Abstract
Using digital holographic cinematography, we quantify and compare the feeding behavior of free-swimming copepods, Acartia tonsa, on nutritional prey (Storeatula major) to that occurring during exposure to toxic and non-toxic strains of Karenia brevis and Karlodinium veneficum. These two harmful algal species produce polyketide toxins with different modes of action and potency. We distinguish between two different beating modes of the copepod’s feeding appendages–a “sampling beating” that has short durations (<100 ms) and involves little fluid entrainment and a longer duration “grazing beating” that persists up to 1200 ms and generates feeding currents. The durations of both beating modes have log-normal distributions. Without prey, A. tonsa only samples the environment at low frequency. Upon introduction of non-toxic food, it increases its sampling time moderately and the grazing period substantially. On mono algal diets for either of the toxic dinoflagellates, sampling time fraction is high but the grazing is very limited. A. tonsa demonstrates aversion to both toxic algal species. In mixtures of S. major and the neurotoxin producing K. brevis, sampling and grazing diminish rapidly, presumably due to neurological effects of consuming brevetoxins while trying to feed on S. major. In contrast, on mixtures of cytotoxin producing K. veneficum, both behavioral modes persist, indicating that intake of karlotoxins does not immediately inhibit the copepod’s grazing behavior. These findings add critical insight into how these algal toxins may influence the copepod’s feeding behavior, and suggest how some harmful algal species may alter top-down control exerted by grazers like copepods.
Introduction
Harmful algal blooms (HABs) are occurring with increasing frequency and magnitude across the globe, and have the potential to alter or disrupt ecosystem functions. In blooms of toxic or unpalatable phytoplankton, zooplankton grazing rates may decrease, thus negating top-down control and altering the transfer of energy to higher trophic levels [1]. Karenia brevis and Karlodinium veneficum are two common dinoflagellates that form HABs. K. brevis blooms are responsible for the infamous ‘red tides’ prominent in the Gulf of Mexico. It causes neurotoxic shellfish poisoning by producing brevetoxins, which are ladder polyethers that trigger numerous physiological symptoms in exposed organisms. K. veneficum is responsible for major fish kills in estuaries and brackish waters worldwide. It produces karlotoxins, which are pore-forming polyketides that cause cell lysis and death by increasing the membrane permeability. A recent study reported that karlotoxin was used by K. veneficum for capturing its prey, Storeatula major [2].
In the current research, we are interested in the interaction of calanoid copepods with toxic strains of K. brevis and K. veneficum. Acartia tonsa (Calanoida, Copepoda) is one of the most abundant zooplankters found within blooms of K. brevis [3], and is expected to be routinely exposed to background levels of K. veneficum in coastal waters of the southeastern USA [4]. Prior studies investigating copepod-dinoflagellate interactions have focused on using the armored dinoflagellate Alexandrium, which produces the neutotoxic saxitoxins. The role of this phycotoxin as a deterrent to zooplankton grazing was originally thought to be physiological impairment [5]–[7] and not particle rejection based on chemosensory response. Subsequently, [8], [9] showed, based on grazing statistics, that copepods could recognize and reject toxic cells prior to ingestion. Prey selectivity was indicated also by a number of behavior studies utilizing high-speed cinematography with 3D translation systems. [10] distinguished between raptorial and suspension feeding modes, and suggested that during raptorial feeding, the copepod detects ciliates from a long distance using their mechano or chemoreceptors. Investigations of turbulence effects on the prey detection and selection were performed successively [11]–[14]. Using tethered copepods when suspension feeding, [15] showed that the feeding current structure (e.g. high shear) could enhance the chemical signal detection. Recently, [16] observed an active prey selection of non-toxic prey over potentially toxic dinoflagellates.
A number of food removal and fecundity experiments focusing on interactions between A. tonsa and K. brevis (e.g. [17]–[19]) revealed reduced grazing for reasons that were not conclusively resolved. [17] found that A. tonsa avoided ingestion of K. brevis (unknown toxicity) and selectively ingested diatoms in a mixed bloom assemblage, causing starvation and reduced fecundity [18]. However, [19] argued that the decreasing grazing was more likely to result from nutritional inadequacy of K. brevis. Based on a grazing and survival experiment, [20] demonstrated that toxic K. brevis diet led to lower grazing rate, as well as higher mortality and egg production in comparison to diets of corresponding non-toxic strains. The only available grazing study of A. tonsa feeding on K. veneficum [4] showed a reduced grazing rate for toxic strains, but not to the same extent as those measured for K. brevis. The karlotoxins also had little effect on the copepod’s survivorship.
In this paper, we study the behavioral response of A. tonsa to varying diets of toxic and non-toxic isolates of K. brevis and K. veneficum, the same organisms used in [4], [20]. In all the current observations, predation occurs via suspension feeding, i.e. the copepod generates a feeding current to attract and capture prey [21]–[23] and not by raptorial feeding. We use digital holographic cinematography to simultaneously follow the 3D motion of numerous free-swimming copepods and prey cells. Statistical analysis of the duration of the copepods' feeding appendage beating enables us to distinguish between two beating modes in the suspension feeding behavior: “sampling beating” that typically occurs for a short duration and involves little fluid entrainment, and “grazing beating” that persists for longer periods and generates currents that entrain nearby prey cells. The fraction of time dedicated to sampling and grazing are strongly influenced by prey species present and the type of toxin they produce. These findings add critical understanding of how the mode of action of algal toxins may influence the copepods’ ability to exert top-down control, and help in terminating HABs [24].
Materials and Methods
In our experiments, groups of twenty wild female A. tonsa were placed in a 30×30×100 mm3 transparent container, and allowed to swim freely. They were exposed to ten diets, as listed in Table 1, which included no-prey as a negative control, the cryptophyte Storeatula major, as a known good nutritional food source (positive control), as well as mono-algal and mixed, toxic and non-toxic strains of K. brevis and K. veneficum. Supporting Information S1 summarizes the origin of the dinoflagellates used in the present study, as well as the properties of the toxins involved.
Table 1. The time fraction and mean duration of feeding appendage beating, hopping and escape reaction.
no prey | S. major | Karenia brevis | Karlodinium veneficum | 20 um polystyrene | |||||||
SP-1 (non-toxic) | 2228 (toxic) | 2228+S.major | 1609 (non-toxic) | 2064 (toxic) | 2064+1609 | ||||||
1∶3 | 3∶1 | 1∶3 | 3∶1 | ||||||||
Total observation time (s) | 437.09 | 398.28 | 484.04 | 575.36 | 416.9 | 366.44 | 457.31 | 391.97 | 711.2 | 658.89 | 54.6 |
Mean beating duration (ms)★ | 58±33 | 287±220 | 194±177 | 140±130 | 140±147 | 89±84 | 150±157 | 117±118 | 198±171 | 164±159 | 41±10 |
Cumulative beating fraction♦ | 0.022 | 0.320 | 0.262 | 0.134 | 0.078 | 0.034 | 0.177 | 0.143 | 0.220 | 0.158 | 0.008 |
Cumulative hopping fraction♦ | 0.0195 | 0.0201 | 0.0304 | 0.0221 | 0.0152 | 0.0191 | 0.0347 | 0.0292 | 0.023 | 0.0191 | 0.0108 |
Cumulative escape fraction♦ | 0.0036 | 0.0063 | 0.0134 | 0.0068 | 0.0123 | 0.0122 | 0.0127 | 0.0042 | 0.01 | 0.0112 | 0.005 |
The mean duration of a single beating event and its standard deviation.
The total duration of a certain behavior divided by the observation time.
Materials
A. tonsa were collected from Narragansett Bay, Rhode Island in July 2010 and in February 2009 from Pivers Island, Beaufort, North Carolina, USA by towing a 150 µm mesh, 0.5 m diameter plankton net, sorted by hand under a dissecting microscope and maintained in filtered seawater with sufficient food (Rhodomonas salina) until the day of experimentation. All the diet experiments were performed using the Narragansett Bay samples, except for the no-prey case, which involved the samples collected at Beaufort. These field locations are not privately-owned or protected in any way and our field studies did not involve endangered or protected species. No specific permits are required for collecting samples in these locations. All algal cultures were grown in standard media as described in [25], [26] (S. major and K. veneficum) and [20] (R. salina and K. brevis) to log phase and harvested for use in the behavioral studies.
Experimental Setup and Procedures
Measurements were performed using inline, cinematic digital holography. This technique enabled us to simultaneously track numerous spatially-separated organisms, e.g. the copepods and prey cells, without loss of spatial resolution and over a long period in a 3D sample volume with a substantial depth. Figure 1 is a schematic of our experimental setup. Similar setups were used in a series of previous studies to examine the flow field around a feeding copepod [27] and the interaction of dinoflagellates with their prey [2], [28], [29]. The sample volume was illuminated by a collimated He-Ne laser beam (632.8 nm wavelength), and holograms at two different magnifications were acquired at 250 frames per second by a 1,024×1,024 pixels CMOS camera. Most of the A. tonsa behavioral data were acquired at a pixel resolution of 19.2 µm (1×). In addition, holograms recorded at a magnification of 5×, i.e. a resolution 4.1 µm/pixel were used to examine the interaction between A. tonsa and prey cells. Enhancement of the holograms before reconstruction included removal of time-invariant non-uniformities and equalization to correct for laser intensity variations. The holograms were numerically reconstructed using in-house developed software [2], [29] every 5 mm, which was sufficient for behavior classification, and every 20 µm for counting the dinoflagellate populations. At 1×, the reconstructed sample volume of 20×20×20 mm was located at the center of the transparent 30×30×100 mm3 container, i.e. the analysis did not include copepods (or prey) located within 5 mm from the walls.
Twenty fresh, well-fed, wild female A. tonsa were placed in the test container at the beginning of each diet experiment, which yielded a copepod concentration of 5 mls/copepod. This concentration, though high, was close to that the highest one used in [30], 6 mls/copepod, for which they showed little effect on copepods’ behavior. The dataset was obtained during three sequential recordings spaced 20 min apart, each consisting of 6144 holograms recorded during 24.6 s, which was the maximum data acquisition time allowed by the storage capacity of the high speed camera. The total duration of each diet experiment was about 40 min. Based on the reconstructed holographic movies, every single copepod was tracked continuously as long as it remained in the central sample volume, and we recorded the timing and duration of each behavior. With multiple simultaneous samples available, the cumulative (total) observation time for a single A. tonsa in each diet experiment ranged from 380 to 710 s (see Table 1). Using the reconstructed holograms, we directly measured the cell concentration in the sample volume at the start and the end of each diet experiment (see details in Supporting Information S2), and calculated corresponding clearance rates for 40 min duration. When referring to grazing rate, we rely on the longer duration grazing experiments [4], [20]. Although the intention was to set the start prey cell concentration at 1000 cells/ml for all diet cases, the measured initial concentrations varied. This concentration effect on the following data analysis was taken into account by using the cell concentration presented in Table 2, which was the mean of start and end concentration for each diet case.
Table 2. Sampling and grazing beating statistics.
no prey | S. major | Karenia brevis | Karlodinium veneficum | |||||||
SP-1 (non-toxic) | 2228 (toxic) | 2228+S.major | 1609 (non-toxic) | 2064 (toxic) | 2064+1609 | |||||
1∶3 | 3∶1 | 1∶3 | 3∶1 | |||||||
Cell concentration(cell/ml) | N/A | 2039 | 1855 | 3373 | 2728 | 1844 | 1776 | 2414 | 2004 | 1723 |
Sampling beating fraction★ | 0.022 | 0.039 | 0.052 | 0.053 | 0.036 | 0.024 | 0.059 | 0.069 | 0.049 | 0.049 |
Grazing beating fraction★ | 0.000 | 0.281 | 0.209 | 0.082 | 0.042 | 0.010 | 0.118 | 0.073 | 0.174 | 0.109 |
Corr. sampling fraction♦ | N/A | 0.036 | 0.044 | 0.081 | 0.045 | 0.021 | 0.048 | 0.076 | 0.044 | 0.038 |
Corr. grazing fraction♦ | N/A | 0.302 | 0.248 | 0.053 | 0.034 | 0.011 | 0.146 | 0.066 | 0.190 | 0.139 |
(ms)• | 54×/1.88 | 75×/2.19 | 61×/1.95 | 65×/1.75 | 69×/1.98 | 52×/1.80 | 56×/2.07 | 61×/1.78 | 71×/1.94 | 69×/2.22 |
(ms)• | N/A | 347×/1.66 | 235×/1.88 | 258×/1.51 | 342×/1.54 | 169×/4.97 | 275×/1.91 | 264×/1.48 | 254×/1.64 | 245×/1.51 |
rms fitting error – grazing◊ | N/A | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.18 | 0.90 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.07 |
The total duration of sampling/grazing divided by the observation time.
Values corrected for differences in prey cell concentration.
For a normally-distributed variable, a domain consisting of ± standard deviation from the arithmetic mean contains 68.3% of the data. For a log-normally-distributed variable, 68.3% of the results fall within the range of the geometric mean ×/(multiply/divide) by the geometric standard deviation. Accordingly, represents a confidence interval of 68.3% [38].
errors resulting from log-normal fits.
Behavior Categorization
Based on our observations, and consistent with prior publications [31]–[33], A. tonsa’s behavior can be categorized as exhibiting free drifting, feeding appendage beating (Figure 2a, referred to as beating hereafter), hopping (Figure 2b), and escape reaction.
Feeding appendage beating
This behavior is characterized by periodic movement of feeding appendages which include the 2nd antennae, 1st maxillae, 2nd maxillae, mandibular palps and maxillipeds. These feeding appendages operate in an integrated fashion to generate a flow field leading to prey entrainment and capture (Video S1). For each type of prey, we select 50 cells located initially within one copepod body length from the predator, in a conical volume extending from its mouthpart. We then measure the beating duration until the cell reaches the feeding appendages or the copepod stops appendage beating, whichever occurs first, along with the corresponding displacement of prey cells.
Hopping
This behavior is characterized by the quick backward movement of the 1st antennae and pereiopods. These motions generate a strong thrust to enable the copepod to be translated quickly in a short distance or re-adjust its body orientation (Video S2).
Escape reaction
This behavior is characterized by the repetitive motion of the pereiopods and retraction of the 1st antennae (Video S3).
A feeding appendage beating event is considered to terminate when the copepod retracts its feeding appendages and does not initiate further motion for a period of at least 12 ms, corresponding to three frames under our current recording speed. This criterion has been used to separate between sequential behavioral events throughout this study.
Results
Duration Statistics on A. tonsa’s Behavior
Two sample behavioral sequences showing the timing and duration of different behaviors demonstrate the frequency and variation possible during A. tonsa feeding on two extreme diet quality regimes. The first is A. tonsa grazing on an S. major CCMP 1868 (Figure 2c) and the second is A. tonsa grazing on a mono-algal diet of toxic K. brevis CCMP 2228 (Figure 2d). In a favorable food environment, the behavioral sequence contains frequent beatings for long durations (>200 ms), with hopping and escape reactions distributed almost uniformly throughout the period of observation. In contrast, when feeding on a toxic diet, the lengthy beatings are not evident, leaving primarily hopping and short-duration beatings. Combining the measurements of all tracked copepods, we use two parameters to quantify the frequency and total time fraction for different behaviors. The mean duration for beating, hopping and escape reaction are the average times for a single event. The cumulative fraction of each behavior refers to the total duration of this behavior of all the samples divided by the total observation time. For each behavior, in particular the beating, the duration of every behavioral event varies substantially. The duration histogram along with its mean and standard deviation are used to investigate the trends for the changing diets.
Diet Induced Variations of A. tonsa’s Behavior
Some of the initial observations, consisting of cumulative time fractions of beating, hopping and escape reaction for all the experiments along with the corresponding mean duration are summarized in Table 1. Despite substantial standard deviations, the dependence of mean beating durations on diets is statistically significant (see Supporting Information S3). The longest mean beating duration and the highest cumulative beating fraction occur on the nutritional diet of S. major, consistent with the findings for suspension feeding [10]. On K. brevis diets, longer duration and higher beating fraction are observed with the non-toxic SP-1 strain than those with the toxic 2228 strain, in agreement with the trend of ingestion rates obtained during published food removal experiments [19], [20]. However, the results for the mixed-diets of S. major and K. brevis 2228 are puzzling, since the mean durations and time fractions do not fall between corresponding values of the mono-algal diets. Instead, mean durations and time fractions decrease to levels that are significantly lower than those occurring with the toxic K. brevis alone, and they decline further with increasing proportion of toxic K. brevis. Consistent with the food removal data from [4], the mean beating durations and cumulative fractions on the non-toxic K. veneficum CCMP 1609 diets are higher than those occurring in the presence of the toxic CCMP 2064 strain. However, the differences between the grazing responses on toxic and non-toxic K. veneficum are smaller compared to those associated with toxic and non-toxic K. brevis diets. Furthermore, in mixtures of toxic and non-toxic K. veneficum, A. tonsa does not significantly reduce its cumulative beating fractions or mean duration, in contrast to the K. brevis findings. Hopping and escape reaction behaviors (Table 1) show little correlation with food quality or toxicity, with the cumulative escape fraction being smaller than that of hopping for all diet cases. More details are provided in Supporting Information S3. In summary, the toxins and diets seem to affect only the A. tonsa’s feeding appendage beatings and not other behaviors in their repertoire. This statement refers to suspension feeding of A. tonsa, and does not contradict prior studies (e.g. [34]), which found that during raptorial feeding, the hopping and escape reactions were diet-dependent.
Modes of Feeding Appendage Beating
The duration histograms of beatings for A. tonsa on no prey (Figure 3a) and S. major (Figure 3b) diets, exhibit substantially different time spans, but the peaks of both are located in the vicinity of 44 ms. This observation holds for all the other diets, as demonstrated in the Supporting Information S4 and S5. With little variation, a single 44 ms beating event consists of 3–4 periodic bouts of the feeding appendages. The relationship between beating duration and cell displacement (Figure 4) shows that, beating events that are shorter than 100 ms, do not cause displacements of more than 50 µm, i.e., one third of the characteristic length of the feeding appendage (∼150 µm). In particular, the durations that correspond to the peak of the histogram hardly induce any displacement. For longer periods, the displacements generally increase with beating duration. Furthermore, when A. tonsa is placed in a dense suspension of 20 µm diameter, nearly neutrally buoyant (specific gravity of 1.05) polystyrene particles, both the mean beating duration and cumulative beating fraction become very short (Table 1), and the corresponding particle displacement is very little. It is obvious from our holographic movies and consistent with published findings [35], that A. tonsa ignores the polystyrene particles as a food source. It should be noted that experiments involving the particles were performed at a different time, and are included here only to confirm that there is essentially no measurable feeding current for beatings with short duration.
When A. tonsa is placed in a no-prey environment, the beating duration is limited to the 0–200 ms range (Figure 3a), and 90% of the events are shorter than 100 ms. For such short beating durations, the copepods do not generate sufficient flow to capture/attract prey, as indicated by the above discussion. Imposing the energy minimization principle for biological organisms, there must be another reason for these beatings. We conjecture that they are used for screening and evaluating the environment by promoting the binding of odorant molecules with chemoreceptors concentrated along the copepod’s feeding appendages [36]. When prey are introduced (Figure 3b and Supporting Information S4), the beating histograms extend to much longer durations, ∼1100 ms in the S. major case, indicating that grazing involves beating durations exceeding the characteristic period of ∼100 ms. Thus, we separate the feeding appendage beating into two distinct modes: sampling and grazing. The short duration sampling mode persists in any environment, including cases of no-prey or polystyrene particles, and appears to have a “universal” probability density function (PDF) peak varying between 36 and 48 ms. In no prey case, the copepod might be responding to odorant molecules, such as dimethyl sulfide, that exist as chemical noise in the seawater [37]. When it senses prey, A. tonsa switches directly to a grazing mode that involves generation of a feeding current, whose duration is dependent on the local food environment.
In the analysis that follows, we use the no-prey distribution (Figure 3a) as a model for sampling behavior. Fitting a log-normal curve [38] to this duration histogram has a root mean squared (rms) error of less than 2%. We then infer that all the sampling durations have log-normal distribution with geometric mean values and standard deviation (superscript ‘s’ indicates sampling) that vary with the prey environment (see Supporting Information S5 for all the duration histograms). Possible reasons for the log-normal distribution, which involve olfactory sensing by chemoreceptors, are present in the Discussion Section. To isolate the sampling part of each histogram, we assume that durations of less than 100 ms are used only for sampling, and that those longer than 200 ms are dedicated only to grazing. The magnitudes of and are estimated from a log-normal least square fit to the data in the 0–100 ms range, and the resulting sampling duration PDF is terminated at 200 ms (Figure 3c). This distribution is then subtracted from the total beating duration histogram. The remaining portion, shown in Figure 3d, is considered to be the grazing duration histogram. In view of its shape, we also perform a least-square fit to the grazing histogram with a log-normal distribution that results in a rms error of 4%.
The same procedures and criteria are used to separate modes for all the diets. A complete set of histograms, curve fits, and their parameters are presented in Supporting Information S5 and S6. Values of and (superscript ‘g’ indicates grazing), as well as and are also provided in Table 2. In the 0–100 ms range, the rms errors of the log-normal sampling fits vary between 0.2–2.6%, with negligible residues left for grazing (Supporting Information S5). Values of vary between 52 to 75 ms, and the corresponding most probable values, 38–48 ms, are very close to the measured ones (Supporting Information S5). Since there is no clear boundary between sampling and grazing on the duration histogram, we recognize that one might question the effect of specific choices used for defining the sampling PDF on the findings of this paper, especially the selected cutoff duration. We have tested another approach consisting of a single cutoff at 100 ms without log-normal fitting, i.e. beatings with duration less than 100 ms are considered as sampling, and ones with longer duration are considered as grazing. Different cutoff thresholds have also been tested for mode separation with details described in Supporting Information S7. As it demonstrates, selecting cutoff duration within a certain range (100–200 ms) only affects the specific values of grazing and sampling fraction, but not their general trends among diets. Fitting log-normal curves to grazing beating duration results in rms errors of less than 8% for all diets, except for the two cases involving mixtures of toxic K. brevis and S. major. There deviations are substantial (Table 2) and as a result, we do not use grazing curve-fits for these sets of data. As for the other seven grazing cases, for the favorable S. major diet is distinctly higher than the rest, while the others values are close. The fitted curves of grazing durations, separated by dinoflagellate species, and with the S. major results as a reference, are presented in Figure 5. We opt to include the corresponding histograms for mono-toxic algae in each plot to demonstrate how well the log-normal fits represent the original distributions. In general, the PDFs broaden and their peak values decrease, indicating an increase in the characteristic grazing duration with increasing food “quality”.
Dependence of Sampling and Grazing on Diet
The fractions of time dedicated to sampling and grazing, referred to as sampling and grazing fractions hereafter are provided in Table 2 for each diet case, and their sum equals the cumulative beating fraction in Table 1. Since the prey cell concentration is expected to affect the feeding activities [39], to compare trends of grazing and sampling fractions, it is essential to account for variations in cell concentrations (Table 2). For sampling, high prey concentration increases the encounter rates and binding rate of odorant molecules and chemoreceptors, presumably requiring a reduced screening time to evaluate the local environment. Conversely, the copepod is expected to increase its grazing rate with increasing concentration until reaching saturation levels [39]. Thus, to compensate for concentration effects, it is reasonable to assume as a first order approximation that the sampling time fraction is inversely proportional and that grazing time fraction is proportional to the prey concentration. Accordingly, the sampling and grazing fractions are linearly interpolated/extrapolated, using case-dependent slope, to 2195 cells/ml, the averaged value of all the present diet cases in which the cell concentrations are available (Table 2). Inherently, the no-prey results, which serve as a limiting reference case, are not corrected.
Figure 6a illustrates how A. tonsa adjusts the time fraction allocated to each beating mode in response to varying diets based on the original (non-fitted) grazing data. The complementing Figure 6b compares the fraction of prey cells removed by copepods during the entire 40 min experiments, which is obtained by comparing the start and end cell counts.
Several trends become evident. First, when there is no food, the sampling time is low, and that of grazing is essentially zero. Switching to the favorable S. major diet, the sampling fraction more than doubles, and the grazing fraction peaks at 23%, with the results for the non-toxic K. brevis SP-1 being quite similar. Conversely, on mono-algal diets of both toxic dinoflagellate strains, the sampling fractions increase by five times from the no-prey level, while the grazing fractions remain low. A. tonsa does not attempt to entrain these prey cells, but continues to sample the water. In fact, the movies (Video S4 in Supporting Information S8) show that when a toxic K. brevis cell becomes inadvertently entangled with its feeding appendages, the copepod pushes the dinoflagellate away. Grazing suppression in the presence of toxic K. brevis is supported by previous food removal experiments [20] and the data in Figure 6b. However, we have occasionally seen A. tonsa ingesting toxic K. brevis cell. Consequently, the number of grazing events decreases with time during the 40 min experiment, presumably due to the toxicity of brevetoxin.
Second, the responses to mixed diets involving K. brevis are very different from those associated with K. veneficum. In mixtures of S. major with toxic K. brevis, both modes decrease in comparison to those of the toxic K. brevis alone, consistent with the fraction of prey cells removed (Figure 6b). In particular, for a dinoflagellate to S. major ratio of 3∶1, grazing almost disappears and sampling is reduced to the no-prey level. The cessation of beating is consistent with the poor long-term survival of A. tonsa in the presence of toxic K. brevis [20]. In contrast, on a mixed diet of K. veneficum cells, A. tonsa increases grazing and reduces sampling fraction to the level that is not significantly different from that of the non-toxic mono-algal diet. This trend agrees with the little difference in the fraction of K. veneficum cell removed among diets containing some non-toxic strain (Figure 6b).
Discussion
Grazing Suppression and Toxicity
A question raised from our results is why are grazing and sampling in mixed diets of K. brevis so low? To provide possible explanations, we note that evidence provided in several studies [9], [17] and our holographic movies (Video S5 in Supporting Information S8) show that A. tonsa can perform selective feeding. Thus, on a mixed diet A. tonsa might attempt to ingest the S. major cells, but in the process, it also ingests water containing brevetoxins, and occasionally toxic cells. Consequently, the copepod is more likely to be affected by the neurotoxic brevetoxins, in comparison to the mono-algal case. Indeed, in mixtures, A. tonsa becomes inactive after a short period (∼10 min), indicating a rapid neurological response, and does not exhibit a subsequent noticeable difference in behavior with time. An expectation that the intoxication level is related to the toxin concentration might explain the increased immobility with fraction of toxic K. brevis in the mixture. However, for the mixed diet of K. veneficum, although the longer grazing duration should increase the karlotoxin intake, evidently, it does not cause an appreciable effect on the beating characteristics during the present experiments. These observations are in agreement with the ability of A. tonsa to survive long periods on a diet of toxic K. veneficum [4], and the slow interactions of karlotoxins with membrane sterols [40].
In previous studies, the decrease in clearance rates of A. tonsa on toxic K. brevis has been attributed to behavioral avoidance [18], nutritional inadequacy [19] and physiological incapacitation [4], [20]. The presently observed short-term (40 min) behavior of a well-fed naive A. tonsa strongly suggests that ingesting the neurotoxic brevetoxin suppresses the feeding appendage beating behavior. On a diet of K. brevis, sampling is greatly enhanced, but on mixed diets, presumably once some of the toxins are ingested, both the sampling and grazing fractions decrease. Furthermore, the beating becomes feeble, i.e. A. tonsa lifts and retracts the feeding appendages only partially, but still maintains a beating frequency of about 60 Hz, the same as all the other cases. Although the low grazing and high sampling beating is also observed on a toxic K. veneficum diet, there is little difference between the copepod’s behavior and prey cell removal in non-toxic and mixed diet environments. Vigorous beating (fully-extended appendages) on mixed diets indicates that karlotoxin does not incapacitate the A. tonsa, at least in a short term. It is worth noting that the distinctly different impacts of brevetoxin and karlotoxin are consistent with mode of action of the toxins and trophic behavior of the dinoflagellates. The mixotrophic K. veneficum utilizes its toxins for immobilizing prey cells [2], but not as an effective means of inhibiting grazing by copepods. Conversely, as a predominantly autotrophic dinoflagellate, K. brevis captures prey infrequently [41], therefore the brevetoxin is employed largely as a grazing deterrent.
Mode Separation and Log-normality
The mode separation is essential to explain copepods’ behavior on no-prey/polystyrene particle environment, in which cases the copepods predominantly perform short duration beatings that generate little food entrainment. One cannot explain this behavior without reasoning that the copepod is sampling the water, and since it detects little favorable food, it does not perform longer beatings that would entrain water and particles. In this way, the copepod utilizes less energy, and also avoids entrainment of toxic prey. On the mono-algal toxic diets, the sampling (but not the grazing) fractions are higher than all the other cases. A possible explanation is that the copepods might be sensing the presence of potential food (chemically or mechanically), and consequently, increase the sampling rate. However, since the feedback is unfavorable, they do not graze. This frequent sampling without entrainment might be a defensive strategy against intoxication. On K. veneficum diets, the mode separation enables us to show that the copepod does not favor the mono-algal diet of this species, and in fact, behaves similarly to that occurring on a mono-algal diet of K. brevis. If we did not separate the sampling and grazing beating modes, we would not be able to distinguish between behaviors on mono-algal toxic K. veneficum and mixed K. veneficum diets. In both cases, the total beating time fractions are close, but the time fractions of (short duration) sampling beatings are very different. On the mono-algal diet, the copepod performs a lot of sampling but little grazing. However, on the mixed diet, its sampling fraction decreases but the grazing fraction increases substantially.
The duration of both modes is well approximated using log-normal distributions, except for two cases, where intoxication disrupts the grazing behavior. The observed relationships between duration and diet are not sensitive to the separation approach (Supporting Information S7), and the trends in grazing beating generally agree with measured prey cell removal. These observations provide a simple approach for parameterizing the correlation between behavior and population dynamics in a pelagic planktonic ecosystem.
We can offer several explanations for why appendage beating behavior obeys a log-normal distribution. For example, a sampling beating event could be terminated when a threshold number of odorant molecules bind to chemoreceptors of A. tonsa to trigger a response. According to the Langmuir absorption isotherm [42], the receptor occupancy X has a hyperbolic dependence on the odorant concentration C, i.e. X = 1/(1+k d/C), where k d is the disassociation constant for a particular ligand-receptor complex. Since for or , X = 1/(1+k d/C) = 0.5×[1–(k d/C–1)/(k d/C+1)]≈0.5×[1–0.51 ln(k d/C)] for k d/C is in the 0.1–10 range. This condition is usually satisfied for ligand-receptor binding [43]. The classical theory of receptor function [44] assumes proportionality of the drug response to receptor occupancy. Based on the above derivation, if X has a normal distribution, then, C, the concentration that would trigger a response, has a log-normal distribution. The chemo-sensing response of A. tonsa can be related to receptor occupancy in the same manner. When the threshold for chemo-sensing has a normal distribution, the concentration that would trigger this response has a log-normal distribution [43]. Equivalently, with increasing beating duration, the chemoreceptors bind to an increasing number of odorants, which in turn, reduces the number of unoccupied receptors, also resulting in hyperbolic dependence between beating duration and receptor occupancy, and a log-normal distribution of sampling duration. It is worth mentioning that the normality of chemo-sensing threshold can be understood from the following reasoning: Since the chemoreceptors of A. tonsa are spatially isolated [36], the amplitude of total odorant induced-current is the linear superposition of currents from individual channels [45]. According to the central limit theorem, when the amplitude of induced current in a single channel is random, the distribution of the total current amplitude and consequently the sensing response threshold, approach normality as the number of receptors increases. It should be noted that the normality of olfactory threshold has been demonstrated in human sensing [46], [47].
To explain the log-normal distribution of grazing duration, one could assume that grazing is terminated when a threshold number of prey cells are captured. We also assume that the filtering flux Q is constant in time, and that the capture zone has a shape of full cone with volume V, height x, cone angle θ, and apex located at the mouthpart of the copepod. Then, the induced feeding current is $Q\equals dV/dt\equals d\lpar \pi x^3 \tan ^2 \lpar \theta /2\rpar /3\rpar /dt$. Simple integration shows that the time required to entrain a fluid element located a distance x from the copepod is proportional to . Based on [29], the nearest neighbor distance between dinoflagellates in a suspension has a normal distribution. Consequently, the grazing duration for capturing a prey cell is a cubic function of a normally-distributed variable, which can also be approximated by a log-normal distribution [43].
Potential Ecological Impact
The food selectivity reported in earlier experiments and observed in our study is characteristically associated with coastal copepods, where both food and nutrients are more abundant [9], [17]. During the initiation of a K. brevis bloom, copepods are presumably exposed to a mixed assemblage of toxic dinoflagellates and other nutritional sources. As the concentration of toxic K. brevis increases to a near mono-specific bloom, selective feeding makes copepods more susceptible to neurotoxic brevetoxins, leading to a feeding inhibition that favors the bloom growth. Conversely, during the initiation of a toxic K. veneficum bloom, exposure to karlotoxins does not deter short-term grazing, and consequently copepods would be more effective in exerting top down control of the early bloom growth. However, as the K. veneficum bloom becomes mono-algal, the grazing slows down but does not cease. This diminishes, but does not eliminate the grazers’ impact on bloom development to the same extent. These findings are consistent with the differences in long-term copepod survival among these toxic dinoflagellates [4], and the macro-scale observation that Karenia blooms last from weeks to months while Karlodinium blooms are generally short lived [25], [48]. However, we remind the reader that the population dynamics that determines the formation and maintenance of harmful algal blooms is affected by a number of complicating factors, such as physical forcing [49] and long-term adaption of the grazers [50], [51].
Supporting Information
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: Support for the Johns Hopkins University (JHU) group has been provided in part by National Science Foundation, and in part by the Office of Naval Research, National Oceanographic Partnership Program (NOPP). Support for RJW was from National Research Council (NRC) Research Associateship Program (RAP) funded by the Center for Coastal Fisheries and Habitat Research (CCFHR) and National Centers for Coastal Ocean Science (NCCOS) of the National Oceanographic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA). This is contribution #4649 from the University of Maryland Center for Environmental Sciences, contribution #12-231 from the Institute of Marine and Environmental Technology and contribution #685 from the Ecology and Oceanography of Harmful Algal Blooms (ECOHAB) program. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Sunda WG, Graneli E, Gobler CJ. Positive feedback and the development and persistence of ecosystem disruptive algal blooms. J Phycol. 2006;42:963–974. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sheng J, Malkiel E, Katz J, Adolf JE, Place AR. A dinoflagellate exploits toxins to immobilize prey prior to ingestion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:2082–2087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0912254107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lester KM, Heil CA, Neely MB, Spence DN, Murasko S, et al. Zooplankton and Karenia brevis in the Gulf of Mexico. Cont Shelf Res. 2008;28:99–111. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Waggett RJ, Tester PA, Place AR. Anti-grazing properties of the toxic dinoflagellate Karlodinium veneficum during predator-prey interactions with the copepod Acartia tonsa. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2008;366:31–42. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ives JD. The relationship between Gonyaulax tamarensis cell toxin levels and copepod ingestion rates. In: Anderson DM, editor. Toxic Dinoflagellates. New York: Elsevier; 1985. pp. 413–418. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ives JD. Possible mechanisms underlying copepod grazing responses to levels of toxicity in red tide dinoflagellates. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 1987;112:131–144. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Huntley M, Sykes P, Rohan S, Marin V. Chemically-mediated rejection of dinoflagellate prey by the copepods Calanus pacificus and Paracalanus parvus: mechanism, occurrence and significance. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1986;28:105–120. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Teegarden GJ, Cembella AD. Grazing of toxic dinoflagellates, Alexandrium spp., by adult copepods of coastal Maine: Implications for the fate of paralytic shellfish toxins in marine food webs. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 1996;196:145–176. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Teegarden GJ. Copepod grazing selection and particle discrimination on the basis of PSP toxin content. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1999;181:163–176. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jonsson P, Tiselius P. Feeding behavior, prey detection and capture efficiency of the copepod Acartia tonsa feeding on planktonic ciliates. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1990;60:35–44. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Saiz E, Kiørboe T. Predatory and suspension feeding of the copepod Acartia tonsa in turbulent environments. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1995;122:147–158. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kiørboe T, Saiz E, Viitasalo M. Prey switching behavior in the planktonic copepod Acartia tonsa. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1996;143:65–75. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kiørboe T, Saiz E, Visser A. Hydrodynamic signal perception in the copepod Acartia tonsa. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1999;179:97–111. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Svensen C, Kiørboe T. Remote prey detection in Oithona similis: hydromechanical versus chemical cues. J Plankton Res. 2000;22:1155–1166. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Moore PA, Fields DM, Yen J. Physical constraints of chemoreception in foraging copepods. Limnol Oceanogr. 1999;44:166–177. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schultz M, Kiørboe T. Active prey selection in two pelagic copepods feeding on potentially toxic and non-toxic dinoflagellates. J Plankton Res. 2009;31:553–561. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Turner JT, Tester PA. Zooplankton feeding ecology: copepod grazing during an expatriate red tide. In: Cosper E, Bricelj VM, editors. Novel Phytoplankton Blooms Causes and Impacts of Recurrent Brown Tides and Other Unusual Blooms. Berlin: Springer; 1989. pp. 359–374. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Turner JT, Tester PA, Hansen PJ. Interactions between toxic marine phytoplankton and metazoan and protistan grazers. In: Anderson DM, Cembella AD, Hallegaeff GM, editors. Physiological Ecology of Harmful Algal Blooms. Berlin: Springer; 1998. pp. 453–474. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Breier CF, Buskey EJ. Effects of the red tide dinoflagellate, Karenia brevis, on grazing and fecundity in the copepod Acartia tonsa. J Plankton Res. 2007;29:115–126. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Waggett RJ, Hardison DR, Tester PA. Toxicity and nutritional inadequacy of Karenia brevis: synergistic mechanisms disrupt top-down grazer control. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2012;444:15–32. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Koehl MAR, Strickler JR. Copepod feeding currents: Food capture at low Reynolds number. Limnol Oceanogr. 1981;26:1062–1073. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Strickler JR. Calanoid copepods, feeding currents, and the role of gravity. Science. 1982;218:158–160. doi: 10.1126/science.218.4568.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Strickler JR. Sticky water: A selective force in copepod evolution. In: Meyers DG, Strickler JR, editors. Trophic Interactions within Aquatic Ecosystems. AAAS Symp. 85. Boulder, CO: Westview Press; 1984. pp. 187–239. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Smayda TJ. Global epidemic of noxious phytoplankton blooms and food chain consequences in large ecosystems. In: Sherman KL, et al., editors. Food Chains, Models and Management of Large Marine Ecosystems. Boulder, CO: Westview Press; 1992. pp. 257–307. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Adolf JE, Bachvaroff TR, Place AR. Can cryptophyte abundance trigger toxic Karlodinium veneficum blooms in eutrophic estuaries? Harmful Algae. 2008;8:119–128. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Adolf JE, Bachvaroff TR, Place AR. Environmental modulation of karlotoxin levels in strains of the cosmopolitan dinoflagellate, Karlodinium veneficum (Dinophyceae). J Phycol. 2009;45:176–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-8817.2008.00641.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Malkiel E, Sheng J, Katz J, Strickler JR. The three-dimensional flow field generated by a feeding calanoid copepod measured using digital holography. J Exp Biol. 2003;206:3657–3666. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Xu W, Jericho M, Meinertzhagen I, Kreuzer H. Digital in-line holography for biological applications. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:11301–11305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191361398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sheng J, Malkiel E, Katz J, Adolf JE, Belas R, et al. Digital holographic microscopy reveals prey-induced changes in swimming behavior of predatory dinoflagellates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:17512–17517. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704658104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dur G, Souissi S, Schmitt F, Michalec FG, Mahjoub MS, et al. Effects of animal density, volume, and the use of 2D/3D recording on behavioral studies of copepods. Hydrobiologia. 2011;666:197–214. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Strickler JR. Observations of swimming performances of planktonic copepods. Limnol Oceanogr. 1977;22:165–170. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rosenberg GG. Filmed observations of filter feeding in the marine planktonic copepod Acartia clausii. Limnol Oceanogr. 1980;25:738–742. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Vanderploeg HA. Zooplankton particle selection and feeding mechanisms. In: Wotton RS, editor. The Biology of Particles in Aquatic Systems. Ann Arbor, MI: Lewis Publishers; 1994. pp. 205–234. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Greene CH. Foraging tactics and prey-selection patterns of omnivorous and carnivorous calanoid copepods. Hydrobiologia. 1988;167:295–302. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Donaghay PL, Small LF. Food selection capabilities of the estuarine copepod Acartia clausi. Mar Biol. 1979;52:137–146. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Friedman, MM, Strickler JR. Chemoreceptors and feeding in calanoid copepods (Arthropoda: Crustacea). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1975;72:4185–4188. doi: 10.1073/pnas.72.10.4185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Steinke M, Stefels J, Stamhuis E. Dimethyl sulfide triggers search behavior in copepods. Limnol Oceanogr. 2006;51:1925–1930. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Limpert E, Stahel WA, Abbt M. Log-normal distributions across the sciences: keys and clues. BioScience. 2001;51:341–352. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Frost BW. Effects of size and concentration of food particles on the feeding behavior of the marine planktonic copepod Calanus pacificus. Limnol Oceanogr. 1972;17:805–815. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Deeds JR, Place AR. Sterol-specific membrane interactions with the toxins from Karlodinium micrum (Dinophyceae) a strategy for self-protection? African Journal of Marine Science. 2006;28:421–425. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Glibert PM, Burkholder JAM, Kana TM, Alexander J, Skelton H, et al. Grazing by Karenia brevis on Synechococcus enhances its growth rate and may help to sustain blooms. Aquatic Microbial Ecology. 2009;55:17–30. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kenakin TP. Waltham, MA: Academic Press. 214 pp; 2006. A pharmacology primer: theory, applications, and methods. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Koch AL. The logarithm in biology II: Distributions simulating the log-normal. J Theoretical Biol. 1969;23:251–268. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(69)90040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ariens EJ. Receptor theory and structure-action relationships. In: Harper NJ, Simmonds AB, editors. Advances in Drug Research. London: Academic Press; 1966. pp. 235–285. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Menini A, Picco C, Firestein S. Quantal-like current fluctuations induced by odorants in olfactory receptor cells. Nature. 1995;373:435–437. doi: 10.1038/373435a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Amoore JE, Steinle S. A graphic history of specific anosmia. In: Wysocki CJ, Kare MR, editors. Chem Senses. New York: Marcel Dekker; 1991. pp. 331–351. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lancet D, Sadovsky E, Seidemann E. Probability model for molecular recognition in biological receptor repertoires: significance to the olfactory system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90:3715–3719. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.8.3715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hall NS, Litaker RW, Fensin E, Adolf JE, Bowers HA, et al. Environmental factors contributing to the development and demise of a toxic dinoflagellate (Karlodinium veneficum) bloom in a shallow, eutrophic, lagoonal estuary. Estuaries and Coasts. 2008;31:402–418. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Stumpf RP, Litaker, RW, Lannerolle L, Tester PA. Hydrodynamic accumulation of Karenia off the west coast of Florida. Cont Shelf Res. 2007;28:189–213. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Colin SP, Dam HG. Testing for resistance of pelagic marine copepods to a toxic dinoflagellate. Evolutionary Ecol. 2005;18:355–377. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Avery DE, Dam HG. Newly discovered reproductive phenotypes of a marine copepod reveal the costs and advantages of resistance to a toxic dinoflagellate. Limnol Oceanogr. 2007;52:2099–2108. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.