Skip to main content
Cambridge Open Access logoLink to Cambridge Open Access
. 2011 Mar 14;15(4):559–571. doi: 10.1017/S146114571100037X

The ethics of elective psychopharmacology

Ahmed D Mohamed 1,2,3,, Barbara J Sahakian 1,2,3,4
PMCID: PMC3325502  PMID: 21396152

Abstract

Pharmacological cognitive enhancers (PCEs) are used to improve cognitive functions, such as attention, learning, memory and planning in patients with impairments in cognition resulting from traumatic brain injury (TBI) or from neuropsychiatric disorders such as Alzheimer's disease (AD), mild cognitive impairment, schizophrenia, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Moreover, PCEs have been shown to improve cognition in healthy volunteers with no psychiatric disorders. This article describes the rationale behind the need for their use in neuropsychiatric patients and illustrates how PCEs can ameliorate cognitive impairments, improve quality of life and wellbeing, and therefore reduce the economic burden associated with these disorders. We also describe evidence that PCEs are being used as cognitive enhancers by healthy people. Crucially, as the lifestyle use of these drugs becomes very popular in the healthy population, a final aim is to present an overview of the current and future neuroethical considerations of enhancing the healthy brain. As information regarding their actual use, benefits and harms in various healthy populations is currently lacking, we propose research that aims to obtain relevant empirical data, monitor the short- and long-term effectiveness and side-effects, and initiate accurate surveys to determine current patterns and quantity of usage of PCE drugs by healthy people. Furthermore, in order to instigate a dialogue between neuroethics and neuropsychopharmacology, we urge scientists to explore and communicate the social and ethical implications of their research to the public. Finally, we discuss and highlight other means of enhancing cognition in both patients and healthy adults, including education and physical exercise.

Keywords: Atomoxetine, methylphenidate, modafinil, neuroethics, pharmacological cognitive enhancers

Pharmacological cognitive enhancers (PCEs)

Cognitive impairment is a core deficit of a number of neuropsychiatric disorders (Goldberg & Green, 2002; Weickert et al. 2000) and drugs that improve facets of cognition such as attention, learning, memory and executive functions are known as PCEs (Morein-Zamir et al. 2008; Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2010). These drugs alter neurotransmitter modulation of cognition leading to improvements in cognitive deficits in patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) (Teitelman, 2001), depression (Vaishnavi et al. 2006), addiction (Shearer & Rodgers, 2009), multiple sclerosis (Kraft & Bowen, 2005; Zifko et al. 2002), Parkinson's disease (Nieves & Lang, 2002), and those suffering from Alzheimer's disease (AD), schizophrenia, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The goal for their use is to ameliorate impaired functional outcomes. There is substantial opportunity for meeting the challenge of improving cognition and mental wellbeing in those with mental health problems and for reducing substantially the factors that contribute to the loss of mental, social and economic capital (Beddington et al. 2008).

A good illustration is AD, which is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by a decline in cognitive and behavioural functioning. It is the commonest cause of dementia and one of the most disabling and burdensome health conditions worldwide (Ferri et al. 2005). There are currently 820 000 people with dementia in the UK, which costs £23 billion per year (Alzheimer's Society, 2007). Globally, if no effective prevention strategies or neuroprotective medications are developed, 81.1 million people will suffer from dementia by 2040 (Ferri et al. 2005). However, a treatment that would reduce severe cognitive impairment in older people by just 1% a year would cancel out all estimated increases in the long-term care costs due to ageing population (Comas-Herrera et al. 2007).

Cognitive enhancers hold significant benefits in ameliorating these cognitive impairments in AD and countering these economic and social burdens (Alzheimer's Research Trust, 2010; Kaduszkiewicz et al. 2005). For example, cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEI), such as donepezil, that inhibit centrally active acetylcholinesterase (AchE) and boost acetylcholine in the brain, compensate for the degeneration of neurons in the neocortex that regulate attention and memory (Stahl, 2009), and are effective in the treatment of mild and moderate AD (Eagger et al. 1991a, b; NICE, 2010).

Selective ChEIs release growth factors, interfere with amyloid deposition, or modulate nicotinic receptors (Pepeu & Giovannini, 2009; Stahl, 2009) while future drugs may exert their beneficial effects by activating various neurotransmitters including noradrenaline (NA), dopamine (DA), serotonin (5-HT), GABA and glutamate (Keowkase et al. 2010). Furthermore, drugs that activate synaptic NMDA receptors work synergistically with AMPA receptors to produce long-lasting changes in the synaptic functioning and enable the encoding of new memories. This promotes the phosphorylation of CREB that slows down the pathological changes observed in AD (Snyder et al. 2005). However, these drugs only minimize the neural damage caused by glutamate's neurotoxic effects and evidence is needed of improved episodic memory in AD patients. Importantly, some drugs have significant side-effects. For example, donepezil is contra-indicated for people with liver problems (Mount & Downton, 2006) while others have modest clinical efficacy in early (Lanctot et al. 2003) and advanced stages (Alzheimer's Association, 2005). Furthermore, about 30–40% of patients with AD may not respond to ChEI, and approximately 29% of patients treated with ChEI leave clinical trials because of adverse events (Birks, 2006). Therefore, it is important to develop novel and effective neuroprotective agents that selectively target the underlying neuropathology associated with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and AD. Developing these drugs could be advantageous to the individual and to society, particularly given the significant ageing population in the UK and the USA. It is not within the scope of this review to cover in detail the range and action of all the current and novel PCEs and therefore the reader is referred to Stahl (2009) and The Academy of Medical Sciences (2008).

Notwithstanding, PCEs might also improve the quality of life in patients with TBI, which is the most common cause of disability in young people (Colantonio et al. 2010; Norup et al. 2010). For example, survivors of TBI often suffer from chronic cognitive deficits (Salmond et al. 2005, 2006) in areas such as sustained attention and learning which implicate impaired cholinergic function (Polo et al. 2002; Salmond & Sahakian, 2005). Furthermore, voxel-based morphometry studies reveal structurally reduced grey-matter density and changes in hippocampus and neocortex in TBI patients. This is further consistent with the cholinergic dysfunction account that commonly contributes to the development of TBI-induced cognitive impairments (Salmond et al. 2005). Consequently, the use of ChEIs that increase cholinergic function may be of benefit to TBI patients (Tenovuo et al. 2009). However, whether PCEs improve apathy, which is often considerably disabling and detrimental to rehabilitative efforts in TBI patients, needs to be determined in future research (Keenan et al. 2005; Padala et al. 2007).

Equally, patients with schizophrenia can also benefit from PCEs through improvements in executive functions (Barnett et al. 2010). Schizophrenia is a complex, lifelong disorder that significantly impairs cognitive and motivational function in approximately 1% of the world's population. Although psychotic symptoms, such as hallucinations and delusions, can be managed with antipsychotic treatment (Canuso et al. 2009), patients continue to suffer dysfunctions in cognition, affect and motivation, which account for substantial decrements in social and occupational functioning (Harvey et al. 2007; Matza et al. 2006; Velligan et al. 2006). Specifically, evidence indicates that these patients are substantially impaired in a wide range of neuropsychological task performances (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998; Reichenberg & Harvey, 2007), and these impairments often impede everyday function and quality of life for many patients (Morein-Zamir et al. 2007). PCEs may prove beneficial as an add-on to antipsychotic medication, as it has been suggested that, in patients with schizophrenia, even small improvements in cognitive functions, such as enhancing the ability to adapt efficiently to new situations and to plan effectively, could help them make the transition to independent living (Altamura & Glick, 2010; Davison & Keefe, 1995). Enhancing patients’ cognition will not only improve their quality of life, but will also enable them to access jobs and integrate with society. In consequence, governments are relieved from the cost burden of ongoing care for these patients (Nicholl et al. 2010). In keeping with this, we have shown that PCE drugs improve cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia (Barnett et al. 2010), and that modafinil (Provigil®), a wake-promoting drug licensed for narcolepsy, can improve cognitive flexibility as measured by extra-dimensional attentional set-shifting in patients with chronic schizophrenia (Turner et al. 2004a). Recently, modafinil has been shown to enhance some aspects of social cognition such as emotional facial recognition in patients with first episode of psychosis (Scoriels et al. 2010). However, many of the studies cited above are acute proof of concept studies and therefore one must be cautious about inferred long-term clinical significance, which still requires confirmation in experimental studies.

Nevertheless, children with ADHD can benefit from PCEs. ADHD is a heritable and disabling disorder characterized by core cognitive and behavioural symptoms of impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention. It is the most prevalent neuropsychiatric childhood disorder which affects around 3–7% of children worldwide (APA, 2000; Polanczyk et al. 2007). Structural abnormalities in fronto-striato-circuitry (Durston et al. 2003) and dysfunction in catecholamine neurotransmission, specifically in NA and DA pathways in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) have been implicated in ADHD (Yang et al. 2007). This leads to inefficient information processing and hypo-activation in the frontal lobes (Stahl, 2008). As a result, ADHD patients have significant impairments in performing working memory (WM) and executive function tasks (Biederman et al. 2007; Chamberlain et al. 2007; Dowson et al. 2004). However, if these impairments are not treated early, they can lead to significant negative life events such as drop out from education, job dismissal, criminal activities, substance abuse, and driving accidents (Barkley, 2006). PCEs such as methylphenidate (Ritalin®) (Dodds et al. 2008), selective NA reuptake inhibitor (SNRI) atomoxetine (Strattera®) (Chamberlain et al. 2007; Donnelly et al. 2009) and modafinil (Turner et al. 2003), increase DA and NA levels in the PFC (Mehta et al. 2004; Stahl, 2009; Wilens, 2006) and alleviate cognitive impairments in ADHD patients (DeVito et al. 2008; Turner et al. 2004b). Indeed, studies in our laboratory that use double-blind placebo-controlled designs showed that methylphenidate improves WM, cognitive flexibility, attention and response inhibition in both children and adults with ADHD (DeVito et al. 2008; Turner et al. 2004b). For example, DeVito et al. (2008) used the Cambridge Gambling Task, a test of decision making and risk taking, and showed that a single 0.5 mg/kg dose of methylphenidate reduced large bets on this task in ADHD boys, who performed similarly to healthy boys without medication. In consequence, PCEs can effectively improve core symptoms, abnormal behaviours, self-esteem, cognition, social and family function in ADHD patients (Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2007). However, methylphenidate is successful in treating only about 60–70% of ADHD children, meaning that 30% of patients with ADHD either do not respond to treatment or the drug causes adverse side-effects, such as headache, stomach pain, loss of appetite, trouble sleeping, dizziness and nausea, which precludes the use of methylphenidate. Atomoxetine may be a more acceptable treatment due to its low abuse liability and minimal adverse side-effects (Heil et al. 2002). Still, unlike methylphenidate which improves spatial WM (SWM), sustained attention, and response time in ADHD patients and healthy volunteers (Elliott et al. 1997; Turner et al. 2004b), atomoxetine only improves response inhibition possibly due to its selective NA modulation (Chamberlain et al. 2006, 2007), and is likely to be less effective in treating the range of cognitive deficits associated with ADHD. Therefore, there is a need for medication with improved efficacy and reduced side-effects for ADHD.

Furthermore, studies employing the same methodology show that modafinil also significantly improves short-term memory span, visual memory, spatial planning, and stop-signal motor inhibition in ADHD adults (Turner et al. 2004b). These improvements are consistent with randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trials with modafinil that demonstrate symptom reduction in ADHD children and adolescents (Biederman et al. 2005, 2006; Greenhill et al. 2006; Swanson et al. 2006). Hence, in psychiatry ethics, developing novel PCEs that improve the wellbeing and quality of life for these patients meets the ‘right to receive effective treatment that would offer them a reasonable opportunity to improve their mental condition’ (Bloch & Green, 2008, p. 490).

Pharmacogenomics

Another important argument for developing novel PCEs relates to the rise of pharmacogenomics and individualized medicine that aim to combat neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases. Pharmacogenomics is the discipline behind how genes influence the body's response to drugs. There is growing evidence that key gene variants can change activity within specific neuronal circuits and, as a result, influence particular cognitive-affective phenomena. For example, the catecholamine-O-methyltrasferase (COMT) gene has been shown to affect responses to COMT inhibitors and to predict WM performance whereas the Val108/158 polymorphism exerts a significant effect on enzyme activity and affects DA-regulated PFC activity during WM tasks, and also modifies the effect of dopaminergic drugs (e.g. the COMT enzyme inhibitor tolcapone) in the PFC (Diaz-Asper et al. 2008; Goldberg et al. 2003). Similarly, the therapeutic response in AD appears to be genotype-specific, with APOE-4/4 carriers being the worst responders to conventional treatments (Cacabelos, 2005). However, although behavioural phenotypes and action of PCEs are generally complex (Diaz-Asper et al. 2006), both reflecting the action of multiple genes and neurotransmitters respectively, it is possible that using pharmacogenomics to develop targeted PCEs for particular subgroups and individual responsiveness will lead to greater efficacy and reduced side-effects.

PCEs improve cognition in healthy individuals

PCEs also improve cognition in non-sleep-deprived healthy adults. For example, Turner et al. (2003) showed that a single oral dose of modafinil (100 mg or 200 mg) significantly improved performance on tests of digit span, visual pattern recognition memory, spatial planning, and stop signal reaction time (SSRT) task, or response inhibition, in healthy volunteers. Modafinil also improved the response time in tests of decision making, delayed matching to sample, and spatial planning (Müller et al. 2004). More recently, it improved accuracy in an attention-shifting task, without reaction time trade-off (Marchant et al. 2009), especially when participants’ resources were most challenged. Consistent with this, Müller et al. (2004) demonstrated that modafinil significantly reduced error rates in a long-delay visuo-spatial task and manipulation conditions, without speed-accuracy trade-off.

Similarly, an acute dose of atomoxetine can improve response inhibition in healthy adults. Chamberlain et al. (2009) used functional magnetic resonance imaging and examined the brain mechanism by which atomoxetine exerts its cognitive enhancing effects in healthy volunteers. They found that atomoxetine led to increased activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus (RIFG) when participants attempted to inhibit their responses in the SSRT task. The RIFG has previously been shown to be activated during inhibitory motor control (Aron et al. 2003). It is thought that atomoxetine improves response inhibition via noradrenergic mechanisms. In contrast, methylphenidate has been shown to enhance SWM performance in healthy adults (Elliott et al. 1997). In a study employing sophisticated neuropsychological tests and brain-imaging measures (see Supplementary online material), Mehta et al. (2000) showed that methylphenidate improves both performance and efficiency in the SWM neural network, which includes the dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex in healthy volunteers (Owen et al. 1996). These areas have been significantly associated with WM and executive functions (Robbins et al. 2000). Moreover, studies using positron emission tomography and contrasting [11C]raclopride binding, with the participants either on or off methylphenidate, have further indicated that methylphenidate influences dopaminergic function, particularly in the striatum (Wang et al. 1999). Other DA agonists improve WM and performance of executive tasks in healthy individuals (Mehta & Riedel, 2006; Roesch-Ely et al. 2005). There is evidence for enhancement of other forms of memory by PCEs. For example, evidence from healthy volunteers show that ampakine CX516 and ChEIs also lead to moderate improvements in recall and short-term memory (Wezenberg et al. 2007). In particular, pilots who took donepezil just before learning specific manoeuvres in a flight simulator outperformed a control group on tests of performance conducted 1 month later (Yesavage et al. 2002). However, the mechanism of action for improvement in attention, memory and executive function of PCEs still remains to be determined in many cases. For example, to exert its cognitive enhancing effects, modafinil has been shown to elevate numerous neurotransmitters including NA, DA and glutamate (Minzenberg & Carter, 2008; Volkow et al. 2009).

Neurotransmitter modulation of cognition

Evidently, as these improvements relate to neurotransmitter modulation and function (Iversen et al. 2009), the effects of some PCEs might follow the Yerkes–Dodson law, which explains the relationship between arousal and performance. This principle might be translated to several neurotransmitter systems where cognitive function often follows an inverted U-shaped curve, with deviations from the optimal level in either direction producing sub-optimal performance (Robbins & Sahakian, 1979; Robinson & Sahakian, 2009). For instance, low levels of NA release engage α2-adrenergic receptors and improve executive function whereas higher levels of NA release engage α1-adrenergic receptors which cause significant stress in humans and animals (Arnsten, 2000; Finlay et al. 1995) and impair prefrontal functionality (Ramos & Arnsten, 2007). A similar U-inverted relationship is evidenced between DA and WM function (Vijayraghavan et al. 2007) as both marked increases and decreases of DA in the PFC have been associated with sub-optimal performance (Cools et al. 2003; Iversen, 2001, p. 31). Consistent with this hypothesis, methylphenidate improves cognitive performance in individuals with greater impairment (Konrad et al. 2004; Mehta et al. 2000) while guanfacine (Tenex®), an α2-adrenergic receptor agonist, has beneficial effects on WM and attentional functions in patients with ADHD, but does not improve WM or executive functions in healthy male volunteers (Müller et al. 2005). Thus, the effects of pharmacological substances on cognition are complex as cognition is a multifaceted construct encompassing numerous mental functions including both cold cognition (such as attention, planning, problem solving, and response inhibition) and hot cognition (such as risky decision making; Roiser et al. 2006). For instance, PCEs may further modulate important human virtues such as creativity. A recent study by Farah et al. (2009) showed that the mixed amphetamine salts, adderall, licensed for the treatment of ADHD, enhanced performance on convergent tasks of creativity for lower-performing individuals and either impaired or did not change it for higher-performing individuals. These results on improvement and impairment on higher cognitive function with PCEs raises the issue of what we mean by a general term ‘enhancement’. As healthy adults fall into a wide spectrum of normality, some individuals may be improved by a PCE drug while others remain unchanged or are even impaired (Randall et al. 2005; Robbins & Sahakian, 1979). Furthermore, there is as yet no robust empirical research to demonstrate that PCEs have effects on divergent thinking in healthy people.

Lifestyle use of PCEs by healthy individuals

The above results demonstrate the potential of PCEs to enhance certain cognitive domains in healthy adults. Therefore, attitudes towards their use by the general population need to be considered. In the next section, we focus our discussion on current and future trends of the use of PCEs by healthy people.

In the past few years there has been an unprecedented rise in the use of PCEs among healthy individuals for cognitive enhancement. Cognitive enhancement can be defined as the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal and external information processing systems (Bostrom & Roache, unpublished data). Healthy university students (Desantis & Hane, 2010) and academics (Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2007) have been using PCEs to improve their cognitive function. More specifically, students are taking PCEs to improve academic performance (Rabiner et al. 2009) and are framing their actions as both physically harmless and morally acceptable (Desantis & Hane, 2010). For example, in the USA, 16% of college students (Babcock & Byrne, 2000) and 8% of undergraduates reported having illicitly obtained and used prescription stimulants (Hall et al. 2005; Lord et al. 2009; Teter et al. 2005). Furthermore, a 2005 survey by the US National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA, 2005) found that 2.5% of 13- to 14-yr-olds, 3.4% of 15- to 16-yr-olds and 5.1% of 17- to 18-yr-olds abused methylphenidate. In 2009, the figures for these groups were 1.8%, 3.6% and 2.1%, respectively (NIDA, 2009). Presumably, these young people are obtaining stimulant drugs from others who have prescriptions or purchasing them via the internet or street dealers.

Currently, the global market share of modafinil is more than US$700 million per year (Norman & Berger, 2008). Consistent with Greely's (2006) claim, that healthy physicians on call, students and academics are increasingly using PCEs to enhance cognitive abilities, it is estimated that around 90% of modafinil is predominantly used off-label by healthy, non-sleep-deprived individuals (Baranski et al. 2004; Vastag, 2004). In contrast, beta-blockers that are prescribed to reduce anxiety in clinical patients have been used by musicians to dampen physiological tremors in order to improve their performances on stage (Tindal, 2004).

In the UK, a newspaper survey of 1000 students showed that 1 in 10 were taking prescription drugs for cognitive enhancement (Lennard, 2009). In England, prescription rates of stimulants have been rising steadily from 220000 in 1998 to 418300 in 2004 (Niyadurupola, 2008). In 2008, the journal Nature conducted a poll about the use of PCEs by healthy academics, in which 1400 scientists from 60 different countries responded (Maher, 2008). One in five respondents used drugs for cognitive enhancement, with 52% of them obtaining the drug by prescription, while 34% obtained the drug via the internet and 14% through their pharmacy. The most popular drug was methylphenidate, with 62% of users; 44% reported taking modafinil mainly to improve concentration, and 15% reported taking beta-blockers for anxiety. Of all respondents, 96% thought that people with neuropsychiatric disorders should be given cognitive enhancing drugs. In contrast, 86% of respondents thought that healthy children under the age of 16 yr should be restricted from taking PCE drugs.

Although some of these data were not rigorously collected, they nonetheless suggest the increased use of PCEs among healthy individuals. Their widespread use is not surprising given that small percentage increments in performance can lead to significant improvements in functional outcome. Indeed, a 10% improvement in memory score could lead to an improvement in an A-level grade or degree class (Academy of Medical Sciences, 2008, p. 150).

Neuroethical issues in cognitive enhancement

Nevertheless, the increase in lifestyle use of PCEs by healthy people raises numerous ethical issues that inform the growing field of neuroethics. Neuroethics is the study of the ethical, legal and social questions that arise when scientific findings about the brain are carried into medical practice, legal interpretations and health and social policy (Marcus, 2002). As such, modifying our inherent self, character and individuality through PCE drugs has important implications for society. For these reasons, their lifestyle use has prompted a significant interest both in the media and the public (Coveney et al. 2009; Stix, 2009). There is a concern that PCEs will threaten our notion of personhood and will dampen essential characteristics of what it means to be human (Farah et al. 2004; President's Council on Bioethics, 2003).

As a consequence, enhancing the brain and higher cognitive processes demands strong ethical considerations and a practical policy framework. To address this, we have argued elsewhere that before PCE drugs are prescribed to healthy people, their long-term safety, side-effects and their effectiveness must be tested to provide important facts necessary for further decision making about their regulation (Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2010). Moreover, we have engaged with the media about the need to establish regulations for the use of PCE drugs by healthy people (Mohamed & Sahakian, 2010). In Nature, we emphasized the need to ensure their safe use by healthy people (Greely et al. 2008) while in Science we advocated that ethical considerations in regard to societal issues associated with the use of PCEs by healthy people should be part of neuroethical training within university neuroscience programmes (Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2009).

But, what are the advantages and disadvantages of healthy people using PCEs? Since PCEs improve those with low cognitive performance (Robbins & Sahakian, 1979), it might be possible to mitigate the adverse environmental effects, such as poverty, on the brain and cognition through their use. This might contribute to removing disparity in society. It may also be that some ‘healthy’ people actually have undiagnosed attentional or other problems and are actually self-medicating with drugs such as ritalin. Furthermore, even healthy adults, who normally function well, are not always performing optimally due to sleep deprivation, jet lag or other stressors, and some might need to perform at their best possible level on every occasion (e.g. surgeons, air traffic controllers). In addition, PCEs might enable us to perform better in other competitive or life threatening situations. For instance, psychostimulants have been employed to boost cognition in soldiers in combat (Caldwell et al. 2000; Moran et al. 2007; Russo et al. 2008). PCEs have also been demonstrated to improve performance in shift workers (Ballon & Feifel, 2006), pilots (Caldwell, 2001) and school pupils with ADHD (Trout et al. 2007). Recently, the US Defence Advanced Research Project Agency (DARPA, 2007) introduced the Augmented Cognition Programme to enhance soldiers’ memory and cognition through technology when under conditions of interrupted sleep and stress. However, if proven to be safe, PCEs may be preferred for cost-effectiveness when compared to other methods of enhancement such as expensive technology.

In contrast, the disadvantages of using PCEs include the potential harms and long-term side-effects that they might have in healthy people, particularly in adolescents where the brain is still in development. There are strong safety concerns, especially in the absence of informative data, for healthy individuals as the risk of adverse side-effects might outweigh the beneficial effects of PCE drugs. The abuse liability of some of the PCEs such as methylphenidate is also a concern. A recent study showed that modafinil blocked DA transporters and increased DA in the caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens in healthy human brain (Volkow et al. 2009), which are areas in a network known to be involved in drug-seeking behaviour and addiction (Volkow & Li, 2004). This indicates the need for awareness about the risks involved in PCE use among healthy people and shows that a full ethical consideration of their use is required. To date, there have been no randomized psychopharmacological trials investigating the long-term effects of PCE drugs on healthy people.

There is still a further safety concern about the risks of purchasing substances advertised as PCEs over the internet (Forman et al. 2006a, b). As these drugs are not prescribed by a qualified doctor, they might not be suitable for some people. For instance, contra-indications of atomoxetine and modafinil include heart problems and hepatic impairments (British National Formulary, 2010). Additionally, if one is taking other medication there might be serious drug–drug interactions which could be dangerous in some cases.

With regard to personal autonomy, there are ethical concerns about healthy people being coerced or even forced into using a PCE. Society might force people to take psychoactive agents in order to perform better or to be in a particular mental state. For example, authorities in the USA ordered a mentally ill inmate in criminal proceedings to take psychotropic medication to improve his competence to stand trial and be executed (Boire & Ruiz-Sierra, 2003; Randall, 2004). There is also a considerable potential for indirect coercion resulting from a highly demanding 24/7 society where people feel compelled to take PCEs in order to meet social or workplace demands. Healthy people may resort to self-medication for inadequate sleep or over-exertion at work. For example, 33% of respondents in Maher's (2008) poll indicated that they would feel pressure to give PCE drugs to their children if other children at school were taking them. However, the use of PCEs to enhance cognition is one solution to improving the individual and society. Indeed, we have argued elsewhere that there are other methods of boosting cognition, including education and exercise (Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2010). For instance, physical exercise can improve learning and memory (Creer et al. 2010; Hillman et al. 2008). Through these non-pharmacological means we might be able to effectively and safely enhance cognition and well-being in society.

Another argument against their use is that they might further exacerbate the ever-growing disparity and inequality in society, especially if only the wealthy can access them. Equally, is it morally justified to use PCEs during exams, and does it give the user unfair advantage over those who are equally capable but not cognitively enhanced with drugs? Many universities as yet have no formal policy about the use of PCEs during exams. If PCEs become easily accessible in the future, will society consider their use as cheating or will they equate them to having a caffeine boost from coffee? Is it possible that once PCE drugs are widely available we might run the risk of becoming a homogeneous society? Could our perception of ourselves change from being human to being mechanistic beings with a modicum of emotion? Are we going to be over-enhanced only to be plagued by unwanted memories? Will using PCEs outdate important human virtues such as hard work and reflection and make us unable to take credit for our minds’ achievements?

The advantages and disadvantages of using PCEs have to be evaluated carefully. With regards to fairness, enhancing cognition might lead to dramatic social benefits by reducing natural inequality and promoting social justice (Savulescu, 2006). This is because increasing cognitive ability on an individual level could have dramatic and positive effects on society and the economy as a whole (Bostrom, 2008). For instance, a 3% population-wide increase in IQ would reduce poverty rates by 25% (Weiss, 1998), and would lead to an annual economic gain of US$165–195 billion and up to 1.5% GDP growth (Salkever, 2005; Schwartz, 1994).

Public engagement in neuroscience

However, if healthy people take PCEs to gain a competitive edge but fail to see any difference in the long-term or notice possible impairments observed in high-functioning adults (Mattay et al. 2003) it could spark controversy and outcry in the public. Hence, determining who can use PCEs and under what circumstances involves complex decision making and ethical judgements. Thus, how neuroscientific discoveries impact on society has given rise to an enormous interest in the field of neuroethics, including the foundation of the Neuroethics Society (http://www.neuroethicssociety.org) which advocates further research on ethical questions that are yet to be answered. For instance, what are the possible long-term harms of using PCE drugs in healthy people, particularly in the developing brain? What are the implications of developments in pharmacogenomics? Without formal regulation of their use, healthy people can purchase PCEs via the internet, with all the inherent dangers in doing so. How would such easy access affect widespread use of these drugs by healthy young and elderly people and also impact on society? How would we, as neuropsychopharmacologists, react if we discover that our colleagues or our children's friends are taking PCEs? How should governments react? These questions in neuropsychopharmacology and neuroethics merit further rigorous research. They also clearly indicate the need to engage in discussion with the public about the social and ethical implications of the use of PCEs by healthy individuals (Morein-Zamir & Sahakian, 2009; Ringach, 2009). For this to happen, neuropsychopharmacologists need to integrate experimental results within a neuroethical framework. In order to do this, they need to innovatively work together with social scientists, philosophers, and ethicists (Morein-Zamir & Sahakian, 2009; Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2009, 2010). This increases neuroscientists’ role and responsibility in society (Farah et al. 2004) and puts them in a leading position to engage policy makers and a broad group of stakeholders, including the general public. This will ensure that technological advances in neuroscience are put to maximal benefit and minimal harm.

Conclusions

PCEs have the potential to ameliorate cognitive dysfunction and to provide important clinical benefits for patients. Further development of more effective PCEs with fewer side-effects, in addition to neuroprotective agents for patients with neurodegenerative diseases such as AD, is clearly worthy of pursuit. Pharmacogenomics will make it possible to target individuals with safe and effective drugs. PCEs can also improve cognitive function such as memory and attention in healthy individuals. However, their long-term cognitive enhancing potential as well as their side-effects in healthy people needs to be rigorously determined. Currently, the unprecedented rise of PCE use among the healthy raises numerous ethical issues. Scientists need to work together with social scientists, philosophers, ethicists, policy makers, and teachers to actively discuss the ethical consequences of PCE usage. This will ensure maximal benefit and minimal harm in the advances in neuroscience. Finally, the use of PCEs to enhance cognition is one solution to improving the individual and society. However, this does not preclude other means of enhancing cognition such as education and exercise (Beddington et al. 2008; Sahakian et al. 2010).

Acknowledgements

B.J.S. is partly funded by a Wellcome Trust Grant Award (program grant 019407) and a consortium joint award from the MRC and Wellcome Trust (BCNI grant G0001354). A.D.M. is a Wellcome Trust-funded Ph.D. student at Clare Hall, Cambridge and is funded by The Oxford Centre for Neuroethics at the University of Oxford (Grant: 086041/Z/08/Z).

We thank Dr Ulrich Müller, Christopher Lewis and Charlotte Housden for discussion.

Note

Supplementary material accompanies this paper on the Journal's website (http://journals.cambridge.org/pnp).

Statement of Interest

B.J.S. consults for Cambridge Cognition Ltd. She has consulted for Novartis, Shire, GlaxoSmithKline, Lilly and Boehringer-Ingelheim. She has also received honoraria for Grand Rounds in Psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital (CME credits) (Boston, 27 April 2007) and for speaking at the International Conference on Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia and Mood Disorders: clinical aspects, mechanisms and therapy (Brescia, 17–19 January 2007). She was on the Medical Research Council Neurosciences and Mental Health Board (2010) and on the Science Co-ordination Team for the Foresight Project on Mental Capital and Wellbeing, 2008 (Office of Science, The Department of Innovation, Universities and Skills). She is currently on Panel LS5 for the European Research Council. As an Associate Editor, she also receives an honorarium from the journal Psychological Medicine.

References

  1. Academy of Medical Sciences. Brain Science, Addiction and Drugs: Working Group Report Chaired by Professor Sir Gabriel Horn. Foresight Brain Science, Addiction and Drugs Project. London: Office of Science and Technology; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ajibola O, Clement PW. Differential effects of methylphenidate and self-reinforcement on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Modification. 1995;19:211–233. doi: 10.1177/01454455950192004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Altamura AC, Glick ID. Designing outcome studies to determine efficacy and safety of antipsychotics for ‘real world’ treatment of schizophrenia: trends and Perspectives. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;13:971–973. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709991271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alzheimer's Association. 2005. http://www.alz.org/national/documents/topicsheet_treatments.pdf. http://www.alz.org/national/documents/topicsheet_treatments.pdf ). FDA-approved treatments for Alzheimer's ( ). Accessed November 2010.
  5. Alzheimer's Research Trust. Dementia 2010: the economic burden of dementia and associated research funding in the United Kingdom. Health Economics Research Centre. Alzheimer's Research Trust; 2010. pp. 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  6. Alzheimer's Society. Dementia UK: 2007. ). Dementia UK 2007: a report into the prevalence and cost of dementia prepared by the Personal Social Services Research Unit (PSSRU) at the London School of Economics and the Institute of Psychiatry at King's College London, for the Alzheimer's Society: the full report, ). Accessed 2 February 2010. [Google Scholar]
  7. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edn Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. , text revision (DSM-IV-TR). [Google Scholar]
  8. Arnsten AFT. Through the looking glass: differential noradrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical function. Neural Plasticity. 2000;7:133–146. doi: 10.1155/NP.2000.133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ. et al. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:115–116. doi: 10.1038/nn1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Babcock Q, Byrne T. Student perceptions of methylphenidate abuse at a public liberal arts college. Journal of American College Health. 2000;49:143–145. doi: 10.1080/07448480009596296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ballon JS, Feifel D. A systematic review of modafinil: potential clinical uses and mechanisms of action. Journal of clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67:544–566. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baranski JV, Pigeau R, Dinich P, Jacobs I. et al. Effects of modafinil on cognitive and meta-cognitive performance. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2004;19:323–332. doi: 10.1002/hup.596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. 3rd edn. Guilford: New York; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  14. Barnett JH, Robbins TW, Leeson VC, Sahakian BJ. et al. Assessing cognitive function in clinical trials of schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:1161–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Beddington J, Cooper CL, Field J, Goswami U. et al. The mental wealth of nations. Nature. 2008;455:1057–1060. doi: 10.1038/4551057a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Biederman J, Petty CR, Fried R, Fontanelli J. et al. Can self-reported behavioral scales assess executive function deficits? A controlled study of adults with ADHD. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2007;195:240–246. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000243968.06789.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Biederman J, Swanson J, Wigal SB. Efficacy and safety of modafinil film-coated tablets in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, flexible-dose study. Pediatrics. 2005;116:777–784. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Biederman J, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, Boellner SW. et al. A comparison of once-daily and divided doses of modafinil in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized, double-blind, and placebo-controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67:727–735. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Birks J. Cholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2006 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005593. . Issue 1. Art. No.: CD005593. doi: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Boire RG, Ruiz-Sierra J. 2003. http://www.cognitiveliberty.org/news/US_v_Sell_decision.htm. http://www.cognitiveliberty.org/news/US_v_Sell_decision.htm ). Supreme court upholds right to refuse mind-altering drugs ( ). Accessed February 2010.
  21. Bostrom N. Cognitive enhancement in the public interest. Reshaping the human condition: exploring human enhancement. Science and Innovation Network and the Parliamentary Office of Science & Technology 2008:29–35. , pp. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bloch S, Green SA. Singer PA, Viens AM. The Cambridge Textbook of Bioethics. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2008. Psychiatric ethics; pp. 490–492. [Google Scholar]
  23. British National Formulary. 2010. http://bnf.org/bnf/bnf/current/ http://bnf.org/bnf/bnf/current/ ). BNF 60 ( ). Accessed November 2010.
  24. Cacabelos R. Pharmacogenomics and therapeutic prospects in Alzheimer's disease. Expert Opinion in Pharmacotherpy. 2005;6:1967–1987. doi: 10.1517/14656566.6.12.1967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Caldwell JA. Efficacy of stimulants for fatigue management: the effects of provigil®and dexedrine® on sleep-deprived aviators. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2001;4:19–37. [Google Scholar]
  26. Caldwell JA, Caldwell JL, Smythe NK III, Hall KK. A double-blind, placebo-controlled investigation of the efficacy of modafinil for sustaining the alertness and performance of aviators: a helicopter simulator study. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2000;150:272–282. doi: 10.1007/s002130000450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Canuso CM, Dirks B, Carothers J, Kosik-Gonzales C. et al. Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of paliperidone extended-release and quetiapine in inpatients with recently exacerbated schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:691–701. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08040613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chamberlain SR, del Campo N, Dowson J, Müller U. et al. Atomoxetine improved response inhibition in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62:977–984. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Chamberlain SR, Hampshire A, Muller U, Rubia K. et al. Atomoxetine modulates right inferior frontal activation during inhibitory control: a pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:550–555. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L. et al. Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science. 2006;311:861–863. doi: 10.1126/science.1121218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Colantonio A, Howse D, Patel J. Young adults with traumatic brain injury in long-term care homes: a population-based study. Brain Impairment. 2010;11:31–36. [Google Scholar]
  32. Comas-Herrera A, Wittenburg R, Pickard L, Knapp M. Cognitive impairment in older people: future demand for long-term care services and the associated costs. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2007;22:1037–1045. doi: 10.1002/gps.1830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. L-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1431–1441. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00117-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Coveney CM, Nerlich B, Martin P. Modafinil in the media: metaphors, medicalisation and the body. Social Science & Medicine. 2009;68:487–495. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Creer DJ, Romberg C, Saksida LM, van Praag H. et al. Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107:2367–2372. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911725107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. DARPA. 2007. http://www.darpa.mil/DARPATech2002/presentations/ipto_pdf/speeches/SCHMORRO.pdf. http://www.darpa.mil/DARPATech2002/presentations/ipto_pdf/speeches/SCHMORRO.pdf ). Augmented cognition: building cognitively aware computational systems ( ). Accessed March 2010.
  37. Davison M, Keefe RSE. Cognitive impairment as a target for pharmacological treatment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 1995;17:123–129. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00037-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Desantis DA, Hane CA. Adderall is definitely not a drug’: justifications for the illegal use of ADHD stimulants. Substance Use & Misuse. 2010;45:31–46. doi: 10.3109/10826080902858334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. DeVito EE, Blackwell AD, Kent L, Ersche K. et al. The effects of methylphenidate on decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:636–639. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Diaz-Asper CM, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Straub RE. et al. Genetic variation in catechol-o-methyltransferase: effects on working memory in schizophrenic patients, their siblings, and healthy controls. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63:72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Diaz-Asper CM, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. Catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphisms and some implications for cognitive therapeutics. NeuroRx. 2006;3:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.nurx.2005.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dodds CM, Muller U, Clark L, Van Loon A. et al. Methylphenidate has differential effects on blood oxygenation level-dependent signal related to cognitive subprocesses of reversal learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:5976–5982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1153-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Donnelly C, Bangs M, Trzepacz P, Jin L. et al. Safety and tolerability of atomoxetine over 3 to 4 years in children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:176–185. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318193060e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dowson JH, McLean A, Bazanis E, Toon B. et al. Impaired spatial working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: comparisons with performance in adults with borderline personality disorder and in control subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2004;110:45–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00292.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson M C. et al. Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:871–878. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01904-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Eagger SA Levy R Sahakian BJ 1991aTacrine in Alzheimer's disease Lancet 337989–992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Eagger SA Levy R Sahakian BJ 1991bTacrine in Alzheimer's disease Lancet 33850–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Matthews K, Bannerjea A. et al. Effects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology. 1997;131:196–206. doi: 10.1007/s002130050284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Farah M, Haimm C, Sankoorikal G, Smith ME. et al. When we enhance cognition with adderall, do we sacrifice creativity? A preliminary study. Psychopharmacology, 2009;202:541–547. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1369-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Farah MJ, Illes J, Cook-Deegan R, Gardener H. et al. Neurocognitive enhancement: what can we do and what should we do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:421–425. doi: 10.1038/nrn1390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Broadaty H. et al. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet. 2005;366:2112–2117. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Finlay JM, Zigmond MJ, Abercrombie ED. Increased dopamine and norepinephrine release in medial prefrontal cortex induced by acute and chronic stress: effects of diazepam. Neuroscience. 1995;64:619–628. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)00331-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Forman R Marlowe D McLellan A 2006aThe internet as a source of drugs of abuse Current Psychiatry Reports 8377–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Forman RF Woody GE McLellan T Lynch KG et al. 2006bThe availability of web sites offering to sell opioid medications without prescriptions American Journal of Psychiatry 1631233–1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Gscheidle T. et al. Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-ethyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:889–896. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Goldberg TE, Green MF. Davis KL. Psychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. New York: Raven Press; 2002. Neurocognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: an overview; pp. 657–669. [Google Scholar]
  57. Greely H. Illis J. Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. The social effects of advances in neuroscience: legal problems, legal perspectives; pp. 245–263. [Google Scholar]
  58. Greely H, Sahakian B, Harris J, Kessler RC. et al. Towards responsible use of cognitive-enhancing drugs by the healthy. Nature. 2008;456:702–705. doi: 10.1038/456702a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Greenhill LL, Biederman J, Boellner SW, Rugino TA. et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of modafinil film-coated tablets in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:503–511. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000205709.63571.c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hall KM, Irwin MM, Bowman KA, Frankenberger W. et al. Illicit use of prescribed stimulant medication among college students. Journal of American College Health. 2005;53:167–174. doi: 10.3200/JACH.53.4.167-174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Harvey CA, Jeffreys SE, McNaught AS, Blizard RA. et al. The Camden schizophrenia surveys III: five-year outcome of a sample of individuals from a prevalence survey and the importance of social relationships. International Journal of Social Psychiatry. 2007;53:340–356. doi: 10.1177/0020764006074529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Heil SH, Holmes HW, Bickel WK, Higgins ST. et al. Comparison of the subjective, physiological, and psychomotor effects of atomoxetine and methylphenidate in light drug users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2002;67:149–156. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Science and society: be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Iversen L. Drugs: A Very Short Story. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  66. Iversen L, Iversen SP, Bloom FE. Introduction to Neuropsychopharmacology. Oxford: University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  67. Kaduszkiewicz H, Zimmermann T, Beck-Bornholdt H-P, van den Bussche H. et al. Cholinesterase inhibitors for patients with Alzheimer's disease: systematic review of randomised clinical trials. British Medical Journal. 2005;331:321–332. doi: 10.1136/bmj.331.7512.321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Keenan S, Mavaddat N, Iddon J, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ. et al. Effects of methylphenidate on cognition and apathy in normal pressure hydrocephalus: a case study and review. British Journal of Neurosurgery. 2005;19:46–50. doi: 10.1080/02688690500080893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Keowkase Aboukhatwa M, Luo Y. Fluoxetine protects against amyloid-beta toxicity, in part via daf-16 mediated cell signalling pathways, in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;59:358–365. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2010.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Konrad K, Gunther T, Hanisch C, Herpertz-Dahlmann B. et al. Differential effects of methylphenidate on attentional functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43:191–198. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200402000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kraft GH, Bowen J. Modafinil for fatigue in MS: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind study. Neurology. 2005;27:1995–1997. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000200985.04239.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, Yau KK, Khan LR. et al. Efficacy and safety of cholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer's Disease. A meta-analysis. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2003;16:169. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lennard N. One in ten takes drugs to study. Varsity. 2009;693:1. [Google Scholar]
  74. Lord S, Downs G, Furtaw P, Chaudhari A. et al. Nonmedical use of prescription opioids and stimulants among student pharmacists. Journal of the American Pharmacists Association. 2009;49:519–528. doi: 10.1331/JAPhA.2009.08027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lowe C, Rabbitt P. Test/re-test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological batteries: theoretical and practical issues. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:915–923. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00036-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Maher B. Poll results: look who is doping. Nature. 2008;452:674–675. doi: 10.1038/452674a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Marchant N, Kamel F, Echlin K, Grice J. et al. Modafinil improves rapid shifts of attention. Psychopharmacology. 2009;202:487–495. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1395-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Marcus D. Neuroethics: Mapping the Field Conference Proceedings. San Francisco, California. New York: The Dana Press; 2002. , 13–14 May 2002, [Google Scholar]
  79. Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR. et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2003;100:6186–6191. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0931309100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Matza LS, Buchanan R, Purdon S, Brewster-Jordan J. et al. Measuring changes in functional status among patients with schizophrenia: the link with cognitive impairment. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006;32:666–678. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mehta MA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Methylphenidate improves working memory and set-shifting in AD/HD: relationships to baseline memory capacity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:293–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mehta MA, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Mavaddat N. et al. Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:1–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0004.2000. , 65RC- [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mehta MA, Riedel WJ. Dopaminergic enhancement of cognitive function. Current Pharmacological Designs. 2006;12:2487. doi: 10.2174/138161206777698891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Minzenberg MJ, Carter CS. Modafinil: a review of neurochemical actions and effects on cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1477–1502. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mohamed AD, Sahakian BJ. It's time to establish rules for the use of cognitive-enhancing drugs by healthy people. http://www.prospectmagazine.co.uk/2010/05/going-mental/ Prospect Magazine, Science and Technology 2010;171 , vol. [Google Scholar]
  86. Moran DS, Eliyahu U, Berlin S, Hadad E, Heled Y. et al. Psychostimulants and military operations. Military Medicine. 2007;172:383–387. doi: 10.7205/milmed.172.4.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW, Turner D, Sahakian BJ. Mental Capital and Wellbeing: Making the Most of Ourselves in the 21st Century. London: 2008. State-of-science review: SR-E9: pharmacological cognitive enhancement; pp. 3–16. . Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. The Government Office for Science, ). Accessed 30 June 2010. [Google Scholar]
  88. Morein-Zamir S, Sahakian BJ. Neuroethics and public engagement training needed for neuroscientists. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;14:49–51. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Morein-Zamir S, Turner DC, Sahakian BJ. A review of the effects of modafinil on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2007;33:1298–1306. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Mount C, Downton C. Alzheimer's disease: progress or profit? Nature Medicine. 2006;12:780–784. doi: 10.1038/nm0706-780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Müller U, Clark L, Lam ML, Moore RM, Murphy CL. et al. Lack of effects of guanfacine on executive and memory functions in healthy male volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2005;182:205–213. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0078-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Müller U, Steffenhagen N, Regenthal R, Bublak P. et al. Effects of modafinil on working memory processes in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2004;177:161–169. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1926-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. NIDA. 2005. http://www.drugabuse.gov/pdf/infofacts/Ritalin05.pdf. http://www.drugabuse.gov/pdf/infofacts/Ritalin05.pdf. ). InfoFacts. Methylphenidate (Ritalin). National Institute on Drug Abuse ( Accessed 4 March 2010.
  94. NIDA. 2009. http://www.drugabuse.gov/mediaguide/commonlyabused.html. http://www.drugabuse.gov/mediaguide/commonlyabused.html ). The Basics of commonly abused drugs. National Institute on Drug Abuse ( ). Accessed 3 June 2010.
  95. NICE. 2010. http://www.nice.org.uk/ http://www.nice.org.uk/ ) National Institute for Clinical Health and Excellence. Accessed November 2010.
  96. Nicholl D, Akhras KS, Diels J, Schadrack J. et al. Burden of schizophrenia in recently diagnosed patients: healthcare utilisation and cost perspective. Current Medical Research and Opinion. 2010;26:943–955. doi: 10.1185/03007991003658956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Nieves AV, Lang AE. Treatment of excessive daytime sleepiness in patients with Parkinson's disease with modafinil. Clinical Neuropharmacology. 2002;25:111–114. doi: 10.1097/00002826-200203000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Niyadurupola G. Zonneveld L, Dijstelbloem H, Ringoir D. Reshaping the Human Condition: Exploring Human Enhancement. The Hague: Rathenau Institute; 2008. Better brains; pp. 77–87. , in collaboration with the British Embassy, Science and Innovation Network and the Parliamentary Office of Science & Technology ( ). Accessed November 2010. [Google Scholar]
  99. Norman C, Berger M. Neuroenhancment: status quo and perspectives. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2008;258:110–114. doi: 10.1007/s00406-008-5022-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Norup A, Siert L, Mortensen LE. Emotional distress and quality of life in relatives of patients with severe brain injury: the first month after injury. Brain Injury. 2010;24:81–88. doi: 10.3109/02699050903508200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Owen A, Eans AC, Petrides M. Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cortex. 1996;6:31–38. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Padala PR, Burke WJ, Bhatia SC. Modafinil therapy for apathy in an elderly patient. Annals of Pharmacology. 2007;41:346–349. doi: 10.1345/aph.1H302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pepeu G, Giovannini MG. Cholinesterase inhibitors and beyond. Current Alzheimer Research. 2009;6:86–96. doi: 10.2174/156720509787602861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J. et al. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2007;164:942–948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Polo MD, Newton P, Rogers D, Escera C. et al. ERPs and behavioural indices of long-term preattentive and attentive deficits after closed head injury. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:2350. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00127-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. President's Council on Bioethics. 2003. http://www.bioethics.gov/background/kasspaper.html. http://www.bioethics.gov/background/kasspaper.html ). Beyond therapy: biotechnology and the pursuit of human improvement ( ). Accessed January 2010.
  107. Rabiner DL, Anastopoulos AD, Costello EJ, Hoyle RH. et al. Motives and perceived consequences of nonmedical ADHD medication use by college students: are students treating themselves for attention problems? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2009;13:259–270. doi: 10.1177/1087054708320399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Ramos BP, Arnsten AF. Adrenergic pharmacology and cognition: focus on the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacology and Therapeutics. 2007;113:523–536. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Randall DC, Shneerson JM, File SE. Cognitive effects of modafinil in student volunteers may depend on IQ. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2005;82:133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Randall K. 2004. http://www.cognitiveliberty.org/dll/singleton_executed.html. http://www.cognitiveliberty.org/dll/singleton_executed.html ). Mentally ill inmate put to death after medical ‘treatment’ prepares execution ( ). Accessed February 2010.
  111. Reichenberg A, Harvey P. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:833–858. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Ringach DL. Scientists talking to the public: is there anyone out there? Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1477–1477. [Google Scholar]
  113. Robbins TW, Mehta MA, Sahakian BJ. Neuroscience. Boosting working memory. Science. 2000;290:2275–2276. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5500.2275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Paradoxical’ effects of psychomotor stimulant drugs in hyperactive children from the standpoint of behavioural pharmacology. Neuropharmacology. 1979;18:931–950. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(79)90157-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Robinson OJ, Sahakian BJ. Acute tryptophan depletion evokes negative mood in healthy females who have previously experienced concurrent negative mood and tryptophan depletion. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2009;205:227–235. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1533-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Roesch-Ely D, Scheffel H, Weiland S. Differential dopaminergic modulation of executive control in healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2005;178:420–430. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2027-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Roiser JP, Rubinsztein JS, Sahakian BJ. Neuropsychology of mood disorders. Psychiatry. 2006;5:158–162. [Google Scholar]
  118. Russo MB, Arnett MV, Thomas ML, Cadwell JA. et al. Ethical use of cogniceuticals in the militaries of democratic nations. American Journal of Bioethics. 2008;8:39–41. doi: 10.1080/15265160802015016. , discussion W34-36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Sahakian B, Morein-Zamir S. Professor's little helper. Nature. 2007;450:1157–1159. doi: 10.1038/4501157a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Sahakian BJ, Malloch G, Kennard C. A UK strategy for mental health and wellbeing. Lancet. 2010;375:1854–1855. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60817-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sahakian BJ, Morein-Zamir S. Neuroscientists need neuroethics teaching. Science. 2009;325:147. doi: 10.1126/science.325_147a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Sahakian BJ, Morein-Zamir S. Neuroethical issues in cognitive enhancement. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2010;25:197–204. doi: 10.1177/0269881109106926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sahakian BJ, Owen A. Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion paper. Journal of The Royal Society of Medicine. 1992;85:399–402. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Salkever DS. Updated estimates of earnings benefits from reduced exposure of children to environmental lead. Environmental Research. 2005;70:1–6. doi: 10.1006/enrs.1995.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Salmond CH, Chatfield DA, Menon DK, Pickard JD. et al. Cognitive sequelae of head injury: involvement of basal forebrain and associated structures. Brain. 2005;128:189–200. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Salmond CH, Menon DK, Chatfield DA, Pickard JD. et al. Changes over time in cognitive and structural profiles of head injury survivors. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:1995. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Salmond CH, Sahakian BJ. Cognitive outcome in traumatic brain injury survivors. Current Opinion in Critical Care. 2005;11:111–116. doi: 10.1097/01.ccx.0000155358.31983.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Savulescu J. Justice, fairness, and enhancement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1093:321–338. doi: 10.1196/annals.1382.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Schwartz J. Low-level lead exposure and children's IQ: a meta-analysis and search for a threshold. Environmental Research. 1994;65:42–55. doi: 10.1006/enrs.1994.1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Scoriels L, Barnett J, Murray G, Sahakian B. et al. The effects of modafinil on cognitive and emotional functions in the first episode of psychosis. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;69:457–464. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Shearer JD, Rodgers SC, Slade T, Van Beek L. et al. A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of modafinil (200 mg/day) for methamphetamine dependence. Addiction. 2009;104:224–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02437.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Snyder EM, Nong Y, Almeida CG, Paul S. et al. Regulation of NMDA receptor trafficking by amyloid-[beta] Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1051–1058. doi: 10.1038/nn1503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Stahl MS. Stahl's Essential Psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific Basis and Practical Applications. 3rd edn. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  134. Stahl MS. Stahl's Essential Psychopharmacology: The Prescriber's Guide. Cambridge University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  135. Stix G. Turbo charging the brain. Scientific American. 2009;301:46–55. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1009-46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Swanson JM, Greenhill LL, Lopez FA. Modafinil film-coated tablets in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, fixed-dose study followed by abrupt discontinuation. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67:137–147. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Teitelman E. Off-label uses of Modafinil. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:1341. doi: 10.1176/ajp.158.8.1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Tenovuo O, Alin J, Helenius H. A randomized controlled trial of rivastigmine for chronic sequels of traumatic brain injury: what it showed and taught? Brain Injury. 2009;23:548–558. doi: 10.1080/02699050902926275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Teter CJ, McCabe SE, Cranford JA, Boyd CJ. et al. Prevalence and motives for illicit use of prescription stimulants in an undergraduate student sample. Journal of American College Health. 2005;53:253–262. doi: 10.3200/JACH.53.6.253-262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Tindal B. 2004. http://www.mozartinthejungle.com/new_york_times_39713.htm. http://www.mozartinthejungle.com/new_york_times_39713.htm ). Better playing through chemistry ( ). Accessed November 2010.
  141. Trout AL, Ortiz Lienemann T, Reid R. A review of non-medication interventions to improve the academic performance of children and youth with ADHD. Remedial and Special Education. 2007;28:207–226. [Google Scholar]
  142. Turner DC Clark L Pomarol-Clotet E McKenna P et al. 2004aModafinil improves cognition and attentional set shifting in patients with chronic schizophrenia Neuropsychopharmacology 291363–1373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Turner DC Dowson J Robbins TW Sahakian BJ 2004bModafinil improves cognition and response inhibition in adult ADHD Biological Psychiatry, 551031–1040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Turner DC, Robbins TW, Clark L, Aron AR. et al. Cognitive enhancement effects of modafinil in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2003;165:260–269. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1250-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Vaishnavi S, Gadde K, Alamy S, Zhang W. et al. Modafinil for atypical depression: effects of open-label and double-blind discontinuation treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;26:373–378. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000227700.263.75.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Vastag B. Poised to challenge need for sleep, ‘wakefulness enhancer’ rouses concerns. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;291:167–170. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.2.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Velligan DI, Mueller J, Wang M, Dicocco M. et al. Use of environmental supports among patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services. 2006;57:219–224. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.57.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, Williams GV. et al. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:376–384. doi: 10.1038/nn1846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J, Alexoff D. et al. Effects of modafinil on dopamine and dopamine transporters in the male human brain: clinical implications. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301:1148–1154. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Volkow ND, Li T-K. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:963–970. doi: 10.1038/nrn1539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J, Pappas NR. et al. Reproducibility of repeated measures of endogenous dopamine competition with [11C] raclopride in the human brain in response to methylphenidate. Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 1999;40:1285–1291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Weickert T, Goldberg T, Gold J. et al. Cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:907–913. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.9.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Weiss B. A risk assessment perspective on the neurobehavioral toxicity of endocrine disruptors. Toxicology and Industrial Health, 1998;14:397–418. doi: 10.1177/074823379801400122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Wezenberg E, Verkes RJ, Ruigt GS, Hulstijn W. et al. Acute effects of the ampakine farampator on memory and information processing in healthy elderly volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1272–1283. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wilens TE. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:2059–2063. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.12.2059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wood SJ, Pantelis C, Proffitt T, Phillips LJ. et al. Spatial working memory ability is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33:1239–1247. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Yang B, Chan RCK, Jing J, Li T. et al. A meta-analysis of association studies between the 10-repeat allele of a VNTR polymorphism in the 3prime-UTR of dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2007;144B:541–550. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Yesavage JA, Mumenthaler MS, Taylor JL. et al. Donepezil and flight simulator performance: effects on retention of complex skills. Neurology. 2002;59:123–125. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Zifko UA, Rupp M, Schwarz S, Zipko HT. et al. Modafinil in treatment of fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Results of an open-label study. Journal of Neurology. 2002;249:983. doi: 10.1007/s00415-002-0765-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES