextually conditioned fear in mice. Learn Mem. 2006;13:584–589. doi: 10.1101/lm.279006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLamb RL, Williams LR, Nanry KP, Wilson WA, Tilson HA. MK-801 impedes the acquisition of a spatial memory task in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:41–45. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90038-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Rao G, Baldwin J, Rasmussen M. Long-term enhancement of hippocampal synaptic transmission and the acquisition of spatial information. J Neurosci. 1986;6:563–571. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-02-00563.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Means LW, Kennard KJ. Working memory and the aged rat: deficient two-choice win-stay water-escape acquisition and retention. Physiol Behav. 1991;49:301–307. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90047-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merrill DA, Karim R, Darraq M, Chiba AA, Tuszynski MH. Hippocampal cell genesis does not correlate with spatial learning ability in aged rats. J Comp Neurol. 2003;459:201–207. doi: 10.1002/cne.10616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miettinen R, Sirvio J, Riekkinen P, Sr, Laakso MP, Riekkinen M, Riekkinen P., Jr Neocortical, hippocampal and septal parvalbumin- and somatostatin-containing neurons in young and aged rats: correlation with passive avoidance and water maze performance. Neuroscience. 1993;53:367–378. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90201-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell DB, Brown AS, Murphy DR. Dissociations between procedural and episodic memory: effects of time and aging. Psychol Aging. 1990;5:264–276. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.5.2.264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyagawa H, Hasegawa M, Fukuta T, Amano M, Yamada K, Nabeshima T. Dissociation of impairment between spatial memory, and motor function and emotional behavior in aged rats. Behav Brain Res. 1998;91:73–81. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00105-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffat SD, Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM, Raz N. Extrahippocampal contributions to age differences in human spatial navigation. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1274–1282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffat SD, Resnick SM. Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:851–859. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.5.851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffat SD, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Age differences in spatial memory in a virtual environment navigation task. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22:787–796. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00251-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mondadori C, Weiskrantz L, Buerki H, Petschke F, Fagg GE. NMDA receptor antagonists can enhance or impair learning performance in animals. Exp Brain Res. 1989;75:449–456. doi: 10.1007/BF00249896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore CI, Browning MD, Rose GM. Hippocampal plasticity induced by primed burst, but not long-term potentiation, stimulation is impaired in area CA1 of aged Fischer 344 rats. Hippocampus. 1993;3:57–66. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretti M, de Souza AG, de Chaves G, de Andrade VM, Romao PR, Gavioli EC, Boeck CR. Emotional behavior in middle-aged rats: Implications for geriatric psychopathologies. Physiol Behav. 2011;102:115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG. Synaptic plasticity and learning: selective impairment of learning rats and blockade of long-term potentiation in vivo by the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist AP5. J Neurosci. 1989;9:3040–3057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-09-03040.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG, Anderson E, Lynch GS, Baudry M. Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature. 1986;319:774–776. doi: 10.1038/319774a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG, Davis S, Butcher SP. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptors: a role in information storage? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1990a;329:187–204. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1990.0164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG, Moser EI, Riedel G, Martin SJ, Sandin J, Day M, O’Carroll C. Elements of a neurobiological theory of the hippocampus: the role of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG, Schenk F, Tweedie F, Jarrard LE. Ibotenate Lesions of Hippocampus and/or Subiculum: Dissociating Components of Allocentric Spatial Learning. Eur J Neurosci. 1990b;2:1016–1028. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1990.tb00014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser EI, Krobert KA, Moser MB, Morris RG. Impaired spatial learning after saturation of long-term potentiation. Science (New York, NY. 1998;281:2038–2042. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5385.2038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moyer JR, Jr, Brown TH. Impaired trace and contextual fear conditioning in aged rats. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:612–624. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mueller SG, Stables L, Du AT, Schuff N, Truran D, Cashdollar N, Weiner MW. Measurement of hippocampal subfields and age-related changes with high resolution MRI at 4T. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:719–726. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mungas D, Beckett L, Harvey D, Farias ST, Reed B, Carmichael O, Olichney J, Miller J, DeCarli C. Heterogeneity of cognitive trajectories in diverse older persons. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:606–619. doi: 10.1037/a0019502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mungas D, Harvey D, Reed BR, Jagust WJ, DeCarli C, Beckett L, Mack WJ, Kramer JH, Weiner MW, Schuff N, Chui HC. Longitudinal volumetric MRI change and rate of cognitive decline. Neurology. 2005;65:565–571. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000172913.88973.0d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nacher J, Alonso-Llosa G, Rosell DR, McEwen BS. NMDA receptor antagonist treatment increases the production of new neurons in the aged rat hippocampus. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:273–284. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00096-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakashiba T, Young JZ, McHugh TJ, Buhl DL, Tonegawa S. Transgenic inhibition of synaptic transmission reveals role of CA3 output in hippocampal learning. Science (New York, NY. 2008;319:1260–1264. doi: 10.1126/science.1151120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakazawa K, Quirk MC, Chitwood RA, Watanabe M, Yeckel MF, Sun LD, Kato A, Carr CA, Johnston D, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science (New York, NY. 2002;297:211–218. doi: 10.1126/science.1071795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakazawa K, Sun LD, Quirk MC, Rondi-Reig L, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neuron. 2003;38:305–315. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00165-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namestkova K, Simonova Z, Sykova E. Decreased proliferation in the adult rat hippocampus after exposure to the Morris water maze and its reversal by fluoxetine. Behav Brain Res. 2005;163:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narang N, Joseph JA, Ayyagari PV, Gerber M, Crews FT. Age-related loss of cholinergic-muscarinic coupling to PLC: comparison with changes in brain regional PLC subtypes mRNA distribution. Brain Res. 1996;708:143–152. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01272-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newcomer JW, Krystal JH. NMDA receptor regulation of memory and behavior in humans. Hippocampus. 2001;11:529–542. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolle MM, Bizon JL, Gallagher M. In vitro autoradiography of ionotropic glutamate receptors in hippocampus and striatum of aged Long-Evans rats: relationship to spatial learning. Neuroscience. 1996;74:741–756. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(96)00147-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris CM, Foster TC. MK-801 improves retention in aged rats: implications for altered neural plasticity in age-related memory deficits. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1999;71:194–206. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris CM, Halpain S, Foster TC. Reversal of age-related alterations in synaptic plasticity by blockade of L-type Ca2+ channels. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3171–3179. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03171.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris CM, Korol DL, Foster TC. Increased susceptibility to induction of long-term depression and long-term potentiation reversal during aging. J Neurosci. 1996;16:5382–5392. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-17-05382.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyffeler M, Yee BK, Feldon J, Knuesel I. Abnormal differentiation of newborn granule cells in age-related working memory impairments. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:1956–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oh MM, Oliveira FA, Disterhoft JF. Learning and aging related changes in intrinsic neuronal excitability. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2 doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.002.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oler JA, Markus EJ. Age-related deficits on the radial maze and in fear conditioning: hippocampal processing and consolidation. Hippocampus. 1998;8:402–415. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:4<402::AID-HIPO8>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliet SH, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA. Two distinct forms of long-term depression coexist in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron. 1997;18:969–982. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80336-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira L, Graeff FG, Pereira SR, Oliveira-Silva IF, Franco GC, Ribeiro AM. Correlations among central serotonergic parameters and age-related emotional and cognitive changes assessed through the elevated T-maze and the Morris water maze. Age (Dordr) 2010;32:187–196. doi: 10.1007/s11357-009-9123-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ooishi Y, Mukai H, Hojo Y, Murakami G, Hasegawa Y, Shindo T, Morrison JH, Kimoto T, Kawato S. Estradiol Rapidly Rescues Synaptic Transmission from Corticosterone-induced Suppression via Synaptic/Extranuclear Steroid Receptors in the Hippocampus. Cereb Cortex. 2011 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ordy JM, Thomas GJ, Volpe BT, Dunlap WP, Colombo PM. An animal model of human-type memory loss based on aging, lesion, forebrain ischemia, and drug studies with the rat. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:667–683. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80131-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, Teather LA. Posttraining injections of MK-801 produce a time-dependent impairment of memory in two water maze tasks. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68:42–50. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer MJ, Irving AJ, Seabrook GR, Jane DE, Collingridge GL. The group I mGlu receptor agonist DHPG induces a novel form of LTD in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:1517–1532. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(97)00181-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paris JJ, Walf AA, Frye CA. II. Cognitive performance of middle-aged female rats is influenced by capacity to metabolize progesterone in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Brain Res. 2010;1379:149–163. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park DC, Royal D, Dudley W, Morrell R. Forgetting of pictures over a long retention interval in young and older adults. Psychol Aging. 1988;3:94–95. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.3.1.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellmar TC, Hollinden GE, Sarvey JM. Free radicals accelerate the decay of long-term potentiation in field CA1 of guinea-pig hippocampus. Neuroscience. 1991;44:353–359. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90060-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penner MR, Roth TL, Barnes CA, Sweatt JD. An epigenetic hypothesis of aging-related cognitive dysfunction. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plancher G, Gyselinck V, Nicolas S, Piolino P. Age effect on components of episodic memory and feature binding: A virtual reality study. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:379–390. doi: 10.1037/a0018680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porras A, Sanz B, Mora F. Dopamine-glutamate interactions in the prefrontal cortex of the conscious rat: studies on ageing. Mech Ageing Dev. 1997;99:9–17. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(97)00084-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potier B, Turpin FR, Sinet PM, Rouaud E, Mothet JP, Videau C, Epelbaum J, Dutar P, Billard JM. Contribution of the d-Serine-Dependent Pathway to the Cellular Mechanisms Underlying Cognitive Aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.001.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power JM, Wu WW, Sametsky E, Oh MM, Disterhoft JF. Age-related enhancement of the slow outward calcium-activated potassium current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in vitro. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7234–7243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-07234.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quartermain D, deSoria VG, Kwan A. Calcium channel antagonists enhance retention of passive avoidance and maze learning in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2001;75:77–90. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1999.3958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quartermain D, Hawxhurst A, Ermita B, Puente J. Effect of the calcium channel blocker amlodipine on memory in mice. Behav Neural Biol. 1993;60:211–219. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90390-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quevedo J, Vianna M, Daroit D, Born AG, Kuyven CR, Roesler R, Quillfeldt JA. L-type voltage-dependent calcium channel blocker nifedipine enhances memory retention when infused into the hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69:320–325. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn JJ, Ma QD, Tinsley MR, Koch C, Fanselow MS. Inverse temporal contributions of the dorsal hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to the expression of long-term fear memories. Learn Mem. 2008;15:368–372. doi: 10.1101/lm.813608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajah MN, Kromas M, Han JE, Pruessner JC. Group differences in anterior hippocampal volume and in the retrieval of spatial and temporal context memory in healthy young versus older adults. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:4020–4030. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapp PR, Amaral DG. Individual differences in the cognitive and neurobiological consequences of normal aging. Trends Neurosci. 1992;15:340–345. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90051-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapp PR, Rosenberg RA, Gallagher M. An evaluation of spatial information processing in aged rats. Behav Neurosci. 1987;101:3–12. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.101.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawlins JN, Tsaltas E. The hippocampus, time and working memory. Behav Brain Res. 1983;10:233–262. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90033-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65:405–415. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rex CS, Kramar EA, Colgin LL, Lin B, Gall CM, Lynch G. Long-term potentiation is impaired in middle-aged rats: regional specificity and reversal by adenosine receptor antagonists. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5956–5966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0880-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riekkinen M, Schmidt B, Kuitunen J, Riekkinen P., Jr Effects of combined chronic nimodipine and acute metrifonate treatment on spatial and avoidance behavior. Eur J Pharmacol. 1997;322:1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(96)00976-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts M, Shapiro M. NMDA receptor antagonists impair memory for nonspatial, socially transmitted food preference. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:1059–1069. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.6.1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robillard JM, Gordon GR, Choi HB, Christie BR, MacVicar BA. Glutathione restores the mechanism of synaptic plasticity in aged mice to that of the adult. PLoS One. 2011;6:e20676. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roman FS, Alescio-Lautier B, Soumireu-Mourat B. Age-related learning and memory deficits in odor-reward association in rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)02030-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose GM, Ong VS, Woodruff-Pak DS. Efficacy of MEM 1003, a novel calcium channel blocker, in delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in older rabbits. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:766–773. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenzweig ES, Barnes CA. Impact of aging on hippocampal function: plasticity, network dynamics, and cognition. Prog Neurobiol. 2003;69:143–179. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00126-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossato JI, Bonini JS, Coitinho AS, Vianna MR, Medina JH, Cammarota M, Izquierdo I. Retrograde amnesia induced by drugs acting on different molecular systems. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:563–568. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe WB, Spreekmeester E, Meaney MJ, Quirion R, Rochford J. Reactivity to novelty in cognitively-impaired and cognitively-unimpaired aged rats and young rats. Neuroscience. 1998;83:669–680. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00464-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan BK, Vollmayr B, Klyubin I, Gass P, Rowan MJ. Persistent inhibition of hippocampal long-term potentiation in vivo by learned helplessness stress. Hippocampus. 2010;20:758–767. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabolek HR, Bunce JG, Giuliana D, Chrobak JJ. Within-subject memory decline in middle-aged rats: effects of intraseptal tacrine. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:1221–1229. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakimura K, Kutsuwada T, Ito I, Manabe T, Takayama C, Kushiya E, Yagi T, Aizawa S, Inoue Y, Sugiyama H, et al. Reduced hippocampal LTP and spatial learning in mice lacking NMDA receptor epsilon 1 subunit. Nature. 1995;373:151–155. doi: 10.1038/373151a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samorajski T, Delaney C, Durham L, Ordy JM, Johnson JA, Dunlap WP. Effect of exercise on longevity, body weight, locomotor performance, and passive-avoidance memory of C57BL/6J mice. Neurobiol Aging. 1985;6:17–24. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(85)90066-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandin M, Jasmin S, Levere TE. Aging and cognition: facilitation of recent memory in aged nonhuman primates by nimodipine. Neurobiol Aging. 1990;11:573–575. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(90)90120-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saucier D, Hargreaves EL, Boon F, Vanderwolf CH, Cain DP. Detailed behavioral analysis of water maze acquisition under systemic NMDA or muscarinic antagonism: nonspatial pretraining eliminates spatial learning deficits. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:103–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimanski LA, Barnes CA. Neural Protein Synthesis during Aging: Effects on Plasticity and Memory. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider A, Huentelman MJ, Kremerskothen J, Duning K, Spoelgen R, Nikolich K. KIBRA: A New Gateway to Learning and Memory? Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.004.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonknecht P, Pantel J, Kruse A, Schroder J. Prevalence and natural course of aging-associated cognitive decline in a population-based sample of young-old subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:2071–2077. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz D, Huston JP, Jezek K, Haas HL, Roth-Harer A, Selbach O, Luhmann HJ. Water maze performance, exploratory activity, inhibitory avoidance and hippocampal plasticity in aged superior and inferior learners. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:2175–2185. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02282.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz RD, Bernabei AA, Spencer CJ, Pugsley TA. Loss of muscarinic M1 receptors with aging in the cerebral cortex of Fisher 344 rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;35:589–593. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90295-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seoane A, Massey PV, Keen H, Bashir ZI, Brown MW. L-type voltage-dependent calcium channel antagonists impair perirhinal long-term recognition memory and plasticity processes. J Neurosci. 2009;29:9534–9544. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5199-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sexton CE, Mackay CE, Lonie JA, Bastin ME, Terriere E, O’Carroll RE, Ebmeier KP. MRI correlates of episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy aging. Psychiatry Res. 2010;184:57–62. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shacter E. Quantification and significance of protein oxidation in biological samples. Drug Metab Rev. 2000;32:307–326. doi: 10.1081/dmr-100102336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shankar S, Teyler TJ, Robbins N. Aging differentially alters forms of long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal area CA1. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:334–341. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shetty PK, Huang FL, Huang KP. Ischemia-elicited oxidative modulation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. J Biol Chem. 2008;283:5389–5401. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M708479200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimizu E, Tang YP, Rampon C, Tsien JZ. NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic reinforcement as a crucial process for memory consolidation. Science (New York, NY. 2000;290:1170–1174. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5494.1170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinoda Y, Kamikubo Y, Egashira Y, Tominaga-Yoshino K, Ogura A. Repetition of mGluR-dependent LTD causes slowly developing persistent reduction in synaptic strength accompanied by synapse elimination. Brain Res. 2005;1042:99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shoji H, Mizoguchi K. Acute and repeated stress differentially regulates behavioral, endocrine, neural parameters relevant to emotional and stress response in young and aged rats. Behav Brain Res. 2010;211:169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shors TJ, Gallegos RA, Breindl A. Transient and persistent consequences of acute stress on long-term potentiation (LTP), synaptic efficacy, theta rhythms and bursts in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Synapse. 1997;26:209–217. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199707)26:3<209::AID-SYN2>3.0.CO;2-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sierra-Mercado D, Dieguez D, Jr, Barea-Rodriguez EJ. Brief novelty exposure facilitates dentate gyrus LTP in aged rats. Hippocampus. 2008;18:835–843. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skelton RW, Bukach CM, Laurance HE, Thomas KG, Jacobs JW. Humans with traumatic brain injuries show place-learning deficits in computer-generated virtual space. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:157–175. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200004)22:2;1-1;FT157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small SA, Schobel SA, Buxton RB, Witter MP, Barnes CA. A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:585–601. doi: 10.1038/nrn3085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TD, Adams MM, Gallagher M, Morrison JH, Rapp PR. Circuit-specific alterations in hippocampal synaptophysin immunoreactivity predict spatial learning impairment in aged rats. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6587–6593. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06587.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon PR, Wood MS, Groccia-Ellison ME, Yang BY, Fanelli RJ, Mervis RF. Nimodipine facilitates retention of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in aged rabbits over long retention intervals. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16:791–796. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00093-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng M, Rossier J, Schenk F. Spaced training facilitates long-term retention of place navigation in adult but not in adolescent rats. Behav Brain Res. 2002;128:103–108. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00266-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele RJ, Morris RG. Delay-dependent impairment of a matching-to-place task with chronic and intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA-antagonist D-AP5. Hippocampus. 1999;9:118–136. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:2<118::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterlemann V, Rammes G, Wolf M, Liebl C, Ganea K, Muller MB, Schmidt MV. Chronic social stress during adolescence induces cognitive impairment in aged mice. Hippocampus. 2010;20:540–549. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoelzel CR, Stavnezer AJ, Denenberg VH, Ward M, Markus EJ. The effects of aging and dorsal hippocampal lesions: performance on spatial and nonspatial comparable versions of the water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;78:217–233. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2001.4054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoub TR, Rogalski EJ, Leurgans S, Bennett DA, deToledo-Morrell L. Rate of entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and mild AD: relation to memory function. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;31:1089–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanila H, Taira T, Piepponen TP, Honkanen A. Effect of sex and age on brain monoamines and spatial learning in rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1994;15:733–741. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(94)90056-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor G, Farr S, Griffin M, Humphrey W, Weiss J. Adult ontogeny of rat working memory of social interactions. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1999;54:M145–151. doi: 10.1093/gerona/54.3.m145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teixeira CM, Pomedli SR, Maei HR, Kee N, Frankland PW. Involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in the expression of remote spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2006;26:7555–7564. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1068-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thibault O, Landfield PW. Increase in single L-type calcium channels in hippocampal neurons during aging. Science (New York, NY. 1996;272:1017–1020. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5264.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas KG, Laurance HE, Luczak SE, Jacobs WJ. Age-related changes in a human cognitive mapping system: data from a computer-generated environment. Cyberpsychol Behav. 1999;2:545–566. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1999.2.545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tombaugh GC, Rowe WB, Chow AR, Michael TH, Rose GM. Theta-frequency synaptic potentiation in CA1 in vitro distinguishes cognitively impaired from unimpaired aged Fischer 344 rats. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9932–9940. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-09932.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tombaugh GC, Rowe WB, Rose GM. The slow afterhyperpolarization in hippocampal CA1 neurons covaries with spatial learning ability in aged Fisher 344 rats. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2609–2616. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5023-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trompet S, Westendorp RG, Kamper AM, de Craen AJ. Use of calcium antagonists and cognitive decline in old age. The Leiden 85-plus study. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:306–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uekita T, Okaichi H. Pretraining does not ameliorate spatial learning deficits induced by intrahippocampal infusion of AP5. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:520–526. doi: 10.1037/a0015672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uttl B, Graf P. Episodic spatial memory in adulthood. Psychol Aging. 1993;8:257–273. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.8.2.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallee M, MacCari S, Dellu F, Simon H, Le Moal M, Mayo W. Long-term effects of prenatal stress and postnatal handling on age-related glucocorticoid secretion and cognitive performance: a longitudinal study in the rat. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:2906–2916. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00705.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Staay FJ. Assessment of age-associated cognitive deficits in rats: a tricky business. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:753–759. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00062-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Staay FJ, de Jonge M. Effects of age on water escape behavior and on repeated acquisition in rats. Behav Neural Biol. 1993;60:33–41. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90690-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Gemert NG, Joels M. Effect of chronic stress and mifepristone treatment on voltage-dependent Ca2+ currents in rat hippocampal dentate gyrus. J Neuroendocrinol. 2006;18:732–741. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Groen T, Kadish I, Wyss JM. Old rats remember old tricks; memories of the water maze persist for 12 months. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:247–255. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00137-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vasquez BJ, Martinez JL, Jr, Jensen RA, Messing RB, Rigter H, McGaugh JL. Learning and memory in young and aged Fischer 344 rats. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1983;2:279–291. doi: 10.1016/0167-4943(83)90001-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veng LM, Mesches MH, Browning MD. Age-related working memory impairment is correlated with increases in the L-type calcium channel protein alpha1D (Cav1.3) in area CA1 of the hippocampus and both are ameliorated by chronic nimodipine treatment. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2003;110:193–202. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(02)00643-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicens P, Redolat R, Carrasco MC. Effects of early spatial training on water maze performance: a longitudinal study in mice. Exp Gerontol. 2002;37:575–581. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5565(01)00217-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Engelhardt J, Doganci B, Jensen V, Hvalby O, Gongrich C, Taylor A, Barkus C, Sanderson DJ, Rawlins JN, Seeburg PH, Bannerman DM, Monyer H. Contribution of hippocampal and extra-hippocampal NR2B-containing NMDA receptors to performance on spatial learning tasks. Neuron. 2008;60:846–860. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:848–858. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Michaelis EK. Selective neuronal vulnerability to oxidative stress in the brain. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss C, Sametsky E, Sasse A, Spiess J, Disterhoft JF. Acute stress facilitates trace eyeblink conditioning in C57BL/6 male mice and increases the excitability of their CA1 pyramidal neurons. Learn Mem. 2005;12:138–143. doi: 10.1101/lm.89005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whishaw IQ, Cassel JC, Jarrad LE. Rats with fimbria-fornix lesions display a place response in a swimming pool: a dissociation between getting there and knowing where. J Neurosci. 1995;15:5779–5788. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-08-05779.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White JA, McKinney BC, John MC, Powers PA, Kamp TJ, Murphy GG. Conditional forebrain deletion of the L-type calcium channel Ca V 1.2 disrupts remote spatial memories in mice. Learn Mem. 2008;15:1–5. doi: 10.1101/lm.773208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson IA, Gallagher M, Eichenbaum H, Tanila H. Neurocognitive aging: prior memories hinder new hippocampal encoding. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:662–670. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson IA, Ikonen S, Gallagher M, Eichenbaum H, Tanila H. Age-associated alterations of hippocampal place cells are subregion specific. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6877–6886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1744-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiltgen BJ, Brown RA, Talton LE, Silva AJ. New circuits for old memories: the role of the neocortex in consolidation. Neuron. 2004;44:101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G. A neuropsychological analysis of memory loss with age. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:487–494. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80102-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Hippocampal and prefrontal cortex contributions to learning and memory: analysis of lesion and aging effects on maze learning in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:544–551. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.4.544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Sekeres M. A study of remote spatial memory in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;31:143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodruff-Pak DS, Chi J, Li YT, Pak MH, Fanelli RJ. Nimodipine ameliorates impaired eyeblink classical conditioning in older rabbits in the long-delay paradigm. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:641–649. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(97)00159-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodside BL, Borroni AM, Hammonds MD, Teyler TJ. NMDA receptors and voltage-dependent calcium channels mediate different aspects of acquisition and retention of a spatial memory task. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;81:105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2003.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu K, Meyers CA, Guerra NK, King MA, Meyer EM. The effects of rAAV2-mediated NGF gene delivery in adult and aged rats. Mol Ther. 2004;9:262–269. doi: 10.1016/j.ymthe.2003.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyss JM, Chambless BD, Kadish I, van Groen T. Age-related decline in water maze learning and memory in rats: strain differences. Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21:671–681. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00132-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu L, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Behavioural stress facilitates the induction of long-term depression in the hippocampus. Nature. 1997;387:497–500. doi: 10.1038/387497a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamazaki M, Matsuoka N, Maeda N, Kuratani K, Ohkubo Y, Yamaguchi I. FR121196, a potential antidementia drug, ameliorates the impaired memory of rat in the Morris water maze. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1995;272:256–263. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang CH, Huang CC, Hsu KS. Novelty exploration elicits a reversal of acute stress-induced modulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity in the rat. J Physiol. 2006;577:601–615. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang YJ, Wu PF, Long LH, Yu DF, Wu WN, Hu ZL, Fu H, Xie N, Jin Y, Ni L, Wang JZ, Wang F, Chen JG. Reversal of aging-associated hippocampal synaptic plasticity deficits by reductants via regulation of thiol redox and NMDA receptor function. Aging Cell. 2010;9:709–721. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2010.00595.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yassa MA, Mattfeld AT, Stark SM, Stark CE. Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:8873–8878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101567108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeier Z, Madorsky I, Xu Y, Ogle WO, Notterpek L, Foster TC. Gene expression in the hippocampus: Regionally specific effects of aging and caloric restriction. Mech Ageing Dev. 2010 doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2010.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelinski EM, Burnight KP. Sixteen-year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:503–513. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang HY, Watson ML, Gallagher M, Nicolle MM. Muscarinic receptor-mediated GTP-Eu binding in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex is correlated with spatial memory impairment in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:619–626. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang JJ, Okutani F, Huang GZ, Taniguchi M, Murata Y, Kaba H. Common properties between synaptic plasticity in the main olfactory bulb and olfactory learning in young rats. Neuroscience. 2010;170:259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Q, Homma KJ, Poo MM. Shrinkage of dendritic spines associated with long-term depression of hippocampal synapses. Neuron. 2004;44:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zornetzer SF, Thompson R, Rogers J. Rapid forgetting in aged rats. Behav Neural Biol. 1982;36:49–60. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(82)90234-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zyzak DR, Otto T, Eichenbaum H, Gallagher M. Cognitive decline associated with normal aging in rats: a neuropsychological approach. Learn Mem. 1995;2:1–16. doi: 10.1101/lm.2.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]