Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Oct 5.
Published in final edited form as: Nature. 2012 Mar 14;484(7392):62–68. doi: 10.1038/nature10918

Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task

Christopher D Harvey 1,3,4,5, Philip Coen 1,4, David W Tank 1,2,3,4
PMCID: PMC3321074  NIHMSID: NIHMS354701  PMID: 22419153

Abstract

The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) plays an important role in many cognitive behaviors; however, the neural circuit dynamics underlying PPC function are not well understood. Here we optically imaged the spatial and temporal activity patterns of neuronal populations in mice performing a PPC-dependent task that combined a perceptual decision and memory-guided navigation in a virtual environment. Individual neurons had transient activation staggered relative to one another in time, forming a sequence of neuronal activation spanning the entire length of a task trial. Distinct sequences of neurons were triggered on trials with opposite behavioral choices and defined divergent, choice-specific trajectories through a state space of neuronal population activity. Cells participating in the different sequences and at distinct time points in the task were anatomically intermixed over microcircuit length scales (< 100 micrometers). During working memory decision tasks the PPC may therefore perform computations through sequence-based circuit dynamics, rather than long-lived stable states, implemented using anatomically intermingled microcircuits.

Introduction

In real world tasks decision-making and working memory often occur in the context of other complex behaviors, including spatial navigation. For example, when driving through a city towards a destination, sensory information defining context and place engages memory and decision circuits to plan turns at upcoming intersections. The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is a prime candidate for the neuronal circuitry combining the cognitive processing elements necessary for such tasks. In primates the PPC is important for perceptual decision-making and categorization13, movement planning4, and spatial attention5. Studies in rats suggest that the PPC is also important for encoding route progression during navigation69. Using a virtual reality system for mice10 and cellular resolution optical imaging methods1112, we developed a T-maze-based navigation paradigm combining all these cognitive elements and characterized the neuronal circuit dynamics in the PPC, which have not been studied in this combined behavioral context.

Neuronal activity patterns in the PPC have been studied using microelectrode recordings during spatial attention, working memory, and perceptual decision tasks. These studies have commonly found cells with sustained firing rate changes spanning entire task periods (cue, delay, response periods)2,45,13. For example, cells with persistent activity throughout the delay period of memory-guided saccade/reach tasks have frequently been recorded1415. Also, studies have identified neurons with ramps of increasing firing rate spanning the accumulation of evidence period in a motion perception task12. Neurons with sustained activity during the same task period often have similar activity time courses, suggesting the presence of classes of cells (e.g. delay cells) and implying that the task-dependent neuronal dynamics are low-dimensional. The low-dimensional dynamics can be reproduced in recurrent attractor network models, in which each cell’s activity is typically an amplitude-scaled version of a prototypical time series1621. In contrast, recent analysis of prefrontal cortex activity has identified heterogeneous neuronal activity time courses, in which a neuron’s activity can be thought of as the sum of a few activity modes2224. In addition, there is growing evidence for sequences of neuronal activation within local circuits, in which each neuron is active for only a fraction of a task period, including during working memory tasks in the prefrontal cortex2528 and the hippocampus29 and during an object construction task in the PPC30. Sequences suggest dynamics that are high-dimensional, without the presence of classes of cells with relatively homogeneous activity time courses.

We explored whether PPC dynamics are best described in terms of cell classes or high-dimensional dynamics. During a navigation-based decision task the dynamics were high-dimensional: neurons were active in choice-specific sequences in which information moves from one neuronal population to another across time in the task; although the neuronal activity patterns could be divided into cue, delay, and response groups, sequences were present within each group. Furthermore, exploiting the ability of cellular resolution optical measurements to provide the relative anatomical location of the recorded cells12,3132, we found that neurons active during behaviorally-distinct task periods and on trials with different behavioral choices were spatially intermixed over microcircuit length scales.

A PPC-dependent navigation-based decision task for head-restrained mice

Using a virtual reality system10, we trained mice to navigate through a virtual T-maze in a task that incorporated both visual discrimination and a memory-guided response (Methods; Fig. 1a). Visual cues present in the initial section of the maze indicated which direction to turn at the T-intersection to receive a water reward (one set of cues to indicate a right turn and a second set for a left turn; Supplementary Fig. 1c). Between the cue section and the T-intersection, mice ran through a delay maze section that was identical on left and right turn trials; visual information about the reward location was thus present only in the cue section. The task resembled traditional delay tasks1415,33 in its cue-delay-response structure but differed in that ongoing sensorimotor activity was present throughout the task, including the delay period. Mice performed the task with high levels of accuracy (83 ± 9% correct, p < 0.0001 vs. 50%, t-test; Fig. 1b). Within and across trials, individual mice ran at highly consistent speeds (Supplementary Fig. 2); however, running speeds varied between mice and the maze position at which mice began rotating the spherical treadmill to make a right or left turn differed across trials (turn onset; Fig. 1c, Supplementary Fig. 3; Methods). To compare behaviorally similar epochs of the task across trials, individual trials were aligned to the time points when the cue was no longer visible (cue offset), the turn onset, and the end of the trial; these alignment points formed the boundaries of cue (trial start to cue offset: 5.1 ± 2.6 s), delay (cue offset to turn onset: 4.2 ± 1.2 s), and turn task periods (turn onset to trial end: 3.0 ± 1.8 s).

Figure 1. A PPC-dependent decision task in virtual reality.

Figure 1

  1. Schematic of the two versions of the virtual T-maze that differed only in the cue period and the reward location. Patterns in the schematic reflect the patterns present on the virtual maze walls.
  2. Behavioral performance on individual training (gray) and imaging (black) sessions.
  3. Rotational velocity of the spherical treadmill about the vertical axis for view angle changes on correct right (red) and left (blue) trials, aligned to the cue offset and the turn onset.
  4. Behavioral performance on a memory-guided task from (a) after receiving no injections (gray), saline (open circles), or muscimol (black) bilaterally in the PPC. Connected dots are from individual mice across daily sessions (n = 3 mice).
  5. Same as in (d) except for a visually-guided task (Supplementary Fig. 1b).

Because the location of the mouse PPC has not been characterized, we first performed retrograde and anterograde labeling experiments to locate it anatomically (Methods, Supplementary Fig. 4). We identified a region consistent with the rat and primate PPC based on the set of areas from where it received axonal projections, the areas to which it sent projections, and its location relative to other cortical regions8,34 (anterior to visual cortex and posterior to somatosensory cortex). We therefore considered this area to be the mouse PPC.

To test if the PPC was required for the behavioral task, we inactivated it using bilateral injections of the GABAA receptor agonist muscimol. Muscimol reversibly decreased behavioral performance from high levels of accuracy in control sessions to near chance levels, but did not affect the rate of trials performed (Fraction correct: no injections 87 ± 7%, saline 85 ± 5%, muscimol 54 ± 5% p < 0.0001 vs. no injections, t-test; Trials per minute: no injections 3.2 ± 0.5, muscimol 3.1 ± 0.3, p > 0.8, t-test; Fig. 1d). In contrast, PPC inactivation did not significantly affect performance on a visually-guided task in which a visual cue was present at the reward site and visible throughout the trial, indicating that the decrease in performance on the memory-guided task was unlikely due to a major visual or motor deficit (no injections: 90 ± 5%, muscimol: 87 ± 5%, p > 0.2, t-test; Fig. 1e, Supplementary Fig. 1b).

Imaging sequences of neuronal activity

We used two-photon microscopy to image layer 2/3 PPC neurons expressing the genetically-encoded calcium indicator GCaMP3, which increases in fluorescence intensity in response to action potential firing35 (Methods). On average, we imaged ~65 cells simultaneously within a ~300 µm by ~150 µm area (range: 37–94 cells). Nearly all imaged cells showed statistically significant Ca2+ transients during the behavioral session (96% of cells had > 0.2 transients per minute; Methods). Of the cells with high levels of activity (> 2 Ca2+ transients per minute on average; Supplementary Fig. 5), ~73% had significant increases in their mean fluorescence intensity traces (ΔF/F, averaged across trials) during a specific time in the trial or inter-trial interval (task-modulated cells; Fig. 2, Supplementary Figs. 6–7). These task-modulated cells had Ca2+ transients for only short time intervals on individual trials (11 ± 8% of time points in trials with a transient) such that only a small fraction of neurons was active simultaneously (Fig. 2b–e, Supplementary Figs. 8, 9a); cells with prolonged activity patterns covering a large fraction of the trial were not observed. The majority (~71%) of task-modulated cells had significantly different levels of activity on correct right and left choice trials (choice-specific cells). Similar choice-specific, task-modulated activity patterns were observed in extracellular electrophysiological recordings (Supplementary Figs. 10–11). Cells were also active on error trials, such that neurons active during the cue period tended to be correlated with the cue identity, and neurons active during the turn period in general were correlated with the behavioral response (Supplementary Fig. 12). Only a small fraction of cells had obvious reward-related signals (~2% of active cells with p < 0.01, t-test, comparing ΔF/F values within ~0.6 s after the reward was given on correct trials or missed on error trials).

Figure 2. Imaging PPC neuronal activity during the T-maze task.

Figure 2

  1. Left: Example image of GCaMP3-expressing neurons in layer 2/3. Right: Example fluorescence intensity traces (ΔF/F; gray portions indicate significant Ca2+ transients, Methods) for three example cells from the left panel on correct right (red) and left (blue) trials.
  2. Activity patterns during the task for cells 1–3 from (a). Top: Color-coded ΔF/F traces for individual correct left and right choice trials. Each row is a single trial aligned to the cue offset, turn onset, and trial end. Bottom: Mean ΔF/F traces for correct right (red) and left (blue) choice trials. Dashed lines indicate mean ± sem.
  3. Normalized mean ΔF/F traces for all the choice-specific, task-modulated cells (one cell per row) imaged in a single mouse and divided by left-preferring (n = 51) and right-preferring (n = 54) cells. Traces were normalized to the peak of each cell’s mean ΔF/F trace on preferred trials and sorted by the peak time. Some cells were imaged on different days and in different fields-of-view.
  4. Same as in (c), except for all mice (n = 404 cells from 6 mice) on preferred and opposite trials.
  5. Fraction of active cells with significantly different activity levels on right and left choice trials as a function of trial time.

When the activity patterns of all the choice-specific, task-modulated cells were ordered according to the time profile of their Ca2+ transients, the active periods across cells were staggered relative to one another in time forming a sequence of neuronal activation covering the entire trial length (Fig. 2c–d, Supplementary Fig. 7c). Different sequences of neurons were activated on left and right trials (Fig. 2c–d and absence of activity in Fig. 2d lower plot, Supplementary Fig. 14e). Although these plots of sequences combined cells from different experiments and averaged across trials, similar properties were observed when considering only the cells imaged in a single mouse and on individual trials (Fig. 2c, Supplementary Fig. 13). Sequences were also apparent in the ~29% of task-modulated cells that did not have choice-specific activity; these cells participated in the sequences for both right and left choice trials (Supplementary Fig. 7b). In total ~73% of the highly active cells participated in sequences. Sequences similar to those during the task were not observed in shuffled versions of the dataset, demonstrating that the sequences were not an artifact created by ordering the data (Supplementary Fig. 14a–d).

Because previous studies of the PPC have categorized cells into classes with cue, delay, or response period activity1415, we examined if neurons in the sequence were grouped based on behavioral periods. The distribution of activity times of all cells in the population (calculated for each cell as the center-of-mass (COM) in time of the mean ΔF/F during the trial, tCOM) had three peaks corresponding to the cue, delay, and turn periods, suggesting a possible grouping by behavioral period (Fig. 3a). Consistently, principal component analysis (PCA) on the mean ΔF/F traces for all cells revealed three intermixed groups, with each group mostly containing cells preferring the same behavioral period (Fig. 3a–b, Supplementary Fig. 15).

Figure 3. Neuronal activity in individual behavioral periods.

Figure 3

  1. Histogram of the times of the center-of-mass of the mean ΔF/F trace (tCOM) for choice-specific, task-modulated cells. Cells were separated into three groups (cue-, delay-, and turn-preferring cells; varying shades of green) based on peaks in the distribution.
  2. PCA on the normalized mean ΔF/F traces for all the choice-specific, task-modulated cells. Left: Scores for each cell plotted for the first two principal components. Right: Cells categorized based on the groupings from (a). n = 404 cells.
  3. Sorted normalized mean ΔF/F traces for cue-preferring (n = 101), delay-preferring (n = 133), and turn-preferring (n = 170) cells, aligned to the trial start, cue offset, and turn onset, respectively, on the preferred trial-type.

Although the population of neurons could be divided into groups, the temporal activity patterns within each individual period were heterogeneous and formed sequences (Fig. 3c). Cells within their preferred period were active for only a fraction of the period (35 ± 16% of time points in preferred periods with a Ca2+ transient) with different cells active at different times. Although cells with activity covering a large fraction of the period were occasionally observed (e.g. Fig. 2c top panel), these cells were rare (4% of cells with activity lasting for > 60% of the period; Supplementary Fig. 9b); the distribution of epoch coverage by an individual neuron’s activity was similar during the delay and cue or turn periods (p > 0.1, KS test). Furthermore, Pearson’s correlations between the non-averaged ΔF/F traces for cell pairs with the same trial-type and behavioral period preferences varied widely, with a large fraction of pairs having low correlation coefficients (Supplementary Fig. 16a). The low correlation coefficients could be due to activity at different times in the period, as expected for sequences, or activity at the same time in the trial except on different trials. However, the probability that both cells in these pairs had Ca2+ transients or both cells did not have transients on the same trial during their preferred epoch was generally high (Supplementary Fig. 16b), suggesting that the diversity primarily resulted from differences in the activity times of cells within trials. Together these data indicate that classes of cells with homogeneous activity patterns were not present. Rather, choice-specific sequences of neurons were activated in all behavioral periods, with a lower density of cells in the sequence at the borders between periods.

To further examine the sequential neuronal activation on individual trials, we calculated correlations between the activity patterns of cells using the non-averaged ΔF/F time series. Cells that were active at similar time points in the trial on average (measured as the difference in tCOM values on correct trials, ΔtCOM) had, on correct trials and in their preferred behavioral periods, highly correlated ΔF/F traces and peaks in their cross correlation at a lag approximately equal to ΔtCOM (Supplementary Fig. 16d–h); these relationships in the non-averaged ΔF/F time series provide further evidence for sequential activity on individual trials. Cells that were active at similar times on correct trials were also highly correlated on error trials and in non-preferred periods (Supplementary Fig. 16c–f). Additionally, cells that were sequentially activated during their preferred behavioral period on correct trials were sequentially active with similar lags during error trials and other time points in the task (Supplementary Fig. 16g–i).

The choice-specific activity could result if mice experienced different visual stimuli and running patterns on right and left trials and if PPC activity was modulated by those differences. To examine this, we first performed a multiple regression analysis to determine the potential effects of the parameters defining the mouse’s running trajectory on the fluorescence changes during the delay period (Supplementary Table 1). These parameters could not explain the choice-specific activity patterns, suggesting that any differences in running trajectories between right and left trials did not trigger the activity we observed. In addition, we performed two sets of experiments to further examine whether the maze visual stimuli alone or the mouse’s running patterns triggered PPC activity. In the first experiments, movies of simulated left and right turn runs through the T-maze, which closely approximated real runs, were played to mice that passively viewed the visual scenes (open-loop experiments; Supplementary Fig. 17a–b). In the second experiments, mice were trained on a virtual linear track to perform a simple running back and forth task (run to one end for a reward, turn around, run to the other end for the next reward). The linear track had several visual patterns on the walls, all of which were the same as patterns in portions of the T-maze (Supplementary Fig. 18a–b). The task required similar running and turning behaviors to the T-maze, except that turns were not memory-guided based on visual cues and a delay period and there was not a two-alternative forced-choice structure. The overall levels of activity during the simulated T-maze runs or during the linear track task were much lower than when mice actively performed the T-maze task (Supplementary Figs. 17c–d, 18c–d). Also, only a small fraction of neurons had significant increases in activity at specific locations in the maze, either during the simulated T-maze runs or in the linear track, suggesting that cells were not activated robustly by location-specific visual scenes or running patterns (simulated runs: 1.3%, linear track: 5.8%, T-maze: 32.3%; Supplementary Figs. 17e–k, 18e–k). Together these results suggest that PPC neurons in the T-maze were not activated only by the visual information or by the running patterns of the mouse.

Choice-specific neuronal circuit trajectories

The heterogeneous and sequential neuronal activity patterns during the T-maze task indicated that we should consider the dynamics of the population rather than classes of cells. We therefore analyzed the dynamics as a trajectory through a state space of neuronal population activity (neuronal circuit trajectory)3638. At each time point, the activity state of the circuit containing n simultaneously imaged neurons was defined as a point in an n-dimensional space, with each dimension representing the activity (ΔF/F values) of a single neuron. Different trajectories (visualized using factor analysis for dimensionality reduction) were traversed for trials with different behavioral choices (Fig. 4a–b, Supplementary Fig. 19a). The trajectories for correct right and left choice trials began the trial at similar positions, gradually diverged to reach a peak separation near the time of the behavioral choice, and converged to the starting point in the inter-trial interval (Fig. 4a–d). To quantify the trajectory divergence, we used a classifier based on the distance from an individual trial trajectory to the mean right and left choice trajectories at single time points. It was possible, from the activity of a small population of neurons located in close anatomical proximity (~65 neurons separated by < 250 µm), to predict the mouse’s choice on single correct trials at better than chance levels during the cue, delay, and turn periods (Methods; Fig. 4e, Supplementary Fig. 19b–c). The activity in the PPC can therefore be considered as divergent, choice-specific trajectories through a state space of neuronal population activity.

Figure 4. Neuronal circuit trajectories on correct and error trials.

Figure 4

  1. Time course of mean choice-specific trajectories on correct right (red) and left (blue) choice trials from one session (plotted for the first three common factors). Points labeled 1, 2, 3 correspond to the mean times of the cue offset, turn onset, and trial end, respectively.
  2. Example individual (thin lines) and mean (thick lines) trajectories for correct trials from the session in (a).
  3. Euclidean distance between the mean trajectories on correct right and left choice trials (n = 29 individual sessions, gray). The black line indicates the mean.
  4. Trajectory selectivity index for individual correct trials from a single session, defined based on distances as (dto mean traj, opposite choicedto mean traj, same choice)/(dto mean traj, same choice + dto mean traj, opposite choice). Values close to 1 and -1 indicate that the individual trial trajectory was near the mean trajectory of the same and opposite behavioral choice, respectively. Mean trajectories were defined using correct trials only. Gray and black lines indicate individual trials and the mean, respectively.
  5. Classification accuracy for determining the behavioral choice of the mouse at different time points in the task during individual sessions (gray, n = 29; black, mean). The classifier was based on a distance-dependent classification scheme (see Methods) using correct trials.
  6. Example individual trial trajectories (gray and black) on right choice and left choice errors trials, plotted with the mean trajectories for correct right (red) and left (blue) choice trials.
  7. Same as for (d), except on error trials.
  8. Same as for (e), except on error trials.

Trajectories were highly variable on error trials. Some trajectories began close to the correct choice trajectory during the cue period and transitioned toward the error choice trajectory later in the trial; such transitions occurred at a wide range of points in the trial (gray traces in Fig. 4f, Supplementary Fig. 19f). Other trajectories were similar to the error choice trajectory throughout the trial (Fig. 4f, black traces). On average, the error trial trajectories were more similar to the correct choice trajectory during the cue period and closer to the error choice trajectory during the turn period (cue offset: trajectory selectivity index = −0.13 ± 0.22, p < 0.001 vs. 0, t-test; trial end: trajectory selectivity index = 0.18 ± 0.15, p < 0.001 vs. 0; Fig. 4g; consistent results based on classification, Fig. 4h). Therefore, individual trajectories transitioned between the mean correct right and left choice trajectories at many time points during trials, but most frequently switched during the delay period.

Anatomical micro-organization of PPC dynamics

To determine how the cells implementing the activity dynamics were anatomically organized, we first examined if cells with different response preferences, such as activity peaks at different times in the trial or different preferences for right and left choice trials, were present in the same regions of the PPC or separated into different areas. Each ~250 µm by ~125 µm area (i.e. field-of-view with simultaneously imaged cells) contained both right and left choice-preferring cells of approximately equal numbers and cells with activity peaks at a wide range of times in the trial (Fig. 5a–c, Supplementary Fig. 20). Next, within each imaged area we compared the activities of pairs of neurons as a function of the distance between the neurons’ cell bodies. The difference in the trial-type selectivity for cells in a pair did not depend significantly on the distance between cells, indicating that left and right choice-preferring cells were intermixed (ρ = 0.04, Spearman’s correlation, p > 0.05; Fig. 5e). Similarly, ΔtCOM for a cell pair did not differ with the distance between the cells, indicating that cells active during different periods of the task were intermingled (ρ = −0.01, p > 0.6; Fig. 5d). However, cells that were separated by less anatomical distance had ΔF/F traces that were significantly more correlated than cell pairs further apart, but the relationship between the correlation coefficient and distance was weak (all pairs of active cells: ρ = −0.16, p < 0.001; pairs with the same trial-type and behavioral period preference: ρ = −0.22, p < 0.001 Fig. 5f). This weak relationship could be due to overlapping fluorescence changes, such as from dendritic signals, or could reflect an actual, weak spatial organization. Regardless, cells with highly different activity patterns were intermixed over short length scales, and an anatomical separation of the response properties we measured was not present.

Figure 5. Anatomical micro-organization in the PPC.

Figure 5

  1. Example field-of-view with cells outlined and choice-specific, task-modulated cells colored. Top: Cells’ trial-type selectivity, defined as (ΔF/Fright trials − ΔF/Fleft trials) / (ΔF/Fright trials + ΔF/Fleft trials). Values close to 1 (red) and -1 (blue) indicate right and left choice preferences, respectively. Bottom: Cells’ time of the center-of-mass of the mean ΔF/F trace (tCOM). tCOM = 0 corresponds to ~3 s before the cue offset.
  2. Box plots of tCOM values for task-modulated cells in individual fields-of-view (box edges, first and third quartiles; vertical line in the box, median; whiskers, range)
  3. Number of right and left choice-preferring cells in each field-of-view (n = 29; unity line is shown).
  4. Difference in tCOM as a function of the distance between cells.
  5. Difference in the trial-type selectivity index as a function of the distance between cells.
  6. Pearson’s correlation between non-averaged ΔF/F traces (all time points) for all pairs of active cells (black) and cell pairs with the same trial-type and behavioral period preference (gray) as a function of the cell-cell distance.

Discussion

The choice-specific sequences of PPC neuronal activation we report here add to the growing list of studies that have identified cortical sequences of activity states during working memory tasks2730. Furthermore, because we demonstrated that PPC activity was necessary only for the memory-guided task, the sequences of activation were likely important at least for the memory aspect of the task. Sequence-based dynamics may therefore be a common framework for circuit function during memory and decision tasks, including during navigation behaviors. Such dynamics could potentially be implemented using feedforward architectures3940 or liquid state machines4142 related to those that have been proposed for working memory.

Our results also offer a way to unite previous work on neural coding in the PPC. Navigation, memory, and choice information may be combined in the sequences such that the identity of the active sequence reflects choice-related information for working memory and movement planning45 and the currently active cell within the sequence reflects spatial or temporal progression through the task7,43, which were highly correlated in our task (Supplementary Fig. 2). It seems unlikely that PPC neurons only provided location information in a context-dependent manner, like hippocampal place cells29,44, because unlike place cells PPC neurons did not encode spatial location during a linear track task (Supplementary Fig. 18) or other tasks67.

A possible explanation for heterogeneity and sequences in our experiments versus stereotypy and low-dimensional dynamics, which have been emphasized in previous recordings and models of PPC activity1,1318,2021, is that the PPC adopts different dynamics depending on the demands of the behavioral task. Because traditional delayed saccade tasks, for example, have one spatiotemporal component during the delay period (fixation before making a response), the PPC may adopt sustained activity patterns. In contrast, during tasks that involve many spatial and temporal components, as are common in natural behaviors and during navigation, the PPC may utilize sequences of activation. Alternatively, sequences of activity may be present in the primate PPC during traditional tasks but have yet to be identified, consistent with emerging evidence for heterogeneous temporal response properties2224,30. In addition, different regions or layers of the PPC may have differing activity dynamics45, or the dynamics of rodent and primate PPC circuits may differ.

Because cells that were active at distinct time points in the task and that participated in different choice-specific sequences were spatially intermingled, our results indicate that functionally distinct subnetworks are anatomically interlaced in the PPC. This extends previous work in sensory cortex, motor cortex, and the hippocampus showing spatial intermixing of heterogeneous response properties in cells encoding qualitatively similar types of information (e.g. orientation selectivity in visual cortex) or cells with activity during similar task epochs12,3132. These findings differ from the predictions of models that have emphasized the spatial clustering of similar response patterns, as in functional columns, and that propose connectivity defined by axonal-dendritic overlap without fine-scale specificity4647. Rather, our results support a model in which microcircuits are formed by highly specific synaptic connectivity and are comprised of neuronal motifs, such as those identified in brain slice recordings in sensory cortices4849 and amongst visual cortical neurons with the same orientation selectivity50. Our results showing that cell pairs that were sequentially active during their preferred periods on correct trials had similar activity relationships even during error trials and the inter-trial interval suggest the presence of such motifs (Supplementary Fig. 16g–i).

Our behavioral paradigm did not isolate the decision-making process. We note however that activity trajectories occasionally switched during a trial between the prototypical correct left and right choice trajectories, including frequently on error trials, suggesting that the mouse’s decision was not necessarily irreversibly reached immediately after a trial’s start (trajectory selectivity switch during the delay period or last 1 s of the cue period: 63% of error trials, 20% of correct trials; Fig. 4d,g, Supplementary Fig. 19e–f). Sequences may therefore play a role in some aspect of decision-related processes, but further experiments will be necessary to assess this possibility.

Our results motivate consideration of a conceptual framework for decision-making and working memory in which sensory information used for the decision activates a neuronal sequence of activity. The sequence begins in a choice-independent state, which could be mediated by neurons that are not choice-specific (Supplementary Fig. 7b), and then moves toward a choice-specific trajectory and away from other trajectories in a manner dependent on the incoming information. A decision is proposed to be reached when the sequence of activity intersects a choice-specific trajectory; different decisions involve intersections with different trajectories. Upon reaching a decision, a working memory can be maintained by continuing along that choice-specific trajectory. Changing decisions would occur through transitions between trajectories, but as time progresses in the task, the state space distance between trajectories increases, in effect creating a larger barrier to change. In this view, decision-making and working memory utilize an ordered progression through a sequence in which information moves from one population of neurons to another over time. The framework we propose is an extension of a point-of-view first considered in describing the dynamics underlying behavioral choices in the leech nervous system37. It has both some similarities and differences from drift/diffusion-to-bound, race, and recurrent network models of decision-making implemented as neuronal integrator winner-take-all circuits2,16. These models are similar to the trajectory-based view in that different decisions correspond to a divergence in state space surrounding a separatrix. However, these models differ from the sequence framework in that decisions are reached when the activity approaches a choice-specific fixed point with working memory maintained as stable activity at that point. Thus, although these circuits can demonstrate divergent trajectories to reach the fixed points associated with different choices, the trajectories are defined by relatively homogeneous changes in the activity of the population, and the same set of neurons participate in all stages of the decision-making and working memory process.

Methods summary

A complete description of the experimental methods and data analysis is available in the Supplementary Information.

Supplementary Material

1
2

Acknowledgements

We thank D. Dombeck for assistance on imaging methods and analysis, C. Domnisoru, M. de Bettencourt, C. Brody, and A. Miri for discussions and M. Goldman, J. Hopfield, D. Aronov, B. Scott, and T. Hanks for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the NIH (R01-MH083686), a fellowship from the Helen Hay Whitney Foundation (C.D.H.), and a Burroughs Wellcome Fund Career Award at the Scientific Interface (C.D.H.)

Footnotes

Author contributions

C.D.H. performed experiments with assistance from P.C. on the retrograde tracing experiments. D.W.T. implemented the imaging instrumentation. C.D.H. analyzed the data with strategy and methods contributions from D.W.T.. C.D.H. and D.W.T. wrote the paper.

References

  • 1.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:1916–1936. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Freedman DJ, Assad JA. A proposed common neural mechanism for categorization and perceptual decisions. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:143–146. doi: 10.1038/nn.2740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Andersen RA, Cui H. Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits. Neuron. 2009;63:568–583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:1–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.McNaughton BL, et al. Cortical representation of motion during unrestrained spatial navigation in the rat. Cereb Cortex. 1994;4:27–39. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Nitz DA. Tracking route progression in the posterior parietal cortex. Neuron. 2006;49:747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Whitlock JR, Sutherland RJ, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:14755–14762. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804216105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Calton JL, Taube JS. Where am I and how will I get there from here? A role for posterior parietal cortex in the integration of spatial information and route planning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;91:186–196. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW. Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature. 2009;461:941–946. doi: 10.1038/nature08499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dombeck DA, Khabbaz AN, Collman F, Adelman TL, Tank DW. Imaging large-scale neural activity with cellular resolution in awake, mobile mice. Neuron. 2007;56:43–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dombeck DA, Harvey CD, Tian L, Looger LL, Tank DW. Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation. Nat Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.1038/nn.2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Curtis CE, Lee D. Beyond working memory: the role of persistent activity in decision making. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:216–222. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Barash S, Bracewell RM, Fogassi L, Gnadt JW, Andersen RA. Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. I. Temporal properties; comparison with area 7a. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66:1095–1108. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2919–2940. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.2919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wang XJ. Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron. 2008;60:215–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wong KF, Wang XJ. A recurrent network mechanism of time integration in perceptual decisions. J Neurosci. 2006;26:1314–1328. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mazurek ME, Roitman JD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1257–1269. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ganguli S, et al. One-dimensional dynamics of attention and decision making in LIP. Neuron. 2008;58:15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Miller P, Brody CD, Romo R, Wang XJ. A recurrent network model of somatosensory parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1208–1218. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Machens CK, Romo R, Brody CD. Flexible control of mutual inhibition: a neural model of two-interval discrimination. Science. 2005;307:1121–1124. doi: 10.1126/science.1104171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Machens CK, Romo R, Brody CD. Functional, but not anatomical, separation of "what" and "when" in prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30:350–360. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3276-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jun JK, et al. Heterogenous population coding of a short-term memory and decision task. J Neurosci. 2010;30:916–929. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2062-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Singh R, Eliasmith C. Higher-dimensional neurons explain the tuning and dynamics of working memory cells. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3667–3678. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4864-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Batuev AS. Two neuronal systems involved in short-term spatial memory in monkeys. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 1994;54:335–344. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Seidemann E, Meilijson I, Abeles M, Bergman H, Vaadia E. Simultaneously recorded single units in the frontal cortex go through sequences of discrete and stable states in monkeys performing a delayed localization task. J Neurosci. 1996;16:752–768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00752.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Baeg EH, et al. Dynamics of population code for working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2003;40:177–188. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00597-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fujisawa S, Amarasingham A, Harrison MT, Buzsaki G. Behavior-dependent short-term assembly dynamics in the medial prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:823–833. doi: 10.1038/nn.2134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsaki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:1322–1327. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Crowe DA, Averbeck BB, Chafee MV. Rapid sequences of population activity patterns dynamically encode task-critical spatial information in parietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30:11640–11653. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0954-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ohki K, Chung S, Ch'ng YH, Kara P, Reid RC. Functional imaging with cellular resolution reveals precise micro-architecture in visual cortex. Nature. 2005;433:597–603. doi: 10.1038/nature03274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Komiyama T, et al. Learning-related fine-scale specificity imaged in motor cortex circuits of behaving mice. Nature. 2010;464:1182–1186. doi: 10.1038/nature08897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Erlich JC, Bialek M, Brody CD. A cortical substrate for memory-guided orienting in the rat. Neuron. 2011;72:330–343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Corwin JV, Reep RL. Rodent posterior parietal cortex as a component of a cortical network mediating directed spatial attention. Psychobiology. 1998;26:87–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tian L, et al. Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nat Methods. 2009;6:875–881. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mazor O, Laurent G. Transient dynamics versus fixed points in odor representations by locust antennal lobe projection neurons. Neuron. 2005;48:661–673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Briggman KL, Abarbanel HD, Kristan WB., Jr Optical imaging of neuronal populations during decision-making. Science. 2005;307:896–901. doi: 10.1126/science.1103736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Churchland MM, Yu BM, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Techniques for extracting single-trial activity patterns from large-scale neural recordings. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:609–618. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goldman MS. Memory without feedback in a neural network. Neuron. 2009;61:621–634. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ganguli S, Huh D, Sompolinsky H. Memory traces in dynamical systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:18970–18975. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804451105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Maass W, Joshi P, Sontag ED. Computational aspects of feedback in neural circuits. PLoS Comput Biol. 2007;3:e165. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sussillo D, Abbott LF. Generating coherent patterns of activity from chaotic neural networks. Neuron. 2009;63:544–557. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Leon MI, Shadlen MN. Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron. 2003;38:317–327. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wood ER, Dudchenko PA, Robitsek RJ, Eichenbaum H. Hippocampal neurons encode information about different types of memory episodes occurring in the same location. Neuron. 2000;27:623–633. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00071-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Burke SN, et al. Differential encoding of behavior and spatial context in deep and superficial layers of the neocortex. Neuron. 2005;45:667–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Braitenberg VB, Schuz A. Anatomy of the cortex: Statistics and geometry. Springer-Verlag; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Binzegger T, Douglas RJ, Martin KA. A quantitative map of the circuit of cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24:8441–8453. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1400-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Yoshimura Y, Dantzker JL, Callaway EM. Excitatory cortical neurons form fine-scale functional networks. Nature. 2005;433:868–873. doi: 10.1038/nature03252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Song S, Sjostrom PJ, Reigl M, Nelson S, Chklovskii DB. Highly nonrandom features of synaptic connectivity in local cortical circuits. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e68. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ko H, et al. Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature. 2011;473:87–91. doi: 10.1038/nature09880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

RESOURCES