Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2012 May 16;3:43. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00043

Cortisol-Induced Increases of Plasma Oxytocin Levels Predict Decreased Immediate Free Recall of Unpleasant Words

Mattie Tops 1,2,3,*, Femke T A Buisman-Pijlman 4, Maarten A S Boksem 5,6, Albertus A Wijers 2, Jakob Korf 7
PMCID: PMC3353157  PMID: 22623919

Abstract

Cortisol and oxytocin have been shown to interact in both the regulation of stress responses and in memory function. In the present study we administered cortisol to 35 healthy female subjects in a within-subject double-blind placebo-controlled design, while measuring oxytocin levels, adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) levels, and free recall of pleasant and of unpleasant words. We found that cortisol administration suppressed ACTH levels and (1) induced a decrease in oxytocin associated with ACTH suppression and (2) an increase in oxytocin that was independent from ACTH suppression. This cortisol-induced increase in plasma oxytocin was associated with a selective decrease in immediate free recall of unpleasant words from primacy positions. The present results add to evidence that cortisol-induced increases in oxytocin could mediate some of the effects of stress and cortisol on memory, and possibly play a role in the regulation of the hypothalamo-pituitary–adrenal stress response. This mechanism could significantly impact affective and social behaviors, in particular during times of stress.

Keywords: oxytocin, cortisol, adrenocorticotropic hormone, memory, stress, hypothalamo-pituitary–adrenal axis, vasopressin

Introduction

As part of the hypothalamo-pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, corticotrophin releasing factor (CRF) is released by the hypothalamus to trigger release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and vasopressin into the bloodstream at the pituitary, which trigger release of glucocorticoids including cortisol from the adrenal cortex. Oxytocin is secreted from the same hypothalamic nuclei which regulate plasma vasopressin and the HPA axis, and neuroanatomic/electrophysiologic data indicate it is modulated by the same hippocampal projections which modulate plasma vasopressin and HPA axis activity (Risold and Swanson, 1996; Herman et al., 2002). Plasma oxytocin levels have been shown to be correlated positively with the integrity of hippocampal-mediated HPA feedback (Goldman et al., 2008). Additionally, in patients with Addison’s disease, elevated ACTH levels were accompanied by higher oxytocin levels (Grenbäck et al., 2007). It has been suggested that cortisol-induced increases in oxytocin may be involved in the feedback regulation of the HPA axis (Liberzon and Young, 1997; Carter, 1998).

Based on findings suggesting that cortisol administration increases plasma oxytocin levels (Kalin et al., 1985; Tops et al., 2007b), we previously postulated the hypothesis that cortisol administration may have beneficial effects on stress coping and emotional memory that are partially mediated by increases in oxytocin (Tops et al., 2007a,b): oxytocin inhibits or downregulates stress responses, seems to inhibit acquisition and/or consolidation of aversive, high arousal experiences, and has analgesic effects (Uvnäs-Moberg, 1998; Lee et al., 2009).

However, these cortisol–oxytocin interactions in stress and memory function appear to be highly context dependent. Heinrichs et al. (2004) found that intranasal oxytocin administration impaired recall depending on the kind of memory test used and the psychobiological relevance of the stimuli, impairing recall of reproduction-related words but not neutral words in an implicit conceptual task. Intranasal has been found to increase the familiarity of previously presented faces (Rimmele et al., 2009). Animal research has shown that oxytocin is positively involved in social memory (Young, 2002), while high levels of oxytocin seem to inhibit acquisition and/or consolidation of aversive, high arousal experiences such as during labor (e.g., Silber et al., 1990). So it seems that effects of oxytocin on memory may, like effects of cortisol (De Kloet et al., 1999; Lupien et al., 2005), depend on arousal, and context. In the present study we investigated whether administration of cortisol to healthy female subjects increases plasma oxytocin levels, and whether increased oxytocin levels are associated with decreased immediate free recall of unpleasant but not of pleasant words. We measured plasma ACTH levels to include as predictor in the analyses, as cortisol administration suppresses plasma ACTH levels (Tops et al., 2006, 2007c) and may affect memory and plasma oxytocin levels through this route as well (Grenbäck et al., 2007). For the same purpose we will measure vasopressin, which is also a cortisol releasing factor. Our first aim is to replicate the finding that cortisol administration increases plasma oxytocin levels. Our second aim is to investigate whether the cortisol-induced increase in oxytocin level is associated with decreased immediate free recall of unpleasant words.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-five healthy female paid volunteers aged 30–57 (M = 45, SD = 8) were enlisted by advertisement in a local newspaper. Each participant passed a health screening. Inclusion criteria included Dutch as their native language, regularly cycling, or using oral contraception, normal or corrected to normal vision, and no personal history of psychiatric, metabolic, or neurological disorders, or substance abuse. Volunteers who reported noxious health behaviors (drug abuse including excessive alcohol, smoking and caffeine, and abnormal sleeping habits, e.g., too little sleep), chronic health problems or psychopathology were excluded from the study. Our subjects self-reported the phase of their menstrual cycle based on days since onset of their last menstrual period, and active contraceptive use. All participants read and signed an informed consent statement approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center of Groningen.

Procedure

The subjects took part in a within-subjects double-blind design in which they were treated with either cortisol or placebo in two separate sessions, the order of which was counterbalanced and were separated by approximately 1 week. The participants arrived between 9:00 and 10:30. First, a capsule was ingested containing either cortisol (35 mg of hydrocortisone) or a placebo (avicel capsule). Participants were then allowed to read while they waited 70 min for a blood sampling. After that, the subjects performed the memory task which took 20 min to complete.

Memory task

Word lists

The to-be memorized pleasant and unpleasant nouns consisted of one to three syllables and were selected from two different databases. These words were rated in a pilot study for the degree of pleasantness of each word. Only words with relatively high emotional impact were used. The memory task consisted of two lists of pleasant words (e.g., “friendship”) and two lists of unpleasant words (e.g., “hate”). Each word list contained 11 words from only one valence category. We constructed two parallel versions of the memory task, of which the order of presentation was counterbalanced across sessions. All lists were matched for frequency of written use, imageability, and word length. The pleasant words were matched with the unpleasant words for emotional impact. Examples of pleasant and unpleasant words translate to English as love, fun, loyalty, regard and resentment, cancer, violence, malaise, respectively. The lists of words can be obtained from the first author.

Task

The to-be memorized words were presented on a 15′′ computer screen for 2 s and were immediately replaced by the next word. Words were selected from one list at a time and were presented in random order. After each list, the participants were given 90 s to write down as many words as they could remember from the list, in any order (Tops et al., 2003, 2004; Rijsdijk et al., 2009), before moving on to the next list. The order of the lists from which the words were selected was also randomized.

Plasma hormone levels

The blood samples were collected in ice-chilled tubes. After centrifugation, the plasma was removed, and the samples were stored at −80°C until analysis. Plasma concentrations of cortisol and ACTH were determined using an in-house radio immunoassay (Pratt, 1978). Vasopressin was extracted from the plasma using Sep-pak C 18 cartridges. The extracts were analyzed with a radioimmunoassay (MP-products, Amersfoort). These analyses were performed at the University Medical Center of Groningen. Before oxytocin was analyzed, plasma was extracted with acetone [Guarantee Reagent (GR); Merck, Darmstadt, Germany] and petroleum benzene (GR; boiling point range 40–60°C; Merck) with a recovery of 92%. Oxytocin levels were measured by radioimmunoassay at the Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences in Uppsala, Sweden, at the Department of Animal Physiology. The antibody KA19 was used for the analysis (Euro Diagnostica, Milab, Malmö, Sweden). The limit of detection was 4.68 pmol/l and interassay coefficients of variation low 22.06% CV = 9.57; medium 37.83% CV = 8.81; high 529.8% CV = 6.81; intrassay% CV < 10 conc = 20.90–1026 (Stock and Uvnäs-Moberg, 1985, 1988).

Statistical analyses

The effects of cortisol administration on cortisol, ACTH (i.e., ACTH suppression) and vasopressin (vasopressin suppression) levels were analyzed using a General Linear Model (GLM) analysis with Condition (placebo vs. cortisol) as within-subject factor. To investigate the effects of cortisol administration on oxytocin levels, taking into account possible effects through ACTH suppression (level in the cortisol condition minus level in the placebo condition), a GLM analysis of oxytocin levels was performed with Condition (placebo vs. cortisol) as within-subject factor and ACTH suppression as continuous predictor.

Finally, to investigate whether cortisol-induced changes in oxytocin level are associated with changes in recall performance, we performed a GLM analysis of recall with Condition and Valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant words) as within-subject factors and with the effect of cortisol administration on oxytocin level (level in the cortisol condition minus level in the placebo condition) as continuous predictor. Means (SD) of hormone levels and recall are presented, while effects of continuous predictors are presented as Pearson’s correlation coefficients.

Results

We tried potential confounders, e.g., age, menstrual cycle phase, oral contraceptive use, and the order of treatments, as covariates. No potential confounders noteworthy altered the results, and they were not included in the final analyses. Cortisol and oxytocin levels were natural-log transformed to better approximate a normal distribution. There were two missing values for ACTH levels, reducing the number of subjects for analyses involving ACTH to 33.

Cortisol, ACTH, and vasopressin

A GLM analysis of cortisol levels with Condition (placebo vs. cortisol) as within-subject factor showed a main effect of Condition [F(1,34) = 181.16, p < 0.000001]. Cortisol levels were higher in the cortisol condition compared to the placebo condition (Table 1). A GLM analysis of ACTH levels with Condition (placebo vs. cortisol) as within-subject factor showed a main effect of Condition (F(1,31) = 34.41, p < 0.00001). ACTH levels decreased in the cortisol treatment condition compared to the placebo condition (Table 1), reflecting a negative feedback mechanism. In contrast, there was no significant suppression effect of cortisol administration on vasopressin (F(1,34) = 2.41, p = 0.130) although levels were somewhat lower in the cortisol condition compared to the placebo condition (Table 1).

Table 1.

Mean plasma hormone levels (SD) per condition (placebo vs. cortisol treatment).

Placebo condition SD Cortisol condition SD
Cortisol (nmol/l) 255 118 1213 456
ACTH (ng/l) 18.21 9.29 12.43 6.51
Vasopressin (ng/l) 0.89 0.66 0.81 0.53
Oxytocin (nmol/l) 52 12 55 14

Oxytocin

A GLM analysis of oxytocin levels with the effect of cortisol administration on ACTH level (“ACTH suppression”; level in the cortisol condition minus level in the placebo condition) as predictor showed that plasma oxytocin levels increased in the cortisol treatment condition compared to the placebo condition (Table 1; F(1,31) = 12.28, p < 0.001). Additionally, there was an interaction between Condition and ACTH suppression (r = 0.58, F(1,31) = 15.48, p < 0.001): subjects who showed strong suppression of ACTH levels actually displayed a decrease in oxytocin levels (see Figure 1A). A similar analyses with the effect of cortisol administration on vasopressin level as predictor yielded no significant results.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Scatterplots of (A) cortisol administration-induced change in plasma ACTH level (N = 33) and (B) cortisol-induced change in recall negativity bias (N = 35), both (in (A) and (B)) as a function of cortisol-induced change in plasma oxytocin level. Levels from the placebo condition are subtracted from levels from the cortisol condition; positive values reflect increases. The recall negativity bias is the proportion unpleasant words recalled minus the proportion pleasant words recalled.

Memory performance

The mean proportion of words recalled was.64 (SD = 0.31). As we previously showed differential effects of cortisol administration on primacy (the first four words in each list) compared to recency (the last four words) recall (Tops et al., 2004), we analyzed primacy recall and recency recall separately. A GLM analysis of primacy recall with Condition and Valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant words) as within-subject factors and with the effect of cortisol administration on oxytocin level as predictor showed an interaction between Condition, Valence, and oxytocin level (F(1,33) = 8.84, p = 0.005). Following up this interaction by separate analyses of pleasant and unpleasant word primacy recall showed no effects for pleasant words, but unpleasant words recall displayed an interaction between Condition and oxytocin level [F(1,33) = 8.44, p = 0.007]. This cortisol-induced change in recall of unpleasant words was proportional to the change in oxytocin level: recall increased when oxytocin decreased and recall decreased when oxytocin increased (r = -0.45, p = 0.007). There was no effect on recall of pleasant words, as shown by the similar correlation between the change in oxytocin and the change in recall negativity bias (recall of unpleasant words minus recall of pleasant words; r = -0.46, p = 0.005; see Figure 1B). There were also no significant results regarding recency recall. When included in the analyses, ACTH, and vasopressin did not significantly predict recall performance.

Discussion

We showed that administration of cortisol increased plasma oxytocin levels, although subjects who showed strong suppression of ACTH levels actually displayed a decrease in oxytocin levels: cortisol appeared to induce both a decrease in oxytocin associated with ACTH suppression and an increase in oxytocin that was independent from ACTH suppression. The observed cortisol-induced increase in plasma oxytocin was associated with a decrease in immediate free recall of unpleasant words from primacy (first) positions, but did not relate to recall of pleasant words or of words from recency (last) positions in lists of words.

Both CRF and oxytocin are secreted from the same hypothalamic nuclei that regulate the HPA axis, and are modulated by the same hippocampal projections which modulate HPA axis activity (Risold and Swanson, 1996; Herman et al., 2002). Plasma oxytocin levels have been shown to be correlated positively with the integrity of the hippocampal-mediated HPA feedback mechanism (Goldman et al., 2008). Indeed, it has been proposed that cortisol-induced increases in oxytocin may be involved in the feedback regulation of the HPA axis (Liberzon and Young, 1997; Carter, 1998) and that there may be important interactions between cortisol-mediated stress and oxytocin-mediated attachment. Stressful experiences (such as pregnancy and parturition), anxiety, neophobia, isolation, and separation often precede the formation of social attachments. Correlations between glucocorticoid levels and attachment offer support for a role of glucocorticoids in the events which eventually lead to pair bonding (Carter, 1998; Henry and Wang, 1998). Lower plasma oxytocin levels during pregnancy have been found to predict postpartum depression and risk for developing postpartum depression, suggesting that increased oxytocin during pregnancy could prevent postpartum depression (Skrundz et al., 2011). In addition, both positive social interactions and social bonds could function to provide a sense of safety and reduce anxiety or stress. In contrast, negative or traumatic social interactions and rejection could feed back to decrease oxytocin levels, heighten fear or anxiety and prevent the formation of social interactions (Carter, 1998).

Results from Dabrowska et al. (2011) suggest that CRF type 2 receptors located on oxytocinergic neurons and axon terminals might regulate the release of oxytocin and hence might be a crucial part of a potential feedback loop between the hypothalamic oxytocin system and the forebrain CRF system that could significantly impact on affective and social behaviors, in particular during times of stress. Other studies suggest that oxytocin release in response to stress may be modulated in part by high circulating glucocorticoids and their negative feedback on the release of hypothalamic CRF (Chen and Du, 1999). However, the positive association in the present study between cortisol-induced change in ACTH levels and change in oxytocin levels suggests that the negative feedback on hypothalamic CRF that is mediated by cortisol did not increase oxytocin levels. Nevertheless, cortisol-mediated feedback on CRF may be involved in both the decrease in oxytocin associated with ACTH suppression and the increase in oxytocin that was independent from ACTH suppression.

Recall from primacy positions, but not recall from recency positions, has been shown to depend on the integrity of the hippocampal formation (Kesner, 1985; Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1990). Although the exact mechanism cannot be inferred from this study, the results suggest the possibility that cortisol administration impacts on oxytocin levels and memory through effects on the hippocampus, or that cortisol administration increases oxytocin levels which impact on memory performance. Moreover, negative feedback by cortisol on the HPA axis may be involved.

In the present study we failed to find any direct effects of cortisol administration on immediate recall. A review of the effects of cortisol on memory in humans concluded that fairly consistently over studies, stress, or cortisol treatment does not affect immediate recall but temporarily blocks retrieval of long-term memory, especially of emotional arousing material, most likely through reduction of activity of the hippocampus (Wolf, 2009). In addition, cortisol appears to only influence memory when arousal is relatively high (e.g., due to venipuncture such as in the present study; Tops et al., 2006). In contrast to the effects on retrieval, cortisol enhances memory consolidation (Wolf, 2009). Because in the present study cortisol was administered before learning, decreased immediate recall most likely reflects effects on acquisition or retrieval. Future studies will have to determine whether association between cortisol-induced oxytocin-changes and reduced immediate recall of unpleasant words involves direct effects of oxytocin on retrieval, and whether such effects of oxytocin on retrieval are also involved in the effects of cortisol on retrieval from long-term memory. Moreover, future studies should employ receptor blockade to demonstrate direct involvement of oxytocin in mediating cortisol effects on memory.

Several aspects define this study as preliminary, such as reliance on single samples for hormone determination per session, inclusion of women only, and inclusion of subjects in various phases of their menstrual cycle. Nevertheless, the correlation between the oxytocin levels of the placebo and cortisol treatment conditions despite cortisol administration (r = 0.45, p = 0.006) suggests reliability of oxytocin measurements. Consistent with larger studies employing between-subjects comparisons, we did not observe any relationships between menstrual cycle phase or oral contraceptive use and the present findings (Light et al., 2005). In contrast, studies employing a within-subject design report lower oxytocin levels in the luteal phase (Salonia et al., 2005), which suggests that menstrual cycle phase variations in oxytocin levels are small and might be easily masked by individual differences (Light et al., 2005).

The present results add to evidence that cortisol-induced increases in oxytocin could mediate some of the effects of stress and cortisol on memory, and may play a role in the regulation of HPA stress response. Cortisol-induced increases in oxytocin may facilitate stress coping and perhaps, by decreasing negative memory, decrease depressive responses to stress. This mechanism could significantly impact affective and social behaviors, in particular during times of stress.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

For reprints contact Mattie Tops, Centre for Child and Family Studies, University of Leiden, Post box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, the Netherlands topsm@fsw.leidenuniv.nl. Mattie Tops was supported by an award from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (Veni, NWO; www.nwo.nl). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  1. Carter C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Chen X. Q., Du J. Z. (1999). Hypoxia induces oxytocin release in the rat. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 20, 373–378 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Dabrowska J., Hazra R., Ahern T. H., Guo J. D., McDonald A. J., Mascagni F., Muller J. F., Young L. J., Rainnie D. G. (2011). Neuroanatomical evidence for reciprocal regulation of the corticotrophin-releasing factor and oxytocin systems in the hypothalamus and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of the rat: Implications for balancing stress and affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 1312–1326 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S., Joëls M. (1999). Stress and cognition: Are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends Neurosci. 22, 422–426 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01438-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Goldman M., Marlow-O’Connor M., Torres I., Carter C. S. (2008). Diminished plasma oxytocin in schizophrenic patients with neuroendocrine dysfunction and emotional deficits. Schizophr. Res. 98, 247–255 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Grenbäck E., Hulting A. L., Bucht E., Petersson M. (2007). Plasma galanin, vasopressin, and oxytocin in patients with Addison’s disease. Horm. Metab. Res. 39, 589–595 10.1055/s-2007-985130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Heinrichs M., Meinlschmidt G., Wippich W., Ehlert U., Hellhammer D. H. (2004). Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory. Physiol. Behav. 83, 31–38 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Henry J. P., Wang S. (1998). Effects of early stress on adult affiliative behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 863–875 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00058-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Herman J. P., Tasker J. G., Ziegler D. R., Cullinan W. E. (2002). Local circuit regulation of paraventricular nucleus stress integration glutamate-GABA connections. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 71, 457–468 10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00681-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Kalin N. H., Gibbs D. M., Barksdale C. M., Shelton S. E., Carnes M. (1985). Behavioral stress decreases plasma oxytocin concentrations in primates. Life Sci. 36, 1275–1280 10.1016/0024-3205(85)90272-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kesner R. P. (1985). Correspondence between humans and animals in coding of temporal attributes: role of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 444, 122–136 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37584.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Lee H. J., Macbeth A. H., Pagani J. H., Young W. S., III. (2009). Oxytocin: the great facilitator of life. Prog. Neurobiol. 88, 127–151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Liberzon I., Young E. A. (1997). Effects of stress and glucocorticoids on CNS oxytocin receptor binding. Psychoneuroendocrinology 22, 411–422 10.1016/S0306-4530(97)00044-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Light K. C., Grewen K. M., Amico J. A. (2005). More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biol. Psychol. 69, 5–21 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lupien S. J., Fiocco A., Wan N., Maheu F., Lord C., Schramek T., Tu M. T. (2005). Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 225–242 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Pratt J. J. (1978). Steroid immunoassay in clinical chemistry. Clin. Chem. 24, 1869–1890 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Rijsdijk F. V., Riese H., Tops M., Snieder H., Brouwer W. H., Smid H. G. O. M., Ormel J. (2009). Neuroticism, recall bias and attention bias for valenced probes: a twin study. Psychol. Med. 39, 45–54 10.1017/S0033291708003231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Rimmele U., Hediger K., Heinrichs M., Klaver P. (2009). Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J. Neurosci. 29, 38–42 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4260-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Risold P. Y., Swanson L. W. (1996). Structural evidence for functional domains in the rat hippocampus. Science 272, 1484–1486 10.1126/science.272.5267.1484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Salonia A., Nappi R. E., Pontillo M., Daverio R., Smeraldi A., Briganti A., Fabbri F., Zanni G., Rigatti P., Montorsi F. (2005). Menstrual cycle-related changes in plasma oxytocin are relevant to normal sexual function in healthy women. Horm. Behav. 47, 164–169 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Silber M., Almkvist O., Larsson B., Uvnäs-Moberg K. (1990). Temporary peripartal impairment in memory and attention and its possible relation to oxytocin concentration. Life Sci. 47, 57–65 10.1016/0024-3205(90)90566-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Skrundz M., Bolten M., Nast I., Hellhammer D. H., Meinlschmidt G. (2011). Plasma oxytocin concentration during pregnancy is associated with development of postpartum depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1886–1893 10.1038/npp.2011.74 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Stock S., Uvnäs-Moberg K. (1985). Oxytocin infusions increase plasma levels of insulin and VIP but not of gastrin in conscious dogs. Acta Physiol. Scand. 125, 205–210 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1985.tb07708.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Stock S., Uvnäs-Moberg K. (1988). Increased plasma levels of oxytocin in response to afferent electrical stimulation in sciatic and vagal nerves and in response to touch and pinch in anaesthetized rats. Acta Physiol. Scand. 132, 29–34 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1988.tb08294.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Tops M., Boksem M. A. S., Wijers A. A., van Duinen H., Den Boer J. A., Meijman T. F., Korf J. (2007a). The psychobiology of burnout: are there two different syndromes? Neuropsychobiology 55, 143–150 10.1159/000106056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Tops M., van Peer J. M., Korf J. (2007b). Individual differences in emotional expressivity predict oxytocin responses to cortisol administration: relevance to breast cancer? Biol. Psychol. 75, 119–123 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Tops M., van Peer J. M., Wijers A. A., Korf J., Tucker D. M. (2007c). Anxiety, cortisol and attachment predict plasma oxytocin. Psychophysiology 44, 444–449 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00510.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Tops M., van der Pompe G., Baas D., Mulder L. J. M., den Boer J. A., Meijman T. F., Korf J. (2003). Acute cortisol effects on immediate free recall and recognition of nouns depend on stimulus valence. Psychophysiology 40, 167–173 10.1111/1469-8986.00018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Tops M., van der Pompe G., Wijers A. A., den Boer J. A., Meijman T. F., Korf J. (2004). Free recall of pleasant words from recency positions is especially sensitive to acute administration of cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 327–338 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00032-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Tops M., van Peer J. M., Wester A. E., Wijers A. A., Korf J. (2006). State-dependent regulation of cortical activity by cortisol: an EEG study. Neurosci. Lett. 404, 39–43 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.05.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Uvnäs-Moberg K. (1998). Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819–835 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00056-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Wolf O. T. (2009). Stress and memory in humans: twelve years of progress? Brain Res. 1293, 142–154 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Young L. J. (2002). The neurobiology of social recognition, approach, and avoidance. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 18–26 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01268-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Zola-Morgan S. M., Squire L. R. (1990). The primate hippocampal formation: evidence for a time-limited role in memory storage. Science 250, 288–290 10.1126/science.2218534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES