Abstract
Research suggests overlap in brain regions undergoing neurodegeneration in Parkinson's and Alzheimer's disease. To assess the clinical significance of this, we applied a validated Alzheimer's disease-spatial pattern of brain atrophy to patients with Parkinson's disease with a range of cognitive abilities to determine its association with cognitive performance and decline. At baseline, 84 subjects received structural magnetic resonance imaging brain scans and completed the Dementia Rating Scale-2, and new robust and expanded Dementia Rating Scale-2 norms were applied to cognitively classify participants. Fifty-nine non-demented subjects were assessed annually with the Dementia Rating Scale-2 for two additional years. Magnetic resonance imaging scans were quantified using both a region of interest approach and voxel-based morphometry analysis, and a method for quantifying the presence of an Alzheimer's disease spatial pattern of brain atrophy was applied to each scan. In multivariate models, higher Alzheimer's disease pattern of atrophy score was associated with worse global cognitive performance (β = −0.31, P = 0.007), including in non-demented patients (β = −0.28, P = 0.05). In linear mixed model analyses, higher baseline Alzheimer's disease pattern of atrophy score predicted long-term global cognitive decline in non-demented patients [F(1, 110) = 9.72, P = 0.002], remarkably even in those with normal cognition at baseline [F(1, 80) = 4.71, P = 0.03]. In contrast, in cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses there was no association between region of interest brain volumes and cognitive performance in patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition. These findings support involvement of the hippocampus and parietal–temporal cortex with cognitive impairment and long-term decline in Parkinson's disease. In addition, an Alzheimer's disease pattern of brain atrophy may be a preclinical biomarker of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease.
Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, dementia, mild cognitive impairment, Parkinson's disease, neurodegeneration
Introduction
Patients with Parkinson's disease are at an increased risk of developing dementia, with cumulative prevalence rates up to 80% reported (Aarsland et al., 2003; Hely et al., 2008) In addition, ∼25% of non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease meet neuropsychological test criteria for mild cognitive impairment (Aarsland et al., 2010), most of whom eventually convert to Parkinson's disease with dementia (Janvin et al., 2006).
The neural substrate of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease is a subject of continued debate (Farlow and Cummings, 2008). Parkinson's disease dementia is associated with diffuse subcortical and cortical Lewy body disease pathology, including the transentorhinal and entorhinal cortices, hippocampus, other limbic cortex regions, and neocortex (Galvin et al., 1999; Braak et al., 2005; Beach et al., 2009). However, many patients with Parkinson's disease also have Alzheimer's disease-related neuropathological changes on autopsy (Lieberman, 1997; Sabbagh et al., 2009; Compta et al., 2011), including in the hippocampus (Apaydin et al., 2002). In addition, lower CSF β-amyloid1–42 (Aβ42) levels have been detected in de novo patients with Parkinson's disease (Alves et al., 2010) and decrease further across the stages of cognitive impairment (Compta et al., 2009), are associated with a range of cognitive deficits in non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease (Compta et al., 2009; Alves et al., 2010), and predict long-term cognitive decline (Siderowf et al., 2010). At a minimum there appears to be some overlap in the neurodegenerative process that occurs in Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's disease, and the pathological processes may even be synergistic (Masliah et al., 2001; Clinton et al., 2010).
Structural brain imaging allows in vivo determination of regional neurodegeneration. In Parkinson's disease, studies using a range of imaging analyses have reported diffuse parietal–temporal and prefrontal cortex atrophy in patients with Parkinson's disease with dementia (Camicioli et al., 2003; Burton et al., 2004; Junqué et al., 2005; Tam et al., 2005; Kenny et al., 2008), but most studies have not controlled for possible confounding variables and some have reported an association between regional brain volumes and cognitive performance (Camicioli et al., 2003; Tam et al., 2005; Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al., 2008; Sanchez-Castaneda et al., 2009; Apostolova et al., 2010). In patients with Parkinson's disease who are non-demented or have mild cognitive impairment, varying degrees of atrophy have been reported for the parietal–temporal cortex, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and amygdala (Brück et al., 2004; Beyer et al., 2007; Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al., 2008; Lyoo et al., 2010; Song et al., 2011), again with mixed evidence for correlation between atrophy and either neuropsychological test performance or conversion to Parkinson's disease with dementia (Brück et al., 2004; Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al., 2008; Aybek et al., 2009; Jokinen et al., 2009; Martin et al., 2009).
Voxel-based morphometry analyses aim to identify group differences, but are not suitable for deriving diagnostic biomarkers on an individual patient basis. Therefore, we used a high-dimensional pattern classification methodology developed to classify individual scans as belonging to either healthy controls or patients with Alzheimer's disease (Fan et al., 2007, 2008a; Davatzikos et al., 2008). The pattern classification method provides an individual-based score called the Spatial Pattern of Abnormalities for Recognition of Alzheimer's disease (SPARE-AD) score, which has been determined via a ‘training’ database of healthy control subjects and patients with Alzheimer's disease and uses atrophy in the following regions: most of the temporal lobe (especially the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, inferior temporal cortex and uncus), precuneus, posterior cingulate and peri-hippocampal white matter.
Given the limited understanding of evolution and patterns of neurodegeneration related to cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease, we report: (i) the relationship between SPARE-AD scores and cognitive performance in a cohort of patients with Parkinson's disease with a range of cognitive abilities; and (ii) the association between baseline SPARE-AD score and long-term cognitive decline in non-demented Parkinson's disease patients, including those with normal cognition.
Materials and methods
Participants
Baseline data (n = 84) were obtained as part of the University of Pennsylvania Center of Excellence for Research on Neurodegenerative Diseases (CERND) (http://cernd.org/), a cross-sectional study that evaluated individuals at risk for late-life dementia with a range of biomarkers. Based on standardized Dementia Rating Scale (DRS-2) score there were 69 non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease at baseline, and 60 of these participants agreed to enrol in the Penn Udall Center for Parkinson's Research at the University of Pennsylvania (http://www.med.upenn.edu/udall/) and were followed longitudinally and administered the DRS-2 annually for at least two additional years (i.e. 2-year follow-up). One of these subjects had a >20% decline in DRS-2 score during this period, was considered an outlier, and was removed from the longitudinal sample. For the remaining 59 non-demented Parkinson's disease subjects followed long-term, none had a >10% increase in DRS-2 score, and only two subjects had a >10% decrease in DRS-2 score from baseline, consistent with what would be expected in a Parkinson's disease population over this time period (Troster et al., 2007).
Disease severity was based on UPDRS (Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale) motor score and Hoehn and Yahr stage (Fahn et al., 1987). Levodopa and dopamine agonist dosages were combined and are presented as levodopa equivalent daily dosage (Hobson et al., 2005). Participants were categorized as having postural instability–gait difficulty subtype (as opposed to non-postural instability–gait difficulty subtype) based on published criteria using UPDRS scores (Jankovic et al., 1990). Psychosis was assessed with the UPDRS Part I ‘Thought Disorder’ item. Given the small number of participants rated as having psychotic symptoms (i.e. hallucinations or delusions) and the association between vivid dreaming and both psychosis (Arnulf et al., 2000; Forsaa et al., 2010; Goetz et al., 2010) and long-term cognitive decline (Santangelo et al., 2007) in Parkinson's disease, we included presence of ‘thought disorder’ as a variable, defined as any positive score on the UPDRS Part I thought disorder item (i.e. vivid dreaming, hallucinations or delusions). Depression severity was assessed with the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) (Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986).
Neuropsychological testing and cognitive classification
The DRS-2 (Jurica et al., 2001), a measure of global cognitive performance, has been validated as an assessment instrument for Parkinson's disease with dementia (Llebaria et al., 2008), discriminates between Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment and Parkinson's disease with dementia (Martin et al., 2008), and predicts long-term conversion to Parkinson's disease with dementia (Levy et al., 2002). Cognitive categories were defined on the basis of recommended (Jurica et al., 2001) age-standardized DRS-2 scores using new robust and expanded norms (Pedraza et al., 2010): (i) Parkinson's disease with normal cognition (DRS-2 score of >8, which corresponds to >28th percentile); (ii) Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment (DRS-2 score 6–8 inclusive, 6–28th percentile); and (iii) Parkinson's disease with dementia (DRS-2 <6, <6th percentile). The DRS-2 total score is constructed from five subscores: memory, attention, initiation/perseveration, construction and conceptualization.
Structural imaging and analyses
Image acquisition
The data sets included standard T1-weighted magnetic resonance images acquired sagittally using volumetric 3D magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo with 1.25 × 1.25 mm in-plane spatial resolution and 1.2 mm thick sagittal slices (8° flip angle) performed on 1.5 T scanners.
Image analysis
CERND MRI analysis was based on an image processing protocol developed at the Section of Biomedical Image Analysis (SBIA) of the Department of Radiology at the University of Pennsylvania and previously described in detail (Goldszal et al., 1998). Global volumes were obtained via an automated segmentation technique that labels the brain into white matter, grey matter, CSF and ventricles, after a sequence of preprocessing steps that remove extracranial material and aligns each scan with the anterior–posterior commissure plane. Quantification of regional brain volumes is performed through an elastic atlas warping algorithm that coregisters a template of brain anatomy with each individual scan (Shen and Davatzikos, 2002). The template has 97 regions of interest based on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, which are transferred to individual scans, so that regional volumetric and functional measurements can be obtained. These regions of interest were then collapsed into 14 larger regions of interest. To limit the number of variables presented, we calculated the average of the right and left volumes for each region of interest and present grey matter volumes only (hippocampus, medial temporal lobe, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, occipital lobe, frontal lobe, insula, anterior cingulate and posterior cingulate).
In order to further characterize local atrophy in the brain, a voxel-based morphometry analysis, named Regional Analysis of Volumes Examined in Normalized Space (RAVENS) (Shen and Davatzikos, 2003), was performed. This approach computes grey matter, white matter and ventricle tissue density maps separately in a common coordinate system after spatial normalization. The RAVENS approach bears similarities with the ‘optimized voxel-based morphometry’ approach, except it uses a high-dimensional image warping algorithm termed HAMMER (Shen and Davatzikos, 2002, 2003). Moreover, it uses tissue-preserving transformations, which ensure that image warping absolutely preserves the amount of grey matter, white matter and CSF tissue present in an individual's scan. Thus, the RAVENS value in a certain region in the reference space is directly proportional to the amount of tissue present in the respective anatomical region of a subject's scan. Voxel dimensions were 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm.
Pattern classification
Although voxel-based morphometry analysis aims to identify group differences, it is not suitable for deriving diagnostic biomarkers on an individual patient basis. Therefore, we applied to our Parkinson's disease sample a high-dimensional pattern classification approach that was generated using healthy controls and patients with Alzheimer's disease, called COMPARE (Classification of Morphological Patterns Using Adaptive Regional Elements) (Fan et al., 2007, 2008a; Davatzikos et al., 2008). This approach considers all brain regions jointly, and identifies a minimal set of regions whose volumes jointly maximally differentiate the two groups under consideration, on an individual scan basis. Leave-one-out cross-validation is used to test this classification scheme on data sets not used for training, and obtain a relatively unbiased estimate of the generalization power of the classifier to new patients. The pattern classification method provides an individually calculated score, called the SPARE-AD score. For a classifier constructed from healthy controls and patients with Alzheimer's disease, a positive SPARE-AD score implies Alzheimer's disease-like pattern of cerebral atrophy, while a negative score reflects a brain structure associated with normal structure (Fig. 1). The software used to generate SPARE-AD scores is available through the Section of Biomedical Image Analysis at the University of Pennsylvania (http://www.rad.upenn.edu/sbia).
Statistical analysis
Chi-square tests, t-tests (with Levene's test for equality of variances), non-parametric tests to compare medians and ANOVA with post hoc analyses (Tukey's test) were used for between-group comparisons on clinical, demographic, neuropsychological and imaging variables. To determine the association between SPARE-AD score or regions of interest and cognitive performance, all clinical and demographic variables of interest (i.e. those previously associated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease) that were associated with global cognitive performance on bivariate analysis were entered into linear regression models.
Linear mixed model analysis (Laird and Ware, 1982) was used to determine if baseline SPARE-AD score and other baseline demographic and clinical variables predicted cognitive decline over a 2-year period in non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease and patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition, and all relevant clinical, demographic and imaging variables that might be associated with long-term cognitive decline were included as covariates. The mixed model procedure accounts for the correlations that are due to the repeated measurements of DRS-2 over time in the same patients. In our implementation of the mixed model, the intercept and the regression coefficient for the follow-up time (visit) were treated as random effects such that each subject has a unique intercept and regression coefficient for the follow-up time. The population mean coefficient for the follow-up time was obtained by averaging across the subject specific regression coefficients for the follow-up time. This population mean coefficient estimated the average yearly change for DRS-2 score. The following covariates were adjusted in the mixed-effect models and their regression coefficients were treated as fixed effects: baseline DRS-2 score, SPARE-AD score, age, Hoehn and Yahr stage (entered as a dichotomous variable based on the median score due to significant skewing toward mild disease in the longitudinal sample), Parkinson's disease duration, and postural instability–gait difficulty subtype in the primary models. In an additional model, sex, education, total levodopa equivalent daily dosage, Geriatric Depression Scale-15 score and presence of thought disorder were also added as covariates, either individually or as a group. The predictive ability of baseline SPARE-AD and other demographic and clinical variables on cognitive decline was examined through their interactions with follow-up time.
Normality assumptions were checked whenever the tests required normality assumption. All statistical tests were two-sided. Statistical significance was set at P ≤ 0.05. All analyses were conducted using the PASW Statistics (version 18.0) software.
Results
Participant characteristics
At baseline, the mean (SD; range) DRS-2 score for the entire sample was 134.2 (12.5; 74–144), and using recommended age-corrected cut-off scores, 51 patients (60.7%) were classified as having Parkinson's disease with normal cognition, 18 (21.4%) as Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment and 15 (17.9%) as Parkinson's disease with dementia. For the 59 non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease (i.e. combination of Parkinson's disease with normal cognition and Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment) followed longitudinally, the average standardized DRS-2 score at baseline was approximately the 50th percentile; for the subset of 43 patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition followed longitudinally, the average standardized DRS-2 score was approximately the 65th percentile. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics for the cross-sectional (n = 84) and longitudinal (n = 59) cohorts of patients are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variable [mean (SD), % or median] | Cross-sectional cohort (n = 84) | Longitudinal cohort (n = 59) |
---|---|---|
Clinical and demographic | ||
Age [mean (SD) years] | 70.6 (6.5) | 70.0 (6.4) |
Sex (% male) | 66.7% | 62.7% |
Education [mean (SD) years] | 15.7 (2.7) | 16.1 (2.3) |
PD duration [mean (SD) years] | 7.7 (4.8) | 7.4 (4.1) |
Hoehn and Yahr stage [median (interquartile range)] | 2.0 (2.0–2.5) | 2.0 (2.0–2.0) |
Total LEDD [mean (SD) mg/day] | 554 (379)a | 525 (373) |
UPDRS motor score [mean (SD)] | 22.0 (10.2) | 20.3 (9.2) |
PIGD subtype (% yes) | 69.9b | 58.6c |
Thought disorder (% yes) | 25.9d | 27.1 |
GDS-15 [mean (SD)] | 2.5 (2.5)e | 2.3 (2.5) |
DRS-2 | ||
Total raw score [mean (SD)] | 134.2 (12.5) | 138.6 (3.9) |
Age-adjusted standardized score [mean (SD)] | 9.2 (3.9) | 10.4 (2.8) |
PD-NC (% of sample) | 60.7 | 72.9 |
PD-MCI (%) | 21.4 | 27.1 |
PDD (%) | 17.9 | – |
a n = 83.
b n = 83.
c n = 58.
d n = 81.
e n = 79.
GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; LEDD = levodopa equivalent daily dosage; PD = Parkinson's disease; PDD = Parkinson's disease with dementia; PD-MCI = Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment; PDNC = Parkinson's disease with normal cognition; PIGD = postural instability–gait difficulty.
Cross-sectional analyses
SPARE-AD score as correlate of cognitive impairment in entire cohort
On bivariate analysis, increasing age, increasing Parkinson's disease duration, higher Hoehn and Yahr stage, higher UPDRS motor score, and postural instability–gait difficulty subtype were associated with worse total DRS-2 score, so these variables were included as covariates in subsequent linear regression models. There was no association between cognitive performance and intracranial volume, sex, education, total levodopa equivalent daily dosage, Geriatric Depression Scale-15 score and presence of thought disorder. As Hoehn and Yahr stage and UPDRS motor score are both measures of disease severity and were highly correlated, only one of these variables was entered into a given model, with Hoehn and Yahr stage used preferentially.
In a linear regression model including SPARE-AD score and the aforementioned covariates, higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.31, P = 0.007) and increasing Hoehn and Yahr stage (β = −0.29, P = 0.01) were independent predictors of worse total DRS-2 score (Table 2, Model 1). In a model that substituted UPDRS motor score for Hoehn and Yahr stage, higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.35, P = 0.002), higher UPDRS motor score (β = −0.23, P = 0.03), and longer Parkinson's disease duration (β = −0.24, P = 0.02) predicted worse global cognition (Table 2, Model 2).
Table 2.
Models | Unstandardized coefficients |
Standardized coefficients | t | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | Std. Error | Beta | |||
Model 1b | |||||
Age | −0.102 | 0.207 | −0.054 | −0.495 | 0.62 |
PD duration | −0.516 | 0.267 | −0.199 | −1.935 | 0.06 |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | −4.851 | 1.824 | −0.288 | −2.659 | 0.01 |
PIGD subtype | −0.633 | 2.638 | −0.023 | −0.240 | 0.81 |
SPARE-AD score | −4.841 | 1.748 | −0.310 | −2.769 | 0.007 |
Model 2c | |||||
Age | −0.068 | 0.213 | −0.036 | −0.320 | 0.75 |
PD duration | −0.625 | 0.263 | −0.241 | −2.380 | 0.02 |
UPDRS motor score | −0.285 | 0.127 | −0.233 | −2.242 | 0.03 |
PIGD subtype | −1.562 | 2.649 | −0.058 | −0.590 | 0.56 |
SPARE-AD score | −5.454 | 1.721 | −0.350 | −3.169 | 0.002 |
a Total raw DRS-2 score.
b Linear regression model with baseline DRS-2 score as dependent variable and Hoehn and Yahr stage as measure of disease severity [F = 8.53 (df = 5,77), P < 0.001].
c Linear regression model with baseline DRS-2 score as dependent variable and UPDRS motor score as measure of disease severity [F = 7.95 (df = 5,77), P < 0.001].
PD = Parkinson's disease; PIGD = postural instability–gait difficulty.
Substituting DRS-2 subtest scores for DRS-2 total score in the original model, only higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.32, P = 0.007) was associated with worse memory. Higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.38, P = 0.004) and longer Parkinson's disease duration (β = −0.23, P = 0.04) predicted worse conceptualization performance, and higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.25, P = 0.04) and increasing Hoehn and Yahr stage (β = −0.23, P = 0.05) predicted worse initiation/perseveration performance. Only increasing Hoehn and Yahr stage predicted worse construction (β = −0.32, P = 0.01) and attention scores (β = −0.41, P = 0.001).
SPARE-AD score across cognitive categories
SPARE-AD scores increased across patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition, Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment and patients with Parkinson's disease with dementia [F = 3.56 (df = 2,81), P = 0.03] with a significant difference between patients with Parkinson's disease with dementia and Parkinson's disease with normal cognition (P = 0.03) (Fig. 2).
Including only non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease in a multivariate model, only higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.28, P = 0.05) predicted worse global cognitive performance (Table 3). Including patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition only, there was a suggestion only for higher SPARE-AD score (β = −0.29, P = 0.06) predicting lower DRS-2 score.
Table 3.
PD-MCI + PD-NC (n = 69) |
PD-NC only (n = 51) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Standardized coefficients (Beta) | t | P-value | Standardized coefficients (Beta) | t | P-value | |
SPARE-AD modelsa | ||||||
Age | −0.155 | −1.168 | 0.25 | −0.213 | −1.467 | 0.15 |
PD duration | −0.053 | −0.415 | 0.68 | 0.079 | 0.545 | 0.59 |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | −0.069 | −0.533 | 0.60 | −0.185 | −1.262 | 0.59 |
PIGD subtype | 0.010 | 0.081 | 0.94 | 0.199 | 1.44 | 0.16 |
SPARE-AD | −0.279 | −1.997 | 0.05 | −0.287 | −1.971 | 0.06 |
Hippocampus modelsb | ||||||
Age | −0.152 | −1.162 | 0.25 | −0.270 | −1.817 | 0.08 |
PD duration | 0.005 | 0.040 | 0.97 | 0.141 | 0.968 | 0.34 |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | −0.124 | −1.013 | 0.32 | −0.249 | −1.693 | 0.10 |
PIGD subtype | 0.028 | 0.224 | 0.82 | 0.210 | 1.462 | 0.15 |
Hippocampal volume | 0.283 | 2.211 | 0.03 | 0.116 | 0.808 | 0.42 |
a Linear regression models with baseline DRS-2 score as dependent variable and SPARE-AD score as measure of atrophy.
b Linear regression models with baseline DRS-2 score as dependent variable and hippocampal volume as measure of atrophy.
PD = Parkinson's disease; PD-MCI = Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment; PD-NC = Parkinson's disease with normal cognition; PIGD = postural instability–gait difficulty.
Substituting regions of interest individually into the model for SPARE-AD score, smaller hippocampal (β = 0.28, P = 0.03), medial temporal lobe (β = 0.25, P = 0.04), and temporal lobe (β = 0.27, P = 0.03) volumes predicted worse DRS-2 score in non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease, but no individual region of interest predicted DRS-2 performance in patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition (Table 3).
Longitudinal analyses
SPARE-AD score as predictor of long-term cognitive decline in non-demented patients
DRS-2 scores decreased significantly from baseline [mean (SD) = 138.6 (3.9)] to the end of Year 2 [135.6 (7.2); F = 22.1 (1,117), P < 0.001] in non-demented Parkinson's disease patients. In the linear mixed model analysis with the primary covariates entered, higher baseline SPARE-AD score [F(1, 110) = 9.72, P = 0.002], increasing age [F(1, 110) = 9.59, P = 0.002], and postural instability–gait difficulty subtype [F(1, 110) = 4.84, P = 0.03] predicted long-term decline in DRS-2 score (Table 4, Model 1).
Table 4.
Model | Regression coefficient | df | F | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1a | ||||
Age | −0.152 | 1, 110 | 9.585 | 0.002 |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | −1.046 | 1, 110 | 1.696 | 0.20 |
PD duration | −0.073 | 1, 110 | 0.869 | 0.35 |
PIGD subtype | 1.318 | 1, 110 | 4.837 | 0.03 |
SPARE-AD score | −1.424 | 1, 110 | 9.717 | 0.002 |
Model 2b | ||||
Age | −0.154 | 1, 105 | 5.962 | 0.02 |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | 1.372 | 1, 105 | 2.715 | 0.10 |
PD duration | −0.073 | 1, 105 | 1.273 | 0.26 |
PIGD subtype | 1.458 | 1, 105 | 5.616 | 0.02 |
SPARE-AD score | −1.456 | 1, 105 | 7.690 | 0.007 |
Sex | 1.108 | 1, 105 | 3.362 | 0.07 |
Education | −0.070 | 1, 105 | 0.072 | 0.79 |
Total LEDD | <−0.001 | 1, 105 | 0.080 | 0.78 |
GDS-15 score | −0.084 | 1, 105 | 0.395 | 0.53 |
Presence of thought disorder | 0.881 | 1, 105 | 1.114 | 0.29 |
a Type III tests in the mixed model with repeated measures of DRS-2 score as dependent variable, and visit, baseline DRS-2 score, age, Hoehn and Yahr stage (high versus low based on median cut-off), Parkinson's disease duration, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype, SPARE-AD score, age × visit interaction, Hoehn and Yahr stage × visit interaction, Parkinson's disease duration × visit interaction, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype × visit interaction and SPARE-AD × visit interaction entered as covariates. The intercept and the regression coefficients for the follow-up time (visit) were treated as random effects such that each subject has a unique intercept and regression coefficient for the follow-up time. Results for age, Hoehn and Yahr stage, Parkinson's disease duration, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype and SPARE-AD score are for variable × visit interaction term.
b Results for age, Hoehn and Yahr stage (high versus low based on median cut-off), Parkinson's disease duration, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype, SPARE-AD score, sex, education, total levodopa equivalent daily dosage, Geriatric Depression Scale-15 score and presence of thought disorder are for variable × visit interaction term.
LEDD = levodopa equivalent daily dosage; PD = Parkinson's disease; PIGD = postural instability–gait difficulty.
Entering other covariates of interest, either as a group (Table 4, Model 2) or individually (data not shown), only higher baseline SPARE-AD score [F(1, 105) = 7.69, P = 0.007], increasing age [F(1, 105) = 5.96, P = 0.02], and postural instability–gait difficulty subtype [F(1, 105) = 5.62, P = 0.02] predicted long-term cognitive decline. Serially substituting all regions of interest into the model for SPARE-AD score, no baseline region of interest volume predicted decline in DRS-2 score over time (data not shown).
SPARE-AD score as predictor of long-term cognitive decline in patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition
In mixed model analyses that included only patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition (n = 43) and the primary covariates (i.e. age, Hoehn and Yahr stage, Parkinson's disease duration, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype and SPARE-AD score), only higher baseline SPARE-AD score [F(1, 80) = 4.71, P = 0.03] and increasing age [F(1, 80) = 6.85, P = 0.01] predicted worsening global cognitive performance over time. Serially substituting all regions of interest into the model for SPARE-AD score, no baseline region of interest volume predicted decline in DRS-2 score over time in patients with Parkinson's disease with normal cognition (data not shown).
Discussion
The specific physiological, biochemical and anatomical changes that underlie the development of cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease are not well understood. Diffuse Lewy body deposition (Aarsland et al., 2005), brain atrophy (Song et al., 2011) and metabolic deficits (Peppard et al., 1992) are the most commonly documented correlates of Parkinson's disease with dementia. However, over half of patients with Parkinson's disease with dementia also have significant Alzheimer's disease-related plaques and neurofibrillary tangles on autopsy (Lieberman, 1997; Sabbagh et al., 2009), with a positive correlation between amount of Alzheimer's disease pathology and severity of Parkinson's disease with dementia (Jellinger et al., 2002), and some patients with Parkinson's disease with dementia are reported to have increased β-amyloid using Pittsburgh Compound B (PiB) PET imaging (Gomperts et al., 2008; Maetzler et al., 2009). Even less is known about biomarkers of preclinical cognitive impairment or at the stage of Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment.
Using multivariable analyses to control for possible confounding variables and applying a validated Alzheimer's disease-pattern of brain atrophy to the MRI scans of patients with Parkinson's disease, we found that this pattern also predicts global cognitive performance in patients with Parkinson's disease across a range of cognitive abilities. The results indicate that the overall pattern of brain neurodegeneration that occurs in Alzheimer's disease, as summarized by SPARE-AD score (i.e. weighted toward hippocampal, medial temporal lobe and parietal–temporal cortex atrophy), is also associated with progression of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease. This association between neurodegeneration in medial temporal lobe structures and cognitive performance is also consistent with recent research highlighting that memory impairment is relatively common in Parkinson's disease, even at the stage of mild cognitive impairment. Whether this neurodegeneration is due primarily to Parkinson's disease pathology, Alzheimer's disease pathology, some combination of the two, or even represents a compensatory mechanism remains to be determined. Our finding that both an Alzheimer's disease pattern of neurodegeneration and increasing Parkinson's disease severity were independent contributors to cognitive performance raises the possibility that both Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease pathology contribute to cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease, which is supported by a recent clinicopathological study (Compta et al., 2011).
Of significance was that the Alzheimer's disease pattern of atrophy predicted cognitive performance even in patients with Parkinson's disease without dementia-level severity of cognitive impairment, remarkably even in those with normal cognition based on robust and expanded normative data. There was no association between region of interest volumes, including the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe, and cognitive performance in patients with normal cognition. This suggests that applying a dementia pattern of atrophy that differentially weighs brain regions may be more sensitive to cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease than a region of interest approach, and may even be a preclinical (i.e. present in patients with normal cognition) biomarker.
Demographic and clinical correlates or risk factors for cognitive decline and development of dementia in Parkinson's disease include increasing age, male sex, lower level of education, increasing severity and longer duration of Parkinson's disease, postural instability–gait difficulty subtype and hallucinations (Aarsland et al., 1996, 2001; Green et al., 2002; Ramirez-Ruiz et al., 2007; Williams-Gray et al., 2007). In addition, dopamine replacement therapy may impair cognition, depending on dosage, cognitive domain assessed, disease severity and genetic factors (Kehagia et al., 2010). Including those variables associated with cognitive performance in our sample as covariates in linear regression models, an Alzheimer's disease pattern of brain atrophy, as well as increasing disease severity and duration, predicted current cognitive performance in the entire sample. In non-demented patients, no demographic or clinical variables were associated with cognitive performance when controlling for severity of brain atrophy, suggesting that some clinical and demographic variables reported to be associated with cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease are confounded by their association with brain atrophy. It is also possible that the relatively low frequency of thought disorder, including psychotic symptoms, and lack of a correlation between thought disorder and cognition was due to the use of the UPDRS Part I thought disorder item, which has limited sensitivity to detect psychosis in Parkinson's disease (Starkstein and Merello, 2007).
In addition to the cross-sectional findings, an Alzheimer's disease-like pattern of brain atrophy also predicted 2-year decline in cognitive performance in non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease, including the subset of patients with normal cognition at baseline. Even when entering into the models numerous other demographic and clinical covariates associated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease, an Alzheimer's disease-like pattern of brain atrophy continued to predict long-term cognitive decline. Other baseline variables that independently predicted cognitive decline were increasing age and postural instability–gait difficulty subtype, raising the possibility that the neuropathological underpinnings of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease are varied and complex [i.e. potentially a mix of Parkinson's disease-related, Alzheimer's disease-related and vascular changes (Compta et al., 2011)], which has implications for clinical course and management, as well as the design of intervention studies for cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Overall, the longitudinal results are similar to those reported for patients with mild cognitive impairment in the general population, a population in which higher SPARE-AD scores predicted long-term declines in Mini-Mental State Examination score (Fan et al., 2008a) and conversion to Alzheimer's disease (Misra et al., 2009). Also consistent with our results, a recent analysis in cognitively normal elderly found that higher SPARE-AD scores were associated with worse memory performance (Davatzikos et al., 2009).
Impairment in the neural circuits connecting the basal ganglia and cortical regions, including the prefrontal cortex, are thought to contribute to cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease (Dubois and Pillon, 1997; Burn and O'Brien, 2003; Carbon et al., 2004). Some studies have reported that prefrontal cortical atrophy occurs early in the process of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease (Song et al., 2011) and that executive impairment in non-demented patients predicts long-term development of Parkinson's disease with dementia (Mahieux et al., 1998; Janvin et al., 2005; Santangelo et al., 2007), while others suggest that prefrontal cortex deficits may occur early in the disease course, be stable and not predict future cognitive decline (Williams-Gray et al., 2009; Kehagia et al., 2010). Our finding that an Alzheimer's disease pattern of brain atrophy, characterized by hippocampal and medial temporal lobe atrophy, predicts long-term cognitive decline in non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease is consistent with prospective research in an incident Parkinson's disease cohort that posterior cortical cognitive impairments predict long-term development of dementia (Williams-Gray et al., 2009).
Limitations of this research include lack of formal diagnostic criteria for MCI and dementia, a sample of patients with primarily mild to moderate stage Parkinson's disease, lack of a validated rating scale to assess psychotic symptoms, and longitudinal data limited to 2 years. However, the mean age of our cohort is when Parkinson's disease dementia typically has its onset (Reid et al., 2011), making our sample vulnerable to cognitive decline over a relatively short time period. Future studies need to enrol larger samples, have longer follow-up periods, use formal mild cognitive impairment and Parkinson's disease with dementia diagnostic criteria to document change in clinical status over time, include multiple biomarkers for comparison or combination e.g. both molecular and structural imaging (Fan et al., 2008b), and corticometry (cortical thickness) versus traditional voxel-based morphometry analyses (Jubault et al., 2011), include clinicopathological correlation and incorporate specific Alzheimer's disease biomarkers, the latter to determine the extent that Alzheimer's disease-specific pathophysiological changes contribute to cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease.
For Alzheimer's disease, a model of dynamic biomarkers of pathological cascade has been proposed, starting with evidence for β-amyloidosis at a presymptomatic stage, followed by neuronal dysfunction and neurodegeneration at the time of clinical manifestation of cognitive impairment (Jack et al., 2010), and a pattern of neuronal dysfunction (i.e. altered perfusion using fluorodeoxyglucose-PET) that is associated with cognitive performance has been reported in patients with Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment (Huang et al., 2008). Our findings suggest at least overlap in the regions undergoing neurodegeneration with cognitive decline in the two disease states, and raise the possibility that use of a pattern classification method to detect brain atrophy in Parkinson's disease may allow preclinical detection of patients at imminent risk of cognitive decline.
Funding
This work was supported by a health research grant [grant number SAP4100027296] awarded by the Department of Health of the Commonwealth of Pennsylvania from the Tobacco Master Settlement Agreement under Act 2001-77, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Strokes [NS-053488], and the National Institute of Aging [R01-14971 and P30-AG10124].
Glossary
Abbreviations
- DRS
Dementia Rating Scale
- RAVENS
Regional Analysis of Volumes Examined in Normalized Space
- SPARE-AD
Spatial Pattern of Atrophy for Recognition of Alzheimer's Disease; UPDRS = Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale
References
- Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Kragh-Sørensen P. Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease: an 8-year prospective study. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:387–92. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Nieman H, Kragh-Sorensen P. Risk of dementia in Parkinson's disease: a community-based, prospective study. Neurology. 2001;56:730–6. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.6.730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Williams-Gray CH, Weintraub D, Marder K, Kulisevsky J, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: A multicentre pooled analysis. Neurology. 2010;75:1062–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f39d0e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland D, Perry R, Brown A, Larsen JP, Ballard C. Neuropathology of dementia in Parkinson's disease: a prospective, community-based study. Ann Neurol. 2005;58:773–6. doi: 10.1002/ana.20635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarsland D, Tandberg E, Larsen JP, Cummings JL. Frequency of dementia in Parkinson's disease. Arch Neurol. 1996;53:538–42. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1996.00550060082020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alves G, Bronnick K, Aarsland D, Blennow K, Zetterberg H, Ballard C, et al. CSF amyloid-B and tau proteins, and cognitive performance, in early and untreated Parkinson's disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81:1080–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.199950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apaydin H, Ahlskog JE, Parisi JE, Boeve BF, Dickson DW. Parkinson disease neuropathology. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:102–12. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.1.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apostolova LG, Beyer M, Green AE, Hwang KS, Morra JH, Chou Y-Y, et al. Hippocampal, caudate, and ventricular changes in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Mov Disord. 2010;25:687–95. doi: 10.1002/mds.22799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnulf I, Bonnet A-M, Damier P, Bejjani B-P, Seilhean D, Derenne J-P, et al. Hallucinations, REM sleep, and Parkinson's disease: a medical hypothesis. Neurology. 2000;55:281–8. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.2.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aybek S, Lazeyras F, Gronchi-Perrin A, Burkhard PR, Villemure J-G, Vingerhoets FJG. Hippocampal atrophy predicts conversion to dementia after STN-DBS in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Related Disorders. 2009;15:521–4. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beach TG, Adler CH, Lue L, Sue LI, Bachalakuri J, Henry-Watson J, et al. Unified staging system for Lewy body disorders: correlation with nigrostriatal degeneration, cognitive impairement and motor dysfunction. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 2009;117:613–34. doi: 10.1007/s00401-009-0538-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beyer MK, Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Aarsland D. A magnetic resonance imaging study of patients with Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment and dementia using voxel-based morphometry. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:254–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.093849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak H, Rüb U, Steur ENHJ, Del Tredici K, de Vos RAI. Cognitive status correlates with neuropathologic stage in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;64:1404–10. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000158422.41380.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brück A, Kurki T, Kaasinen V, Vahlberg T, Rinne JO. Hippocampal and prefrontal atrophy in patients with early non-demented Parkinson's disease is related to cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:1467–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.031237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burn DJ, O'Brien JT. Use of functional imaging in parkinsonism and dementia. Mov Disord. 2003;18(Suppl. 6):S88–95. doi: 10.1002/mds.10568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, Williams ED, O'Brien JT. Cerebral atrophy in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia: A comparison with Alzheimer's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies and controls. Brain. 2004;127:791–800. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camicioli R, Moore MM, Kinney A, Corbridge E, Glassberg K, Kaye JA. Parkinson's disease is associated with hippocampal atrophy. Mov Disord. 2003;18:784–90. doi: 10.1002/mds.10444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carbon M, Ma Y, Barnes A, Dhawan V, Chaly T, Ghilardi MF, et al. Caudate nucleus: influence of dopaminergic input on sequence learning and brain activation in Parkinsonism. NeuroImage. 2004;21:1497–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clinton LK, Blurton-Jones M, Myczek K, Trojanowski JQ, LaFerla FM. Synergistic interactions between Aβ, tau, and a-synuclein: acceleration of neuropathology and cognitive decline. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7281–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0490-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compta Y, Marti MJ, Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Junque C, Valldeoriola F, Munoz E, et al. Cerebrospinal tau, phospo-tau, and beta-amyloid and neuropsychological functions in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24:2203–10. doi: 10.1002/mds.22594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compta Y, Parkkinen L, O'Sullivan SS, Vandrovcova J, Holton JL, Collins C, et al. Lewy- and Alzeheimer-type pathologies in Parkinson's disease dementia: which is more important? Brain. 2011;134:1493–505. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davatzikos C, Fan Y, Wu X, Shen D, Resnick SM. Detection of prodromal Alzheimer's disease via pattern classification of magnetic resonance imaging. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:514–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davatzikos C, Xu F, An Y, Fan Y, Resnick SM. Longitudinal progression of Alzheimer's-like patterns of atrophy in normal older adults: the SPARE-AD index. Brain. 2009;132:2026–35. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B, Pillon B. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. 1997;244:2–8. doi: 10.1007/pl00007725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahn S, Elton RL, the UPDRS Development Committee . Unified Parkinson's disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne D, Goldstein M, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson's disease. Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan Health Care Information; 1987. pp. 153–63. [Google Scholar]
- Fan Y, Batmanghelich N, Clark CM, Davatzikos C. the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Spatial patterns of brain atrophy in MCI patients, identified via high-dimensional pattern classification, predict subsequent cognitive decline. NeuroImage. 2008a;39:1731–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan Y, Resnick SM, Wu X, Davatzikos C. Structural and functional biomarkers of prodromal Alzheimer's disease: A high-dimensional pattern classification study. NeuroImage. 2008b;41:277–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan Y, Shen D, Gur RC, Gur RE, Davatzikos C. COMPARE: Classification of morphological patterns using adaptive regional elements. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2007;26:93–105. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.886812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farlow M, Cummings J. A modern hypothesis: the distinct pathologies of dementia associated with Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's disease: similarities and differences. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25:301–8. doi: 10.1159/000119104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forsaa EB, Larsen JP, Wentzel-Larsen T, Goetz CG, Stebbins GT, Aarsland D, et al. A 12-year population-based study of psychosis in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:996–1001. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvin JE, Uryu K, Lee VM-Y, Trojanowski JQ. Axon pathology in Parkinson's disease and Lewy body dementia hippocampus contains α−, β−, and γ-synuclein. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1999;96:13450–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG, Ouyang B, Negron A, Stebbins GT. Hallucinations and sleep disorders in PD: ten-year prospective longitudinal study. Neurology. 2010;75:1773–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fd6158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldszal AF, Davatzikos C, Pham DL, Yan MXH, Bryan RN, Resnick SM. An image processing system for qualitative and quantitative volumetric analysis of brain images. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1998;22:827–37. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199809000-00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomperts SN, Rentz DM, Moran E, Becker JA, Locascio JJ, Klunk WE, et al. Imaging amyloid deposition in Lewy body diseases. Neurology. 2008;71:903–10. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000326146.60732.d6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green J, McDonald WM, Vitek JL, Evatt M, Freeman A, Haber M, et al. Cognitive impairments in advanced PD without dementia. Neurology. 2002;59:1320–4. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031426.21683.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hely MA, Reid WGJ, Adena MA, Halliday GM, Morris JGL. The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson's disease: The inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Mov Disord. 2008;23:837–44. doi: 10.1002/mds.21956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobson DE, Lang AE, Martin WR, Razmy A, Rivest J, Fleming J. Excessive daytime sleepiness and sudden-onset sleep in Parkinson disease: a survey by the Canadian Movement Disorders Group. JAMA. 2005;287:455–63. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.4.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang C, Mattis P, Perrine K, Brown N, Dhawan V, Eidelberg D. Metabolic abnormalities associated with mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2008;79:1470–7. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000304050.05332.9c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Ramírez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, Martí MJ, Valldeoriola F, Bargalló N, et al. Hippocampal head atrophy predominance in Parkinson's disease with hallucinations and with dementia. J Neurol. 2008;255:1324–31. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0885-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–28. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jankovic J, McDermott M, Carter J, Gauthier S, Goetz C, Golbe L, et al. Variable expression of Parkinson's disease: a base-line analysis of the DATATOP cohort. Neurology. 1990;40:1529–34. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.10.1529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Aarsland D, Hugdahl K. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: progression to dementia. Mov Disord. 2006;21:1343–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.20974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janvin CC, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Cognitive predictors of dementia in Parkinson's disease: a community-based, 4-year longitudinal study. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2005;18:149–54. doi: 10.1177/0891988705277540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jellinger KA, Seppi K, Wenning GK, Poewe W. Impact of coexistent Alzheimer pathology on the natural history of Parkinson's disease. J Neural Transm. 2002;109:329–39. doi: 10.1007/s007020200027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jokinen P, Brück A, Aalot S, Forsback S, Parkkola R, Rinne JO. Impaired cognitive performance in Parkinson's disease is related to caudate dopaminergic hypofunction and hippocampal atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009;15:88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jubault T, Gagnon J-F, Karama S, Ptito A, Lafontaine A-L, Evans AC, et al. Patterns of cortical thickness and surface area in early Parkinson's disease. NeuroImage. 2011;55:462–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Junqué C, Ramírez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, Summerfield C, Martí MJ, Pastor P, et al. Amygdalar and hippocampal MRI volumetric reductions in Parkinson's disease with dementia. Mov Disord. 2005;20:540–4. doi: 10.1002/mds.20371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurica PJ, Leitten SL, Mattis S. Odessa, Fl: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001. Dementia Rating Scale-2 (DRS-2): Professional Manual. [Google Scholar]
- Kehagia AA, Barker RA, Robbins TW. Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:1200–13. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70212-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny ER, Burton EJ, O'Brien JT. A volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study of entorhinal cortex volume in dementia with Lewy bodies. A comparison with Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26:218–25. doi: 10.1159/000153432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird NM, Ware JH. Random effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. 1982;38:963–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy G, Jacobs DM, Tang M-X, Côté LJ, Louis ED, Alfaro B, et al. Memory and executive function impairment predict dementia in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2002;17:1221–6. doi: 10.1002/mds.10280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman AN. Point of view: dementia in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 1997;3:151–8. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8020(97)00017-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llebaria G, Pagonabarraga J, Kulisevsky J, García-Sánchez C, Pascual-Sedano B, Gironell A, et al. Cut-off score of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale for screening dementia in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23:1546–50. doi: 10.1002/mds.22173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyoo CH, Ryu YH, Lee MS. Topographical distribution of cerebral cortical thinning in patients with mild Parkinson's disease without dementia. Mov Disord. 2010 doi: 10.1002/mds.22975. 10.1002/mds.22975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maetzler W, Liepelt I, Reimold M, Reischl G, Solbach C, Becker C, et al. Cortical PIB binding in Lewy body disease is associated with Alzheimer-like characteristics. Neurobiol Dis. 2009;34:107–12. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahieux F, Fénelon G, Flahault A, Manifacier M-J, Michelet D, Boller F. Neuropsychological prediction of dementia in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;64:178–83. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.2.178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin RC, Okonkwo OC, Hill J, Griffith HR, Triebel K, Bartolucci A, et al. Medical decision-making capacity in cognitively impaired Parkinson's disease patients without dementia. Mov Disord. 2008;23:1867–4. doi: 10.1002/mds.22170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin WR, Wieler M, Gee M, Camicioli R. Temporal lobe changes in early, untreated Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24:1949–4. doi: 10.1002/mds.22680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masliah E, Roskenstein E, Veinbergs I, Sagara Y, Mallory M, Hashimoto M, et al. β-Amyloid peptides enhance a-synuclein accumulation and neuronal deficits in a transgenic mouse model linking Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:12245–50. doi: 10.1073/pnas.211412398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misra C, Fan Y, Davatzikos C. Baseline and longitudinal patterns of brain atrophy in MCI patients, and their use in prediction of short-term conversion to AD: Results from ADNI. NeuroImage. 2009;44:1415–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedraza O, Lucas JA, Smith GE, Petersen RC, Graff-Radford NR, Ivnik RJ. Robust and expanded norms for the Dementia Rating Scale. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2010;25:347–58. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peppard RF, Martin WRW, Carr GD, Grochowski E, Schulzer M, Guttman M, et al. Cerebral glucose metabolism in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:162–1268. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360060019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez-Ruiz B, Junque C, Marti M, Valldeoriola F, Tolosa E. Cognitive changes in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;23:281–8. doi: 10.1159/000100850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid WGJ, Hely MA, Morris JGL, Loy C, Halliday GM. Dementia in Parkinson's disease: a 20-year neuropsychological study (Sydney Multicentre Study) J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011 doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.232678. 10.1136/jnnp.2010.232678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabbagh M, Adler CH, Lahti TJ, Connor DJ, Vedders L, Peterson LK, et al. Parkinson's disease with dementia: comparing patients with and without Alzheimer pathology. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23:295–7. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31819c5ef4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Castaneda C, Rene R, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Campdelacreu J, Gascon J, Falcon C, et al. Correlations between gray matter reductions and cognitive deficits in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson's disease with dementia. Mov Disord. 2009;24:1740–6. doi: 10.1002/mds.22488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santangelo G, Trojano L, Vitale C, Ianniciello M, Amboni M, Grossi D, et al. A neuropsychological longitudinal study in Parkinson's patients with and without hallucinations. Mov Disord. 2007;22:2418–25. doi: 10.1002/mds.21746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to Assessment and Intervention. New York: The Haworth Press; 1986. pp. 165–73. [Google Scholar]
- Shen D, Davatzikos C. HAMMER: hierarchical attribute matching mechanism for elastic registration. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2002;21:1421–39. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2002.803111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen DB, Davatzikos C. Very high resolution morphometry using mass-preserving deformations and HAMMER elastic registration. NeuroImage. 2003;18:28–41. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siderowf A, Xie SX, Hurtig H, Weintraub D, Duda J, Chen-Plotkin A, et al. CSF amyloid B 1-42 predicts cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease. 2010;75:1055–61. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f39a78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song SK, Lee JE, Park H-J, Sohn YH, Lee JD, Lee PH. The pattern of cortical atrophy in patients with Parkinson's disease according to cognitive status. Mov Disord. 2011;26:289–96. doi: 10.1002/mds.23477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein SE, Merello M. The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale: validation study of the mentation, behavior, and mood section. Mov Disord. 2007;22:2156–61. doi: 10.1002/mds.21521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam CWC, Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O'Brien JT. Temporal lobe atrophy on MRI in Parkinson disease with dementia: A comparison with Alzheimer disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2005;64:861–5. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000153070.82309.D4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troster AI, Woods SP, Morgan EE. Assessing cognitive change in Parkinson's disease: Development of practice effect corrected reliable change indices. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22:711–8. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams-Gray CH, Evans JR, Goris A, Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins TW, et al. The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson's disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort. Brain. 2009;132:2958–69. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CEG, Robbins TW, Barker RA. Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson's disease cohort. Brain. 2007;130:1787–98. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]