Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Oct 19.
Published in final edited form as: Nature. 2012 Mar 22;484(7394):381–385. doi: 10.1038/nature11028

Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall

Xu Liu 1,*, Steve Ramirez 1,*, Petti T Pang 1, Corey B Puryear 1, Arvind Govindarajan 1, Karl Deisseroth 2, Susumu Tonegawa 1,**
PMCID: PMC3331914  NIHMSID: NIHMS364287  PMID: 22441246

Abstract

A specific memory is thought to be encoded by a sparse population of neurons1,2. These neurons can be tagged during learning for subsequent identification3 and manipulation4,5,6. Moreover, their ablation or inactivation results in reduced memory expression, suggesting their necessity in mnemonic processes. However, a critical question of sufficiency remains: can one elicit the behavioral output of a specific memory by directly activating a population of neurons that was active during learning? Here we show that optogenetic reactivation of hippocampal neurons activated during fear conditioning is sufficient to induce freezing behavior. We labeled a population of hippocampal dentate gyrus neurons activated during fear learning with channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2)7,8 and later optically reactivated these neurons in a different context. The mice showed increased freezing only upon light stimulation, indicating light-induced fear memory recall. This freezing was not detected in non-fear conditioned mice expressing ChR2 in a similar proportion of cells, nor in fear conditioned mice with cells labeled by EYFP instead of ChR2. Finally, activation of cells labeled in a context not associated with fear did not evoke freezing in mice that were previously fear conditioned in a different context, suggesting that light-induced fear memory recall is context-specific. Together, our findings indicate that activating a sparse but specific ensemble of hippocampal neurons that contribute to a memory engram is sufficient for the recall of that memory. Moreover, our experimental approach offers a general method of mapping cellular populations bearing memory engrams.


How is a distinct memory formed and stored in the brain? Recent studies indicate that defined populations of neurons correspond to a specific memory trace1, suggesting a cellular correlate of a memory engram. Selective ablation or inhibition of such neuronal populations erased the fear memory response5,6, indicating that these cells are necessary for fear memory expression. However, to prove that a cell population is a cellular basis of a specific fear memory engram, one has to conduct a mimicry experiment to show that direct activation of such a population is sufficient for inducing the associated behavioral output9,10.

The hippocampus is thought to be critical in the formation of the contextual component of fear memories11,12,13,14. Modeling15 and experimental16,17 studies have demonstrated an essential role of the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus in discriminating between similar contexts. Cellular studies of immediately early gene (IEG) expression showed that sparse populations of DG granule cells (2–4%) are activated in a given context18. Moreover, although the same population of DG granule cells is activated repeatedly in the same environment, different environments19 or different tasks20 activate different populations of DG granule cells. These lines of evidence point to the DG as an ideal target for the formation of contextual memory engrams that represent discrete environments.

To label and reactivate a subpopulation of DG cells active during the encoding of a memory, we targeted the DG of c-fos-tTA transgenic mice3 with the AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP virus and an optical fiber implant (Fig. 1a). This approach directly couples the promoter of c-fos, an IEG often used as a marker of recent neuronal activity21, to the tetracycline transactivator (tTA), a key component of the doxycycline (Dox) system for inducible expression of a gene of interest22. In our system, the presence of Dox inhibits c-fos-promoter-driven-tTA from binding to its target tetracycline-responsive element (TRE) site, which in turn prevents it from driving ChR2-EYFP expression. In the absence of Dox, training-induced neuronal activity selectively labels active c-fos-expressing DG neurons with ChR2-EYFP, which can then be reactivated by light stimulation during testing (Fig. 1b, c). We confirmed that our manipulation restricts the expression of ChR2-EYFP largely to the DG area of the hippocampus (Fig. 1d–g).

Figure 1. Basic experimental protocols and selective labeling of the DG cells by ChR2-EYFP.

Figure 1

a, The c-fos-tTA mouse was injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and implanted with an optical fiber targeting DG. b, When off Dox, training induces the expression of c-fos-tTA, which binds to TRE and drives the expression of ChR2-EYFP, labeling a subpopulation of activated cells (yellow) in DG. c, Basic experimental scheme. Mice were habituated in context A with light stimulation while on Dox for five days, then taken off Dox for two days and fear conditioned (FC) in context B. Mice were put back on Dox and tested for five days in context A with light stimulation. d, Representative image showing the expression of ChR2-EYFP in a mouse that was taken off Dox for two days and underwent FC training. An image of each rectangular area in (d) is magnified showing DG (e), CA1 (f), and CA3 (g). The green signal from ChR2-EYFP in the DG spreads throughout entire granule cells, including dendrites (e), while the green signal confined to the nuclei in CA1 and CA3 is due to shEGFP expression from the c-fosshEGFP construct of the transgenic mouse (f, g). Blue is nuclear marker DAPI.

First, to characterize the inducible and activity-dependent expression of ChR2-EYFP, we examined its expression timeline under various treatments (Fig. 2a–h). We observed a complete absence of ChR2-EYFP expression in DG neurons while mice were on Dox (Fig. 2a). Two days off Dox was sufficient to induce ChR2-EYFP expression in home-caged mice (Fig. 2b). The number of ChR2-EYFP-positive cells increased significantly in response to two days off Dox followed by fear conditioning (FC; Fig. 2c). We found that the vast majority of c-fos-positive cells were also ChR2-EYFP-positive (Supplementary Fig. 1), confirming that activity-dependent labeling with ChR2-EYFP recapitulated the induction of endogenous c-fos. A similar increase in ChR2-EYFP expression was seen in a group of mice that was exposed to the same context and tone as the FC group but had no shocks delivered (NS; Fig. 2d). ChR2-EYFP expression lasted at least five days (Fig. 2e) and was gone by 30 days (Fig. 2f). Kainic-acid–induced seizures resulted in complete labeling of DG cells with ChR2-EYFP (Fig. 2g), indicating that the relatively sparse labeling in the FC or NS groups was not due to the low infection rate of the virus, but reflected the natural low activity of DG neurons during the training sessions18,23. Notably, NS and FC treatments resulted in similar proportions of ChR2-EYFP-positive cells (Fig. 2h). ChR2-EYFP expression following FC seemed to be restricted to the excitatory neurons, as no overlap was detected between ChR2-EYFP-positive neurons and GABA-positive inhibitory neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Activity dependent expression and stimulation of ChR2-EYFP.

Figure 2

a–f, Representative images of DG from c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and sacrificed after the following treatments: a, On Dox. b, Off Dox for two days in home cage. c, Same as (b) followed by fear conditioning (FC). d, Same as (c) except no shock was delivered (NS). e, Same as (c), five days post-training. f, Same as (c), 30 days post-training. g, Same as (b) followed by kainic acid injection to induce seizure. Residual green signal in (a) and (f) are from nuclear-localized c-fos-shEGFP (see Fig. 1 legend). h, Percentage of ChR2-EYFP–positive cells after various treatments represented by (a) to (g) (n = 5 subjects each; F6,28 = 94.43, *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001). i, j, Representative DG cells after light stimulation in c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP (i) or AAV9-TRE-EYFP (j). k, Percentage of c-fos–positive cells among ChR2-EYFP–positive cells or EYFP-positive cells after light stimulation (n = 3 subjects each; ***P < 0.001). l, Light-evoked single unit activity of a DG neuron from a c-fos-tTA mouse injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP. Peri-event histogram (top) and raster plot (bottom) show reliable and precisely time-locked spiking relative to the onset of 15 ms light pulses (blue bar). Inset shows an overlay of waveforms for all the spikes during light stimulation. m, Spike probability and peak latency for all the light-responsive cells (n = 10) recorded as in (l). n, Multi-unit activity in the DG from a c-fos-tTA mouse injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP in response to trains of 10 light pulses (15 ms; blue bars) at 20 Hz. Scale bar in (a) 250 μm.

We injected c-fos-tTA mice with either AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP or AAV9-TRE-EYFP, subjected them to fear conditioning while off Dox, and then put them back on Dox to test for light-evoked responses from DG cells the following day. The mice were anesthetized for in vivo recordings and blue light pulses (473 nm, 0.1 Hz, 15 ms pulse duration) were delivered to the DG. Consistent with the sparse labeling of DG neurons (Fig. 2h), we identified only 10 DG neurons that responded to light stimulation from nine c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP (the ChR2 group). In these neurons, we detected a reliable increase of spike probability precisely time-locked to the onset of light pulses (Fig. 2l, m). These cells also showed robust responses to trains of 20 Hz light stimulation with a slight decrease in spike probability over time that remained significantly higher above baseline (Fig. 2n). We did not find any light-responsive cells in the 10 c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-EYFP (the EYFP group; data not shown). Most of the ChR2-EYFP-positive cells in the ChR2 group of mice were also positive for endogenous c-fos after optical stimulation, although not all c-fos-positive cells expressed ChR2-EYFP. Very few neurons expressing EYFP in the EYFP group of mice were c-fos-positive (Fig. 2i–k and Supplementary Fig. 3). The proportion of c-fos-positive cells in the downstream CA3 region was greater in the ChR2 group compared with the EYFP group after optical stimulation of DG neurons, and this number was comparable to the proportion of CA3 c-fos-positive cells obtained by exposing a separate group of fear conditioned mice to the conditioned context (Supplementary Fig. 4).

Next, we tested whether activating a population of DG neurons labeled by ChR2-EYFP during the encoding of a fear memory was sufficient for memory recall. The experimental group (Exp) consisted of c-fos-tTA mice unilaterally injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and implanted with an optical fiber targeting the DG (Fig. 1a). Mice were kept on Dox and underwent five days of habituation to record their basal level of freezing in one context (context A) during both light-off and light-on epochs. Next, they were taken off Dox and underwent fear conditioning in a distinct chamber (context B) in which a tone was paired with shock. The mice were then subjected to five days of testing with light-off and light-on epochs in context A while on Dox (Fig. 1c). During the habituation sessions, the Exp mice showed very little freezing during either light-off or light-on epochs. In contrast, after fear conditioning, freezing levels during light-on epochs were significantly higher compared with light-off epochs, which indicated light-induced fear memory recall (Fig. 3a). Increased freezing during light-on epochs was observed across all five days of test sessions with no discernible day-dependent difference (Supplementary Fig. 5g). These data suggest that DG cells that express endogenous c-fos during training, and therefore become labeled by ChR2-EYFP, define an active neural population that is sufficient for memory recall upon subsequent reactivation.

Figure 3. Optical stimulation of engram-bearing cells induces post-training freezing.

Figure 3

a, c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and trained with FC (Exp group) show elevated freezing during three min light-on epochs. Freezing for each epoch represents 5-day average (Supplementary Fig. 5a, g). Freezing levels for the two light-off and light-on epochs are further averaged in the inset. (n = 12, F1,22 = 37.98, ***P < 0.001). b, Mice trained similar to (a) but without foot shock (NS group) do not show increased light-induced freezing (n = 12). c, Mice injected with AAV9-TRE-EYFP and trained with FC (EYFP group) do not show increased light-induced freezing (n = 12). d, Mice trained similar to (a) but kept off Dox for one day before FC training (Exp-1day group) showed greater freezing during test light-on epochs compared to Exp group (n = 5, F1,8 = 38.26, ***P <0.001). e, Mice trained similar to (a) but bilaterally injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and implanted with optical fibers (Exp-Bi group) showed even higher levels of freezing during test light-on epochs (n = 6, F1,10 = 85.14, ***P < 0.001). f, Summary of freezing levels of the five groups during test light-on epochs (F4,42 = 37.62, *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001).

To rule out the possibility that post-training freezing by optical stimulation was due to the activation of DG cells unrelated to fear learning, we injected another group of mice (NS) with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and administered the same habituation, training, and test sessions as the Exp group, except that no shock was delivered during the training session. Despite the fact that a similar level of ChR2-EYFP expression was detected in the NS group compared with the Exp group, both in terms of proportion of cells labeled (Fig. 2h) and ChR2-EYFP fluorescence intensity per cell (Supplementary Fig. 6), light did not induce post-training freezing in the NS group (Fig. 3b). This indicates that the freezing observed in the Exp group requires optical activation of a specific subset of ChR2-EYFP-positive DG cells that are associated with FC and that activating a population of DG cells not associated with FC does not induce freezing. Yet another group of mice (EYFP) were injected with AAV9-TRE-EYFP and underwent identical habituation, training, and testing sessions as the Exp group. The proportion of cells expressing EYFP was comparable to that seen in the Exp group expressing ChR2-EYFP (Supplementary Fig. 7). However, the EYFP group did not show increased post-training freezing (Fig. 3c). This result rules out the possibility that increased freezing in the Exp group was due to any non-specific effects of post-training optical stimulation.

The light-induced freezing levels of the Exp group were relatively low (~15%) compared with those typically reported from exposure to a conditioned context (~60%)3. One possibility is that light activation of background-activity–induced ChR2-EYFP (Fig. 2b) interfered with the expression of the specific fear memory. We confirmed that limiting the off Dox period from two days to one day reduced the background expression of ChR2-EYFP by at least twofold (compare Supplementary Fig. 8a Home cage with Fig. 2h Home cage). A group of mice (Exp-1day) that went through the same design outlined in Figure 1c but with this modification showed greater freezing levels (~25%) during the light-on epoch of test sessions compared to the Exp group (Fig. 3d, f). Another possible factor contributing to the modest light-induced freezing in the Exp group may be the limited number of cells optically stimulated. To test this possibility, we bilaterally injected a group of mice (Exp-Bi) with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and bilaterally implanted optical fibers targeting the DG, and then subjected these mice to the same scheme as that shown in Figure 1c. During the light-on epochs of the test sessions, the Exp-Bi group exhibited levels of freezing (~35%) that were almost as high as those induced by the conditioned context (Fig. 3e, f, Supplementary Fig. 9, and Supplementary movies).

We next examined whether the light-induced fear memory recall was context-specific. First, to test whether two different contexts activate similar or distinct populations of DG cells, we took the mice off Dox for two days and then exposed them to a novel context (context C, an open field) to label the active DG cells with ChR2-EYFP. After being put back on Dox, the mice were fear conditioned in a different context (context B) and sacrificed 1.5 hours later (Fig. 4a). The expression of ChR2-EYFP was used to identify cells previously activated in context C whereas endogenous c-fos was used to identify cells recently activated in context B. Immunohistochemical analyses revealed a chance level of overlap between ChR2-EYFP–positive and c-fos–positive cells, suggesting that two independent DG cell populations were recruited for the representation of the two distinct contexts (Fig. 4b–g). To test the context-specificity of light-induced recall of a fear memory, we subjected a new group of mice (an open field-fear conditioned group, OF-FC) to habituation sessions in context A, followed by two days off Dox and context C exposure to label neurons active in context C with ChR2-EYFP. Next, we put the mice back on Dox and performed fear conditioning in context B (Fig. 4h). These mice were then placed back in context A and tested for light-induced freezing. Light failed to evoke a significant increase in freezing responses (Fig. 4i). Similarly low levels of freezing were observed in another group of mice (FC-OF) in which fear conditioning in context B while on Dox preceded exposure to context C while off Dox (Supplementary Fig. 10). Together, these results indicate that light reactivation of cells labeled in context C did not induce fear memory recall associated with context B.

Figure 4. Labeling and stimulation of independent DG cell populations.

Figure 4

a, c-fos-tTA mice injected with AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP were taken off Dox and exposed to context C to label activated cells with ChR2-EYFP (yellow), then put back on Dox and trained with FC in context B to activate endogenous c-fos (red). Representative images of DG from these mice are shown in (b) to (e). b, ChR2-EYFP–labeled cells activated in context C. c, c-fos–labeled cells activated in context B. d, Nuclear marker DAPI. e, Merge. The white and red circles show examples of ChR2-EYFP–positive and c-fos–positive cells, respectively. The c-fos–positive cells in (e) appear yellow because they express both endogenous c-fos (red) and the nuclear-localized c-fos-shEGFP (green) (see Figure 1 legend). f, Percentage of ChR2-EYFP–positive, endogenous c-fos–positive, and double-positive cells among total cells (DAPI+) (n = 5). g, Observed percentage of double-positive cells is the same as what would be expected if the two cell populations were independent (i.e. a product of the observed percentage of ChR2-EYFP–single-positive and c-fos–single-positive cells.) h, Behavior setup for mice exposed to an open field in context C while off Dox and subsequently fear conditioned in context B while on Dox (OF-FC). i, OF-FC mice (n = 5) do not show increased light-induced freezing. Scale bar in (b) 10 μm.

Here we have shown that optical activation of hippocampal cells that were active during fear conditioning elicits freezing behavior. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration that directly activating a subset of cells involved in the formation of a memory is sufficient to induce the behavioral expression of that memory. Our results and previous studies that addressed the necessity of similarly sparse cell populations in the amygdala5,6 argue that defined cell populations can form a cellular basis for fear memory engrams. The memory engram that we selectively labeled and manipulated is likely contextual in nature, as previous studies have demonstrated that hippocampal interventions affect conditioned freezing responses to a context but not a tone12,13,24. Indeed, recent findings show that optogenetic inhibition of the hippocampal CA1 region during training or testing both inhibited the recall of contextual fear memory, while leaving auditory-cued fear memory recall intact25. However, we cannot completely rule out the possibility that the fear memory recalled in our experiments may have some tone memory component.

Our observation that freezing responses were elicited by optical stimulation in the experimental groups (Exp, Exp-1day, and Exp-Bi), but not in the OF-FC or FC-OF group, strongly supports a dual memory engram hypothesis of contextual fear conditioning26,27,28. In this hypothesis, hippocampal cells are recruited to form contextual memory engrams, but these contextual engrams alone do not represent a complete fear memory. For a fear memory to be formed, the information from the contextual memory engram must be transferred to the basolateral amygdala (BLA) coincidentally with the information representing a foot shock. In the OF-FC or FC-OF scheme, two distinct contextual memory engrams were formed in the DG, which were represented by two distinct sets of DG cells. One of these two contextual engrams (the one for context B) was associated with the representation of the shock, but not the other engram (the one for context C). Since only the latter, but not the former, was labeled by ChR2, optical stimulation could not elicit fear memory expression.

Although we have demonstrated the “sufficiency” of a DG memory engram for the behavioral expression of fear memory, it does not necessarily mean that this engram is “necessary” for behavioral recall. During contextual fear conditioning, it is likely that multiple contextual memory engrams are formed in a series of hippocampal regions. Each of these engrams may contribute to the formation of the complete fear memory in the BLA and may also be capable of reactivating it independently as we observed in the case of the DG engrams. Since the hippocampus is not a linear feed-forward network but contains several parallel circuits, inhibiting the formation or activation of contextual engrams in one region may not necessarily block the expression of the fear memory. For instance, disruption of contextual memory engrams in the DG could be circumvented by CA1 engrams, which could be generated through the direct input from the entorhinal cortex and may be sufficient to activate the fear memory engram in the BLA. Indeed, we recently generated a mouse mutant, which permitted us to demonstrate that the DG input to the CA3 is dispensable in the formation and retrieval of contextual fear memory17.

The approach and methods described in this work will be a powerful tool for mapping multiple component engrams, each contributing to an overall memory. A multifaceted analysis of these engrams and their interplay will reveal the nature of the overall memory engram.

Methods Summary

Virus-mediated gene expression

The pAAV-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and pAAV-TRE-EYFP plasmids were constructed by standard methods and packed as AAV9 viruses. The viruses were stereotaxically injected into the DG (0.15 μl).

Immunohistochemistry

Subjects were sacrificed after various treatments and brain slices were prepared for immunohistochemistry and confocal microscopy. Coronal sections were immunostained for EYFP and/or c-fos. All imaging and analyses were performed blind to the experimental conditions.

In vivo recording

An optrode consisting of a tungsten electrode glued to a 200 μm optical fiber coupled to a 473 nm laser was used for optical stimulation and extracellular electrical recordings in head-fixed, isoflurane-anesthetized mice.

Behavioral tests

Mice used for behavioral tests were injected with AAV9 virus and implanted with an optical fiber targeting the DG. All mice were habituated in context A while on 40 mg/kg Dox for five days for 12 min/day with light stimulation (473 nm, 20 Hz, 15 ms) during minutes 4–6 and 10–12. The groups of mice were taken off Dox for one (Exp-1day) or two days (Exp, Exp-Bi, EYFP, and NS) and fear conditioned in context B with a tone, with (Exp, Exp-1day, Exp-Bi, and EYFP) or without (NS) shock. The OF-FC group was taken off Dox for two days and exposed to context C without shock, then fear conditioned in context B while on Dox. The FC-OF group was fear conditioned in context B while on Dox, then taken off Dox for two days and exposed to context C without shock. All groups were put back on Dox and tested in context A for five days in a manner similar to habituation sessions. All groups except the NS group were then put back to context B for a contextual fear probe trial one day after the last light stimulation, followed by a cued tone probe trial in context D the next day. Freezing levels were scored by experimenters blind to all treatment conditions.

Methods

Subjects

The c-fos-tTA mice were generated from TetTag mice by breeding them with C57BL/6J mice and selecting those carrying only the c-fos-tTA transgene and not the bi-cistronic tetO promoter driving tau-LacZ and tTAH100Y transgenes. These mice also contained a separate transgene consisting of a c-fos promoter driving the expression of nuclear-localized 2-hr half-life EGFP (shEGFP), which is distinct from the whole-cell-localized ChR2-EYFP. Mice had food and water ad libitum and were socially housed until the beginning of the surgery. The mice were 8–12 weeks old at the time of surgery and had been raised on food containing 40 mg/kg Dox for four weeks prior to surgery. Mice were single housed post-surgery and throughout the rest of the experiments. All procedures relating to mouse care and treatment conformed to the institutional and NIH guidelines.

Virus construct and packaging

The pAAV-TRE-ChR2-EYFP plasmid was constructed by cloning TRE-ChR2-EYFP into an AAV backbone using SpeI restriction site at the 5' terminus and blunt end at the 3' terminus of the insert. The pAAV-TRE-EYFP plasmid was constructed by removing the ChR2 fragment from the pAAV-TRE-ChR2-EYFP plasmid using NheI and AgeI restriction sites, blunting with T4 DNA polymerase, and self-ligation of the vector, which retained the ATG start codon of the EYFP gene from the ChR2-EYFP fusion gene. The recombinant AAV vectors were serotyped with AAV9 coat proteins and packaged by the Gene Therapy Center and Vector Core at the University of Massachusetts Medical School. Viral titers were 1 × 1013 GC/ml for AAV9-TRE-ChR2-EYFP and 1.5 × 1013 GC/ml for AAV9-TRE-EYFP.

Stereotactic injection and optical fiber implant

All surgeries were performed under stereotaxic guidance. Mice were anesthetized using 500 mg/kg Avertin. The virus was injected using a glass micropipette attached to a 10 μl Hamilton microsyringe (701LT; Hamilton, Reno, NV) through a microelectrode holder (MPH6S; WPI, Sarasota, FL) filled with mineral oil. A microsyringe pump (UMP3; WPI, Sarasota, FL) and its controller (Micro4; WPI, Sarasota, FL) were used to control the speed of the injection. The needle was slowly lowered to the target site and remained for 10 min before the beginning of the injection. Mice for timeline studies and head-fixed electrophysiology recordings were injected bilaterally (−2.2 mm AP; ±1.3 mm ML; −2.0 mm DV29) with 0.15 μl AAV9 virus at a rate of 0.1 μl/min. After the injection the needle stayed for five additional minutes before it was slowly withdrawn. The mice used for behavior tests were unilaterally or bilaterally injected with the virus same as described above. After withdrawing of the needle, a Doric patchcord optical fiber (200 μm core diameter; Doric lenses, Quebec, Canada) precisely cut to the optimal length was lowered above the injection site (−2.2 mm AP; ±1.3mm ML; −1.6 mm DV). Three jewelry screws were screwed into the skull surrounding the implant site of each hemisphere to provide extra anchor points. A layer of adhesive cement (C&B Metabond; Parkell, Edgewood, NY) was applied followed with dental cement (Teets cold cure; A-M Systems, Sequim, WA) to secure the optical fiber implant. A cap made from the bottom part of a 15 ml Falcon tube (for unilateral implant) or the top part of an Eppendorf tube (for bilateral implant) was inserted to protect the implant and the incision was closed with sutures. The mouse was given 1.5 mg/kg Metacam as analgesics and remained on a heating pad until fully recovered from anesthesia. Mice were allowed to recover for two weeks before all subsequent experiments. All fiber placements (Supplementary Fig. 11) and viral injection sites were verified histologically. As criteria we only included mice with ChR2-EYFP expression limited to the dentate gyrus, which lead to the exclusion of two mice throughout the study.

ChR2-EYFP and EYFP expression timeline

Fourteen days post-surgery, subjects were either kept on Dox and immediately sacrificed or taken off Dox for one or two days. The mice from the latter two groups were either sacrificed with no further treatments (home cage), or underwent fear conditioning or no shock protocols as described in the behavior section below. After each treatment, mice were sacrificed 1.5 hours, 24 hours, five days, or 30 days later as described in the main text and underwent immunohistochemistry procedures. For seizure experiments, mice were taken off Dox for two days and injected i.p. with 20 mg/kg kainic acid. The mice were sacrificed six hours after the first behavioral onset of seizure.

In vivo recording

Mice were anesthetized by isoflurane inhalation and placed in the stereotactic system with anesthesia maintained with 0.5–1% isoflurane throughout the recording. An optrode consisting of a tungsten electrode (1 MΩ) glued to an optical fiber (200 μm core diameter, Doric Lenses, Inc.), with the tip of the electrode extending beyond the tip of the fiber by 500 μm was used for simultaneous optical stimulation and extracellular recordings. The optrode was lowered to the dentate gyrus (−2.2 mm AP; 1.3 mm ML; −2.0 mm DV) using a hydraulic micromanipulator (Model 640; David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA). The optical fiber was connected to a 200 mW 473 nm laser (MBL F473; Opto Engine, Midvale, UT) and controlled by a function generator (33220A; Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA). The power intensity of light emitted from the optrode was calibrated to about 9 mW, which was consistent with the power intensity used in the behavioral assays. To identify ChR2-labeled cells, light pulses of 15 ms were delivered at 0.1 Hz at the recording sites approximately every 5–10 μm throughout the dentate gyrus. After light responsive cells were detected, two types of light stimuli were tested: 15 ms light pulse every 10 s and a train of ten 15 ms light pulses at 20 Hz every 10 s. Unit activity was band-pass filtered (500 Hz-5 KHz) and acquired with an Axon Digidata 1440A acquisition system running Clampex 10.2 software. Data was analyzed with custom software written in Matlab. Following the recording, endogenous c-fos expression was induced by delivering two epochs of 3-min light stimulation (9 mW, 20 Hz, 15 ms) separated by three min to dentate gyrus, same as in behavioral experiments (see below). Mice were sacrificed and perfused 90 min later.

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were overdosed with Avertin and perfused transcardially with cold phosphate buffer saline (PBS), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were extracted from the skulls and kept in 4% PFA at 4 °C overnight. Fifty μm coronal slices were taken using a vibrotome and collected in cold PBS. For immunostaining, each slice was placed in PBST (PBS + 0.2% Triton X-100) with 5% normal goat serum for one hour and then incubated with primary antibody at 4 °C for 24 hours (rabbit anti-c-fos 1:5000, Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA; rabbit anti-GABA 1:5000, Abcam, Cambridge, MA; chicken anti-GFP 1:500, Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA). Slices then underwent three wash steps for 10 minutes each in PBST, followed by one hour incubation with secondary antibody (1:200 AlexaFlour488 anti-chicken, Invitrogen; 1:200 AlexFlour568 anti-rabbit, Invitrogen). Slices were then incubated for 15 minutes with 4', 6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI; 1:10,000) and underwent three more wash steps of 10 min each in PBST, followed by mounting and coverslipping on microscope slides.

Cell counting

To characterize the expression timeline of ChR2-EYFP and EYFP, the number of EYFP immunoreactive neurons in dentate gyrus were counted from six coronal slices (spaced 120 μm from each other) per mouse (n = 5 for ChR2 group, n = 3 for EYFP group). Coronal slices were taken from dorsal hippocampus centered on coordinates covered by our optical fiber implants (−1.94 mm to −2.46 mm AP; Supplementary Fig. 11). Confocal fluorescence images were acquired on a Leica TCS SP2 AOBS scanning laser microscope using a 20×/0.70 NA oil immersion objective. The image analysis module Visiomorph DP within VIS (Visiopharm, Copenhagen, Denmark) calculated the number of ChR2-EYFP-positive or EYFP-positive cells per section by thresholding EYFP immunoreactivity above background levels and using DAPI staining to distinguish between nuclei. The analysis module also permitted isolation of only ChR2-EYFP–positive and EYFP-positive neurons by setting size and fluorescence thresholds to filter out nuclear-localized c-fos-shEGFP-positive cells. The cell body layer of DG granule cells was outlined as a region of interest (ROI) according to the DAPI signal in each slice. A similar protocol was followed for c-fos–positive cell counts in DG and CA3, except a Cy3 filter was applied for the latter. For quantification comparisons, we used a one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey's multiple comparisons using an α = 0.05. Data was analyzed using Microsoft Excel with the Statplus plug-in and Prism (GraphPad Software).

To analyze the overlap between c-fos and ChR2-EYFP–expressing or EYFP-expressing cells, a z-stack method was used in conjunction with ImageJ30 to montage 10 optical stacks (1 μm each, step size 10 μm) taken under a 20×/0.70 NA oil immersive objective. Separate GFP and Cy3 filtered images were digitally combined to produce composite images. Equal cutoff thresholds were applied to all captures to remove background autoflouresence. All imaging and analyses were performed blind to the experimental conditions. To quantify the expression levels of ChR2-EYFP per cell, an experimenter blind to each condition used ImageJ to calculate the fluorescence intensity signal as integrated density for 10 randomly chosen DG cells per hippocampal slice (n = 3 slices per mouse, 5 mice per condition; Supplementary Fig. 6).

Behavior assays

All the behavior tests were conducted during the light cycle of the day. Four different contexts were used in the behavior assays. Context A was a 30 × 25 × 33 cm conditioning chamber within a room with black walls, black curtains, and dim lighting. The chamber had a white plastic floor and was scented with 0.25% benzaldehyde. Context B was a 29 × 25 × 22 cm conditioning chamber within a second room with white walls and bright lighting. The chamber had a gridded floor and a triangular roof, and was scented with 1% acetic acid. Context C was a 41 × 41 × 31 cm unscented open field arena within a third room with white walls and intermediate lighting. Context D was a 29 × 25 × 22 cm conditioning chamber in the same room as context C. It had a white acrylic glass floor and was unscented. The experimental groups (Exp, Exp-1day, and Exp-Bi) and EYFP control (EYFP) groups underwent exactly the same training protocol. During the habituation session, each mouse was introduced to context A daily for five days while on 40 mg/kg Dox food. Each day the mouse was loaded into the chamber and the optical fiber implant was connected to a 473 nm laser (MBL F473; Opto Engine, Midvale, UT) controlled by a function generator (33220A; Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA). The mouse was then allowed to explore the chamber for 12 min. The 12 min session was divided into four 3-min epochs, with the first and third epoch as the light-off epochs, and the second and fourth epochs as the light-on epochs. During the light-on epochs, the mouse received light stimulation (9 mW, 20 Hz, 15 ms) for the entire three min duration. At the end of the 12 min, the mouse was immediately detached from the laser and returned to its home cage. Following the fifth habituation session, the mouse was kept on regular food without Dox for one (Exp-1day) or two (Exp, Exp-Bi, and EYFP) days until the training session. On the training day the mouse received three training trials separated by three hours in home cage. For each training trial, the mouse was kept in the conditioning chamber in context B for 500 s. A tone (20 s, 75 dB, 2000 Hz) was turned on at 180 s, 260 s, 340 s, and 420 s, each of which co-terminated with a foot shock (2 s, 0.75 mA). After the third training trial, the mouse was returned to its home cage and placed on food containing 1 g/kg Dox over night to rapidly turn off any additional ChR2-EYFP or EYFP expression. The test session started the next day and the mouse was switched back to food containing 40 mg/kg Dox. The procedure for the 5-day test session was exactly the same as the habituation session in context A. The day after the last test session, the mouse was returned to the original context B and exposed to the chamber for 300 s for a retrieval session to assay contextual fear memory. The next day, the mouse was introduced to context D for cued fear memory retrieval. This session lasted for 780 s, with a tone (60 s, 75 dB, 2000 Hz) turned on at 180 s, 420 s and 660 s. The no shock (NS) group went through the same habituation, training, and test sessions as the Exp group, except that no foot shock was given during the training session. The open field-fear conditioned (OF-FC) group went through the same habituation sessions. After the fifth habituation session, Dox was removed from the mouse's diet for two days, followed by exposure to the open field arena in context C to allow for 10 minutes of active exploration. The mouse was subsequently returned to its home cage and placed on 1 g/kg Dox food over night. The following day, the mouse was fear conditioned in context B in the same manner as described above. Test sessions were administered across five days in context A on 40 mg/kg Dox food, and the OF-FC group also underwent context and tone probe trials after the five days of testing. The fear conditioned-open field (FC-OF) group went through the same habituation sessions. The day after the fifth habituation, the mouse was kept on 40 mg/kg Dox food and went through the fear conditioning procedure in context B as described above. The mouse was placed off Dox for two days after fear conditioning. Then the mouse was exposed to the open field arena in context C and allowed to freely explore for 10 min, after which the mouse was returned to home cage with 1 g/kg Dox food over night, followed by test sessions across five days in context A on 40 mg/kg Dox food. Freezing behavior for training, context, and tone probe trials was recorded with a digital camera and measured with FreezeFrame software (ActiMetrics, Wilmette, IL). Light stimulation during the habituation and test sessions interfered with the motion detection of the program, and thus the freezing of these sessions was manually scored. Two experimenters scored each video independently in a double-blinded manner. The over-all scores showed a < 3% difference between the two experimenters and for simplicity only one set of score from one experimenter was reported. The manual scoring and computer scoring of the same training videos gave similar freezing scores. For each group, within each session (habituation and test) and within each epoch (light-on and light-off), a one-way ANOVA with repeated measures followed by Tukey's multiple comparisons (α = 0.05) revealed no difference across five days (Supplementary Fig. 5). We therefore averaged the freezing level across five days for each mouse. A Two-way ANOVA with repeated measures followed by Tukey's multiple comparisons (α = 0.05) revealed that only the experimental groups (Exp, Exp-1day, and Exp-Bi) showed an increase in averaged freezing levels for light-on epochs of test sessions compared to light-off epochs of test sessions and light-on epochs of habituation sessions (Fig. 3a, d, and e).

Supplementary Material

1
2

Acknowledgements

We thank S. Huang, G. Lin, M. Ragion, and X. Zhou for help with the experiments, T. Ryan, A. Rivest, J. Young, R. Redondo, and G. Dragoi for comments and discussions on the manuscript, and all the members of the Tonegawa lab for their support. This work is supported by NIH grants R01-MH078821, P50-MH58880 to S.T. and RIKEN Brain Science Institute.

Footnotes

Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at www.nature.com/nature.

Author Contributions X.L., S.R., A.G. and S.T. contributed to the study design. X.L., S.R., P.T.P. contributed to the data collection and interpretation. X.L. cloned all constructs. X.L. and S.R. conducted the surgeries and the behavior experiments. S.R. conducted the expression timeline experiments. P.T.P. conducted the electrophysiology experiments. C.B.P. contributed to the setup of the electrophysiology apparatus and wrote the Matlab software to analyze the data. K.D. provided the original ChR2 construct. X.L., S.R. and S.T. wrote the paper. All authors discussed and commented on the manuscript.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Josselyn SA. Continuing the search for the engram: examining the mechanism of fear memories. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35:221–228. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Silva AJ, et al. Molecular and cellular approaches to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science. 2009;326:391–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1174519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Reijmers LG, Perkins BL, Matsuo N, Mayford M. Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory. Science. 2007;317:1230–1233. doi: 10.1126/science.1143839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Han JH, et al. Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science. 2007;316:457–460. doi: 10.1126/science.1139438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Han JH, et al. Selective erasure of a fear memory. Science. 2009;323:1492–1496. doi: 10.1126/science.1164139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zhou Y, et al. CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1438–1443. doi: 10.1038/nn.2405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Boyden ES, et al. Millisecond-timescale, genetically targeted optical control of neural activity. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1263–1268. doi: 10.1038/nn1525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tye KM, et al. Amygdala circuitry mediating reversible and bidirectional control of anxiety. Nature. 2011;471:358–362. doi: 10.1038/nature09820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Martin SJ, Morris RG. New life in an old idea: the synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis revisited. Hippocampus. 2002;12:609–636. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gerber B, Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M. An engram found? Evaluating the evidence from fruit flies. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:737–744. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lever C, et al. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature. 2002;416:90–94. doi: 10.1038/416090a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:274–285. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 1992;256:675–677. doi: 10.1126/science.1585183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ramamoorthi K, et al. Npas4 regulates a transcriptional program in CA3 required for contextual memory formation. Science. 2011;334:1669–1675. doi: 10.1126/science.1208049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Treves A, Rolls ET. Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus. 1994;4:374–391. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McHugh TJ, et al. Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science. 2007;317:94–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1140263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nakashiba T, et al. Young Dentate Granule Cells Mediate Pattern Separation whereas Old Granule Cells Facilitate to Pattern Completion. Cell. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Schmidt B, Marrone DF, Markus EJ. Disambiguating the similar: The dentate gyrus and pattern separation. Behav Brain Res. 2011;226:56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Chawla MK, et al. Sparse, environmentally selective expression of Arc RNA in the upper blade of the rodent fascia dentata by brief spatial experience. Hippocampus. 2005;15:579–586. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Satvat E, et al. Changes in task demands alter the pattern of zif268 expression in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7163–7167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0094-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kubik S, Miyashita T, Guzowski JF. Using immediate-early genes to map hippocampal subregional functions. Learn Mem. 2007;14:758–770. doi: 10.1101/lm.698107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shockett PE, Schatz DG. Diverse strategies for tetracycline-regulated inducible gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:5173–5176. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.11.5173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lee I, Kesner RP. Differential contributions of dorsal hippocampal subregions to memory acquisition and retrieval in contextual fear-conditioning. Hippocampus. 2004;14:301–310. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Goshen I, et al. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell. 2011;147:678–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Seidenbecher T, Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape HC. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science. 2003;301:846–850. doi: 10.1126/science.1085818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Schafe GE, Doyere V, LeDoux JE. Tracking the fear engram: the lateral amygdala is an essential locus of fear memory storage. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10010–10014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3307-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rogan MT, Staubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Paxinos G, Franklin K. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rasband WS. ImageJ. U. S. National Institutes of Health; Bethesda, Maryland, USA: 1997–2011. http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/ [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

RESOURCES