Skip to main content
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science logoLink to Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
. 2011 Dec 16;52(13):9534–9538. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8426

Object Frequency Characteristics of Visual Acuity

J Jason McAnany 1,, Kenneth R Alexander 1, Jennifer I Lim 1, Mahnaz Shahidi 1
PMCID: PMC3341120  PMID: 22110062

This study demonstrates that different object frequency information mediates visual acuity (VA) in individuals with different VA levels, which is in contrast to the assumption of scale invariance underlying the MAR scale. This scale dependence can complicate the interpretation of VA measurement.

Abstract

Purpose.

To examine the extent to which visual acuity (VA) for broadband optotypes is scale invariant by determining whether the same object frequencies mediate VA for individuals with different levels of VA.

Methods.

LogMAR (minimum angle of resolution) VA for briefly presented tumbling E's was measured in 10 visually normal individuals and in five patients with VA loss. The E's were either unblurred or blurred with Gaussian low-pass filters that had cutoff frequencies spanning a 1.2-log unit range. The data were fit with a standard equivalent intrinsic blur model to estimate each subject's unblurred VA (MAR0 in minutes of arc) and equivalent intrinsic blur (σint in minutes of arc). From these estimates, the high-frequency cutoff of the band of retinal frequencies (cpdcrit in cycles per degree) and object frequencies (cplcrit in cycles per letter) mediating VA were derived.

Results.

LogMAR0 was related linearly to log σint with a slope of 1.47, which is steeper than that predicted by scale invariance. Log cpdcrit was related linearly to logMAR0 by a slope of −0.64, which is shallower than that predicted by scale invariance. This lack of scale invariance is due to a linear relationship between log cplcrit and logMAR0 that had a slope of 0.36.

Conclusions.

The overall pattern of results is not consistent with the expectation of scale invariance underlying the MAR scale. Optotypes that conform to the expectations of scale invariance are needed to improve vision assessment and to provide equivalency of VA defined in terms of MAR and cpd.


The size of letter optotypes on standard visual acuity (VA) charts is typically specified as the logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR). A fundamental assumption of the MAR metric is that scale invariance holds for VA. That is, the MAR scale assumes that individuals use the same information to identify letters at the acuity limit, regardless of their VA. In particular, the MAR scale assumes that letter identification is based on the stroke width of the letter at all VA levels.

The stroke width of standard Sloan optotypes corresponds to an object frequency of 2.5 cycles per letter (cpl), given that there are five strokes in each letter and two strokes (one light bar and one dark bar) per cycle. If VA is based on stroke width, then the MAR value of a letter can readily be equated to retinal spatial frequency (cycles per degree or cpd) based on the angular subtense of the stroke width. For example, a letter with an MAR of 1 (0 logMAR or 20/20 Snellen equivalent) would correspond to a retinal spatial frequency of 30 cpd, because one cycle would subtend 2 arcmin.

However, evidence indicates that VA for letter optotypes, which are broadband in object frequency content, may not be based on object frequencies near 2.5 cpl. For example, object frequencies less than approximately 1.25 cpl were found to mediate VA in the normal periphery for a task that involved discrimination between letter pairs.1 A study of the Fourier components of the Landolt C and tumbling E concluded that, whereas VA for the tumbling E could be based on object frequencies near 2.5 cpl, VA for the Landolt C was most likely based on object frequencies in the range of 1.3 cpl.2 This proposal for the Landolt C was verified experimentally using band-limited targets in a study of the effect of crowding on VA.3 VA measurements using low- and high-pass–filtered tumbling E's showed that the VA for this target in the normal visual field periphery is based on object frequencies lower than the 2.5 cpl proposed previously.4

The finding that VA for broadband targets may be governed by object frequencies lower than 2.5 cpl would not be an issue if scale invariance holds for VA. That is, if individuals with different levels of VA all use the same low object frequencies to identify letters at the acuity limit, then the equivalent retinal frequency would simply be a scalar transform of the nominal retinal frequency that is derived from stroke width. However, there is evidence from studies of contrast sensitivity that the object frequencies used for letter identification vary with letter size.57 Specifically, there is a linear relationship between log object frequency and log angular subtense of the letter that has a slope of approximately ⅓, such that contrast sensitivity is mediated by low object frequencies for letters of small angular subtense but higher object frequencies for larger letters. Given that individuals with decreased VA require larger-than-normal letters at the acuity limit, it is possible that they may use higher object frequencies than normally sighted individuals, and scale invariance would not hold for VA.

The present study evaluated the extent to which VA is scale invariant by determining whether the same object frequencies mediate VA for individuals who have different VAs. A novel approach was used, in which the high-frequency cutoff of the object frequency region mediating VA was derived by using an equivalent intrinsic blur paradigm. With this approach, the target is successively blurred with Gaussian low-pass filters until VA is affected, under the assumption that, if the removal of specific high object frequencies impairs VA, then those frequencies must be necessary for the task. Gaussian filters were used in the present study because they are the basis for models of equivalent intrinsic blur810 and have also been used in studies of the object frequencies mediating contrast sensitivity for letter optotypes.11,12 The amount of Gaussian blur necessary to reduce VA by a factor of √2 was used to estimate the high-frequency cutoff of the object frequencies mediating VA. This degree of Gaussian blur has been termed “equivalent intrinsic blur” and is assumed to be an estimate of the amount of blur within an individual's visual system.8,9

In the present study, the VA of both normally sighted individuals and patients with VA loss due to eye disease was measured with a tumbling E. The patients were included to broaden the range of VA. Equivalent intrinsic blur was measured in the spatial domain (in minutes of arc) and converted to the frequency domain (cpd) to estimate the high-frequency cutoff of the band of retinal frequencies mediating VA. The corresponding high-frequency cutoff of the band of object frequencies mediating VA was obtained by converting the cpd estimate to cpl to establish whether the same object frequencies mediate performance at the acuity limit for individuals with different VA levels.

Methods

Subjects

Ten normally sighted individuals and five patients diagnosed with diabetes mellitus (DM) participated in the study. Subjects with DM were recruited from the Retina Service at the University of Illinois at Chicago and had VA loss due to diabetic macular edema and/or vein occlusions. These patients were included in the study to broaden the range of VA, and comparison among the patients with DM was not undertaken. Table 1 provides the characteristics of the subjects, including sex, age, diagnosis, refraction, and chart acuity (Lighthouse Distance Acuity Chart; Lighthouse International, New York, NY). The chart acuity measurements were made through the best optical correction and through a 3.0-mm artificial pupil that was used to control the retinal illuminance and also to optimize the optical quality of the eye by minimizing the effects of higher order aberrations and diffraction. The study conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, and the experiments were approved by an institutional review board at the University of Illinois at Chicago. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject before testing.

Table 1.

Subject Characteristics

Subject No. Sex Age (y) Diagnosis Refraction Sphere (D) Refraction Cylinder (D × Angle) Chart VA (logMAR)
1 M 46 Normal −1.50 0.00 −0.08
2 F 48 Normal −1.00 +0.50 × 90° −0.08
3 M 53 Normal −7.75 +0.50 × 0° −0.01
4 F 53 Normal −7.00 +0.75 × 90° 0.03
5 F 54 Normal −5.75 +0.25 × 100° 0.06
6 F 56 Normal −0.50 0.00 −0.10
7 M 56 Normal 0.00 0.00 −0.07
8 F 57 Normal −3.25 +1.00 × 100° −0.01
9 M 58 Normal 0.00 0.00 −0.07
10 M 63 Normal −4.00 0.00 −0.16
11 M 44 CRVO; CSDME 0.00 0.00 0.66
12 F 58 PDR; CSDME 0.00 0.00 0.22
13 F 64 NPDR; CSDME +2.00 0.00 0.28
14 F 68 NPDR; CSDME −0.75 +0.25 × 100° 0.58
15 M 71 BRVO −0.50 0.00 0.34

CRVO, central retinal vein occlusion; CSDME, clinically significant diabetic macular edema; PDR, proliferative diabetic retinopathy; NPDR, non-proliferative diabetic retinopathy; BRVO, branch retinal vein occlusion.

Instrumentation and Stimuli

The instrumentation and stimuli have been described in detail elsewhere.10 Briefly, stimuli were generated by computer (Macintosh G4; Apple Computer, Cupertino, CA) with commercial software (MatLab; The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions.13 The stimuli were displayed on a 22-in. cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor (FE2111SB; NEC, Irving, TX) with a screen resolution of 1024 × 768 and an 85-Hz refresh rate, driven by a video card with 10-bit DAC resolution (ATI Radeon 9000 Pro; AMD, Sunnyvale, CA).

The stimuli consisted of unblurred and blurred tumbling E optotypes. The E's were constructed according to the principles of the Sloan font,14 such that the stroke width was one fifth of the overall optotype size and the three bars were of equal length. The stroke width ranged from 0.6 to 20 arcmin in 16 steps spaced approximately 0.1 log unit apart. The E at each size was blurred with a set of three 2-D Gaussian filters with standard deviations (σstim) of 2, 8, and 32 pixels, corresponding to 0.4, 1.6, and 6.4 arcmin at the test distance of 4.5 m. Stimuli were presented for approximately 60 ms (five video frames) in the center of an adapting field that subtended 3.4° horizontally and 2.6° vertically. The luminance of the adapting field was 106 cd/m2 and the luminance of the unblurred test stimulus was 1.4 cd/m2, yielding a Weber contrast of −99%. The blurred stimuli were presented without rescaling the contrast. The stimulus luminances were measured with a photometer (LS 110; Minolta Osaka, Japan), and the temporal characteristics of the display were confirmed with an oscilloscope and photocell.

Procedure

Before all measurements, the pupil of the tested eye was dilated with 2.5% phenylephrine hydrochloride drops. The subject's pupil was centered on the 3.0-mm artificial pupil using a two-dimensional, two-color alignicator.15 The subject's task was to judge the orientation of the tumbling E, which was randomly facing either to the right or up on each trial. A brief warning tone signaled the start of each stimulus presentation, and the subject verbally reported the orientation, which was recorded by the examiner. The subjects were given practice trials to become familiar with the task.

Threshold logMAR (logMARt) for each value of σstim (0, 0.4, 1.6, 6.4 arcmin) was determined using a two-alternative, forced-choice staircase procedure. An initial estimate of logMARt was obtained by presenting the optotype at a suprathreshold size and then decreasing the size by 0.1 log unit until an incorrect response was recorded. After this initial search, logMARt was determined using a two-down, one-up decision rule, which provides an estimate of the 71% correct point on a psychometric function.16 Each staircase continued until 10 reversals had occurred, and the mean of the last 6 reversals was taken as logMARt. The staircase length was typically 40 to 50 trials, which produced stable measurements (the SEM of the last eight reversals was typically less than 0.05 log unit). One staircase measurement of logMARt was obtained from each subject for each value of σstim. The conditions were presented in order of increasing σstim, but for a given staircase, E's of different sizes were convolved with a constant filter width (σstim) so that the degree of blur varied across trials within the staircase.

Analysis

LogMARt for each subject was plotted as a function of log σstim, and the data were fit with the log form of the following equation8:

graphic file with name z7g01311-2283-m01.jpg

where MAR0 represents VA in the absence of blur (i.e., σstim = 0), and σint (corresponding to equivalent intrinsic blur) is the value of σstim at which MAR = MAR0 · √2. This value of MAR was defined as MARcrit. MAR0 and σint were adjusted to minimize the mean squared error between the fitted function and the data.

Object frequency was derived from MARcrit and σint in two steps. First, the Gaussian function representing equivalent intrinsic blur was converted from the spatial domain to the frequency domain. In the frequency domain, the SD of the Gaussian function is a measure of the high-frequency cutoff of the band of retinal frequencies mediating VA (cpdcrit). The value of σint was converted to cpdcrit as follows:

graphic file with name z7g01311-2283-m02.jpg

Next, the high-frequency cutoff of the band of object frequencies mediating VA (cplcrit) was derived from MARcrit and cpdcri using the following relationship:

graphic file with name z7g01311-2283-m03.jpg

Results

Figure 1 presents logMARt as a function of log σstim for one representative normally sighted subject (subject 10). For reference, the right y-axis shows the corresponding Snellen equivalents of the logMARt values. The curve represents the least-squares best fit of equation 1 to the data (R2 values for the individual subjects ranged from 0.8 to 1.0). According to equation 1, logMARt is approximately constant for small values of log σstim, whereas logMARt increases linearly with a slope of 1 for substantially larger values of log σstim. In this figure, logMAR0 and log σint are indicated by the arrows. For reference, the value of logMARcrit is also indicated. For this subject, MAR0 and σint were approximately 0.7 and 1.1 arcmin, respectively.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Threshold logMAR as a function of log σstim for one representative normally sighted subject (subject 10). The right y-axis shows the Snellen equivalents of the logMARt values. Solid line represents least-squares best fit of equation 1 to the data. Arrows: log σint, logMARcrit, and logMAR0.

Figure 2 shows the relationship between logMAR0 and log σint for the 10 normally sighted subjects (circles) and five DM patients (squares). The right y-axis shows the corresponding Snellen equivalents of the logMAR0 values. The individual values of logMAR0 and log σint are listed in Table 2. The values of logMAR0 in this table correlated significantly with the chart acuity values given in Table 1 (r = 0.92, P < 0.01), demonstrating the validity of using logMAR0 as an index of VA.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

LogMAR0 as a function of log σint for the normally sighted subjects and patients with DM. The right y-axis shows the Snellen equivalents of the logMAR0 values and the top x-axis shows the linear values of σint. Solid line represents least-squares best-fit linear regression line.

Table 2.

Parameters Derived from Equation and Estimates of Retinal and Object Frequency for Each Subject

Subject No. LogMAR0 σint (arcmin) Retinal Frequency (cpdcrit) Object Frequency (cplcrit)
1 −0.13 0.87 10.96 0.95
2 −0.10 1.02 9.33 0.89
3 −0.01 0.89 10.72 1.23
4 0.04 1.35 7.08 0.91
5 0.14 1.38 6.92 1.10
6 0.10 1.45 6.61 0.95
7 −0.10 1.02 9.33 0.87
8 0.04 1.23 7.76 1.02
9 −0.11 0.98 9.77 0.89
10 −0.14 1.10 8.71 0.72
11 0.61 3.16 3.02 1.45
12 0.48 2.09 4.57 1.62
13 0.37 1.95 4.90 1.35
14 0.57 2.75 3.47 1.55
15 0.64 3.02 3.16 1.62

As expected from previous studies,8,10,17 logMAR0 was related linearly to log σint, such that subjects with lower values of logMAR0 (better VA) had lower equivalent intrinsic blur. The data of the normally sighted subjects alone were best fit with a linear regression line with a slope of 1.0, which is in agreement with previous studies.8,10,17 However, when the patients with VA loss are included, the best-fit regression line (Fig. 2, solid line) has a slope of 1.47 (R2 = 0.94), which is significantly steeper than 1.0 (t = 5.1, P < 0.01).

The relationship between log cpdcrit and logMAR0 is shown in Figure 3, with the individual values of cpdcrit given in Table 2. The dashed line in Figure 3 has a slope of −1.0 and describes the relationship between retinal spatial frequency and VA, assuming that scale invariance holds and that a logMAR of 0 (20/20 Snellen VA) is equivalent to a retinal spatial frequency of 30 cpd. It is apparent from Figure 3 that there was a linear relationship between log cpdcrit and logMAR0, but the slope of the best-fit regression line (solid line, R2 = 0.94) was −0.64. This slope is significantly shallower than the slope of −1.0 (t = −8.7, P < 0.01) predicted by scale invariance (dashed line).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Log retinal frequency (cpdcrit) as a function of logMAR0 for the normally sighted subjects and patients with DM. The right y-axis shows the linear values of cpdcrit and the top x-axis shows the Snellen equivalents of the logMAR0 values. Solid line represents least-squares best-fit linear regression line; dashed line represents scale invariance, with the assumption that 30 cpd is equivalent to 0 logMAR.

The basis for the relatively shallow slope of the function shown in Figure 3 can be appreciated by inspection of Figure 4, which plots log cplcrit as a function of logMAR0 for the normally sighted subjects and DM patients. The individual values of cplcrit are given in Table 2. The solid line in Figure 4 represents the best-fit regression line (R2 = 0.92), which has a slope of 0.36. This slope is significantly steeper than a slope of 0 (t = 8.7, P < 0.01). This non-0 slope indicates a dependence of object frequency on VA, such that observers with worse VA (higher logMAR0) used higher object frequencies than subjects with better VA. This lack of scale invariance accounts for the absence of a one-to-one relationship between log cpdcrit and logMAR0 (Fig. 3).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Log object frequency (cplcrit) as a function of logMAR0 for the normally sighted subjects and patients with DM. The right y-axis shows the linear values of object frequency and the top x-axis shows the Snellen equivalents of the logMAR0 values. Solid line represents least-squares best-fit linear regression line.

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated the extent to which the object frequency information mediating VA for broadband optotypes is scale invariant across individuals who have different VA levels. If the object frequency information mediating VA depends on the individual's VA level, then this would complicate the relationship between VA and retinal frequency. The potential difficulty of relating MAR and retinal frequency was noted previously in a study of the effects of optical blur on letter and grating acuity.18

In the present study, the high-frequency cutoff (cplcrit) of the band of object frequencies mediating VA for subjects with different VA levels was derived using an equivalent intrinsic blur paradigm. An estimate of equivalent intrinsic blur was obtained for each subject, and this estimate was used to derive the high-frequency cutoff of the band of retinal frequencies (cpdcrit) mediating VA. The value of cpdcrit was then converted to the corresponding object frequency (cplcrit). The results showed that the object frequency information mediating VA is not scale invariant. Subjects with worse VA (higher values of logMAR0) had higher values of cplcrit than subjects with better VA. Because of this lack of scale invariance, VA defined in terms of MAR and VA defined in terms of equivalent retinal frequency were not proportionally related by a slope of −1.0 (Fig. 3). For example, subjects with MAR values that differed by 1.0 log unit differed in equivalent retinal frequency by only 0.66 log units.

The value of cplcrit for our sample of subjects ranged from 0.7 to 1.6 (Fig. 4). These values are lower than those reported in a previous study that investigated the effect of low-pass and high-pass filtering on orientation judgments of the tumbling E in the normal visual field periphery.4 That study reported that object frequencies between approximately 1.25 and 2.25 cpl mediated performance for the tumbling E. However, a direct comparison of our results with those of this previous study is complicated by the fact that, whereas the previous investigators based their estimate of object frequency on the filter cutoffs required to affect VA, the values reported here are dependent on the point on the function relating logMARt to log σstim that is selected for analysis (Fig. 1). We chose to use the point at which MARt was elevated by √2 above MAR0, which corresponds to the standard measure of equivalent intrinsic blur. Selecting a lower point on the curve would result in a higher estimate of cplcrit, which would be more similar to the values reported previously.4 It is important to note, however, that altering the chosen point on the curve would not affect the non-0 slope of the line relating log cplcrit and logMAR0 (Fig. 4). Consequently, there is a lack of scale invariance for VA, regardless of the point that is selected as the basis for the derivation of critical object frequency.

The slope of the line relating log cplcrit and logMAR0 was approximately ⅓ (Fig. 4). A similar slope has been reported in previous studies of contrast sensitivity for broadband optotypes, including orientation judgments of the Sloan N (a two-alternative, forced-choice task similar to that of the present study), letter detection, letter discrimination, and letter identification tasks.57,11,12 The contrast sensitivity data of these previous studies together with the VA data of the present study suggest that an increase in object frequency with increasing target size is a general characteristic of the measurement of visual function with broadband optotypes. A linear relationship between object frequency and letter size with a slope of approximately one third was also observed in a study of contrast sensitivity in amblyopic subjects.19 This latter finding suggests that the lack of scale invariance for VA found in the present study would probably generalize to other patient populations beyond the DM patients studied here. However, additional work is needed to confirm this hypothesis.

In conclusion, the present results demonstrate that scale invariance cannot necessarily be assumed in VA measurements that use standard broadband optotypes. This lack of scale invariance complicates the interpretation of acuity measurements for individuals with different VA values. Scale invariance could be achieved by using band-limited optotypes, and there have been previous attempts to use band-limited optotypes in VA measurements. However, these targets typically have limitations. For example, VA has been measured with sine-wave grating targets,20,21 but these stimuli are typically unfamiliar to patients, and VA measurements made in the periphery with these stimuli can be affected by spurious resolution and aliasing.22 VA has also been measured with “vanishing optotypes” that have pseudo–high-pass spatial characteristics.23,24 However, untrained subjects, patients with central field loss, or patients with unsteady fixation may have trouble localizing these targets in space at sizes near the acuity limit, which would increase spatial uncertainty. Thus, further study is needed to identify optotypes that maintain the desirable characteristics of letters but conform to the expectations of scale invariance, which would provide a better assessment of VA.

Footnotes

Supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grants K99EY019510 (JM), R01EY008301 (KA), R01EY014275 (MS), and P30EY001792; the Department of Veterans Affairs (MS); the Cless Family Foundation Fund for Retina Research (JL), and Research to Prevent Blindness unrestricted departmental and Senior Scientific Investigator (MS) awards.

Disclosure: J.J. McAnany, None; K.R. Alexander, None; J.I. Lim, None; M. Shahidi, None

References

  • 1. Anderson RS, Thibos LN. The filtered Fourier difference spectrum predicts psychophysical letter discrimination in the peripheral retina. Spat Vis. 2004;17:5–15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bondarko VM, Danilova MV. What spatial frequency do we use to detect the orientation of a Landolt C? Vision Res. 1997;37:2153–2156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hess RF, Dakin SC, Kapoor N. The foveal ‘crowding’ effect: physics or physiology? Vision Res. 2000;40:365–370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Anderson RS, Thibos LN. Sampling limits and critical bandwidth for letter discrimination in peripheral vision. J Opt Soc Am. 1999;16:2334–2342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Alexander KR, Xie W, Derlacki DJ. Spatial-frequency characteristics of letter identification. J Opt Soc Am. 1994;11:2375–2382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Majaj NJ, Pelli DG, Kurshan P, Palomares M. The role of spatial-frequency channels in letter identification. Vision Res. 2002;42:1165–1184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Chung ST, Legge GE, Tjan BS. Spatial-frequency characteristics of letter identification in central and peripheral vision. Vision Res. 2002;42:2137–2152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Levi DM, Klein SA. Equivalent intrinsic blur in spatial vision. Vision Res. 1990;30:1971–1993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Watt RJ, Morgan MJ. Spatial filters and the localization of luminance changes in human vision. Vision Res. 1984;24:1387–1398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. McAnany JJ, Shahidi M, Applegate RA, Zelkha R, Alexander KR. Contributions of optical and non-optical blur to variation in visual acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:716–723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. McAnany JJ, Alexander KR. Spatial frequencies used in Landolt C orientation judgments: relation to inferred magnocellular and parvocellular pathways. Vision Res. 2008;48:2615–2624 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Alexander KR, McAnany JJ. Determinants of contrast sensitivity for the tumbling E and Landolt C. Optom Vis Sci. 2010;87:28–36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10:433–436 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. NAS-NRC Recommended and standard procedures for the clinical measurement and specification of visual acuity: report of working group 39. Adv Ophthalmol. 1980;41:103–148 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Thibos LN, Bradley A, Still DL, Zhang X, Howarth PA. Theory and measurement of ocular chromatic aberration. Vision Res. 1990;30:33–49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. García-Pérez MA. Forced-choice staircases with fixed step sizes: asymptotic and small-sample properties. Vision Res. 1998;38:1861–1881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Coppens JE, van den Berg TJ. A new source of variance in visual acuity. Vision Res. 2004;44:951–958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Thorn F, Schwartz F. Effects of dioptric blur on Snellen and grating acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 1990;67:3–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Chung ST, Levi DM, Legge GE, Tjan BS. Spatial-frequency properties of letter identification in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2002;42:1571–1581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Teller DY, McDonald MA, Preston K, Sebris SL, Dobson V. Assessment of visual acuity in infants and children: the acuity card procedure. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1986;28:779–789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Campbell FW, Green DG. Optical and retinal factors affecting visual resolution. J Physiol. 1965;181:576–593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Wang YZ, Bradley A, Thibos LN. Aliased frequencies enable the discrimination of compound gratings in peripheral vision, Vision Res. 1997;37:283–290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Howland B, Ginsburg A, Campbell F. High-pass spatial frequency letters as clinical optotypes. Vision Res. 1978;18:1063–1066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Anderson RS, Ennis FA. Foveal and peripheral thresholds for detection and resolution of vanishing optotype tumbling E's. Vision Res. 1999;39:4141–4144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science are provided here courtesy of Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology

RESOURCES