Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2012 May 15;3:48. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00048

Action Mechanisms of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Alzheimer’s Disease and Memory Loss

Niels Hansen 1,*
PMCID: PMC3351674  PMID: 22615703

Abstract

The pharmacological treatment of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is often limited and accompanied by drug side effects. Thus alternative therapeutic strategies such as non-invasive brain stimulation are needed. Few studies have demonstrated that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a method of neuromodulation with consecutive robust excitability changes within the stimulated cortex area, is beneficial in AD. There is also evidence that tDCS enhances memory function in cognitive rehabilitation in depressive patients, Parkinson’s disease, and stroke. tDCS improves working and visual recognition memory in humans and object-recognition learning in the elderly. AD’s neurobiological mechanisms comprise changes in neuronal activity and the cerebral blood flow (CBF) caused by altered microvasculature, synaptic dysregulation from ß-amyloid peptide accumulation, altered neuromodulation via degenerated modulatory amine transmitter systems, altered brain oscillations, and changes in network connectivity. tDCS alters (i) neuronal activity and (ii) human CBF, (iii) has synaptic and non-synaptic after-effects (iv), can modify neurotransmitters polarity-dependently, (v) and alter oscillatory brain activity and (vi) functional connectivity patterns in the brain. It thus is reasonable to use tDCS as a therapeutic instrument in AD as it improves cognitive function in manner based on a disease mechanism. Moreover, it could prove valuable in other types of dementia. Future large-scale clinical and mechanism-oriented studies may enable us to identify its therapeutic validity in other types of demential disorders.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, cerebral blood flow, frontotemporal dementia, memory loss, network connectivity, neurotransmitter modulation, synaptic and non-synaptic after-effects, transcranial direct current stimulation

Introduction

As the pharmacological treatment in Alzheimer disease (AD) is limited (Bauer, 2006), alternative therapeutic approaches are worth pursuing, such as non-invasive brain stimulation with transcranial direct current.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is the application of weak electrical currents by saline-soaked surface sponge electrodes to different cortical areas. tDCS can polarity-dependently modulate cortical excitability with prolonged after-effects (Nitsche et al., 2005) and modify neuronal excitability by tonic de-or hyperpolarization of the resting membrane potential (Creutzfeld et al., 1962; Purpura and McMurtry, 1965). The electrode positioning is determined according to the EEG 10–20 system.

tDCS has demonstrated efficacy in improving recognition memory in AD (Boggio et al., 2009, 2011) and it is a useful tool in cognitive neurorehabilitation, as improvements in cognitive functions were described in patients with depression (Fregni et al., 2006), Parkinson’s disease (Boggio et al., 2006) and stroke (Monti et al., 2008).

Alzheimer’s disease is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder (Thies and Bleiler, 2011) presenting a decrease in acethylcholine activity resulting in cognitive impairment (Schliebs and Arendt, 2011) in many cognitive activities such as memory, language, and executive functions.

The concept of benefiting from modulating cortical excitability via tDCS with consecutive improvement in cognitive functions in AD is thus tempting. We describe tDCS application in clinical studies in patients with dementia (see Table 1) and studies on cognitive functions (see Table 2) as well as potential underlying mechanisms in this article.

Table 1.

Clinical studies of tDCS in dementia.

Study Design n Age (years) Disease diagnosis MMSE Medication Parameters Brain target Effect
ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE
Boggio et al. (2009) Cross over, sham controlled 10 79 ± 9 NINCDS, ADRADA 17 ± 5 AChEIs + others Anodal/sham, 2 mA, 30 min Left DLPFC Improved visual recognition memory after atDCS
Boggio et al. (2011) Sham controlled 15 78 ± 7, 81 ± 10 Adas-Cog, VRT, VAT, ADAS 21 ± 3, 19 ± 3 No data Anodal, sham 2 mA, 30 min TC bilateral Improved visual recognition memory after atDCS
Ferrucci et al. (2008a) Cross over, sham controlled 10 75 ± 7 DSM-IV, NINCDS- ADRADA 23 ± 2 AchEI Anodal/cathodal/sham, 1.5 mA, 15 min Left/right TPC Accuracy of the word-recognition memory increased after atDCS
FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA
Huey et al. (2007) Double-blind, sham controlled 10 61 (46−80) Criteria Lund/Manchester 1994 MDRS No data AChEI + memantine Active/sham, 2 mA, 20 min FC No improvement in verbal fluency after active tDCS

AChEI, acetylcholine esterase inhibitors; Adas-Cog, Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale-cognitive sub scale; ADAS, Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; DSM- IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV); FC, frontal cortex; MDRS, Mattis Dementia Rating Scale; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; NINCDS-ADRADA, National Institute of Neurological Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer disease and Related Disorders Association; TC, temporal cortex; TPC, temporoparietal cortex; VAT, visual attention task; VRT, visual recognition task.

Table 2.

Studies of tDCS on cognitive functions.

Study Healthy subjects/age (age: mean [± standard deviation] or range) Stimulation electrode Polarity Duration/intensity Side effects Effects
Andrews et al. (2011) 10, 20−51 years Left DLPFC Anodal/sham 10 min, 1 mA No Improvement in a WM task after atDCS
Boggio et al. (2006) 18 Patients with PD, 45−71 years M 1, left DLPC Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 or 2 mA No Improvement in WM of Parkinson’s disease patients after atDCS of the left DLPFC
de Vries et al. (2011) 38, 23 ± 2 years Broca’s area Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No atDCS facilitates the acquisition of grammatical knowledge
Ferrucci et al. (2008b) 13, 75 ± 7 years Cerebellum Anodal/cathodal/sham 15 min, 2 mA Headache (one patient) atDCS and ctDCS impairs practice-dependent proficiency in WM
Fiori et al. (2011) 10 Subjects, 3 patients, 45−70 years Wernicke’s area Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No atDCS improved accuracy on the picture-naming task, both normal and patients had a shorter naming latency during atDCS
Flöel et al. (2008) 19, 26 ± 3 years Cp5 Anodal/cathodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No Enhanced language learning by atDCS
Flöel et al. (2011) 20, 62 ± 9 years Right temporoparietal cortex Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No Improved recall one week after learning with atDCS
Fregni et al. (2005) 15, 19−22 years M1, DLPFC Anodal/cathodal/sham 10min, 1mA No atDCS leads to enhancement of WM performance
Iyer et al. (2005) 103, 19−70 years F3 Anodal/cathodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA Skin redness Enhanced verbal fluency by atDCS
Javadi and Walsh (2011) 32, 23 ± 2 years Left DLPFC, M1 Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No Enhancement of verbal memorization after atDCS or impairment of verbal memorization after ctDCS
Kincses et al. (2004) 22, 28 ± 5 years Fp3 Anodal/cathodal 10 min, 1 mA No atDCS enhanced probabilistic classification learning
Marshall et al. (2004) 13, 19−28 years F3 and F4 Anodal/sham Alternating 15 s off/15 s on over 30 min No atDCS during slow wave sleep improves verbal declarative memory
Marshall et al. (2005) 12, 19−27 years F3 and F4 Anodal/cathodal Alternating 15 s off/15 s on over 15 min No Impaired performance in WM task by anodal and ctDCS
Ohn et al. (2008) 15, 27 ± 4 years F3 Anodal/sham 30 min, 1 mA No atDCS enhanced performance in a WM task
Penolazzi et al. (2010) 11, 27 ± 5 years Right F4–C4, Left F3–C3, alternating between atDCS and ctDCS Anodal/cathodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No Right atDCS and left ctDCS facilitated the recall of pleasant images regarding pleasant and neutral images
Ross et al. (2011) 14, 55−69 years Both anterior temporal lobes Anodal/sham 15 min, 1.5 mA No Numerical improvement in face naming after atDCS
Sparing et al. (2008) 15, 27 ± 4 years Cp5 Anodal/cathodal/sham 7 min, 2 mA No Improved picture naming by atDCS
Teo et al. (2011) 12, 27 ± 9 years F3 of the DLPFC Anodal/sham 20 min, 1 mA No Current strength may affect WM performance
Zaehle et al. (2011) 10, 25 ± 2 years Left DLPFC Anodal/cathodal 15 min, 1 mA No Increase in WM performance and amplified oscillatory power in theta and alpha bands after atDCS, interference with WM performance after ctDCS

Abbreviations for electrode placement according to the 10–20 electrode system (Cp5, Cz, Fp3, C3/4: see Recommendations for the practice of clinical. Neurophysiology: guidelines of the International Federation Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 1999;52:1–304), atDCS, anodal transcranial direct current stimulation; min, minutes, ctDCS, cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation, DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; mA, milli Ampere; min, minutes, PD, Parkinson’s disease; TPC, temporoparietal cortex.

Methodological Aspects

The review section about the action mechanisms of tDCS in AD is based on a non-systematic approach, whereas the review section on clinical studies with tDCS in AD and memory is based on a somewhat systematic approach based on the PubMed database. A literature search for original and review articles on tDCS in demential disorders was performed through December 2011 seeking clinical studies on tDCS in AD and for demential disorders, by screening the PubMed database. The keywords were used in combination with “Alzheimer disease” AND “tDCS,” “dementia” AND “tDCS” as well as “memory” AND “tDCS.” The studies were published between 2004 and 12/2011. The exclusion criteria of articles in the article titles searched were only “brain stimulation,” “depression,” only “memory” or “Alzheimer disease,” or “demential disorder” without “tDCS,” “motor learning,” and only “tDCS” without “memory” or “Alzheimer disease” or “demential disorder.” Thirty-eight papers were screened from 75 articles according to the aforementioned criteria and 33 thereof were used as the basis for the Section “tDCS in Demential Disorders.”

tDCS in Demential Disorders

Alzheimer’s disease

The effect of anodal tDCS (atDCS) over the left temporal cortex (TC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) was investigated on recognition and working memory (WM) in 10 AD patients (Boggio et al., 2009), revealing enhancement in a visual recognition memory task after atDCS of the DLPFC and left TC (Boggio et al., 2009). In another study, an improvement in a word-recognition memory in 10 patients with probable AD was proven after atDCS of the temporoparietal areas (Ferrucci et al., 2008a). In contrast, cathodal tDCS (ctDCS) lead to decreased word-recognition memory. The effect of atDCS persisted up to 30 min after stimulation, indicating a long-lasting increase in brain excitability (Ferrucci et al., 2008a). Long-term enhancement of visual recognition memory for up to 4 weeks after therapy was found after atDCS in 15 AD patients (Boggio et al., 2011).

Frontotemporal dementia

A study demonstrated that active tDCS does not result in a beneficial effect in verbal fluency in 10 patients with frontotemporal dementia presenting mainly behavioral (and in one patient language) symptoms (Huey et al., 2007). The lack of effect may be due to the small current that reaches the frontal cortex due to brain atrophy and neuronal loss with concomitant incapability of the affected cortex to respond to brain polarization (Huey et al., 2007).

Safety and side effects of tDCS

General observations

There is evidence that tDCS applied to the scalp over the prefrontal cortex over 20 min does not alter local and global cortical function (Iyer et al., 2005). The current intensity of 1 mA did not result in significant effects on cortical function, whereas verbal fluency increased with 2 mA-atDCS and decreased with 2 mA-ctDCS (Iyer et al., 2005). In a systematic review, itching, tingling, headache, burning sensation, and discomfort were the most often reported adverse effects of active tDCS vs. sham tDCS (Brunoni et al., 2011). Skin irritation and skin burning can occur after tDCS application due to the electrochemical products’ skin contact generated by the direct current (Durand et al., 2002; Palm et al., 2008). In addition, magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) in normal subjects failed to detect changes in acetylaspartate, thus showing that atDCS induced no neurotoxic effects (Rango et al., 2008). Furthermore, in motor cortical areas, tDCS induced no relevant changes in serum neuron-specific enolase, a neuronal damage marker, indicating that tDCS induced no harmful effects (Nitsche et al., 2003).

Observations in AD

In studies of tDCS in AD, no adverse effects from tDCS application were noted (Boggio et al., 2009). Only an itching sensation, but no side effects were reported in the study of 10 AD patients (Ferrucci et al., 2008a). No adverse effects, nor tDCS effects on the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive sub scale (Adas-Cog), or visual attention task (VAT) scores were observed (Boggio et al., 2011).

tDCS in Learning and Memory

Object-location learning in the elderly

The ability to memorize the location of objects is known to worsen by aging and in neurodegenerative dementia. atDCS over the temporoparietal cortex in 20 elderly healthy subjects resulted in improved retention of object-location learning for up to 1 week after learning (Flöel et al., 2011). This finding has relevance concerning memory deficits in normal and pathological aging.

Enhancing declarative memory by tDCS

Anodal tDCS enhances slow oscillatory EEG activity that in turn can enhance declarative memories (Marshall et al., 2004). As shown in 32 human healthy subjects, declarative memory can be improved by anodal and impaired by ctDCS of the DLPFC (Javadi and Walsh, 2011).

Enhancing implicit memory by tDCS

Both declarative and implicit memory are known to improve via tDCS. For instance, atDCS of Broca’s area enhanced implicit learning of an artificial grammar in 38 healthy subjects (de Vries et al., 2011), an interesting finding supporting tDCS as a potential instrument in the rehabilitation of aphasic patients.

Modulation of WM by tDCS

Enhancing WM by tDCS

Several studies address the physiological effects of tDCS in the WM as a part of declarative memory playing a pivotal role in long-term memory, language, and executive function (Baddeley, 1992).

In 10 patients with cognitive defects after a first-ever stroke, atDCS of the DLPFC led to enhanced WM performance (Jo et al., 2009). In a neurodegenerative disease like Parkinson’s, atDCS of the left DLPFC was also shown to improve WM in 18 patients (Boggio et al., 2006). atDCS to the DLPFC lead to WM enhancement in healthy subjects (Fregni et al., 2005; Andrews et al., 2011) and rats, frontal ctDCS enhanced visual–spatial WM (Dockery et al., 2011). Interestingly, atDCS led to amplified oscillation in theta and alpha electroencephalography (EEG) bands and increased WM performance in humans (Zaehle et al., 2011). WM representations are supported by oscillatory brain activity (Lisman and Idiart, 1995). In particular, theta EEG band activity has been associated with memory encoding and retrieval (Jensen and Tesche, 2002). Thus amplified theta band activity is related to WM’s executive function, indicating the continuous information processing required during WM performance.

As neuroimaging studies revealed a widespread effect in cortical activity by tDCS (Lang et al., 2005), it is likely to imply a tDCS influence on the entire WM system, and not only on the DLPFC. Furthermore, there is some evidence that WM performance can be improved in a manner dependent on current strength in 14 healthy subjects (Teo et al., 2011). No current strength or time-course effect was observed in the accuracy of WM tasks. However, a significant current by time interaction was found in a WM task (Teo et al., 2011). However, the effect-dependence on current intensity of tDCS in memory function is not proven by this single study of one WM task; it requires further examination in healthy subjects and those with diverse cognitive functions.

A time-dependent enhancement of verbal memory resulted after atDCS of the DLPFC (Ohn et al., 2008). Name recall can benefit from atDCS of the anterior temporal lobes (Ross et al., 2011), and word retrieval improved in healthy and non-fluent aphasic patients after atDCS (Fiori et al., 2011).

Impairment of WM after tDCS

Bilateral prefrontal ctDCS and atDCS during a memory task can impair neuronal processes related to a WM paradigm (Marshall et al., 2005). Furthermore, cerebellar tDCS of both polarities impaired use-dependent improvement in a WM task (Ferrucci et al., 2008b). WM also revealed impairment by ctDCS to the right parietal lobe (Berryhill et al., 2010).

Potential Mechanisms of Action of tDCS in AD

Effects of tDCS on neuronal activity by altering the membrane potential

In an AD mouse model, ß-amyloid peptide was shown to disturb the resting membrane potential in muscle fibers (Mukhamediarov et al., 2011). Furthermore, ß-amyloid 1–42 peptide caused membrane depolarization leading to hyperexcitability of affected neurons in a human neuronal cell model of AD (Blanchard et al., 2002).

atDCS might be an instrument to alter the neuronal depolarization frequently altered in AD according to in vitro studies, as atDCS leads to the increased cortical excitability promoting neuronal depolarization (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000). Increasing cortical excitability is a relevant tool in AD, as AD patients reveal temporoparietal hypoactivity (as characterized by focal slow wave activity in magnetoencephalography; Fernandez et al., 2002).

Motor cortex (Di Lazzaro et al., 2004) and global cortical hyperexcitability is found in AD (Rossini et al., 2007), correlating with cognitive severity in a TMS study (Alagona et al., 2001). As ctDCS led to reduced cortical excitability caused by neuronal hyperpolarization (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000), it might also be beneficial in AD by lowering its somewhat increased cortical excitability.

Non-synaptic mechanisms based on changes in the membrane potential underlying the after-effects of atDCS and ctDCS (Ardolino et al., 2005) might be responsible for modulating cognitive function in AD. The local changes in ionic concentrations could be due to alterations in transmembrane proteins and from changes in H+ ions induced by exposure to a constant electrical field (Ardolino et al., 2005).

Synaptic after-effects of tDCS

NMDA receptor-dependent after-effects

tDCS induces prolonged after-effects sharing similarities with long-term potentiation (LTP)- and long-term depression (LTD)-like changes in cortical excitability (Paulus, 2004). In an in vitro and in vivo AD mouse model, LTP as the putative mechanism of learning and memory is evidently impaired by ß-amyloid peptide (Gengler et al., 2010; Middei et al., 2010). ß-amyloid peptide disruption of LTP is N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent in the mouse hippocampus in vivo and in vitro (Yamin, 2009).

tDCS-induced after-effects are partly NMDA receptor-dependent (Liebetanz et al., 2002), suggesting that tDCS after-effects may alter NMDA receptor-dependent cortical plasticity that may be disturbed in AD.

GABAergic interneurons

Anodal after-effects are probably mediated in part by gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)Aergic interneurons as a reduction in short-interval intracortical inhibition and an increase in I-wave facilitation after tDCS intracortical facilitation (Nitsche et al., 2005; Stagg et al., 2009; Stagg and Nitsche, 2011). As in AD, GABAergic cortical inhibitory interneurons play a role in the disease’s early stage (Koliatsos et al., 2006); modulation of these interneurons by tDCS is a possible disease-modifying mechanism. Hippocampus changes in GABA B receptor protein were found in 16 elderly subjects with AD, indicating alterations between the excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitter systems with consecutively dysfunctional hippocampal circuitry (Iwakiri et al., 2005). A MRS study provides evidence that atDCS causes reduced GABA concentration within the stimulated cortex, whereas ctDCS leads to impaired glutamatergic neuronal activity with a correlated reduction in GABA concentration due to a relationship between these two neurotransmitters (Stagg et al., 2009). Thus tDCS might reduce the disequilibrium between excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters systems in AD.

Glutamatergic synapses

In AD, glutamate receptors may be dysregulated by ß-amyloid accumulation resulting in the disrupted glutamatergic activity that coincides with cognitive decline (Parameshwaran et al., 2008). The dysregulation of glutamatergic activity might be altered by atDCS, as there is evidence that glutamatergic synapses are involved in anodal after-effects (Stagg et al., 2009; Stagg and Nitsche, 2011), and MRS data support that glutamate and glutamine levels were elevated in the parietal cortex after atDCS (Clark et al., 2011).

Therefore, ctDCS may have the potential to affect cognitive functions in AD by modulating glutamatergic synapses.

Effects of tDCS on human regional cerebral blood flow

There is evidence in AD that characteristics of the cerebral microvasculature have changed, leading to altered cerebral blood flow (CBF; van Beek et al., 2012). atDCS induced an increase in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF), whereas ctDCS resulted in a decrease in rCBF during and after stimulation (Zheng et al., 2011). As tDCS modulates CBF in many cortical and subcortical regions with sustained and widespread changes in neuronal activity (Lang et al., 2005), it is an auspicious instrument in AD.

Modulating oscillatory brain activity and functional connectivity pattern via tDCS

AD led to an altered temporal correlation in parietal and prefrontal oscillations (Montez et al., 2009), more severe deceleration of spontaneous oscillatory activity (Rossini et al., 2007; de Waal et al., 2011), a functional disconnection (Gili et al., 2011), in particular between the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in AD (Grady et al., 2001), and network connectivity changes (Zhou et al., 2010). It therefore makes sense to use tDCS as a therapeutic tool in AD, as it can reconfigure cerebral networks (Peña-Gómez et al., 2011) and cause changes in functional cerebral connectivity patterns suggesting alterations in brain synchronization (Polanía et al., 2011). As the cognitive dysfunction in brain diseases like AD is based on abnormal neural synchronization (Polanía et al., 2011), it may be beneficial to cause changes in brain synchronization via tDCS.

More specifically, atDCS over the primary motor cortex combined with inhibitory ctDCS of the contralateral frontopolar cortex caused an increased functional connectivity pattern within the premotor, motor, and sensorimotor areas of stimulated hemispheres in 10 healthy human subjects. Furthermore, intra- and interhemispheric connectivity changes became apparent after atDCS, indicating changes in brain topological functional organization (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006). Another study demonstrated that ctDCS decreased while atDCS augmented normalized beta and gamma frequency EEG bands, suggesting transient reorganization of cortical activity (Antal et al., 2004). As gamma activity is also part of high-level information processing, it is an adjuvant method to influence higher-order cognitive function (Antal et al., 2004).

Modulating cortical neurotransmitters via tDCS

Neuronal loss implicates the impairment of serotonergic neuromodulation as a basic mechanism of promoting dementia in AD (Yang et al., 1999). Furthermore, there is dopaminergic modulation of LTD-like plasticity in AD (Koch et al., 2011). Cholinergic systems with ascending projections are also degenerated in neurodegenerative dementia (Schmitt, 2005; Fregni et al., 2006). Modulating these neurotransmitter systems via tDCS would therefore seem to be a mechanism-based treatment of AD. Dopaminergic (Nitsche et al., 2006), serotonergic (Nitsche et al., 2009) and cholinergic (Kuo et al., 2007) neuromodulations have been demonstrated by atDCS and ctDCS (Stagg and Nitsche, 2011), indicating another disease-modifying treatment option of tDCS.

There are other mechanisms that determine the response of humans to tDCS, i.e., the BDNF polymorphism (Antal et al., 2004). BDNF modulation is an interesting target in AD, as ß-amyloid processing is involved in the BDNF pathway and (Forero et al., 2006) the BDNF ValMet 66 polymorphism is a neural risk for AD (Voineskos et al., 2011), suggesting BDNF as a factor shaping the cortical excitability response to tDCS in AD patients.

Limitations of Current Knowledge of tDCS in Demential Disorders

There are few studies on the effects of tDCS in demential disorders (AD and frontotemporal dementia, see Table 1). The efficacy of tDCS in other demential disorders (for instance vascular dementia or Lewy body dementia) has thus not yet been proven. Furthermore, only a small battery of cognitive functions, i.e., selective attention, WM, visual and word-recognition memory, instruction remembering and word recall has been evaluated so far (see tDCS in Demential Disorders). tDCS effects on other cognitive functions like calculating, cognitive flexibility, language, orientation, short- and long-term memory, and writing will have to be evaluated in studies with larger cohorts and longer control periods. The tDCS effects studied thus far are short-lived (maximum up to 1 month; Boggio et al., 2011) and there are no observations regarding longer-duration interventions. Nor have the long-term side effects of tDCS been assessed. This is particularly important, as tDCS applied over longer periods might interact with mechanisms involved in neurodegeneration with either beneficial (delayed deterioration of cognition) or harmful effects (accelerated cognitive deterioration). The interaction of tDCS with pharmacological treatment has not yet been addressed systematically in studies. However, current data indicate there is no significant interaction between medication outcome and its interaction with tDCS (Boggio et al., 2011).

Perspectives of tDCS in AD

tDCS may enhance our understanding of the neurobiological substrates underlying the cognitive decline in AD. Factors such as cognitive reserve, genetic variants, learning capacity, volumetric studies of cortical thinning and white matter volume, and integrity will have to be thoroughly and systematically investigated in future studies of tDCS on cortical functions in AD. The therapeutic efficacy of tDCS must be examined by outcome scales commonly used in trials of pharmacological agents such as the ADAS-Cog (Freitas et al., 2011). Moreover, multiple target tDCS or tDCS targeting new brain areas must be developed to overcome multiple cognitive deficits in AD. A multi-electrode stimulation set-up was recently demonstrated that increased focalty and intensity at the brain target (Dmochowksi et al., 2011).

Conclusions

tDCS is an easy to perform and non-invasive alternative therapeutic tool for neurodegenerative diseases such as AD. Its effects comprise the enhancement of cognitive functions in explicit and implicit memory. The mechanisms of tDCS are based on changes in membrane polarization, cerebral blood flow, functional connectivity, and brain oscillatory activity that may be altered in AD and other demential disorders.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alagona G., Bella R., Ferri R., Carnemolla A., Pappalardo A., Costanzo E., Pennisi G. (2001). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in Alzheimer disease: motor cortex excitability and cognitive severity. Neurosci. Lett. 13, 57–60 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02288-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrews S. C., Hoy K. E., Enticott P. G., Daskalakis Z. J., Fitzgerald P. B. (2011). Improving working memory: the effect of combining cognitive activity and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Stimul. 4, 84–89 10.1016/j.brs.2010.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antal A., Varga E. T., Kincses T. Z., Nitsche M. A., Paulus W. (2004). Oscillatory brain activity and transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. Neuroreport 15, 1307–1310 10.1097/00001756-200402090-00017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ardolino G., Bossi B., Barbieri S., Priori A. (2005). Non-synaptic mechanisms underlie the after-effects of cathodal transcutaneous direct current stimulation of the human brain. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 568, 653–663 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.088310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley A. (1992). Working memory. Science 255, 556–559 10.1126/science.1736359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauer J. (2006). Cholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 25, CD005593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berryhill M. E., Wencil E. B., Branch Coslett H., Olson I. R. (2010). A selective working memory impairment after transcranial direct currrent stimulation to the right parietal lobe. Neurosci. Lett. 479, 312–316 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blanchard B. J., Thomas V. L., Ingram V. M. (2002). Mechanism of membrane depolarization caused by the Alzheimer Abeta 1-42 peptide. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 293, 1197–1203 10.1016/S0006-291X(02)00346-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boggio P. S., Ferrucci R., Mameli F., Martins D., Martins O., Vergari M., Tadini L., Scarpini E., Fregni F., Priori A. (2011). Prolonged visual memory enhancement after direct current stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Stimul. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boggio P. S., Ferrucci R., Rigonatti S. P., Covre P., Nitsche M., Pascual-Leone A., Fregni F. (2006). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 249, 31–38 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Boggio P. S., Khoury L. P., Martins D. C., Martins O. E., de Macedo E. C., Fregni F. (2009). Temporal cortex direct current stimulation enhances performance on a visual recognition memory task in Alzheimer disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 80, 444–447 10.1136/jnnp.2007.141853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brunoni A. R., Amadera J., Berbel B., Volz M. S., Rizzerio B. G., Fregni F. (2011). A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 14, 1133–1145 10.1017/S1461145710001690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark V. P., Coffman B. A., Trumbo M. C., Gasparovic C. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) produces localized and specific alterations in neurochemistry: a 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neurosci. Lett. 500, 67–71 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Creutzfeld O. D., Fromm G. H., Kapp H. (1962). Influence of transcortical d-c currents on cortical neuronal activity. Exp. Neurol. 5, 436–452 10.1016/0014-4886(62)90056-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. de Vries M. H., Barth A. C., Maiworm S., Knecht S., Zwiserlood O., Flöel A. (2011). Electrical stimulation of Broca’s area enhances implicit learning of an artificial grammar. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2427–2436 10.1162/jocn.2009.21385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. de Waal H., Stam C. J., de Haan W., van Straaten E. C., Scheltens P., van der Flier W. M. (2011). Young Alzheimer patients show distinct regional changes of oscillatory brain dynamics. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1008: e25–e31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Di Lazzaro V., Oliviero A., Pilato F., Saturno O., Dileone M., Marra C., Daniele A., Ghirlanda S., Gainotti G., Tonalli P. A. (2004). Motor cortex hyperexcitability to transcranial magnetic stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 75, 555–559 10.1136/jnnp.2003.018127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dmochowksi J. P., Datta A., Bikson M., Su Y., Parra L. C. (2011). Optimized multi-electrode stimulation increases focalty and intensity at target. J. Neural Eng. 8, 046011. 10.1088/1741-2560/8/4/046011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dockery C. A., Liebetanz D., Birbaumer N., Malinowska M., Wesierska M. J. (2011). Cumulative benefits of frontal transcranial direct current stimulation on visuospatial working memory training and skill learning in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 452–460 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Durand S., Fromy B., Bouyé P., Saumet J. L., Abraham P. (2002).Vasodilatation in response to repeated anodal current application in the human skin relies on aspirin-sensitive mechanisms. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 540, 261–269 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013364 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fernandez A., Maestu F., Arno C., Gil P., Fehr T., Wienbruch C., Rockstroh B., Elbert T., Ortiz T. (2002). Focal temporoparietal slow activity in Alzheimer’s disease revealed by magnetoencephalography. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 764–770 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01366-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ferrucci R., Mameli F., Guidi I., Mrakic-Sposta S., Vergari M., Marceglia S., Cogiamanian F., Barbieri S., Scarpini E., Priori A. (2008a). Transcranial direct current stimulation improves recognition memory in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 71, 493–498 10.1212/01.wnl.0000317060.43722.a3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ferrucci R., Marceglia S., Vergari M., Cogiamanian F., Mrakic-Sposta S., Mameli F., Zago S., Barbieri S., Priori A. (2008b). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation impairs the practice-dependent proficiency increase in working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1687–1697 10.1162/jocn.2008.20112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fiori V., Coccia M., Marinelli C. V., Vecchi V., Bonifazi S., Ceravolo M. G., Provinviali L., Tomaiuolo F., Marangolo P. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation improves word retrieval in healthy and nonfluent aphasic subjects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2309–2323 10.1162/jocn.2010.21579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Flöel A., Rösser N., Michka O., Knecht S., Breitenstein C. (2008). Noninvasive brain stimulation improves language learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1415–1422 10.1162/jocn.2008.20098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Flöel A., Suttorp W., Kohl O., Kürten J., Lohmann H., Breitenstein C., Knecht S. (2011). Non-invasive brain stimulation improves object-location learning in the elderly. Neurobiol. Aging [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Forero D. A., Cascadesus G., Perry G., Arboleda H. (2006). Synaptic dysfunction and oxidative stress in Alzheimer’s disease: emerging mechanisms. J. Cell. Mol. Med. 10, 796–805 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2006.tb00439.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fregni F., Boggio P. S., Nitsche M., Bermpohl F., Antal A., Feredoes F., Marcolin M. A., Rigonatti S. P., Silva M. T., Paulus W., Pascual-Leone A. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enchances working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 23–30 10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fregni F., Boggio P. S., Nitsche M. A., Marcolin M. A., Rigonatti S. P., Pascual-Leone A. (2006). Treatment of major depression with transcranial direct current stimulation. Bipolar Disord. 8, 203–204 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00291.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Freitas C., Mondragón-Llorca H., Pascual-Leone A. (2011). Noninvasive brain stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and perspectives for the future. Exp. Gerontol. 46, 611–627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gengler S., Hamilton A., Hölscher C. (2010). Synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus of a APP/PS 1 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease is impaired in old but not young mice. PLoS ONE 5, e9764. 10.1371/journal.pone.0009764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gili T., Cercignani M., Serra L., Perri R., Giove F., Maraviglia B., Caltagirone C., Bozzali M. (2011). Regional brain atrophy and functional disconnection across Alzheimer’s disease evolution. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 82, 58–66 10.1136/jnnp.2009.199935 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Grady C. L., Furey M. L., Pietrini P., Horwitz B., Rapoport S. I. (2001). Altered brain functional connectivity and impaired short-term memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 124, 739–756 10.1093/brain/124.4.739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Huey C. D., Probasco J. C., Moll J., Stocking J., Ko M. H., Wassermann E. M. (2007). No effect of DC brain polarization on verbal fluency in patients with advanced frontotemporal dementia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 1417–1418 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.02.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Iwakiri M., Mizukami K., Ikonomovic M. D., Ishikawa M., Hidaka S., Abrahamson E. E., DeKosky S. T., Asada T. (2005). Changes in hippocampal GABABR1 subunit expression in Alzheimer’s patients: association with Braak staging. Acta Neuropathol. 109, 467–474 10.1007/s00401-005-0985-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Iyer M. B., Mattu U., Grafman J., Lomarev M., Sato S., Wassermann E. M. (2005). Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology 64, 872–875 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152986.07469.E9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Javadi A. H., Walsh V. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates declarative memory. Brain Stimul. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jensen O., Tesche C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1395–1399 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jo J. M., Kim Y., Ohn S. H., Joen B., Lee K. H. (2009). Enhancing the working memory of stroke patients using tDCS. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 404–409 10.1097/PHM.0b013e3181a0e4cb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kincses T. Z., Antal A., Nitsche M. A., Bártfai O., Paulus W. (2004). Facilitation of probabilistic classification learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the human. Neuropsychologia 42, 113–117 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00124-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Koch G., Esposito Z., Codecà C., Mori F., Kusayanagi H., Monteleone F., Di Lorenzo F., Bernardi G., Martorana A. (2011). Altered dopamine modulation of LTD-like plasticity in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 703–707 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Koliatsos V. E., Kecojevic A., Troncoso J. C., Gastard M. C., Bennett D. A., Schneider J. A. (2006). Early involvement of small inhibitory cortical interneurons in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 112, 147–162 10.1007/s00401-006-0068-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kuo M. F., Grosch J., Fregni F., Paulus W., Nitsche M. A. (2007). Focusing effect of acetylcholine on neuroplasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 14442–14447 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2391-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lang N., Siebner H. R., Ward N. S., Lee L., Nitsche M. A., Paulus W., Rothwell J. C., Lemon R. N., Frackowiak R. S. (2005). How does transcranial DC stimulation of the primary motor cortex alter regional neuronal activity in the human brain? Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 495. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04233.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Liebetanz D., Nitsche M. A., Tergau F., Paulus W. (2002). Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain 125, 2238–2247 10.1093/brain/awf238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lisman J. E., Idiart M. A. (1995). Storage of 7 ± 2 short term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267, 1512–1515 10.1126/science.7878473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Marshall L., Mölle M., Hallschmidt M., Born J. (2004). Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J. Neurosci. 3, 9985–9992 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Marshall L., Mölle M., Siebner H. R., Born J. (2005). Bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation slows reaction time in a working memory task. BMC Neurosci. 8, 23. 10.1186/1471-2202-6-23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Middei S., Roberto A., Berretta N., Panico M. B., Lista S., Bernardi G., Mercuri N. B., Ammassari-Teule M., Nistico R. (2010). Learning discloses abnormal structural and functional plasticity at hippocampal synapses in the APP23 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Learn. Mem. 19, 236–240 10.1101/lm.1748310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Montez T., Poil S. S., Jones B. F., Manshanden I., Verbunt J. P., van Dijk B. W., Brussaard A. B., van Ooyen A., Stam C. J., Scheltens P., Linkenkaer-Hansen K. (2009). Altered temporal correlations in parietal alpha and prefrontal theta oscillations in early-stage Alzheimer disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1614–1619 10.1073/pnas.0811699106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Monti A., Cogiamanian F., Marceglia S., Ferrucci R., Mameli F., Mrakic-Sposta S., Vergari M., Zago S., Priori A. (2008). Improved naming after transcranial direct current stimulation in aphasia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 79, 451–453 10.1136/jnnp.2007.135277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mukhamediarov M. A., Volkov E. M., Leushina A. V., Kochunova I. U. O., Palotas A., Zefirov A. L. (2011). Ionic and molecular mechanisms of beta-amyloid-induced depolarization of the mouse skeletal muscle fibres. Ross. Fiziol. Zh. Im. I M Sechenova 97, 795–803 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nitsche M. A., Kuo M. F., Karrasch R., Wächter B., Liebetanz D., Paulus W. (2009). Serotonin affects transcranial direct current-induced neuroplasticity in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 503–508 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nitsche M. A., Lampe C., Antal A., Liebetanz D., Lang N., Tergau F., Paulus W. (2006). Dopaminergic modulation of long-lasting direct current-induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1651–1657 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04676.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nitsche M. A., Nitsche M. S., Klein C. C., Tergau F., Rothwell J. C., Paulus W. (2003). Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 600–604 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00235-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nitsche M. A., Paulus W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 527, 633–639 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nitsche M. A., Seeber A., Frommann K., Klein C. C., Rochford C., Nitsche M. S., Fricke K., Liebetanz D., Lang N., Antal A., Paulus W., Tergau F. (2005). Modulating parameters of excitability during and after transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 568, 291–303 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ohn S. H., Park C. I., Yoo W. K., Ko M. H., Choi K. P., Kim G. M., Lee Y. T., Kim Y. H. (2008). Time-dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working memory. Neuroreport 8, 43–47 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f2adfd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Palm U., Keeser D., Schiller C., Fintescu Z., Nitsche M., Reisinger E., Padberg F. (2008). Skin lesions after treatment with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Brain Stimul. 1, 386–387 10.1016/j.brs.2008.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Parameshwaran K., Dhanasekaran M., Suppiramaniam V. (2008). Amyloid beta peptides and glutamatergic synaptic dysregulation. Exp. Neurol. 210, 7–13 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Paulus W. (2004). Outlasting excitability shifts induced by direct current stimulation of the human brain. Suppl. Clin. Neurophysiol. 57, 708–714 10.1016/S1567-424X(09)70411-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Peña-Gómez C., Sala-Lonch R., Junqué C., Clemente I. C., Vidal D., Bargalló N., Falcón C., Valls-Solé J., Pascual-Leone A., Bartrés-Faz D. (2011). Modulation of large-scale brain networks by transcranial direct current stimulation evidenced by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Stimul. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Penolazzi B., Di Domenico A., Marzoli D., Mammarella N., Fairfield B., Franciotti R., Brancucci A., Tommasi L. (2010). Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on episodic memory related to emotional visual stimuli. PLoS ONE 5, e10623. 10.1371/journal.pone.0010623 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Polanía R., Nitsche M. A., Paulus W. (2011). Modulating functional connectivity patterns and topological functional organization of the human brain with transcranial direct current stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1236–1249 10.1002/hbm.21104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Purpura D. P., McMurtry J. G. (1965). Intracellular activities and evoked potential changes during polarization of motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 171, 1–25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rango M., Cogiamanian F., Marceglia S., Barberis B., Arighi A., Biondetti P., Priori A. (2008). Myoinositol content in the human brain is modified by transcranial direct current stimulation in a matter of minutes: a 1H-MRS study. Magn. Reson. Med. 60, 782–789 10.1002/mrm.21709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ross L. A., McCoy D., Coslett H. B., Olson I. R., Wolk D. A. (2011). Improved proper name recall in aging after electrical stimulation of the anterior temporal lobes. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:6. 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rossini P. M., Rossi S., Babiloni C., Polich J. (2007). Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: from normal aging to neurodegeneration. Prog. Neurobiol. 83, 375–400 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Schliebs R., Arendt T. (2011). The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav. Brain Res. 22, 555–563 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Schmitt H. P. (2005). Neuro-modulation, aminergic neuro-disinhibition and neuro-degeneration. Draft of a comprehensive theory for Alzheimer disease. Med. Hypotheses 65, 1106–1119 10.1016/j.mehy.2005.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sparing R., Dafotakis M., Meister I. G., Thirugnanasambandam N., Fink G. R. (2008). Enhancing language performance with non-invasive brain stimulation-a transcranial direct current stimulation study in healthy humans. Neuropsychologia 46, 261–268 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stagg C. J., Best J. G., Stephenson M. C., O’Shea J., Wylezinska M., Kincses Z. T., Morris P. G., Matthews P. M., Johansen-Berg H. (2009). Polarity-sensitive modulation of cortical neurotransmitters by transcranial stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29, 5202–5206 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4432-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Stagg C. J., Nitsche M. A. (2011). Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroscientist 17, 37–53 10.1177/1073858410386614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Teo F., Hoy K. E., Daskalakis Z. J., Fitzgerald P. B. (2011). Investigating the role of current strength in tDCS modulation of working memory performance in healthy controls. Front. Psychiatry 2:45. 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thies W., Bleiler L. (2011). Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 208–244 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Uhlhaas P. J., Singer W. (2006). Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52, 155–168 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. van Beek A. H., Lagro J., Olde-Rikkert M. G., Zhang R., Claassen J. A. (2012). Oscillations in cerebral blood flow and cortical oxygenation in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 428–431 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Voineskos A. N., Lerch J. P., Felsky D., Shaikh S., Rajji T. K., Miranda D., Lobaugh N. J., Mulsant B. H., Pollock B. G., Kennedy J. L. (2011). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism and prediction of neural risk for Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 198–206 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yamin G. (2009). NMDA receptor-dependent signaling pathways that underlie amyloid beta-protein disruption of LTP in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. Res. 87, 1729–1736 10.1002/jnr.21998 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yang Y., Beyreuther K., Schmitt H. P. (1999). Spatial analysis of the neuronal density of aminergic brainstem nuclei in primary neurodegenerative and vascular dementia: a comparative immunocytochemical and quantitative study using a graph method. Anal. Cell. Pathol. 19, 125–138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zaehle T., Sandmann P., Thorne J. D., Jäncke L., Herrmann C. S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 12, 2. 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zheng X., Alsop D. C., Schlaug G. (2011). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on human regional cerebral blood flow. Neuroimage 1, 26–33 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zhou J., Greicius M. D., Gennatas E. D., Growdon M. E., Jang J. Y., Rabinovici G. D., Kramer J. H., Weiner M., Miller B. L., Seeley W. W. (2010). Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133, 1352–1367 10.1093/brain/awq075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES