Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Apr 23.
Published in final edited form as: Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011 Oct;1235:18–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06200.x

Age differences in risky choice: a meta-analysis

Rui Mata 1, Anika K Josef 1, Gregory R Samanez-Larkin 2, Ralph Hertwig 1
PMCID: PMC3332530  NIHMSID: NIHMS334921  PMID: 22023565

Abstract

Does risk taking change as a function of age? We conducted a systematic literature search and found 29 comparisons between younger and older adults on behavioral tasks thought to measure risk taking (N =4,093). The reports relied on various tasks differing in several respects, such as the amount of learning required or the choice framing (gains vs. losses). The results suggest that age-related differences vary considerably as a function of task characteristics, in particular the learning requirements of the task. In decisions from experience, age-related differences in risk taking were a function of decreased learning performance: older adults were more risk seeking compared to younger adults when learning led to risk-avoidant behavior, but were more risk averse when learning led to risk-seeking behavior. In decisions from description, younger adults and older adults showed similar risk-taking behavior for the majority of the tasks, and there were no clear age-related differences as a function of gain/loss framing. We discuss limitations and strengths of past research and provide suggestions for future work on age-related differences in risk taking.

Keywords: aging, risk, meta-analysis, decisions from description, decisions from experience


The 21st century is likely to become the century of centenarians: most babies born since 2000 in countries with already long life expectancies, such as Germany and France, may live to be 100 years old.1 Rising life expectancy will likely lead to longer working lives and the need for people to make decisions that are fraught with uncertainty late in life. Risk and uncertainty pervades decisions about wealth and health across the life span, such as the decision to invest in stocks, bonds, or other investment vehicles, given the possibility of ever-faster cycles of booms and busts, or the decision about which of several medical treatment options to pursue (e.g., surgery, radiation, and chemotherapy), given their different chances of success and risks of severe side effects.2 In addition, aging is associated with significant motivational and cognitive changes that are likely to affect decisions in predictable ways, and thus research in this domain provides an important test bed concerning the role of these factors on decision making. Is there evidence that decision making under risk changes systematically with advancing age?

Reviews on aging and decision making abound, yet there has been no systematic attempt to quantify age-related differences in risky choice across tasks and studies.35 We aim to close this gap by applying meta-analytic methods to quantify adult age-related differences in risky choice. The review is organized as follows. First, we provide a brief overview of the theory and measurement of risky choice. Second, we present hypotheses about the link between aging and risky choice. Third, we report a meta-analytic summary of empirical studies on risky choice in younger and older adults. Finally, we discuss limitations of past studies and make recommendations for future work on the impact of aging on decisions under risk.

The economic and psychological definitions of risk

In economics, risk is usually defined as the variance or probability of possible outcomes.6 As Frank Knight, a founder of the Chicago School of Economics, has pointed out, one should distinguish between different types of “probability situations,” namely, a priori probabilities, statistical probabilities, and estimates.7 According to Knight, a priori probabilities refer to situations in which the probability of an outcome is known or can easily be assigned via mathematical calculation. In turn, statistical probabilities refer to situations in which the probabilities must be gauged empirically through experience with similar outcomes. Finally, estimates refer to situations of extreme uncertainty in which there is no basis to empirically derive probabilities, for example, because one is dealing with unique events. In such cases, it is not clear how to assess the likelihood of an outcome. In the remainder of this paper, we will be concerned with the distinction between a priori and statistical probabilities.

Recent work on the psychology of risky choice seems to echo the distinction between a priori and statistical probabilities by emphasizing the different psychological requirements of decisions from description and experience: many studies in decision research have assessed risky behavior on the basis of people’s choices between gambles or lotteries in which full information about probabilities and outcomes is provided—decisions from description. In contrast, decisions from experience provide no explicit information about probabilities, and individuals must rely on experience acquired through feedback. The evidence is mounting for the need to distinguish between these two types of decisions because of systematic differences between the two paradigms: when individuals make decisions from experience, rare events tend to have less impact than they deserve according to their objective probabilities; in turn, in decisions from description, people behave as if the rare events have more impact than they deserve.811 A number of factors may contribute to these differences, including the reliance on modest samples of experience, memory effects (e.g., recency), and sampling strategies used to acquire information in decisions from experience.11

The distinction between experience and description is mirrored in the panoply of tasks that have been used to measure risky behavior (see Table 1 for task descriptions).12,13 A number of tasks provide explicit information about outcome magnitudes, whether outcomes are positive (gains) or negative (losses), as well as their respective probabilities, but differ nevertheless in many other respects. For example, some tasks, like the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT), present people with unavoidable risk by forcing them to choose between two or more monetary gambles that differ in their variance.14 Other tasks demand a decision between a sure outcome and a risky gamble with a certain probability of gain or loss, thus allowing people to avoid risk altogether by choosing the safe outcome.15 In these tasks, risk aversion is reflected in preferring the less risky (e.g., variable) of the two risky prospects or the safe prospect over the risky one. To facilitate assessment of risk, probabilities are sometimes represented as frequencies in the form of cups or bowls, or the problems are presented in well-known formats, such as card games.16,17 Finally, learning paradigms rely on participants’ ability to learn the probabilities of outcomes from experience, and they differ in whether the riskier option is the more or less advantageous one. For example, some tasks, like the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT),18 require participants to learn to avoid the initially attractive risky options with negative expected value in favor of the initially less-attractive risky options with positive expected value, whereas others, such as the balloon analogue risk task (BART),19 favor learning to make riskier but potentially more lucrative choices.

Table 1.

Behavioral measures of risk included in the meta-analysis

Category Task Task Description N
Decisions from experience Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) Subjects choose one card per trial from four decks. Two of the decks are “advantageous” (positive expected value) and two decks are “disadvantageous” (negative expected value). Subjects learn about the decks’ characteristics from the gains and losses associated with each card. Risk taking is measured as the average number of selections from advantageous minus disadvantageous decks (see Supporting Information). 10
Behavioral Investment Allocation Strategy (BIAS) Subjects allocate their money between two stocks and one bond. The bond’s payoff stays constant over all trials. In each block of trials, there is a “good” stock leading to better outcomes than the “bad” stock and subjects can learn about the stocks’ characteristics over trials from the gains and losses associated with each investment. Risk seeking is measured as the number of stock choices that deviate from those of a rational actor with the same experience with the task. 2
Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) Subjects pump a balloon for as many pumps as they want without knowing when it will explode. Each pump increases the amount of money won but also the chance of explosion. The subject can stop pumping to save the money earned before the balloon explodes, but if the balloon explodes the accumulated money for that balloon is lost. Subjects learn about the probability of a balloon exploding over trials. Risk taking is measured as the number of pumps in unexploded balloons. 2
Decisions from description Sure thing vs. risky gamble Subjects have to decide between a sure amount and a risky gamble involving points or money, with decisions being made in the gain or loss domain. Tasks differ regarding whether probabilities are presented numerically or as bowls or cups. Risk taking is measured as the number of choices of the sure amount. 12
Blackjack Subjects are given a two-card hand and decide whether they want to take a third card. Each card is worth a number of points and the aim is to collect as many points as possible without exceeding a total sum of 21 points. Risk taking is measured as the proportion of trials in which participants take a third card. 4
Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) Subjects bet concerning the location of a token. A token is hidden behind one of six red or blue boxes varying in their ratio (5:1, 4:2, 3:3). On each trial, subjects bet where the token is and every choice is associated with a potential gain or loss. The higher probability color is associated with lower gains (losses) than lower probability color. Risk taking is measured as the proportion of trials in which participants choose the lower probability color. 2

Note: N, number of studies that used the respective task included in the meta-analysis.

Risky choice and aging

Economists, biologists, and anthropologists have theorized about how life cycle variables, such as reproductive potential and age, affect risky choice.2022 Overall, these theories propose that risk taking is a function of reproductive competition, which is usually more intense for younger men than women or older adults, thus suggesting that aging should be associated with reduced risk taking. Epidemiological data concerning sexual behavior and crime statistics suggest that risk taking peaks in young adulthood and declines with advancing age.20 In this paper, we assess the empirical evidence from behavioral studies to quantify age-related differences in risk-taking behavior. In addition, we help assess how specific cognitive processes or task requirements contribute to such differences. In other words, we aim to clarify some of the proximal mechanisms underlying age-related differences in risky choice.

Aging is associated with structural and neuromodulatory changes thought to underlie age-related decline in some aspects of attention, memory, learning, and cognitive control.23,24 Age-related decline in some basic cognitive abilities may lead to changes in risky choice in different ways. First, decline in some aspects of cognitive control, such as working memory, may affect strategy selection and application. Older adults seem to rely on simpler decision strategies that require less information search and/or integration of information.2527 Additionally, older adults make more errors when integrating information relative to younger adults, at least when many pieces of information must be combined.2729 In addition, deficits in reward learning can lead to difficulties in learning associations between objects and their payoffs and ultimately adapting decision behavior to specific task structures.27,3032 In contrast, age differences in both behavior and neural activity largely disappear when comparing decision tasks without a strong learning component.33 Learning deficits seem particularly likely to affect decisions from experience in which decision makers must learn about options from feedback over time, and can thus contribute to different patterns of age-related differences in decisions from experience versus description (see also a related review by Eppinger, Hammerer, and Li in this volume71).

Aging is also associated with motivational and affective changes,34,35 and both emotional and motivational states have been shown to affect risky choice.36,37 Motivational theories suggest that aging may be associated with increased focus on emotional regulation, which can lead to biases in information processing.38 For example, younger and older adults have been shown to differ in the anticipation of losses (but not gains) in simple decision tasks, with older adults showing less responsiveness to losses relative to younger adults.39 However, there is also some evidence that older adults are more likely to work toward preventing losses relative to younger adults, who tend to strive for gains.40 Age-related differences in anticipation or desired prevention of losses are thus also a potential mechanism leading to developmental differences in risky choice.

A meta-analysis of adult age differences in risky choice

The goal of the present work was to summarize extant research findings on age-related differences in risky choice as well as to test the hypothesis that the differences vary as a function of the nature of the task. Specifically, we aimed to test the expectation that age-related differences in risky choice are to a large extent a function of the learning requirements of a task. The underlying rationale is that, relative to decisions from description, decisions from experience are more likely to tax learning and memory components that are significantly affected by age-related cognitive decline. In addition, we tested the hypothesis that the domain (gains vs. losses) contributes to age-related differences in risky choice.

Our analysis followed two main steps. First, we conducted a systematic literature search for original empirical reports that compared risky choice of younger and older adults. We focused particularly on studies using a behavioral measure of risk and obtained 29 age comparisons, the majority of which focused on decisions concerning monetary rewards. Second, we used meta-analytic methods to summarize the differences between age groups. They provide an objective, quantitative, and transparent way to synthesize research and, therefore, help to go beyond qualitative reviews when summarizing any effect of interest.41

Method

Literature search

We used the following methods to locate articles dealing with the topic of age-related differences in risky choice:

  1. a computerized literature search using PsycINFO, EBSCO, and Google with a combination of keywords relating to risk, including the names of specific behavioral measures of risk mentioned in literature reviews35,12,13 (e.g., the game of dice task, the CGT, cups task, the probability-associated gambling task, blackjack, BART, the Devil’s task, the risky gains task, IGT, the behavioral investment allocation strategy, risk, risk taking, risky choice, or gamble), and keywords related to age-related differences and aging (aging, older adults, elderly, or age differences); the literature search was limited to articles published in the period from 1900 to January 31, 2011;

  2. a computerized search in PsycINFO and EBSCO for articles citing the original paper that reported any of the risk measures mentioned above;

  3. a search for papers cited in reviews on aging and decision making;35 and

  4. a check of the references of the articles found through the searches above.

Inclusion criteria

We limited our analysis to those studies that met the following criteria (see Supporting Information for additional details and a list of excluded papers):

  1. studies that reported data concerning at least one age group comparison between a group of younger (ca. 18–35 years) and older (ca. 65–85 years) adults, or this comparison was obtainable from the authors;

  2. studies that reported results on a behavioral measure of risk, as opposed to self-report questionnaires or individuals’ reports of personality traits related to risk taking;12 the rationale was that behavioral measures would allow us to assess age-related differences in risky behavior rather than people’s descriptive or normative beliefs about age-related differences concerning risky behavior; and

  3. studies that reported a measure of risky choice for each age group (mean, standard deviation) in numerical or graphical format, a test statistic that could be used to compute an effect size corresponding to age-related differences in risky behavior, or alternatively, this information was obtainable from the authors.

Tables 1 and 2 give an overview of the tasks and articles that investigate age-related differences in risky choice that are included in this meta-analysis.1517,32,4264

Table 2.

Studies included in the meta-analysis

Study Task(s) N
Mean (SD) or range of age
Mean (SD) of education
Older Younger Older Younger Older Younger
Ashman, Dror, Houlette & Levy42 Blackjack 16 16 75.0 (6.8) 25.0 (4.4)
Baena, Allen, Kaut & Hall43 IGT 39 39 60–90 18–35
Bruine de Bruin, Parker & Fischhoff29 Sure/risky 56 63 76.7 (5.8) 24.9 (5.6)
Deakin, Aitken, Robbins & Sahakian44 CGT 45 41 53–79 17–27
Denburg, Tranel & Bechara45 IGT 40 40 56–85 26–55
Denburg et al.46 IGT 79 73 48.6 (11.9) 74.0 (5.6) 15.8 (2.6) 15.8 (2.0)
Dror, Katona & Mungur16 Blackjack 18 18 59–91 18–22
Fein, McGillivray & Finn47 IGT 52 112 73.3 (7.4) 37.8 (10.7) 16.1 (2.1) 16.4 (1.8)
Henninger, Madden & Huettel48 IGT, CGT, BART 54 58 70.7 (4.4) 23.4 (3.0) 15.9 (2.7) 15.0 (1.9)
Holliday49 Sure/risky 24 24 69.4 (3.8) 24.2 (2.3) 12.6 (2.3) 14.9 (2.3)
Isella, Mapelli, Morielli, Franceschi & Ildebrando50 IGT 40 40 65.4 (8.6) 27.9 (4.7) 8.7 (3.6) 13.1 (2.1)
Kim, Goldstein, Hasher & Zacks51 Sure/risky 186 186 58–78 17–28
Lamar & Resnick52 IGT 20 23 69.1 (5.0) 28.4 (5.9) 15.5 (3.4) 15.7 (2.6)
Lauriola & Levin53 Sure/risky 23 26 61–80 21–40
Lee, Leung, Fox, Gao & Chan54 Sure/risky 9 12 65.2 (4.2) 29.2 (6.2) 16.2 (1.2) 17.2 (1.8)
MacPherson, Phillips & Della Sala55 IGT 30 30 69.9 (5.5) 28.8 (6.0) 12.4 (3.2) 14.0 (3.2)
Mayhorn, Fisk & Whittle56 Sure/risky 58 58 70.3 (4.8) 20.3 (3.2)
Mikels & Reed15 Sure/risky 22 22 71.6 (4.5) 19.8 (1.2) 15.8 (3.5) 14.6 (1.2)
Rafaely, Dror & Remington (Experiment 1)57 Blackjack 45 42 70.5 (5.5) 20.9 (3.2) 13.3 (2.5) 14.6 (1.4)
Rafaely, Dror & Remington (Experiment 2)57 Blackjack 53 50 71.0 (5.2) 21.0 (4.4)
Rolison, Hanoch & Wood58 BART 44 40 76.6 (5.9) 19.3 (1.8) 16.5 (–) 13.4 (–)
Rönnlund, Karlsson, Laggnäs & Lindström 59 Sure/risky 192 192 69.1 (7.4) 23.9 (3.5) 9.9 (3.9) 14.4 (2.2)
Samanez-Larkin, Kuhnen, Yoo & Knutson32 BIAS 42 35 72.5 (5.0) 25.7 (4.4) 16.1 (2.6) 16.2 (2.1)
Samanez-Larkin, Wagner & Knutson60 BIAS 59 49 70.6 (4.3) 27.3 (4.6)
Sproten, Diener, Fiebach & Schwieren61 Sure/risky 24 51 68.0 (7.3) 25.0 (3.5) 12.2 (1.4) 12.6 (1.3)
Watabene & Shibutani62 Sure/risky 168 661 72.5 (4.1) 44.8 (11.5)
Weller, Levin & Denburg17 Sure/risky 61 358 65–85 18–22
Wood, Busemeyer, Koling, Cox & Davis63 IGT 63 87 77.0 (5.2) 23.0 (5.9) 14.7 (2.3) 14.2 (1.2)
Zamarian, Sinz, Bonatti, Gamboz & Delazer64 IGT, Sure/risky 52 33 69.3 (7.0) 36.1 (13.7) 10.4 (2.5) 10.7 (1.3)
Total N 1614 2479
Average agea 69 28
Average education 13 15

Note: IGT, Iowa Gambling Task; CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; BART, balloon analogue risk task; BIAS, behavioral investment allocation strategy.

a

We used the midpoint for studies reporting age ranges only.

Data analysis

We first calculated Cohen’s d for each study separately by computing the difference between the younger group mean minus the older group mean on the risk measure for each task and dividing this difference by the pooled standard deviation (see Supporting Information for details concerning the definition of risk used for each task and respective calculations). If means and standard deviations were not provided, we calculated d from available statistics. Although in some tasks higher scores represent risk-seeking behavior (e.g., BART), in other tasks higher scores represent risk-averse behavior (e.g., IGT). In order to standardize the meaning of the effects, we reversed the sign of effects for the former such that a positive effect size indicated more risk seeking of older adults relative to younger adults and a negative value indicated the opposite regularity. We then converted Cohen’s d into Hedge’s g to prevent biased estimation of effect sizes from studies with small sample sizes.41 A few studies contributed with more than one effect size because the same individuals performed different tasks, and in such cases we calculated effect sizes separately for each task. Similarly, some studies presented participants with separate gain and loss problems. In such cases, we calculated an overall effect size across the two domains, as well as separate effects sizes for gains and losses.

Results

We were interested in assessing whether age-related differences in decisions from experience are similar to those found for decisions from description. Figures 1 and 2 present forest plots with the effect sizes (Hedge’s g) and respective 95% confidence intervals (CI) for each study that investigated age-related differences in decisions from experience and description, respectively. A significance level of 0.05 can be inferred when zero is not contained within the 95% CI. The figures also show summary effect sizes for each task under a fixed-effects model. We also obtained summary effects based on random-effects models, but the pattern of results is very similar to those from fixed-effects analyses, so we report the simpler version here.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Decisions from experience. Forest plot for studies using decisions from experience paradigms. The positions of the squares on the x-axis indicate the effect size for each study; the bars indicate the 95% CI of the effect sizes, and the sizes of the squares are inversely proportional to the respective standard errors (i.e., larger squares indicate smaller standard errors).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Decisions from description. Forest plot for studies using decisions from description paradigms. The positions of the squares on the x-axis indicate the effect size for each study; the bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the effect sizes, and the sizes of the squares are inversely proportional to the respective standard errors (i.e., larger squares indicate smaller standard errors).

The meta-analysis suggests that there are significant age-related differences in risky choice in decisions from experience, but that the pattern of age differences varies as a function of the task (see Fig. 1). Older adults were more risk seeking relative to younger adults in both the IGT (0.28; 95% CI: 0.25, 0.31) and the BIAS (0.46; 95% CI: 0.39, 0.53), but older adults were more risk averse relative to younger adults in the BART (−0.38; 95% CI: −0.44, −0.31). When aggregating across tasks (but excluding a study in which the same participants completed multiple tasks),63 one could conclude that older adults are more risk seeking relative to younger adults (0.28; 95% CI: 0.25, 0.31).

Concerning decisions from description (see Fig. 2), younger adults were slightly more risk seeking relative to older adults in two paradigms. The effects were negative but quite small for the Sure/Risky gamble tasks (−0.03; 95% CI: −0.05, −0.001) and Blackjack (−0.07; 95% CI: −0.11, −0.03); in turn, the effect for the CGT was somewhat larger but suggests that older adults were more risk seeking relative to younger adults (0.47; 95% CI: 0.40, 0.54). Overall, aggregating across studies in decisions from description suggests no systematic age-related differences (−0.001; 95% CI: −0.02, 0.02).

We also calculated an overall summary effect size that included all studies involving decisions from description and experience (excluding studies in which the same participants completed multiple tasks48,63) and as expected from the heterogeneous pattern of results, we found a negligible effect of age (0.07; 95% CI: 0.05, 0.09). Overall, these findings suggest that pooling across description and experience categories as well as tasks can obscure significant effects of age on risky choice. We discuss the heterogeneity of results as a function of task characteristics in the discussion.

We also aimed to assess whether the domain (gains vs. losses) had an impact on age-related differences in risky choice. Figure 3 presents the effect sizes (Hedge’s g) and respective 95% CI for studies for which it was possible to compute effects separately for gain and loss domains. All studies involve a choice between a sure option and a risky gamble. As can be seen in Figure 3, the results show no effect of age regarding risky choice in either the gain domain (−0.05; 95% CI: −0.08, −0.03) or the loss domain (−0.05; 95% CI: −0.08, −0.02). In sum, existing studies suggest that younger and older adults show similar risk behavior in decisions from description regardless of gain/loss domain (a related discussion of the impact of gain versus loss frames appears in the review by Strough, Karns, and Schlosnagle in this volume).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Decisions from description: gains vs. losses. Forest plot for studies using decisions from description and that distinguish between gain and loss domains. The positions of the squares on the x-axis indicate the effect size for each study; the bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the effect sizes, and the sizes of the squares are inversely proportional to the respective standard errors (i.e., larger squares indicate smaller standard errors).

Discussion

We conducted a systematic literature review and applied meta-analytic methods to quantify age-related differences in risky choice. We considered both tasks involving decisions from experience and decisions from description. In tasks involving decisions from experience, information about risk is not explicitly provided and participants must learn about the likelihood of outcomes across a number of trials. In decisions from description, the risk associated with each option is explicitly presented to participants. The results show that the pattern of age-related differences varies considerably as a function of the task used. In two tasks involving decisions from experience, the IGT and the BIAS, older adults were more risk seeking relative to younger adults. However, in another learning task, the BART, older adults proved to be more risk averse relative to younger adults. How can we reconcile these apparently contradictory findings within the decisions from experience category? In our view, the key to this riddle is that the IGT, BART, and BIAS differ systematically in their payoff structure. In the IGT, the suboptimal course of action is tantamount to choosing the negative expected value options: learning thus means to avoid taking cards from an initially attractive but ultimately disadvantageous deck of cards in favor of the more advantageous decks, particularly a low-risk (variance) deck.63 This requires reversal learning, which may be impaired with aging.65 In contrast, in the BART, participants’ initial impulse is to pump substantially less than would be optimal (and thereby avoid the loss of the accumulated income): learning in the BART thus usually leads to taking more risk (i.e., more pumping on each balloon) and a more profitable payoff.19 Clearly, there are likely to be various processes or traits implicated in age differences in BART, including a focus on immediate reward (points accrued), insensitivity to loss (balloons exploded), or a combination of the two. But regardless of which processes cause cautious pumping, learning deficits will play out differently in the BART and the IGT task, respectively. In the IGT, impaired learning purportedly results in fewer choices of the less risky but positive expected value deck and, thus, overall more risk-seeking behavior. In the BART, impaired learning results in fewer pumps and thus overall more risk-averse behavior. Even more complex dynamics are required to maximize earnings in the BIAS task. Participants learn to initially prefer the certain option and then shift toward the best of the two risky options.32 In the BIAS, a learning deficit may lead to more risk-seeking throughout the task. Consequently, the seemingly conflicting pattern of age-related differences within decisions from experience is consistent with an age-related decline in learning abilities.

The results from the decisions from description suggest that younger and older adults show similar risk-taking behavior when learning components are excluded from task demands, at least for two out of the three tasks we considered, namely those tasks involving a choice between a gamble and a safe amount or a task modeled on the Blackjack card game. Our analysis revealed significant age differences in the CGT. One explanation for this pattern of results is the strict trade-off involved between probability and outcome in the CGT task. A risky choice in the CGT corresponds to choosing a low probability outcome that leads to a high reward, while the safe (high probability) choice leads to a lower reward. This trade-off may be particularly challenging for older adults if they use simple strategies that focus on the highest payoff (e.g., maximax) and ignore or underweight probability magnitudes.66 The safe/risky and blackjack paradigms do not involve such a strict trade-off and therefore a focus on reward magnitude may not lead to strong age differences.

In sum, our findings do not match expectations from life-history theories,16,44,61 which predict general differences in risk-taking behavior with increased age. Instead, our results emphasize the importance of understanding how different task characteristics engender age-related differences in risky choice. Our results thus present a starting point for future work examining the conditions that foster competent decision making by the elderly.

Limitations and recommendations for future work

Our work has a number of limitations due to the current state of the field. First, we based our analyses on a relatively small number of papers and tasks, which may provide an incomplete or, even worse, a biased view of age-related differences in risky choice. Second, life-history theories suggest that the pattern of age-related differences in risky behavior change considerably from young adulthood to middle age and less so afterwards.20 Unfortunately, few studies have tested participants across the full adult age range, which prevented us from studying the link between age and risky behavior across the complete adult development continuum. However, the design that most studies have adopted, namely, contrasting a sample of young adults with a sample of older adults, should boost the chances of finding differences in risky behavior with age (to the extent they exist), because according to life-history theories, risk taking should differ most between younger and older adults. Third, the studies analyzed cannot assess the role of cohort effects in risk-taking behavior, yet historical trends could prove a powerful determinant of financial behavior.67 Finally, a further limitation concerns our inability to gauge age-related differences as a function of theoretically meaningful but unavailable manipulations, such as reward or probability magnitudes. For example, the well-known fourfold pattern of risky choice suggests that both domain (gain vs. loss) and probability magnitudes determine choice.68 However, the studies we analyzed did not systematically distinguish between low- and high-probability outcomes, thus ruling out an analysis that considers both domain (gain/loss) and probability magnitude.

Another limitation of the literature to date, and by extension of our review, is that it has rarely quantified age-related differences in decision making that may be related to the accumulation of life experience. It is important to keep in mind that the decisions from experience tasks described here require relatively rapid learning from a limited amount of feedback in a novel environment. This experience-based learning should be distinguished from the learning that occurs over the lifetime and accumulates. It is possible that this form of decision making based on learning from limited experience indexes a more fluid component of cognition, whereas decisions that are made based on accumulated life experience may index a more crystallized component of cognition.69 We hope that future studies will more systematically examine how both of these experience measures may differentially contribute to risky decision making.

Still another limitation of this review is its almost exclusive focus on more traditional incentive-based tasks (where participants attempt to maximize points or monetary earnings). Age-related differences in the motivational value of money may have contributed to poorer learning from feedback. Consequently, it would be important to assess whether a similar pattern of results would emerge in other domains for which the objective is not the maximization of money but of some other dimensions equally treasured by young and old people (e.g., social relationships, sexual behavior, recreational activities). Unfortunately, there are currently far fewer studies that focus on these types of risky choice, and what does exist does not fit well within the taxonomy of tasks that we adopted from the Decision Making Individual Differences Inventory.12 As this area of literature continues to expand, we will be increasingly able to examine the domain specificity or generality of age-related differences in risky choice.

Our review also identified some general limitations of previous research on the topic of risky choice and aging. First, we found that many of the studies were underpowered. Specifically, we calculated the power of each study given its sample size and an effect of the magnitude indicated by our aggregation of studies separately for each task (α = 0.05), and found that more than 90% of studies were underpowered (power < 0.8). Meta-analyses can overcome power limitations by pooling across studies, but such a state of affairs is undesirable for individual studies because failure to detect significant results may be due to lack of power rather than negligible effects. For example, one paper reported no significant differences in risk taking between an older (n =18) and a younger (n =18) group, even though the age effect was of medium size (g = −0.45).16 Second, there is room for methodological improvement, for example, concerning the provision of clear incentives.70 Only 51% of the studies paid participants for participation, and an even smaller amount, 28%, paid participants in a performance-contingent fashion. We conducted exploratory analyses to assess the impact of performance-contingent payment on the size of age-related differences but found no evidence for a moderating role of hypothetical versus actual payment. Nevertheless, the use of performance-contingent payment in future research could help ensure that any age-related differences found are not due to unclear reward structures or varying effects of hypothetical payoffs.

Finally, this review points to voids in the literature. Only a minority of the studies identified in our search presented participants with different decision tasks and thus were able to compare performance on decisions from description and experience.48,63 Unfortunately, even these studies did not fully equate the structure or information provided in the different tasks. Consequently, it is hard to interpret the differences found. Future work with paradigms that equate decisions from experience and description would allow researchers more direct assessment and comparison of how search, integration, and decision components can lead to age-related differences in risky choice. Further, a main conclusion from our analysis is that task characteristics are pivotal in determining age-related difference in risky choice, and it remains to be studied which aspects of decisions from description and experience contribute to moderating age effects. For example, there may be different formats of decisions from experience that can increase or decrease task difficulty for those with declining capacity to learn.59

Conclusion

Increases in life expectancy pose challenges to modern societies that can be met partly by assuring that older adults can make sound health and financial decisions. We conducted a systematic literature search and quantified age-related differences in decisions under risk. In decisions involving learning from experience, age-related differences in risk taking were a function of decreased learning performance: older adults were more risk seeking compared to younger adults when learning resulted in risk-averse behavior, but proved more risk averse when learning resulted in risk-seeking behavior. In decisions from description, the majority of studies suggest negligible differences between younger and older adults’ risky behavior, but there are exceptions as a function of the experimental paradigm used. Overall, our findings suggest both that the pattern of age-related differences in risk taking is complex but systematic as a function of task demands and that more work is needed to identify the task properties (in the laboratory and in the wild) as well as the proximal mechanisms that give rise to age-related differences in risky choice.

Supplementary Material

Supplement

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Wändi Bruine de Bruin, Jerome R. Busemeyer, Hugh Chung, George Fein, Yaniv Hanoch, Debra E. Henninger, Valeria Isella, Stephanie Kovalchik, Marco Lauriola, Sarah E. MacPherson, Jonathan Rolison, Eldad Yechiam, and Laura Zamarian for providing information or data, Benjamin Scheibehenne for helpful discussions concerning meta-analysis, and Laura Wiles for editing the manuscript. This work was partly supported by a grant from the Stiftung Suzanne und Hans Biäsch zur Förderung der Angewandten Psychologie to R.M., a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation to R.H. (100014-126558), and G.R.S.L. was supported by US National Institute on Aging post-doctoral fellowship AG039131.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Supporting information

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.

Please note: Wiley-Blackwell is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.

References

  • 1.Vaupel JW. Biodemography of human ageing. Nature. 2010;464:536–542. doi: 10.1038/nature08984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.The Economist. The new tech bubble. The Economist Newspaper Limited; May 12, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Peters E, et al. Adult age differences in dual information processes: implications for the role of affective and deliberative processes in older adults’ decision making. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2:1–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mohr PN, Li SC, Heekeren HR. Neuroeconomics and aging: neuromodulation of economic decision making in old age. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:678–688. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hanoch Y, Wood S, Rice T. Bounded rationality, emotions and older adult decision making: not so fast and yet so frugal. Hum Dev. 2007;50:333–358. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Weber EU. Risk attitude and preference. Wiley Inter-discip Rev: Cogn Sci. 2010;1:79–88. doi: 10.1002/wcs.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Knight FH. Risk, Uncertainty and Profits. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1921. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hertwig R, et al. Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:534–539. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hau R, et al. The description-experience gap in risky choice: the role of sample size and experienced probabilities. J Behav Decis Mak. 2008;21:493–518. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hills TT, Hertwig R. Information search in decisions from experience: do our patterns of sampling foreshadow our decisions? Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1787–1792. doi: 10.1177/0956797610387443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hertwig R, Erev I. The description-experience gap in risky choice. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Appelt KC, et al. The Decision Making Individual Differences Inventory and guidelines for the study of individual differences in judgment and decision-making research. Judgment Decis Mak. 2011;6:252–262. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schonberg T, Fox CR, Poldrack RA. Mind the gap: bridging economic and naturalistic risk-taking with cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rogers RD, et al. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1999;20:9029–9038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-20-09029.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mikels JA, Reed AE. Monetary losses do not loom large in later life: age differences in the framing effect. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:457–460. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dror IE, Katona M, Mungur K. Age differences in decision making: to take a risk or not? Gerontology. 1998;44:67–71. doi: 10.1159/000021986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Weller JA, Levin IP, Denburg NL. Trajectory of risky decision making for potential gains and losses from ages 5 to 85. J Behav Decis Mak. 2010 In Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bechara A, et al. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lejuez CW, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) J Exp Psychol. 2001;8:75–84. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.8.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Daly M, Wilson M. Crime and conflict: homicide in evolutionary psychological perspective. Crime Justice. 1997;22:51–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Low BS. Why Sex Matters: A Darwinian Look at Human Behavior. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rogers AR. Evolution of time preference by natural-selection. Am Econ Rev. 1994;84:460–481. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bäckman L, et al. Age-related cognitive deficits mediated by changes in the striatal dopamine system. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:635–637. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.4.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Volkow ND, et al. Association between decline in brain dopamine activity with age and cognitive and motor impairment in healthy individuals. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:344–349. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.3.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mata R, Nunes L. When less is enough: cognitive aging, information search, and decision quality in consumer choice. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:289–298. doi: 10.1037/a0017927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mata R, Schooler LJ, Rieskamp J. The aging decision maker: cognitive aging and the adaptive selection of decision strategies. Psychol Aging. 2007;22:796–810. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mata R, von Helversen B, Rieskamp J. Learning to choose: cognitive aging and strategy selection learning in decision making. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:299–309. doi: 10.1037/a0018923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bruine de Bruin W, Parker AM, Fischhoff B. Explaining adult age differences in decision making competence. J Behav Decis Mak. 2010;24:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bruine de Bruin W, Parker AM, Fischoff B. Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;92:938–956. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chasseigne G, et al. Aging and probabilistic learning in single- and multiple-cue tasks. Exp Aging Res. 2004;30:23–45. doi: 10.1080/03610730490251469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mell T, et al. Altered function of ventral striatum during reward-based decision making in old age. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:1–10. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.034.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Samanez-Larkin GR, et al. Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates age-related suboptimal financial risk taking. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1426–1434. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4902-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hosseini SMH, et al. Aging and decision making under uncertainty: behavioral and neural evidence for the preservation of decision making in the absence of learning in old age. Neuro Image. 2010;52:1514–1520. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Blanchard-Fields F. Everyday problem solving and emotion: an adult developmental perspective. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16:26–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Carstensen LL. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science. 2006;312:1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lerner JS, Keltner D. Fear, anger, and risk. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81:146–159. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Scholer AA, et al. When risk seeking becomes a motivational necessity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99:215–231. doi: 10.1037/a0019715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Samanez-Larkin GR, et al. Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nature Neurosci. 2007;10:787–791. doi: 10.1038/nn1894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ebner NC, Freund AM, Baltes PB. Developmental changes in personal goal orientation from young to late adulthood: from striving forgains to maintenance and prevention of losses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21:664–678. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Borenstein M, Hedges LV. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; Chichester, West Sussex: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ashman O, et al. Preserved risk-taking skills in old age. North Am J Psychol. 2003;5:397–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Baena E, et al. On age differences in prefrontal function: the importance of emotional/cognitive integration. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:319–333. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Deakin J, et al. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:590–598. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Denburg N, Tranel D, Bechara A. The ability to decideadvantageously declines prematurely in some normal older persons. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1099–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Denburg N, et al. Poor decision making among older adults is related to elevated levels of neuroticism. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:64–172. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9094-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fein G, McGillivray S, Finn P. Older adults make less advantageous decisions than younger adults: cognitive and psychological correlates. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13:480–489. doi: 10.1017/S135561770707052X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Henninger DE, Madden DJ, Huettel SA. Processing speed and memory mediate age-related differences in decision making. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:262–270. doi: 10.1037/a0019096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Holliday SG. Risky-choice behavior: a life-span analysis. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1988;27:25–33. doi: 10.2190/139A-98AX-EL11-D120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Isella V, et al. Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability. Behav Neurol. 2008;19:59–63. doi: 10.1155/2008/893727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kim S, et al. Framing effects in younger and older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005;4:215–218. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.4.p215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lamar M, Resnick SM. Aging and prefrontal functions: dissociating orbitofrontal and dorsolateral abilities. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:553–558. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lauriola M, Levin IP. Personality traits and risky decision-making in a controlled task: an Exploratory study. Pers Individ Differ. 2001;31:215–226. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lee TMC, et al. Age-related differences in neutral activities during risk taking revealed by functional MRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:7–15. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.MacPherson SE, Phillips LH, Della Sala S. Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. 2001;17:598–609. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Mayhorn CB, Fisk AD, Whittle JD. Decisions, decisions: analysis of age, cohort, and time of testing on framing of risky decision options. Hum Factors. 2002;44:515–521. doi: 10.1518/0018720024496935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rafaely V, I, Dror E, Remington B. Information selectivity in decision making by younger and older adults. Int J Psychol. 2006;41:117–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rolison JJ, Hanoch Y, Wood S. Risky decision making in younger and older adults: the role of learning. Psychol Aging. 2011 Jul 18; doi: 10.1037/a0024689. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rönnlund M, et al. Risky decision making across three arenas of choice: are younger and older adults differently susceptible to framing effects? J Gen Psychol. 2005;132:81–92. doi: 10.3200/GENP.132.1.81-93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Samanez-Larkin GR, Wagner AD, Knutson B. Expected value information improves financial risk taking across the adult life span. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6:207–217. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sproten A, et al. Working Papers. University of Heidelberg; 2010. Aging and Decision Making: How Aging Affects Decisions Under Uncertainty. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Watabene S, Shibutani H. Aging and decision making: differences in susceptibility to the risky-choice framing effect between older and younger adults in Japan. Japan Psychol Res. 2010;52:163–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wood S, et al. Older adults as adaptive decision makers: evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:220–225. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Zamarian L, et al. Normal aging affects decisions under ambiguity, but not decisions under risk. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:645–657. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kovalchik S, Allman J. Measuring reversal learning: introducing the Variable Iowa Gambling Task in a study of young and old normals. Cogn Emotion. 2006;20:714–728. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Brandstätter E, Gigerenzer G, Hertwig R. The priority heuristic: making choices without trade-offs. Psychol Rev. 2006;113:409–432. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Malmendier U, Nagel S. Depression babies: do macroeconomic experiences affect risk taking? Quart J Econ. 2011;126:373–416. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tversky A, Kahneman D. Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. J Risk Uncertainty. 1992;5:297–323. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Agarwal S, et al. The age of reason: financial decisions over the life cycle and implications for regulation. Brookings Pap Econ Act. 2009;42:51–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Hertwig R, Ortmann A. Experimental practices in economics: a methodological challenge for psychologists? Behav Brain Sci. 2001;24:383–451. doi: 10.1037/e683322011-032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Eppinger B, Hämmerer D, Li SC. Neuromodulation of reward-based learning and decision making in human aging. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2011;1235:1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06230.x. This volume. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement

RESOURCES