Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 May 18.
Published in final edited form as: J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011 Mar 11;66(Suppl 1):i26–i35. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr013

A life course approach to the development of mental skills

Marcus Richards 1, Stephani L Hatch 2
PMCID: PMC3355296  EMSID: UKMS47870  PMID: 21398418

Abstract

A wide variety of factors across the life course jointly influence cognitive and emotional development. Indeed, research from a variety of disciplines strongly suggests that cognition and mental health are intertwined across the life course, by their common antecedents and underlying physiology in development, and in their interplay across adult and later life. We suggest that cognitive and socio-emotional function fuse to form skills for life supporting self-regulation, competence and quality of life that persist into later life, through linked reciprocal processes of genetic influence, nurturing, schooling, work, and lifestyle.

Keywords: cognitive, emotional, life course, skills, self-regulation, mastery, wisdom

A. Introduction

Research from a variety of disciplines shows that cognition and mental health are intertwined across the life course, by their common antecedents and underlying physiology in development, and their interplay across adult and later life. We suggest that cognitive and socio-emotional function gradually fuse to form skills for life, with implications for mental and physical health in ageing, as implied by Rutter a quarter of a century ago:

“… what is needed for optimal cognitive development is a combination of active learning experiences that promote cognitive competence together with a social context in which the style of interaction and relationships promotes self-confidence and an active interest in seeking to learn independently of instruction.” (Rutter, 1985, p. 699).

This echoes earlier views still; the sociologists Bowles and Gintis (1976) were among the first to highlight the importance of ‘non-cognitive’ skills (e.g. conscientiousness, tenacity) in educational and occupational achievement; this has gained currency in other disciplines, particularly economics (e.g. Heckman & Rubinstein, 2001). As we will attempt to show, this fusion of cognitive and emotional processes has long-term resonance for the development of human agency. This article is not specifically oriented to the life course origins of cognitive decline and psychiatric illness (Whalley, Dick & McNeill, 2006; Rutter, Kim-Cohen & Maughan, 2006). Rather, we are concerned with a life course approach to the development of competence and quality of life, which we regard as returns to life skills.

With regard to terminology, life course epidemiology, which investigates the independent effects or interactive effects of intrauterine, childhood, adolescent and adult risk and protective factors on adult health and disease (Kuh, Ben Shlomo, Lynch, Hallqvist & Power, 2003), borrows concepts from lifespan psychology and life course sociology traditions. The former refers to individual development (ontogenesis) of biologically-based capabilities, in this context those of higher mental function; whereas the latter addresses the embedding of individual lives into social structures over the lifetime (Mayer, 2003). We should note however that the issue of social change is beyond our present reach. The historian Gillian Sutherland (2010) vividly illustrates the complexities of social and policy-led changes since World War 2 in a single structure (education) in a single country (the UK) and its differing regions, in a manner suggesting that the assimilation of cohort effects into the present article would be a formidable task.

B. Early influences on cognitive and emotional development

1. Genetic influence

As O’Donovan and Owen (2009) note, “with its role in human adaptability and survival, it would be remarkable if traits that result from variation in brain function were not influenced in part by genes”. In fact evidence suggests that the brain is at its most environmentally plastic in very young childhood, with heritability of general cognitive ability at approximately 30% at this stage, rising over the life course to as much as 80% in older adults (Deary, Johnson & Houlihan, 2009). This increase may partly result from a gradual matching to environment, i.e. a reciprocal causation between cognitive ability and environment leading to gene-environment correlation (Dickens & Flynn, 2001; Rodgers & Wänström, 2007); and also from increasing genetic influence on neural processes with age (Lenroot et al., 2009). Later still in the life course, the operation of neural defense and repair mechanisms, themselves under genetic control, may become important as the brain accumulates insults (Deary et al., 2009). In comparison, the heritability of major depressive disorder is estimated to be approximately 50%, with around 55% of the genetic risk shared with the dimensional trait of neuroticism (Levinson, 2006). Like cognition, there is some evidence that the heritability of emotional problems increases with age, explained in part by an increase in gene-environment correlation through negative life events (Rice, Harold & Thapar, 2003).

In additions to any effects of the DNA sequence, genetic influence on mental development also occurs through epigenetic alteration of gene expression during interaction with the environment. In animal studies, offspring of high-nurturing mothers (in terms of licking or grooming) tend to have relatively low levels of anxiety (Gottlieb, Wahlsten & Lickliter, 2006; Meany & Szyf (2005). They also show an attenuated hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis response to stress, and higher levels of glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in the hippocampus (with parallel implications for cognition), a difference in methylation that persists across the life course. This epigenetic alteration almost certainly occurs in humans, and information is accumulating about the way this affects risk of neurocognitive disorders (Urdinguio, Sanchez & Esteller, 2009). For example, parental age shows an inverted-U relationship with cognitive development (Malaspina et al., 2005), where the effect of older fatherhood in particular is thought to reflect de novo mutations or abnormal methylation of paternally imprinted genes. Parental age is also positively associated with risk of psychiatric disorder (Durkin et al., 2008). In both cases these effects are of pubic health concern in view of steadily increasing mean parental age in middle and high income countries (Durkin et al., 2008).

Looking ahead to old age, epigenetic alteration has been implied or implicated in the development of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Deary et al., 2002; Wang, Oelze & Schumacher, 2008; Chouliaras et al., 2010), although it is currently unclear when in the life course such epigenetic drift begins.

2. Influence of the uterine environment

Neural development begins soon after conception, and by the time of birth head circumference, an index of brain volume, is far closer to its final size than the rest of the body. A traditional focus in life course epidemiology, most commonly linked to the work of David Barker, is the effect of fetal growth on mature health. Birth weight is positively and independently associated with cognitive development in the general population (Shenkin, Starr & Deary, 2004), almost certainly because endocrine systems target areas of the brain that underlie mental function while simultaneously driving skeletal growth (Berger, 2001). However, this does not appear to have long-term consequences for cognitive aging (Richards, Hardy, Kuh & Wadsworth, 2001), although there is evidence of an inverse association between birth weight and risk of late life depression, independently of childhood and adult social circumstances (Thompson, Syddall, Rodin, Osmond & Barker, 2001).

The extent to which fetal growth mediates the effect of maternal exposures during pregnancy on offspring cognitive and emotional development is unclear. However, a range of such exposures are themselves linked to mental development, including aspects of the maternal hormonal milieu, such as the positive and negative influence, respectively, of insulin-like growth factors (Gunnell, Miller, Rogers & Holly, 2005) and elevated cortisol (LeWinn et al., 2009). The latter is associated with maternal stress, itself a predictor of offspring emotional and cognitive problems (Talge, Neal & Glover, 2007). Maternal morbid conditions such as gestational diabetes and hypertensive disorders are negatively associated with offspring cognitive function, although this may be partly because they are risk factors for preterm birth (Chatzi et al., 2009). However, greater attention has been given to agents thought to be teratogenic, particularly tobacco and alcohol. These are negatively associated with cognitive and behavioral development, although confounding by maternal IQ (Batty, Der & Deary, 2006) and other inherited factors (Thapar & Rutter, 2009) are serious challenges in this research, and many prenatal exposures also persist into the postnatal environment, so that their precise timing is not always easy to identify. Such caveats almost certainly apply to maternal nutrition, in spite of the PERLIP Working Group and the Early Nutrition Programming Project conclusion that (Koletzko, Cetin & Brenna, 2007) intake of fish or other sources of long-chain omega-3 fatty acids is positively associated with offspring cognitive development.

We should note, however, that availability of micronutrients critical to mental development is deficient in many geographical regions, as documented by the International Child Development Steering Group (Walker et al., 2007). In particular, iron deficiency anaemia, which can lead to behavioural disturbance (Golub, Hogrefe, Germann, Capitanio & Lozof, 2006), is one of the most prevalent forms of malnutrition in the world; and iodine deficiency is the most common preventable cause of learning disability (Walker et al., 2007).

3. Early childhood development

While the most rapid neural development occurs during fetal growth, dramatic development of this nature also occurs during first five years of postnatal life. At this time environmental factors, including nutrients, play a critical role in the ‘blooming and pruning’ of the cytoarchitecture of the brain (Levitt, 2003). This leads us into postnatal life itself.

Maturation

Skeletal growth continues to be positively associated with cognitive development, independently of birth weight (e.g. Richards, Hardy, Kuh & Wadsworth, 2002). Neural development during infancy, as marked for example by growing head circumference (Gale, O’Callaghan, Bredow & Martyn, 2006) and motor milestone attainment (Murray, Jones, Kuh & Richards, 2007), are associated with cognitive development; milestone attainment is also associated with emotional and behavioral problems at the population level (Liu, Sun, Neiderhiser, Uchiyama & Okawa, 2001), although more evidence of this kind is required. The emerging physical health of the child also becomes an important influence on mental development (e.g. Pless, Cripps, Davies & Wadsworth, 1989); in this way cognition may represent an “archaeological type record” of a range of early insults (Whalley & Deary, 2001). Nutrition, of course, plays an essential role in mental development at this stage. Breast feeding benefits cognitive development (Anderson, Johnstone & Remley, 1999), with inconsistent evidence of parallel effects on emotional development (Allen, Lewinsohn & Seely, 1998; Robinson et al., 2008; Waylen, Ford, Goodman, Samara & Wolke, 2009). The dominant explanation is that benefit results from essential long chain fatty acids, although it should be emphasized that the association is strongly confounded by maternal IQ (Richards et al., 1998; Der, Batty & Deary, 2006) and in any case does not appear to track very strongly over the life course (Richards, Hardy & Wadsworth, 2002). There are also non-nutritional reasons why breast feeding may be associated with mental health, such as reciprocity and closeness (Dignam, 1995).

Socialization

The world of most infants and children centers on the family, above all on the caregiver. The classic writings of Harlow, Bowlby, Ainsworth and others emphasize the fundamental importance of mother-infant attachment in early life, and the long-term impact on psychopathology of insecure attachment or loss of the caregiver. The theoretical approach of Bowlby recapitulates the role of genetics, since, in collaboration with ethologists such as Hinde, he emphasized the evolutionary imperative of attachment formation for survival. The security of this attachment requires intense reciprocal regulation and organization (Cairns & Cairns, 2006). A well-studied manifestation of this is the use of games such as peek-a-boo and hide-and-seek, which help to provide a ‘scaffold’ for language structure and function (Ratner & Bruner, 1978) while playing a role in the development of emotional self-regulation (Stifter & Moyer, 1991). In fact this reciprocity characterizes human development across the life course (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006), a topic to which we shall return. It is in this context that we should note that separation from a caregiver through parental divorce is one of the most common stressors faced by children in Western cultures (Maughan & McCarthy, 1997), and is associated with lower academic achievement and poorer emotional adjustment (Amato and Keith, 1991; Amato, 1994). This is a complex stressor, usually involving parental distress, financial difficulties, possible relocation, and the introduction of new family members (Maughan & McCarthy, 1997; Fors, Lennartsson & Lundberg, 2009).

Linked and reciprocal interactions between caregiver and child require some discussion of socialization, defined as the manner in which individuals selectively acquire skills, knowledge, values, motives, behaviors and roles (Bush & Simmons, 1992). Socialization processes occur within multiple domains over the life course, including school, peers, neighbourhood, and workplace, but are of course shaped by the family, as well as the caregiver’s families of origin, the immediate social environment, and their social status positions based on gender, class, and race or ethnicity. For example, racial and ethnic socialization involves an intergenerational transfer of attitudes, beliefs, and experiences that are likely to include themes important for exposure to unfair treatment, such as promotion of mistrust (i.e., encouraging social distance from and wariness of the dominant group) and preparation for racial bias (Hughes et al., 2006).

An important aspect of socialization is parenting style. The influential model of parenting proposed by Baumrind (e.g. 1991) distinguishes three styles: authoritative (high expectation of compliance with rules; open dialogue; and a child-centered approach characterized by warmth and involvement); authoritarian (high use of coercive discipline; low use of open dialogue; with a high control-low trust parent-centered approach); and permissive (low expectations; lack of control; and inconsistent approaches to discipline). Evidence suggests that the authoritative style is most associated with the development of competence and adjustment, where autonomy-granting is particularly important (McLeod, Wood & Weisz, 2007). Conversely, authoritarian and permissive parenting hinders the development of competence and self-regulation. This again raises the issue of interaction andreciprocity, first suggested in this context by Sameroff (1975), and more recently by the circular model of Kuczynski (2003). The ‘transactional loop’ between parent and child can take the form of a ‘vicious circle’, for example between a strained parent and an aggressive child (Patterson, Reid & Dishion, 1998), as well as a positive recursive process. Of course there are non-linear effects on cognitive development of very poor or extreme exposures, as suggested for example by Turkheimer and Gottesman (1991) and by Rutter (1985), and there is little question that the most extreme manifestations of negative parenting, severe neglect and physical and sexual abuse, are major risk factors for severe long-term psychiatric disorder (Anda et al., 2006; McLaughlin et al., 2010).

Family and neighborhood environment

The socioeconomic environment of the family has an important distal influence on cognitive and socio-emotional development. Chronic poverty is associated with lower cognitive performance and more behavioral problems in children, and later poverty tends to be more detrimental than early poverty (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2005). Indeed chronic economic hardship over the life course is associated with depression and self-reported cognitive difficulties in late middle age and early old age (Lynch, Kaplan & Shame, 1997). Earlier in the life course effects may be partly mediated by negative parenting, since poverty also causes maternal depression, although in fact these independently impair cognitive development and emotional wellbeing of children (Kiernan & Huerta, 2008). Other mechanisms through which material poverty can impair mental development include elevated exposure to acute and chronic stressors (McLoyd, 1998). It is long known that overcrowding can lead to autonomic and neuroendocrine dysregulation, and contemporary work tends to confirm the negative effect of this on cognitive and emotional development (Evans, 2006). In fact a long-term evaluation of early childhood care and education programs in the USA found that, while Head Start participation was associated with higher reading and math skills through the school years (in girls), the home environment had a larger impact (Joo, 2010).

There are additional distal effects of the neighborhood on school achievement and mental health (Aneshensel & Sucof, 1996), independently of individual and family-level characteristics (Ross, 2000), although negative mental health effects tend to be expressed more as externalizing behaviors than as poor emotional adjustment (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Potential mechanisms include quality of services (e.g. education, health, transport, recreation, retail); control of noxious or hazardous exposures (e.g. noise, pollution, street traffic, crime); and more subtle factors such as community responsibility for individuals (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Evans, 2006). Combined deprivation in all these respects may explain the striking decline in IQ between ages six to 11 years urban children in Detroit, USA, after controlling for key factors such as maternal IQ and education; whereas those of suburban children in the same metropolitan area remained relatively unchanged (Breslau et al., 2001). Importantly, these two study areas are strongly segregated by race; analysis suggests that severely disadvantaged neighborhoods can reduce later verbal ability in African-American children to a degree equivalent to missing a year or more of schooling (Sampson, Sharkey & Raudenbush, 2008). This is almost certain to have a long-term impact on cognitive aging, and there is also little question of the deleterious impact of material and physical environments on adult mental health (March et al., 2008; Kim, 2008), with some evidence of accumulation in this respect (Wheaton & Clarke, 2003).

C. Self-regulation in development and the emergence of skills for life

1. The concept of self-regulation

In considering the above early influences on the twin outcomes of cognitive and emotional development, some factors appear to shape one of these outcomes more than the other, yet most operate as common-cause factors. Through these common causes begins the intertwining of cognitive and emotional processes that persists into old age, with important implications for competence and quality of life. It is now necessary to look more closely at this process of entwinement itself. A reference point is the concept of self-regulation, defined as “self-generated thoughts, feelings and actions that are planned and cyclically adapted to the attainment of personal goals” (Zimmerman, 2000, p. 14). This reflects the neuropsychological construct of executive function, based on specific skills underlying goal-oriented behavior such as self-initiation and inhibition, and mental structuring and set-switching, which is conventionally thought of as cognition. Yet it also implies interpersonal feeling and functions, such as belonging, social engagement and respect for others, that arise from socio-emotional development and underpin psychological wellbeing (Duckworth, Akerman, MacGregor, Salter, & Vorhaus, 2009). Integrating these two strands, Schunk and Ertmer (2000) suggest that self-regulated learning involves:

“setting goals for learning, attending to and concentrating on instruction, using effective strategies to organize, code and rehearse information to be remembered, establishing a productive work environment, using resources effectively, monitoring performance, managing time effectively, seeking assistance when needed, holding positive beliefs about one’s capabilities, the value of learning, the factors influencing learning and the anticipated outcomes of actions, and experiencing pride and satisfactions with one’s efforts” (Schunk & Ertmer, 2000, p. 631).

This echoes the quote by Rutter (1985) at the beginning of this article, and captures the emerging theme of agency. This emergence could be seen as the development of human capital, with non-cognitive behaviors as important productive assets as cognitive skills (Farkas, 2003). This view does not of course take into account difficulties or even failure in establishing self-regulation and the long-term consequences of this. Clearly lack of inhibition in terms of poor impulse control is a feature of slower socio-emotional development. This may be seen in childhood conduct problems; these have a widespread and serious adverse impact on life chances, from skill formation to economic attainment and social functioning (Richards & Abbott, 2009), with negative implications for mental aging. How do these processes unfold over the life course? One clue is in the life course continuity in cognitive and emotional functions themselves.

2. Continuity and cross-linkage in cognition and mental health

Cognitive ability in childhood (Deary, Whalley, Lemmon, Crawford, & Starr, 2000; Richards & Sacker, 2003) and early adulthood (Plassman et al., 1995; Snowdon et al., 1996) correlates highly with cognitive ability in midlife and old age. To some extent this is a matter of tracking, since this correlation is observed even when the influence of education and father’s and own occupation are controlled (Richards & Sacker, 2003). However, as we shall see there is little question that education and occupation themselves contribute to this continuity through recursive processes.

Emotional problems in adolescence also show continuity into adulthood (Clark, Rodgers, Caldwell, Power, & Stansfeld, 2008). Rutter et al. (2006) estimate that depressed adolescents have 2-7 times the odds of being depressed in adulthood, with 40-70% showing major depressive disorder during this phase of the life course. Indeed, in the British 1946 birth cohort 71% of those with emotional problems in adulthood also showed evidence of these problems in adolescence; conversely, only 14% of those who showed these problems in adolescence reported no emotional problems in adulthood (Colman, Ploubidis, Wadsworth, Jones & Croudace, 2007). Prospective evidence also suggests lifetime continuity in psychological wellbeing (Richards & Huppert, in press). Rutter et al. (2006) review possible mechanisms that drive or mediate negative mental health continuity. These include increasing gene-environment correlation with age (as previously highlighted, and applying equally to cognition); and the effect of cognitive attributional biases that increase vulnerability to depression (Abramson et al., 2002) and mediate the association between depressive symptoms and subjective memory problems in older people (Crane, Bogner, Brown & Gallo, 2007). We should also refer to life course continuity in personality, which increases with age (Caspi & Roberts, 2001), and is thought to be maintained by a combination of genetic influence, environmental stability, and person-environment interactions (Willis & Blaskewicz Boron, 2008). Of particular relevance to this context is neuroticism, which arguably represents long-term predisposition to emotional stress (Wilson et al., 2005).

Finally, in addition to their own continuities cognitive and emotional processes also show longitudinal cross-linkage. Lower cognitive ability in childhood or early adulthood is associated with increased risk of emotional problems in midlife (Richards et al., 2001; Zammit et al., 2004; Hatch et al., 2007; Koenen et al., 2009), which in turn are associated with cognitive impairment in later life (e.g. Steffens et al., 2006). Neuroticism also shows long-term linkage to later cognitive function (Willis & Blaskewicz Boron, 2008); these authors raise the possible mediating role of health behaviors, although this association may also reflect a long-standing correlation between the stable aspects of these traits since childhood (Gale, Deary, Kuh, Huppert & Richards, 2010). What broader processes might support this linkage? We briefly referred to the role of education and occupation in the context of cognitive continuity (Richards & Sacker, 2003), and have referred throughout to reciprocity. Drawing together these strands, at least three reciprocal life domains are relevant: education, work, and leisure, themselves interdependent through ‘linked lives’ (Elder, 1994).

3. The role of education

Education is the first major socializing institution outside the family, albeit with more narrowly defined goals in this respect (Hatch & March, 2010). Cognition is an important determinant of educational achievement (Deary, Strand, Smith & Fernandes, 2006), yet education itself can augment cognitive skills independently of prior cognitive ability (Snow & Yallow, 1982; Hernstein & Murray, 1994; Richards & Sacker, 2003; Hatch, Feinstein, Link, Wadsworth, & Richards, 2007). Human capital theory again provides a model for this, focusing on the way in which schooling teaches specific knowledge, teaches practical skills for the workplace, refines other cognitive skills, socializes the individual for success, and shapes confidence, motivation and other aspects of self-regulation discussed above (Kohn & Slomczynski, 1993). As with parenting, this should not be seen purely in terms of input; as well as having a clear focus on academic goals, effective classroom management, and adequate but discriminating use of classroom teaching and motivational techniques, schools that successfully promote academic achievement tend to encourage student participation in, and responsibility for, the running of school life (Rutter, 1985). These aspects of the classroom may also an important long-term determinant of racial disparities in cognitive function. In a large sample of older community-dwelling people African Americans scored lower than Whites on a range of verbal and nonverbal tests, even though these groups were matched for years of education; yet these differences were largely explained by reading level, which these authors suggest reflects quality rather than quantity of schooling (Manly, Jacobs, Touradji, Small & Stern, 2002). In this context there are large historical racial inequalities in the provision of schooling itself in the USA (Glynmour & Manly, 2008), in the type of schooling within which an individual is enrolled, and in educational attainment (Kao & Thompson, 2003). The long-term effect of these inequalities on skills for - and quality of - life is therefore a matter of particular concern.

Beyond these processes that are internal to the institution, status attainment is influenced by how concordant or discordant the expectations of educators, families, and peers become, particularly during the school to work transition. Education also provides a readily identifiable credential that selects the individual into the workforce and stratifies adult socioeconomic status (Collins, 1979), with significant consequences for cognitive aging. This credential also signals to employers (Rosenbaum et al., 1990) that the individual possesses the very qualities of self-regulation described by Schunk & Ertmer (2000) above. There are almost certainly racial disparities in the signaling power of a particular credential too; based on 2008 USA Census Bureau statistics, mean income returns to a bachelor’s degree for African American and Hispanic males were nearly 30% lower than those for Whites (Williams, Mohammed, Leavell & Collins, 2010).

4. Work and leisure

With regard to work itself, the longitudinal study of Kohn & Schooler (1983) showed that, echoing education, while cognitive ability is a determinant of intellectually demanding work, work complexity is also beneficial to cognitive function. This was replicated elsewhere by Hauser & Roan (2007), with the important additional control for adolescent cognitive ability. Of particular importance from the life course perspective, the latter effect appears to be greater for older compared to younger workers, possibly because of the reduction in routine, and the growing reliance on occupational self-direction with age and experience (Schooler, Mulatu & Oates, 1999). These authors also raise the possibility, to our knowledge not yet resolved, of an additional recursive process whereby higher cognitive ability leads to longer time spent in the labor force, further increasing its benefit to subsequent cognitive function. Parallel phenomena are observed for leisure activities, whether complex and intellectually challenging (e.g. number or books and magazines read and their intellectual level; Schooler & Mulatu, 2001) or physical exercise (Richards, Hardy & Wadsworth, 2003; Richards, Stephens & Mishra, 2010). Consistent with this, a bidirectional association between sedentary lifestyle and depression is observed in adolescents and older people (Barbour & Blumenthal, 2005; Ortega, Ruiz, Castillo & Sjös, 2008; Roshanaei-Moghaddam, Katon & Russo, 2009). What are the broader implications of this kind of reciprocity? We have referred to the emergence of self-organization; we now turn to a related concept with developmental origins yet strong implications for mental aging: mastery.

5. Mastery and wisdom

The construct of mastery was developed by Pearlin (e.g. Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). This refers to the ability to manage life circumstances, and to control those circumstances that significantly impact the individual, and thus reflects the dynamics of self-regulated learning. It arises partly from status achievement, which in turn confers a greater sense of control and self-direction; and partly out of successful coping with stressors across the life course, although of course these two strands are themselves inter-connected. Mastery can develop relatively early in the life course, for example through effective management of problems within interpersonal roles such as relationship formation and child-rearing (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). On the other hand mastery declines with age, and can be impaired by exposure to difficult conditions in salient areas of life that are resistant to personal control, for example job loss through bankruptcy of an employer, or the demands on caregiving from a family member with severe limiting illness (Pearlin, Nguyen, Schieman & Milkie, 2007). Importantly, even where the construct of mastery appears to have universal underlying meaning, the criteria by which particular relevant skills are valued can vary widely, and should be considered in reference to the occupation of social statuses and changing social context. For example, the skills that are necessary for a low-income person to survive are very different to those deployed by a high-income individual to signal status (Farkas, 2003). Success or otherwise in mastering challenge also has long-term implications; in a large community-dwelling sample of older people an association between past circumstances (educational and SES attainment, and various stressors) and current mastery was found to be mediated by the belief of the individual to have directed and managed the trajectories that connect their past to their present (Pearlin et al., 2007).

In considering mastery the emphasis seems to be on skill, competence and control; however, the reciprocal interaction of these processes with emotion may lead to the development of wisdom as defined by Kramer (1990), expressed in problem solving, ability to advise others, engage in management of social institutions, life decision-making, and spiritual reflection. As also defined by Baltes (e.g. Baltes & Staudinger, 1993), wisdom involves rich factual and procedural knowledge, but also three broader aspects of maturity: lifespan contextualizing; value relativism and tolerance; and the recognition and management of uncertainty, including the limits of one’s own knowledge. In terms of implicit theory, wisdom is generally assumed to be a characteristic of older age, although this is not always supported by empirical evidence (Staudinger, 1999; Webster, 2003). As Sternberg & Lubart (2001) note, “People become wiser at a given age with respect to the problems that confront them at that point in their lives” (p. 504).

Baltes & Staudinger (1993) suggest a research framework for the life course antecedents of wisdom, integrating individual characteristics such as cognition and mental health, experiential factors such as coping with life problems (similar to the way in which mastery may evolve from the need to overcome adversity as well as succeed during advantage, as acknowledged above), and socioeconomic factors such as education and occupation. Wisdom is explicitly seen as a “fine-tuned coordination of cognition, motivation and emotion”, which, again like mastery, integrates past, present and future. We might regard this process as akin to that of contemporary notions of wellbeing, although in the eudemonic sense of self-realization and purposeful engagement rather than the hedonic sense of happiness and life satisfaction (see Ryan & Deci, 2001). At their best these processes may evolve into generativity, a sense of optimism as skills for life are passed from one generation to the next. Yet wisdom can have negative aspects, for example when it generates a burden of responsibility, apprehension, and over-concern for the common good (Coleman & O’Hanlon, 2004) It can also become impaired by the same kinds of stressful events over which individuals have little control that hinder mastery (Meacham, 1990).

D. Conclusions

The central message of this article is that cognitive and socio-emotional function are intertwined across the life course, and, we suggest, fuse to form skills for life supporting self-regulation, competence and quality of life that persist into later life. In attempting to understand the entwining of cognition and emotion over the life course, and the consequent emergence of competence, we have followed a thread connecting reciprocal phenomena that run from genes to environmentally altered gene expression to genetic matching to environment; through proximal and distal effects of environments themselves: caregiver, family, neighborhood, school, workplace, and the choices and effects of adult lifestyle. However, it is equally important to acknowledge that the formation of skills for life within this framework is not a universal, homogenous process; many components of this chain represent points at which skill formation can become impaired, with negative implications for mental aging. This raises the question of intervention. We began our story in early life, where developmental processes are at their most malleable. Thus Heckman, who himself recognized that cognitive, linguistic, social and emotional skills are interdependent, estimated that financial returns to investment in disadvantaged children are greatest when this investment occurs in early life (Heckman, 2006). Our concern is with the returns of competence and quality of life to cognitive and emotional skills, which we argue may require a more encompassing view across the life course in regard to optimizing the development of these skills. This is reflected in life course models of abnormal mental development and aging (e.g. Richards & Deary, 2005; Whalley et al., 2006; Rutter et al., 2006), but applies with equal force to normative processes, and the transactional emergence of human agency.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Diana Kuh and Leonard Pearlin for constructive criticism during the preparation of this article.

Funding This work was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MR); and the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Kings College London Institute of Psychiatry (SH).

References

  1. Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Hankin BL, Haeffel GJ, MacCoon DG, Gill BE. Cognitive-vulnerability-stress models of depression in a self -regulatory and psychological context. In: Gotlib IH, Hammen CL, editors. Handbook of Depression. Guilford Press; New York: 2002. pp. 268–294. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen NB, Lewinsohn PM, Seely JR. Prenatal and perinatal influences on risk for psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychopathology. 1998;10:513–529. doi: 10.1017/s0954579498001722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amato PR. Life-span adjustment of children to their parents’ divorce. The Future of Children. 1994;4:143–164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amato PR, Keith B. Parental divorce and adult well-being: a meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1991;53:43–58. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, Dube SR, Giles WH. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;256:174–186. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anderson JA, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1999;70:525–35. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/70.4.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aneshensel CS, Sucoff CA. The neighborhood context of adolescent mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:293–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. The search for a psychology of wisdom. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1993;2:75–80. [Google Scholar]
  9. Barbour KA, Blumenthal JA. Exercise training and depression in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005;26(suppl. 1):119–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Batty GD, Der G, Deary IJ. Effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring’s cognitive ability: empirical evidence for complete confounding in the US national longitudinal survey of youth. Pediatrics. 2006;118:943–50. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Baumrind D. The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1991;11:56–95. [Google Scholar]
  12. Berger A. Insulin-like growth factor and cognitive function. British Medical Journal. 2001;322:203. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7280.203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bowles S, Gintis H. Schooling in capitalist America. Basic Books; New York: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  14. Breslau N, Chilcoat HD, Susser ES, Matter T, Liang K-Y, Peterson EL. Stability and change in children’s intelligence quotient scores: a comparison of two socioeconomically disparate communities. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;154:711–717. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.8.711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The bioecological model of human development. In: Lerner RM, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th edn Vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ: pp. 793–828. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bush DM, Simmons RG. Socialisation processes over the life course. In: Rosenberg M, Turner RH, editors. Social Psychology: sociological perspectives. 2nd edn Transaction Publishers; London: 1992. pp. 133–164. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cairns RB, Cairns B,D. The making of developmental psychology. In: Lerner RM, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th edn Vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ: 2006. pp. 89–165. [Google Scholar]
  18. Caspi A, Roberts BW. Personality development across the life course: the argument for change and continuity. Psychological Inquiry. 2001;12:49–66. [Google Scholar]
  19. Chatzi L, Plana E, Daraki V, Karakosta P, Alegkakis D, Tsatsanis C, Kafatos A, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Metabolic syndrome in early pregnancy and risk of preterm birth. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;170:829–36. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chouliaras L, Rutten BP, Kenis G, Peerbooms O, Visser PJ, Verhey F, van Os J, Steinbusch HW, van den Hove DL. Epigenetic regulation in the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease. Progress in Neurobiology. 90:498–510. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clark C, Rodgers B, Caldwell T, Power P, Stansfeld S. Childhood and adulthood psychological ill health as predictors of midlife affective and anxiety disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;64:668–678. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Colman I, Ploubidis GB, Wadsworth MEJ, Jones PB, Croudace TJ. A longitudinal typology of symptoms of depression and anxiety over the life course. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62:1265–1271. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Coleman PG, O’Hanlon A. Ageing and development: theories and research. Arnold; London: 2004. pp. 54–66. [Google Scholar]
  24. Collins R. The credential society: an historical sociology of education and stratification. Academic Press; New York: 1979. [Google Scholar]
  25. Crane MK, Bogner HR, Brown GK, Gallo JJ. The link between depressive symptoms, negative cognitive bias and memory complaints in older adults. Aging and Mental Health. 2007;11:708–15. doi: 10.1080/13607860701368497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deary IJ, Whalley LJ, Lemmon H, Crawford JR, Starr J,M. The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence. 2000;28:49–55. [Google Scholar]
  27. Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Pattie A, Starr JM, Hayward C, Wright AF, Carrothers A, Whalley LJ. Cognitive change and the APOE epsilon 4 allel. Nature. 2002;418:932. doi: 10.1038/418932a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence. 2006;35:13–21. [Google Scholar]
  29. Deary IJ, Johnson W, Houlihan LM. Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Human Genetics. 2009;126:215–232. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. British Medical Journal. 2006;333:929–930. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38978.699583.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dickens WT, Flynn JR. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review. 2001;108:346–69. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dignam DM. Understanding intimacy as experienced by breastfeeding women. Health Care for Women International. 1995;16:477–85. doi: 10.1080/07399339509516200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Duckworth K, Akerman R, MacGregor A, Salter E, Vorhaus J. Self -regulated learning: a literature review. 2009 www.learningbenefits.net/Publications/ResRepIntros/ResRep33intro.htm.
  34. Durkin MS, Maenner MJ, Newschaffer CJ, Lee LC, Cunniff CM, Daniels JL, Kirby RS, Leavitt L, Miller L, Zahorodny W, Schieve LA. Advanced parental age and the risk of autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Epidemiology. 168:1268–1276. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Elder GH., Jr Time, human agency, and social change: perspectives in the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1994;57:4–15. [Google Scholar]
  36. Evans GW. Child development and the physical environment. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:423–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Farkas G. Cognitive skills and noncognitive traits and behaviors in stratification processes. Annual Review of Sociology. 2003;29:541–562. [Google Scholar]
  38. Fors S, Lennartsson C, Lundberg O. Childhood living conditions, socioeconomic position in adulthood, and cognition in later life: exploring the associations. Journal of Gerontology: Series B, Social Sciences. 2009;64:750–757. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gale CR, O’Callaghan FJ, Bredow M, Martyn CN, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team The influence of head growth in fetal life, infancy, and childhood on intelligence at the ages of 4 and 8 years. Pediatrics. 2006;118:1486–92. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gale CR, Deary IJ, Kuh D, Huppert F, Richards M, the HALCyon Study Team Neuroticism in adolescence and cognitive function in midlife in the British 1946 birth cohort: the HALCyon Program. Journal of Gerontology: psychological Sciences. 2010;65B:50–56. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Glynmour MM, Manly JJ. Lifecourse social conditions and racial and ethnic patters of cognitive aging. Neuropsychological Reviews. 2008;18:223–254. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9064-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Golub MS, Hogrefe CE, Germann SL, Capitanio JP, Lozof B. Behavioral consequences of developmental iron deficiency in infant rhesus monkeys. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2006;28:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gottlieb G, Wahlsten D, Lickliter R. The significance of biology for human development: a developmental psychobiological systems view. In: Lerner RM, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th edn Vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ: 2006. pp. 210–257. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gunnell D, Miller LL, Rogers I, Holly JM, the ALSPAC Study Team Association of insulin-like growth factor I and insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-3 with intelligence quotient among 8- to 9-year-old children in theAvon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e681–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hatch SL, March D. Concepts and challenges in capturing dynamics of the wider social environment. In: Morgan C, Bhugra D, editors. Principles of Social Psychiatry. 2nd edition John Wiley & Sons; 2010. pp. 65–76. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hatch SL, Feinstein L, Link B, Wadsworth MEJ, Richards M. The continuing benefits of education: adult education and midlife cognitive ability in the British 1946 birth cohort. Journal of Gerontology Series B. 2007;62:S404–S414. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.6.s404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hatch SL, Jones PB, Kuh D, Hardy R, Wadsworth MEJ, Richards M. Childhood cognitive ability and adult mental health in the British 1946 birth cohort. Social Science and Medicine. 2007;64:2285–2296. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hauser RM, Roan C,L. Work complexity and cognitive functioning at midlife: Cross-validating the Kohn-Schooler hypothesis in an American cohort. University of Wisconsin, Center for Demography and Ecology; Madison: 2007. (CDE Working Paper No. 2007-08) [Google Scholar]
  49. Heckman JJ. Skill Formation and the Economics of Investing in Disadvantaged Children. Science. 2006;312:1900–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1128898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heckman JJ, Rubenstein Y. The importance of noncognitive skills: lessons from the GED testing program. American Economics Review. 2001;91:145–149. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hernstein RJ, Murray C. The bell curve: intelligence and class structure in American life. The Free Press; New York: 1994. p. 591. table. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hughes D, Rodriguez J, Smith EP, Johnson DJ, Stevenson HC, Spicer P. Parents’ racial/ethnic socialization practices: A review of research and agenda for future study. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:747–770. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Joo M. Long-term effects of Head Start on academic and school outcomes of children in persistent poverty: girls vs. boys. Children and Youth Services Review. 2010;32:807–814. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kao G, Thompson JS. Racial and ethnic stratification in educational achievement and attainment. Annual Review of Sociology. 2003;29:417–42. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kiernan KE, Huerta MC. Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. The British Journal of Sociology. 2008;59:783–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-4446.2008.00219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kim D. Blues from the neighborhood? Neighborhood characteristics and depression. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2008;30:101–117. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A. Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:50–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kohn M, Schooler C. Work and personality: an enquiry into the impact of social stratification. Ablex; Norwood, NJ: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  59. Kohn M, Slomcznski KM. Social structure and self-direction. A comparative analysis of the United States and Poland. Blackwell; Cambridge, MA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  60. Koletzko B, Cetin I, Brenna JT. Dietary fat intakes for pregnant and lactating women. British Journal of Nutrition. 2007;98:873–7. doi: 10.1017/S0007114507764747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kramer DA. Conceptualizing wisdom: the primacy of affect-cognition relations. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Wisdom: it’s nature, origins and development. Cambridge University Press; New York: 1990. pp. 279–313. [Google Scholar]
  62. Kuczynski L. Beyond bidirectionality: bilateral conceptual frameworks forunderstanding dynamics in parent-child relations. In: Kuczynski L, editor. Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relationships. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2003. pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
  63. Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, J Hallqvist J, Power C. Life course epidemiology. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57:778–783. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.10.778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lenroot RK, Schmitt JE, Ordaz SJ, Wallace GL, Neale MC, Lerch JP, Kendler KS, Evans AC, Giedd JN. Differences in genetic and environmental influences on the human cerebral cortex associated with development during childhood and adolescence. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:163–174. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: the effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:309–337. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Levinson DF. The genetics of depression: a review. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Levitt P. Structural and functional maturation of the developing primate brain. Journal of Pediatrics. 2003;143:S35–S45. doi: 10.1067/s0022-3476(03)00400-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. LeWinn KZ, Stroud LR, Molnar BE, Ware JH, Koenen KC, Buka SL. Elevated maternal cortisol levels during pregnancy are associated with reduced childhood IQ. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;38:1700–10. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Liu X, Sun Z, Neiderhiser JM, Uchiyama M, Okawa M. Low birth weight, developmental milestones, and behavioral problems in Chinese children and adolescents. Psychiatry Research. 2001;101:115–29. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00244-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Shame SJ. Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. New England Journal of Medicine. 1997;337:1889–1895. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199712253372606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication II. Archives of general Psychiatry. 2010;67:124–132. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Reviews. 2007;27:155–172. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American psychologist. 1998;53:185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Malaspina D, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Fenning S, Davidson M, Harlap S, Wolitzky R, Rabinowitz J, Susser E, Knobler HY. Paternal age and intelligence: implications for age-related genomic changes in male germ cells. Psychiatric Genetics. 2005;15:117–125. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200506000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:341–8. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. March D, Hatch SL, Morgan C, Kirkbride JB, Bresnahan M, Fearon P, Susser E. Psychosis and place: A systematic review. Epidemiological Reviews. 30:84–100. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Maughan B, McCarthy G. Childhood adversities and psychosocial disorders. British Medical Bulletin. 1997;53:156–169. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a011597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Mayer KU. The sociology of the life course and lifespan psychology: diverging or converging pathways? In: Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U, editors. Understanding human development: dialogues with lifespan psychology. Kluwer; Norwell, MA: 2003. pp. 463–482. [Google Scholar]
  79. Meacham JA. The loss of wisdom. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Wisdom: it’s nature, origins and development. Cambridge University Press; New York: 1990. pp. 181–211. [Google Scholar]
  80. Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2005;7:103–123. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2005.7.2/mmeaney. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Murray GK, Jones PB, Kuh D, Richards M. Infant developmental milestones and subsequent cognitive function. Annals of Neurology. 2007;62:128–136. doi: 10.1002/ana.21120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network (NICHD) Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children’s cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Development. 2005;76:795–810. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. O’Donovan MC, Owen MJ. Genetics and the brain: many pathways to enlightenment. Human Genetics. 2009;126:1–2. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0705-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Castillo MJ, Sjöström M. Physical fitness in childhood and adolescence: a powerful marker of health. International Journal of Obesity (London) 2008;32:1–11. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Patterson GR, Reid JB, Dishion TJ. Antisocial boys. In: Oatley K, editor. Human emotions: a reader. Blackwell; Malden, MA: 1998. pp. 330–336. [Google Scholar]
  86. Pearlin LI, Schooler C. The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1978;22:337–356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pearlin LI, Nguyen KB, Schieman S, Milkie MA. The life-courseorigins of mastery among older people. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2007;48:164–179. doi: 10.1177/002214650704800205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Plassman B, Welsh K, Helms M, Brandt J, Page W, Breitner J. Intelligence and education as predictors of cognitive state in late life: a 50 -year follow-up. Neurology. 1995;45:1446–50. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.8.1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Pless IB, Cripps HA, Davies JMC, Wadsworth MEJ. Chronic physical illness in childhood: psychological and social effects in adolescence and adult life. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1989;31:746–755. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1989.tb04070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Ratner N, Bruner J. Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language. 1978;5:391–401. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900002063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rice F, Harold GT, Thapar A. Negative life events as an account of age-related differences in the genetic aetiology of depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:977–87. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Richards M, Sacker A. Lifetime antecedents of cognitive reserve. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25:614–24. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.614.14581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: a Model for cognitive aging and decline? Annals of Neurology. 2005;58:617–622. doi: 10.1002/ana.20637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Richards M, Abbott R. Childhood mental health and life chances in post-war Britain. Insights from three national birth cohorts. 2009 http://www.centreformentalhealth.org.uk/pdfs/life_chances_report.pdf.
  95. Richards M, Huppert FA. Do positive children become positive adults? Evidence from a longitudinal birth cohort study. Journal of Positive Psychology. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2011.536655. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Richards M, Wadsworth MEJ, Rahimi-Foroushani A, Hardy R, Kuh D, Paul A. Infant nutrition and cognitive development in the first offspring of a national UK birth cohort. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1998;40:163–167. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh DL, Wadsworth MEJ. Birthweight and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort. British Medical Journal. 2001;322:199–202. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7280.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Richards M, Maughan B, Hardy R, Hall I, Strydom A, Wadsworth MEJ. Long-term affective disorder in people with mild learning disability. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:523–527. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.6.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth M. Postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31:342–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth M. Long-term effects of breast-feeding in a national birth cohort: educational attainment and midlife cognitive function. Public Health Nutrition. 2002;5:631–5. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth M. Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Social Science and Medicine. 2003;56:785–92. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00075-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Richards M, Stephens A, Mishra GD. Health returns to cognitive capital in the British 1946 birth cohort. Journal of Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2010;1:281–296. [Google Scholar]
  103. Robinson M, Oddy WH, Li J, Kendall GE, de Klerk NH, Silburn SR, Zubrick SR, Newnham JP, Stanley FJ, Mattes E. Pre- and postnatal influences on preschool mental health: a large-scale cohort study. Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:1118–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01955.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Rodgers JL, Wänström L. Identification of a Flynn Effect in the NLSY:moving from the center to the boundaries. Intelligence. 2007;35:187–196. [Google Scholar]
  105. Rosenbaum JE, Kariya T, Settersen R, Maier T. Market and network theories of the transition from high school to work: their application to industrialized societies. Annual Review of Sociology. 1990;16:263–299. [Google Scholar]
  106. Roshanaei-Moghaddam B, Katon WJ, Russo J. The longitudinal effects of depression on physical activity. General Hospital Psychiatry. 2009;31:306–15. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Ross CE. Neighborhood disadvantage and adult depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2000;41:177–187. [Google Scholar]
  108. Rutter M. Family and school influences on cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1985;26:683–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1985.tb00584.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:276–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potential: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sameroff A. Transactional models of early social relations. Human Development. 1975;18:773–783. [Google Scholar]
  112. Sampson RJ, Sharkey P, Raudenbush SW. Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2008;105:845–52. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710189104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Schooler C, Mulatu MS. The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16:466–482. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.16.3.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Schooler C, Mulatu MS, Oates G. The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging. 1999;14:483–506. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Schunk D, Ertmer P. Self-regulation and academic learning: self-efficacy enhancing interventions. In: Boekaerts M, Pintrich P, Zeider M, editors. Handbook of self-regulation. Elsevier Academic Press; Burlington, MA: 2000. pp. 631–649. [Google Scholar]
  116. Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:989–1013. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Snow RE, Yalow E. Education and intelligence. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Handbook of human intelligence. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1982. pp. 493–585. [Google Scholar]
  118. Snowdon DA, Kemper SJ, Mortimer JA, Greiner LH, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1996;275:528–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Staudinger UM. Older and wiser? Integrating results on the relationship between age and wisdom-related performance. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1999;23:641–64. [Google Scholar]
  120. Steffens D, Otey E, Alexopoulos G, Butters MA, Cuthbert B, Ganguli M, Geda YE, Hendrie HC, Krishnan RR, Kumar A, Lopez OL, Lyketsos CG, Mast BT, Morris JC, Norton MC, Peavy GM, Petersen RC, Reynolds CF, Salloway S, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Yesavage J. Perspectives on depression, mild cognitive impairment, and cognitive decline. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:130–38. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. Wisdom and creativity. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the psychology of aging. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 2001. pp. 500–522. [Google Scholar]
  122. Stifter CA, Moyer D. The regulation of affect: gaze aversion activity during mother-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development. 1991;14:111–123. [Google Scholar]
  123. Sutherland G. Setting the scene. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2010;1:201–208. [Google Scholar]
  124. Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V. Antenatal maternal stress and long -term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:245–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Thapar A, Rutter M. Do prenatal factors cause psychiatric disorder? Be wary of causal claims. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;195:100–101. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.062828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Thompson C, Syddall H, Rodin I, Osmond C, Barker D. Birth weight and the risk of depressive disorder in late life. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:450–455. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.5.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Turkheimer E, Gottesman II. Individual differences and the canalization of human behavior. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:18–22. [Google Scholar]
  128. Urdinguio RG, Sanchez JV, Esteller M. Epigenetic mechanisms in neurological diseases: genes, syndromes, and therapies. Lancet Neurology. 8:1056–1072. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70262-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, Carter JA. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Wang S-C, Oelze B, Schumacher A. Age-specific epigenetic drift inlate-onset Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e2698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Waylen A, Ford T, Goodman R, Samara M, Wolke D. Can early intake of dietary omega-3 predict childhood externalizing behaviour? Acta Paediatrica. 2009;98:1805–1808. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01434.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Webster JD. An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale. Journal of Adult Development. 2003;10:13–22. [Google Scholar]
  133. Whalley LJ, Deary IJ. Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal. 2001;322:1–5. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7290.819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Whalley LJ, Dick FD, McNeill G. A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. Lancet Neurology. 2006;5:87–96. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Wheaton B, Clarke P. Space meets time: integrating temporal and contextual influences on mental health in early adulthood. American Sociological Review. 2003;68:680–706. [Google Scholar]
  136. Williams DR, Mohammed SA, Leavell J, Collins C. Race, socioeconomic status, and health: complexities, ongoing challenges, and research opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186:69–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05339.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Willis SL, Blaskewicz Boron J. Midlife cognition. The association of personality with cognition and risk of cognitive impairment. In: Hofer SM, Alwin DF, editors. Handbook of cognitive aging. Interdisciplinary perspectives. Sage Publications; Los Angeles: 2008. pp. 647–660. [Google Scholar]
  138. Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA, Li Y, Bienias JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Proneness to psychological distress and risk of Alzheimer disease in a biracial community. Neurology. 2005;64:380–2. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000149525.53525.E7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Zammit S, Allebeck P, David AS, Dalman C, Hemmingsson T, Lundberg I, Lewis G. A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:354–60. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Zimmerman B. Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective. In: Boekaerts M, Pintrich P, Zeider M, editors. Handbook of self-regulation. Elsevier Academic Press; Burlington, MA: 2000. pp. 13–39. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES