Abstract
Pain caused by nerve injury (i.e. neuropathic pain) is associated with development of neuronal hyperexcitability at several points along the pain pathway. Within primary afferents, numerous injury-induced changes have been identified but it remains unclear which molecular changes are necessary and sufficient to explain cellular hyperexcitability. To investigate this, we built computational models that reproduce the switch from a normal spiking pattern characterized by a single spike at the onset of depolarization to a neuropathic one characterized by repetitive spiking throughout depolarization. Parameter changes that were sufficient to switch the spiking pattern also enabled membrane potential oscillations and bursting, suggesting that all three pathological changes are mechanistically linked. Dynamical analysis confirmed this prediction by showing that excitability changes co-develop when the nonlinear mechanism responsible for spike initiation switches from a quasi-separatrix-crossing to a subcritical Hopf bifurcation. This switch stems from biophysical changes that bias competition between oppositely directed fast- and slow-activating conductances operating at subthreshold potentials. Competition between activation and inactivation of a single conductance can be similarly biased with equivalent consequences for excitability. “Bias” can arise from a multitude of molecular changes occurring alone or in combination; in the latter case, changes can add or offset one another. Thus, our results identify pathological change in the nonlinear interaction between processes affecting spike initiation as the critical determinant of how simple injury-induced changes at the molecular level manifest complex excitability changes at the cellular level. We demonstrate that multiple distinct molecular changes are sufficient to produce neuropathic changes in excitability; however, given that nerve injury elicits numerous molecular changes that may be individually sufficient to alter spike initiation, our results argue that no single molecular change is necessary to produce neuropathic excitability. This deeper understanding of degenerate causal relationships has important implications for how we understand and treat neuropathic pain.
Author Summary
Neuropathic pain results from damage to the nervous system. Much is known about the multitude of molecular and cellular changes that are triggered by nerve injury (and which correlate with development of neuropathic pain), but little is understood about how those changes cause neuropathic pain. Rather than identifying what changes occur after nerve injury (which has already been the focus of countless studies), our study focuses on identifying which changes are functionally important. Specifically, we explain how certain molecular changes, acting alone or in combination, cause a triad of neuropathic changes in primary afferent excitability. Through computational modeling and nonlinear dynamical analysis, we demonstrate that the entire triad of excitability changes arises from a single switch in the nonlinear mechanism responsible for spike initiation. Going further, we demonstrate that many distinct molecular changes are sufficient to produce that switch but that no single molecular change is necessary if more than one sufficient change co-occurs after nerve injury, which appears to be the case. The issue becomes whether molecular changes combine to reach some tipping point whereupon cellular excitability is qualitatively altered. This highlights the importance of nonlinearities for neuropathic pain and the need for more computational pain research.
Introduction
Many primary afferents become hyperexcitable after nerve injury. The resulting spontaneous and evoked hyperactivity contributes to neuropathic pain directly and by driving central sensitization [1], [2], [3]. Beyond simply becoming more excitable (i.e. having a lower activation threshold), three qualitative changes in excitability stand out: a change in spiking pattern ( Fig. 1A ), membrane potential oscillations ( Fig. 1B ) and bursting ( Fig. 1C ) [4], [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12]. These changes co-occur and have been documented in dorsal root ganglion (DRG) neurons of various sizes, including putative high- and low-threshold afferents. Hyperexcitability in low-threshold afferents is thought to underlie allodynia [5], [13], [14], [15], which implicates central plasticity (e.g. unmasking of polysynaptic spinal circuits through disinhibition [16], [17]) such that normally innocuous stimulation (causing exaggerated responses among hyperexcitable low-threshold afferents) can lead to activation of ascending pain pathways, thus enabling innocuous stimulation to elicit pain. The central nervous system is obviously required for pain perception, and central plasticity certainly contributes to the development of neuropathic pain, but it is generally agreed that reversing peripheral hyperexcitability could relieve or markedly attenuate many varieties of neuropathic pain [3]. Doing so has proven easier said than done.
Countless molecular changes have been documented to occur after nerve injury and are correlated with cellular hyperexcitability and pain [[for reviews], [ see 18], [19,20]. Still more injury-induced changes are likely to occur but have yet to be described ( Fig. 1D ). Moreover, causal relationships are harder to ascertain than correlations. Knockout studies [e.g. 21], [22] can demonstrate the necessity of certain molecules for mediating changes in cellular excitability, but those studies do not address sufficiency. If control of primary afferent excitability is degenerate, meaning distinct molecular changes yield equivalent cellular outcomes [23], then a certain molecular change may be sufficient but unnecessary to produce hyperexcitability. Specifically, if more than one molecular change is sufficient to cause cellular hyperexcitability, then blocking any one of those molecular changes will not prevent hyperexcitability if another molecular change can fill in. This possibility should be cause for alarm given that multiple changes co-occur following nerve injury (see above), yet this has not been considered in recent discussions on the lack of progress in translational pain research [24], [25].
Thus, beyond considering which molecular changes are necessary, it is also important (1) to identify which molecular changes are sufficient to produce cellular hyperexcitability and why, and (2) to explain how co-occurring molecular changes interact. Addressing these issues is complicated by the complex nature of excitability. Complexity stems from nonlinearities, which is to say that components of a system (e.g. ion channels within a neuron) compete, cooperate, or interfere with one another [26]. A nonlinear system is not the sum of its parts, which means reductionist approaches provide incomplete, if not incorrect, explanations of complex phenomena. Dynamical systems theory provides a more integrative approach but has not been used hitherto to help explain primary afferent hyperexcitability.
In this study, we sought to identify what sorts of molecular changes could, in theory, cause primary afferent hyperexcitability. This is fundamentally different from identifying what changes occur after nerve injury. The two approaches are complementary ( Fig. 1D ), but whereas the latter has seen widespread use, the former has not. Hence, the current study provides a novel perspective that should facilitate the interpretation of past results. Rather than simulating known molecular changes, we worked in the reverse direction by first reproducing known changes in cellular excitability and then using theory to help identify their potential molecular bases. Specifically, we reproduced previously reported [4] injury-induced changes in cellular excitability (see Fig. 1A–C ) in the simplest possible conductance-based model. Then, through dynamical analysis of that model, we pinpointed a switch in spiking mechanism as the common dynamical basis for all three excitability changes. In turn, we investigated the potential molecular bases for the switch in spiking mechanism. As anticipated, several distinct molecular changes were found to produce qualitatively identical changes in cellular excitability (by switching the spike initiation mechanism), thus highlighting degeneracy in the molecular basis for cellular excitability. We also found that molecular changes can add together and offset one another in terms of their influence on excitability. The implications of these insights for understanding and treating neuropathic pain are discussed.
Results
Injury-induced change in spiking pattern
The first of three pathological changes that we sought to reproduce in our computer model was the switch from onset-only spiking to repetitive spiking during sustained depolarization (see Fig. 1A ). Starting with a minimal conductance-based model (see Eqn. 1–5 in Methods), we set parameters in order for the model to produce onset-only spiking ( Fig. 2A top) and then we varied parameters one at a time in order to switch the model to repetitive spiking ( Fig. 2A bottom). We initially focused on the effects of varying parameter βw, and we henceforth refer to the model with βw = −21 mV as “normal” and to the model with βw = −13 mV as “neuropathic”. The biophysical meaning of a change in βw and the effects of varying other parameters (in the same model or in different models) are considered later in the Results.
Based on our past work [27], we hypothesized that each spiking pattern was associated with a dynamically distinct mechanism of spike initiation. To test this, we conducted bifurcation analysis in the normal and neuropathic models. A bifurcation refers to a qualitative change in steady-state behavior, e.g. transition from quiescence to repetitive spiking. Bifurcation analysis involves systematically varying a parameter of interest to determine at what value(s) of that parameter the system switches steady states, and how that switch occurs. When we varied the stimulus current I stim, the neuropathic model transitioned from quiescence to repetitive spiking through a subcritical Hopf bifurcation ( Fig. 2B bottom) whereas the normal model exhibited no bifurcation ( Fig. 2B top). Both models exhibited a range of I stim within which a single, onset-only spike was generated independently of a bifurcation (see below).
We further characterized spike initiation using phase plane analysis. Our starting model comprised just two variables, V and w, whose interaction is entirely responsible for how the model behaves. Those interactions can be visualized by plotting V against w to create a phase plane, and can be analyzed by considering how the nullclines intersect, where V- and w-nullclines represent locations in phase space where V or w do not change. Points where the two nullclines intersect, referred to as fixed points, are especially important for explaining spike initiation. Stimulation caused a vertical shift in the V-nullcline, which, in the neuropathic model, caused the stable fixed point to become unstable ( Fig. 2C bottom) – this is the geometric explanation for the Hopf bifurcation responsible for repetitive spiking. In the normal model, stimulation caused a similar shift in the V-nullcline, which allowed one spike to be generated despite the fixed point remaining stable ( Fig. 2C top). Rather than occurring through a bifurcation, spike generation in the latter case depended on the trajectory to the newly positioned (but still stable) fixed point, as explained here. A quasi-separatrix (QS) corresponds a manifold in phase space from which trajectories diverge. The V-nullcline and QS shift instantaneously upon the onset of stimulation. Although the QS can move, trajectories cannot cross a stationary QS and, instead, tend to diverge from it; therefore, if the QS shifts far enough during stimulation that the starting point of the trajectory (which corresponds to the original fixed point) ends up below the shifted QS, the system will move to its new fixed point via an indirect route around the end of the QS. See Prescott et al. [27] for more detailed explanations of these dynamical mechanisms.
The biophysical meaning of these dynamical mechanisms in terms of competition between fast-activating inward current (a positive feedback process) and slower-activating outward current (a negative feedback process) are explained in Fig. S1. Equivalent changes can be observed in other minimal 2-D models, for instance, if delayed rectifier potassium channel activation is replaced with fast sodium channel inactivation (Fig. S2).
Injury-induced development of membrane potential oscillations (MPOs)
The second of three pathological changes that we sought to reproduce in our computer model was the development of MPOs (see Fig. 1B ). We hypothesized that the same dynamical mechanism responsible for repetitive spiking also explains MPOs because the fixed point identified in Fig. 2C is a focus (i.e. it has complex eigenvalues), which means trajectories spiral into or away from that point depending on whether the point is stable or unstable, respectively (see inset of Fig. 2C top for sample trajectory). Any noise within the system (e.g. channel noise [28]; primary afferent somata bear no synapses, thus excluding synaptic noise) will continuously perturb the system away from its stable fixed point; as the system relaxes, it will return to the fixed point via a spiral trajectory, thereby producing oscillations on the time series [29]. Thus, one would expect noise-dependent MPOs whose amplitude is inversely proportional to the stability of the fixed point. Since a Hopf bifurcation represents destabilization of the fixed point (see above), noise-dependent MPOs should become prominent near the bifurcation. In biophysical terms, this corresponds to inward current starting to activate but activation of outward current (or inactivation of inward current) catching up, thus preventing spike initiation but producing an MPO in the process.
Based on these theoretical insights, we made three predictions: (1) noise-dependent MPOs should occur in the neuropathic model when it operates near a subcritical Hopf bifurcation, (2) noise-dependent MPOs should not occur in the normal model because there is no Hopf bifurcation, (3) noise-independent MPOs should not occur in either model because there is no stable, subthreshold limit cycle on the bifurcation diagrams in Fig. 2B . To test these predictions, we ran simulations with and without noise in the normal and neuropathic 2-D models (without having adjusted any model parameters). All three predictions were confirmed ( Fig. 3A ). These results argue that repetitive spiking and MPOs are two manifestations of the same dynamical mechanism, namely a subcritical Hopf bifurcation, and become manifest when I stim is above or just below the bifurcation point, respectively. In the absence of a Hopf bifurcation, the system can neither spike repetitively nor exhibit MPOs.
Previous efforts to model injury-induced MPOs in DRG neurons have not considered noise, and have thus necessarily focused on solutions involving stable subthreshold limit cycles [8], [30], [31]. To contrast noise-independent and -dependent MPOs, we adjusted our 2-D model to make it produce MPOs in the absence of any noise. In this model, MPOs arose from a stable limit cycle produced through a supercritical Hopf bifurcation ( Fig. 3B ). Compared with noise-dependent MPOs, noise-independent MPOs were much larger and more regular as evidenced by large, narrow peaks on the power spectra (colored curves on Fig. 3C ). Published power spectra of experimental MPOs are broad [5], [10], [30], like the black power spectrum in Fig. 3C , and are thus consistent with noise-dependent MPOs. Most published studies (see Introduction) have not included power spectral analysis, but the irregularity of MPOs in experimental time series points to a noise-dependent oscillatory mechanism.
Past experiments have shown that MPOs can occur over a broad range of membrane potentials, and that MPO amplitude and peak frequency tend to increase with mean depolarization [4], [10]. Figure 4 demonstrates that our model can reproduce these MPO properties on the basis of a noise-dependent oscillatory mechanism. MPO amplitude grows as the average membrane potential approaches spike threshold ( Fig. 4A ). By comparison, for equivalent mean input, increasing noise amplitude shifts the power spectrum upward but does not change its shape (compare solid and dotted red curves), consistent with larger noisy perturbations but equivalent relaxation toward the fixed point; indeed, the imaginary part of the fixed point's complex eigenvalue, which reflects the rate of winding around the fixed point, is unchanged by noise. That said, stronger noise (which is arguably unphysiological in the case of DRG neurons and would overwhelm the intrinsic oscillatory process) increases the likelihood of the voltage trajectory crossing spike threshold; thus, probability of spike initiation depends jointly on noise amplitude and the difference between mean depolarization and voltage threshold [see 32]. If threshold is shifted (e.g. by slow cumulative sodium channel inactivation, which is represented here by h; Fig. 4B ), the voltage range across which MPOs occur becomes quite broad and is associated with a shift in peak MPO frequency ( Fig. 4C ). Implementation of cumulative sodium channel inactivation as a dynamical process in our model (see Eqns. 6–8) demonstrates the feasibility of a large shift in spike threshold ( Fig. 4D ). Given that noise is ubiquitous, noise-dependent oscillations near a subcritical Hopf bifurcation provide a robust dynamical explanation of MPOs without invoking any mechanism beyond that already required to explain repetitive spiking.
Injury-induced development of bursting
The last of three pathological changes that we sought to reproduce in our model was the development of bursting (see Fig. 1C ). Bursting is a slow process relative to the timescale of individual spikes. Because there is no variable with a sufficiently slow time constant in our 2-D model, we did not expect and nor did we observe bursting in that model. However, the subcritical Hopf bifurcation is known to allow elliptic bursting when a slow process, like spike frequency adaptation, causes the system to drift back and forth across the bifurcation [33], [34]. This occurs because a subcritical Hopf bifurcation has a region of bistability within which the neuron is quiescent or spiking depending on recent history (which is reflected in the adaptation) – this is an example of hysteresis. We reasoned, therefore, that adding adaptation to our neuropathic 2-D model (thus making it 3-D) should give rise to elliptic bursting, and that this might reproduce the bursting observed experimentally in nerve-injured DRG neurons. To test this, we added adaptation mediated through an AHP current I AHP (see Eqns. 9 and 10) without changing any other parameters. As expected, the neuropathic model exhibited bursts whose frequency and duration increased with depolarization ( Fig. 5A ). An enlarged view of the membrane potential ( Fig. 5A inset) shows the growth of MPOs preceding burst initiation, which has been observed experimentally [4], [7], [12], [35] and is a characteristic feature of elliptic bursting caused by the trajectory spiraling away from the fixed point until it jumps to the stable limit cycle. Noise was included in these simulations and introduces randomness into the initiation and termination of each burst, but it is unnecessary for bursting. On the other hand, bursting is dependent on the subcritical Hopf bifurcation; therefore, adding adaptation to the normal model predictably failed to produce bursting (data not shown).
Figure 5B explains the role of hysteresis in burst generation. The inset shows a typical burst with each phase of the response colored differently. Note that the burst starts and stops at different values of z, which is the activation variable for I AHP. The same response was projected onto a bifurcation diagram created by treating z as the bifurcation parameter rather than letting it evolve freely as a variable. The boundaries of the bistable region correspond to values of z at which the burst starts and stops. Fast-slow analysis can accurately predict whether adaptation will, for a certain stimulus intensity, lead to bursting or to tonic spiking at a reduced rate (Fig. S3).
Importantly, we did not tune parameters each time we sought to reproduce a different property; instead, MPOs and bursting were accurately reproduced in our model using the set of parameters chosen on the basis of spiking pattern in Fig. 2 . Our results argue, therefore, (1) that spiking pattern, MPOs and bursting are mechanistically linked through their mutual dependence on the spike initiation mechanism, and (2) that our model captured the crucial nonlinearity (i.e. spike initiation) including how it is altered under neuropathic conditions.
Continuum of injury-induced changes
Beyond reproducing normal and neuropathic states, a good model should account for the transition between states by reproducing the continuum of pathological change. We are unaware of experimental data that quantitatively describe the progress of such changes, but one might reasonably assume that affected cells become more hyperexcitable as pathology worsens. Therefore, we tested values of βw between the nominally “normal” value of −21 mV and the “neuropathic” value of −13 mV using our standard 2-D model. Figure 6A shows the progressive increase in excitability and the change in spiking pattern as βw was varied. Specifically, the minimum I stim required to elicit spiking decreased as βw was increased, and the I stim range associated with onset-only spiking became progressively narrower as repetitive spiking became predominant. Both trends are consistent with qualitative experimental data [4].
Next, we considered how MPOs changed within this same range of βw. All simulations included noise and I stim was chosen to be just below the threshold for repetitive spiking. We found that MPO amplitude decreased as βw was increased ( Fig. 6B , black curve). Because intermediate values of βw are associated with a supercritical Hopf bifurcation (see Fig. 3B ) that gives rise to large, noise-independent MPOs (indicated by * on Fig. 6B ) over a narrow range of I stim, we re-tested with I stim further from threshold in order to isolate noise-dependent MPOs, but the same trend was observed ( Fig. 6B , gray curve). Adjusting as few as one other parameter could prevent the supercritical Hopf bifurcation from occurring at any βw value, but the same trend of decreasing MPO amplitude was still observed (data not shown). Peak MPO frequency also decreased over the same range of βw ( Fig. 6C , solid lines), which parallels the trend in minimal sustainable firing rate (dotted line) as expected from the type 2 excitability associated with the Hopf bifurcation [27], [36]. One might reasonably have expected MPOs to become larger and faster as pathology worsens, but the model clearly predicted the opposite. This unintuitive trend is explained by the neuron becoming more prone to repetitive spiking (see Fig. 6A ) such that the large and fast MPOs observed after mild pathological change (i.e. small changes in βw) are replaced by repetitive spiking as pathology worsens (i.e. larger changes in βw); in other words, the upper limits of MPO amplitude and frequency are reduced as the lower limit (i.e. stimulus threshold) for repetitive spiking decreases.
Results presented thus far demonstrate that a switch in spike initiation mechanism (from QS-crossing to subcritical Hopf bifurcation) is sufficient to explain the complete triad of excitability changes outlined in Fig. 1A–C . Despite extensive exploration of the parameter space of two separate 2-D models (see also Fig. S2), we could not find another dynamical explanation for the combination of changes, consistent with the limited number of bifurcation mechanisms that are possible in a 2-D model [37], which argues that the identified switch is also necessary for the excitability changes. In a higher dimensional system, the same switch in spiking mechanism is sufficient to explain excitability changes but we cannot prove that it is necessary, although parsimony argues in favor of our explanation. We now shift our focus from what a switch in spike initiation can explain, to what explains the switch in spike initiation. Specifically, we will now consider the biophysical meaning of a neuropathic change in parameter βw and whether such a change is biologically plausible. Later, we will consider the effects of changing other parameters.
Biophysical basis for a neuropathic change in spike initiation
Strictly speaking, βw represents the voltage at half-maximal activation of the slow current, I slow (see Eqn. 4). Increasing βw from −21 mV to −13 mV caused a rightward shift in I slow-V curve of the model neuron ( Fig. 7A ), which corresponds to a shift in the w-nullcline ( Fig. 7A inset); shifting the w-nullcline changes how the V- and w-nullclines intersect, which has direct implications for spike initiation dynamics (see Fig. 2 ). These results therefore argue that an injury-induced change in the voltage-dependency of I slow could account for repetitive spiking; indeed, variations in the voltage-sensitivity of ion channel gating occur and are associated with hyperexcitability [e.g. 38], [39], [40]. However, most previous research has focused on whether expression of certain ion channels is up- or down-regulated after nerve injury [41]. Varying βw can also account for changes in ion channel density if one understands how our “minimal” computer model was constructed; specifically, I slow represents the sum of all ion currents with slow gating kinetics [42], meaning I slow comprises multiple currents that could be modeled individually. To explain expression changes, we can ungroup I slow into parts that have more specific molecular correlates. To this end, we replaced I slow with a delayed rectifier K+ current I K,dr and a subthreshold inward or outward current I sub whose activation properties are shown in Fig. 7B . This converts the model from 2-D to 3-D, i.e. with three activation variables instead of just two (compare Eqns. 1 and 11). Increasing inward I sub (akin to upregulating Nav1.3 expression [43]) or decreasing outward I sub (akin to downregulating Kv1 expression [44]) shifted the (I K,dr+I sub)−V curve in the 3-D model ( Fig. 7C and D , respectively) the same way as making βw less negative shifted the I slow-V curve in the 2-D model (cf. Fig. 7A ); in other words, distinct molecular changes are equally capable of causing repetitive spiking, consistent with the equivalent effects of such changes on spike initiation (see bifurcation diagrams in Fig. 7C and D ).
These results demonstrate that a single parameter change in our minimal 2-D model can represent more than one biological change. Furthermore, other parameters in the 2-D model affect spike initiation. To investigate this, we varied other parameters one at a time and measured excitability. Starting from the same “normal” state (see Fig. 2A top), each parameter change was able to produce comparable “neuropathic” states as evidenced by a Hopf bifurcation ( Fig. 8A–D , right panels). Phase planes (left panels) illustrate how different parameter changes morph either the V- or w-nullclines – the important observation is that each change produced the same geometrical alteration in how the two nullclines intersect, which is a direct reflection of how fast and slow currents compete during spike initiation (see Fig. S1). Like for the biological meaning of a change in βw (see Fig. 7 ), we can reasonably posit that other parameter changes in the 2-D model can each represent more than one biological change.
These data emphasize the degeneracy of the molecular substrate for cellular excitability, i.e. that distinct molecular pathologies can yield the same pattern of cellular hyperexcitability (see Introduction). In fact, changing any parameter in the 2-D model can potentially produce the neuropathic change in spike initiation because our minimal model contains only the necessary and sufficient variables required to explain spike generation. If a conductance that does not influence spike initiation (e.g. one that activates only at suprathreshold potentials) is added to the 2-D model (thus producing a 3-D model comparable to that used in Fig. 7 ), altering that conductance will not affect excitability (Fig. S4). Notably, injury-induced changes in such currents could occur and would be correlated with cellular hyperexcitability (and neuropathic pain) without the molecular and cellular changes being causally related (see Fig. 1D ). This illustrates the insensitivity of the pathological process to certain parameters [45]. On the other hand, a change in a functionally important parameter might also fail to produce cellular hyperexcitability if that change is offset by a simultaneous change in a second parameter ( Fig. 9A ). We illustrate this in Fig. 9 using our 2-D model, but the importance of this observation is even greater for higher-dimensional models. Consider that biological changes accounted for by the same parameter might offset one another so that the parameter itself (in the 2-D model) does not change; for example, a large increase in inward I sub could be offset by a large increase in outward I sub (in the 3-D model) such that βw remains unchanged (in the 2-D model). The last two examples highlight the importance of co-variability across parameters [45]. Notably, small co-variations may also combine to cause hyperexcitability ( Fig. 9B ). Insensitivity and co-variability are both routinely overlooked in studies reporting injury-induced changes in ion channel expression, thus compromising the definitive interpretation of those studies.
Discussion
In this study, we combined computer modeling and analysis based on dynamical systems theory to rigorously explain neuropathic changes in the excitability of primary somatosensory afferents. Beyond reproducing the quantitative increase in excitability, our results demonstrate that three qualitative changes in excitability (i.e. repetitive spiking, MPOs and bursting) all arise from a switch in the nonlinear mechanism responsible for spike initiation. In dynamical terms, spike initiation switches from a quasi-separatrix-crossing under normal conditions to a Hopf bifurcation under neuropathic conditions. These dynamical mechanisms represent different outcomes in the competition between positive and negative feedback, and generalize to different feedback mechanisms (e.g. Na+ channel inactivation vs. K+ channel activation). In biophysical terms, this switch occurs when competition between currents contributing to spike initiation becomes biased. A multitude of different changes in different channels are sufficient to produce the equivalent switch in spike initiation dynamics by biasing the competition in favor of inward current. A switch in spike initiation dynamics is thus sufficient (and very likely necessary) to explain neuropathic changes in excitability; by comparison, many different molecular changes may be sufficient to switch the spike initiation mechanism, but no single molecular change is strictly necessary if more than one sufficient change is triggered by nerve injury (see Introduction and Fig. 10 ).
Following a “top-down” modeling approach, we focused first on finding generic solutions to explain excitability changes, and only thereafter did we consider how such solutions might be biologically implemented. This contrasts a “bottom-up” approach of incorporating previously described injury-induced molecular changes into a computer model to investigate whether those molecular changes can account for changes in excitability. The two approaches are complementary but, prior to this study, the former had not been used to investigate primary afferent hyperexcitability, the implication being that no one has previously sought to specifically identify which molecular changes are sufficient to explain documented changes in excitability. Animal models of neuropathic pain can arguably recreate clinically relevant human conditions quite accurately, but such models exhibit a vast array of changes at multiple levels, which can make interpreting those changes prohibitively difficult, especially with respect to causality; irrelevant correlations are probably quite common ( Fig. 10 scenario 1). Our approach depends upon the empirical data collected in animal models of neuropathic pain (1) to identify cellular changes requiring explanation, and (2) to identify candidate molecular pathologies with which to explain those cellular changes. But with overwhelming amounts of such data already collected, an alternative approach such as ours is needed to help interpret those data.
Invariably, the question seems to arise: pathological alteration of which ion current is responsible (i.e. necessary) for hyperexcitability? Our results demonstrate that the answer is not straightforward (see Fig. 10 ). Any change in any current has context-dependent effects insofar as that altered current interacts with other currents. Indeed, a single mutation in Nav1.7 channels can increase or decrease excitability depending on the other channels present in the neuron [46]. Therefore, the more important issue is if and how interactions between currents are altered, and more specifically, whether that alteration manifests a switch in spike initiation dynamics. We would argue that one should ask: of all the changes caused by nerve injury, which are sufficient (alone or in combination) to produce cellular hyperexcitability? Experimentally testing the sufficiency of each change and combinations thereof for producing each aspect of excitability is impractical. Empirical testing in computer models is more feasible, but ideally a more strategic, theory-based approach could be employed. Our identification of spike initiation as the crucial nonlinearity linking simple molecular changes with complex cellular changes constrains the search to those currents affecting spike initiation. Only currents active at subthreshold potentials can contribute (directly) to the spike initiation process. Suprathreshold currents like high-voltage-activated Cav1 and 2 channels are present in somatosensory afferents and contribute to processes like synaptic transmission. Those currents can be altered by nerve injury [47] and such changes could contribute to neuropathic pain, but any association with primary afferent hyperexcitability is likely to be purely correlative and therapies targeting those currents would be ineffective at reversing hyperexcitability; however, the analgesic efficacy of gabapentenoid drugs acting on the α2-δ subunit of Cav1 and 2 channels [48], [49] clearly implicates other mechanisms as critical factors for pain processing. But even if primary afferent hyperexcitability is neither necessary nor sufficient to produce neuropathic pain (see Introduction), primary afferent hyperexcitability is an important contributing factor [3]. Our data highlight the nuances and complicated nature of the molecular basis for just this one contributing factor.
Although we have ruled in subthreshold currents as playing a critical role in spike initiation and excitability, it cannot be assumed that even large changes in a subthreshold current will necessarily lead to cellular hyperexcitability, let alone neuropathic pain. Cellular excitability may or may not be affected depending on concurrent changes in other currents that could offset the first change ( Fig. 10 , scenarios 3 and 4). This raises an important point: most studies have focused on characterizing injury-induced changes in one channel, or perhaps a few channels, but a definitive explanation of whether such changes are sufficient to cause cellular hyperexcitability requires one to account for co-variations by measuring expression levels of all channels involved in spike initiation. Worse yet, this complete analysis should be done on a cell-by-cell basis since different neurons may achieve equivalent excitability based on different combinations of channel densities [50], [51], [52]. Beyond expression levels, we must also consider that channel function can be modulated [53].
In terms of developing new analgesics that are more effective against neuropathic pain, it is arguably less important to understand precisely which molecular changes combine to cause hyperexcitability than it is to understand which molecular processes should be targeted when trying to therapeutically reverse hyperexcitability. In other words, one need not reverse underlying molecular changes if cellular excitability can be normalized via other (potentially more druggable) targets. By this logic, one should focus on the sufficiency (and likely necessity) of a dynamical change in spike initiation for explaining hyperexcitability rather than being preoccupied with the degeneracy of the molecular basis for that switch. Indeed, whereas the diversity of molecular changes contributing to hyperexcitability makes it difficult to understand the exact pathogenic process (see above), that same diversity broadens the range of drug targets whereby hyperexcitability might be corrected. However, degeneracy of the molecular substrate for cellular excitability has another important implication: if neuropathic pain results from maladaptive plasticity [20], [54], then the therapeutic efficacy achieved through effects on one ion channel could be offset (i.e. nullified) by misguided homeostatic plasticity in any one of several other ion channels. This might help explain the relative intractability of neuropathic pain, and suggests that we must be prepared to block or reverse changes in each and every ion channel affected by maladaptive homeostatic plasticity or, better yet, that we address the homeostatic plasticity rules themselves rather than the diverse agents upon which those rules act. The diversity of molecular pathologies contributing to neuropathic pain is arguably well recognized, but the implications described above are not. Our results caution against that oversight.
Our results also demonstrate how simple molecular changes can manifest complex changes in cellular excitability because of how molecular processes interact. The nonlinear nature of those interactions is key for enabling small quantitative changes in the interacting processes to cause large qualitative changes in the outcome. Indeed, this is the case for spike initiation insofar as there is a critical point (defined by a balance of time- and voltage-dependent feedback mechanisms) that determines whether an all-or-none spike will be produced. Notably, if a simple conductance-based model can accurately reproduce phenomena of interest, then it is necessarily true that a more complicated, biologically realistic model (i.e. with more variables and parameters) will be able to do the same. That said, finding the required set of parameter values within the larger parameter space of the more complicated model may prove prohibitively difficult. Nonetheless, either type of model could be used to study the phenomena – the choice of model depends on the specific questions and how one intends to answer them. In our case, we started with a simple model because we sought to rigorously characterize (pathological disruption of) fundamental mechanisms of excitability using tools like phase-plane analysis. Once that characterization was complete, we increased the biological realism of our model (see Fig. 7 ) to facilitate its biological interpretation. Our results demonstrate the utility of such an approach.
To summarize, our results demonstrate that a switch in spike initiation mechanism is sufficient and most likely necessary to explain a constellation of neuropathic changes in primary afferent excitability. Pathological alteration of the dynamical mechanism responsible for spike initiation, rather than pathological alteration of any one ion channel, is key for explaining cellular hyperexcitability. Indeed, the molecular basis for a change in spike initiation dynamics is highly degenerate insofar as a multitude of different molecular changes, either alone or in combination, can manifest the same change in cellular excitability. Ultimately, strategically intervening to reduce neuropathic pain requires that we thoroughly understand and capitalize on rather than be thwarted by the complexity and degeneracy of the underlying mechanisms.
Methods
Two-dimensional model
Simulations were conducted in modified versions of the Morris-Lecar model. All models are single compartment, which is important for enabling our analysis but is also an accurate representation of acutely isolated somata used in most electrophysiological experiments on dorsal root ganglion neurons, including those reproduced in Fig. 1 . The simplest, 2-D version of our model consists of a fast activation variable V and a slower recovery variable w [27], [33], [55]. V denotes membrane potential and m and w are gating variables.
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
such that m changes instantaneously with V whereas w changes with a time constant τw. Unless otherwise indicated, parameters were C = 2 µF/cm2, E Na = 50 mV, E K = −100 mV, E leak = −70 mV, ϕw = 0.15, g fast = 20 mS/cm2, g slow = 20 mS/cm2, g leak = 2 mS/cm2, βm = −1.2 mV, γm = 18 mV, γw = 10 mV, and βw was varied. These values were taken directly from [27] except that βw was freely adjusted in Fig. 2 in order to reproduce different spiking patterns; thereafter, parameter values were not adjusted to reproduce other response properties. I stim represents injected current. Where indicated, Gaussian white noise n(t) with 0 mean and 20 mV2 variance was added. This weak noise approximates effects of stochastic channel opening. DRG somata do not experience synaptic noise because they do not bear any synapses.
Three-dimensional model with slow sodium channel inactivation
Slow Na+ channel inactivation was incorporated into the 2-D model by inserting an inactivation variable h according to
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
where βh = 30 mV, γh = −5 mV, τh = 2000 ms, γm = 15 mV, and all other parameters were unchanged from our standard 2-D models.
Three-dimensional model with adaptation
The model with adaptation mediated by an AHP current I AHP [56] was modeled according to
(9) |
(10) |
where z controls activation of I AHP current with parameters gadapt = 0.5 mS/cm2, βz = 0 mV, γz = 4 mV, τz = 300 ms. All other parameters were unchanged from our standard 2-D models.
Three-dimensional “ungrouped” model
In the standard 2-D model, g fast and g slow represent the sum of all conductances with fast or slow activation kinetics, respectively. To help interpret the biophysical meaning of changing βw in the 2-D model (see Results), we ungrouped I slow into a delayed rectifier K+ current I K,dr and a subthreshold current I sub that is either inward or outward depending on E sub. Activation of I K,dr and I sub are controlled by w* and y, respectively, as described by
(11) |
where differential equations for w* and y are the same form as Eqn. 2, and steady state values and time constants are calculated as per Eqns. 4 and 5 with parameters ϕw* = 0.15, βw* = −21 mV or −13 mV, γw* = 10 mV, ϕy = 0.3, Esub = ENa = 50 mV for inward current and ϕy = 0.15, Esub = EK = −100 mV for outward current. Values of g sub, βy and γy were adjusted to reproduce effects of varying βw in the 2-D model and are indicated in the legend of Fig. 7 . All other parameters were as in the 2-D model.
Simulation methods and dynamical analysis
Simulations were run in XPP [57] using the Euler method with 0.01–0.05 ms time step. Phase plane analysis was conducted in XPP and bifurcation analysis was conducted with AUTO using the XPP interface. Excellent introductions to these forms of nonlinear dynamical analysis are available in [26], [33], [34], [37]. In Fig. 2C , the quasi-separatrix (QS) was plotted on the V-w phase plane by integrating backward in time starting from the point indicated by *. That point was chosen based on where forward trajectories (with slightly different starting values) eventually diverge after clinging to the QS. The region of divergence marks the end of the QS and is therefore a good location from which to begin plotting the reverse trajectory representing the QS.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Stéphanie Ratté for comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
This work was supported by NIH grant NS 074146 and by scholar awards from the Rita Allen Foundation and the Edward Mallinckrodt Jr. Foundation to SAP. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Devor M. Neuropathic pain and injured nerve: peripheral mechanisms. Br Med Bull. 1991;47:619–630. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a072496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gracely RH, Lynch SA, Bennett GJ. Painful neuropathy: altered central processing maintained dynamically by peripheral input. Pain. 1992;51:175–194. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90259-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gold MS, Gebhart GF. Nociceptor sensitization in pain pathogenesis. Nat Med. 2010;16:1248–1257. doi: 10.1038/nm.2235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Liu CN, Devor M, Waxman SG, Kocsis JD. Subthreshold oscillations induced by spinal nerve injury in dissociated muscle and cutaneous afferents of mouse DRG. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:2009–2017. doi: 10.1152/jn.00705.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Song Y, Li HM, Xie RG, Yue ZF, Song XJ, et al. Evoked bursting in injured Abeta dorsal root ganglion neurons: A mechanism underlying tactile allodynia. Pain. 2012;153:657–665. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ma C, LaMotte RH. Multiple sites for generation of ectopic spontaneous activity in neurons of the chronically compressed dorsal root ganglion. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14059–14068. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3699-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Xing JL, Hu SJ, Long KP. Subthreshold membrane potential oscillations of type A neurons in injured DRG. Brain Res. 2001;901:128–136. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02329-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Amir R, Liu CN, Kocsis JD, Devor M. Oscillatory mechanism in primary sensory neurones. Brain. 2002;125:421–435. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ma C, LaMotte RH. Enhanced excitability of dissociated primary sensory neurons after chronic compression of the dorsal root ganglion in the rat. Pain. 2005;113:106–112. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Amir R, Michaelis M, Devor M. Membrane potential oscillations in dorsal root ganglion neurons: role in normal electrogenesis and neuropathic pain. J Neurosci. 1999;19:8589–8596. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-08589.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Liu CN, Michaelis M, Amir R, Devor M. Spinal nerve injury enhances subthreshold membrane potential oscillations in DRG neurons: relation to neuropathic pain. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:205–215. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.1.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jian Z, Xing JL, Yang GS, Hu SJ. A novel bursting mechanism of type a neurons in injured dorsal root ganglia. Neurosignals. 2004;13:150–156. doi: 10.1159/000076569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Liu CN, Wall PD, Ben Dor E, Michaelis M, Amir R, et al. Tactile allodynia in the absence of C-fiber activation: altered firing properties of DRG neurons following spinal nerve injury. Pain. 2000;85:503–521. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00251-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Koltzenburg M, Torebjork HE, Wahren LK. Nociceptor modulated central sensitization causes mechanical hyperalgesia in acute chemogenic and chronic neuropathic pain. Brain. 1994;117(Pt 3):579–591. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.3.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Campbell JN, Raja SN, Meyer RA, Mackinnon SE. Myelinated afferents signal the hyperalgesia associated with nerve injury. Pain. 1988;32:89–94. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90027-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Torsney C, MacDermott AB. Disinhibition opens the gate to pathological pain signaling in superficial neurokinin 1 receptor-expressing neurons in rat spinal cord. J Neurosci. 2006;26:1833–1843. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4584-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Keller AF, Beggs S, Salter MW, De Koninck Y. Transformation of the output of spinal lamina I neurons after nerve injury and microglia stimulation underlying neuropathic pain. Mol Pain. 2007;3:27. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-3-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Campbell JN, Meyer RA. Mechanisms of neuropathic pain. Neuron. 2006;52:77–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ji RR, Strichartz G. Cell signaling and the genesis of neuropathic pain. Sci STKE. 2004;2004:reE14. doi: 10.1126/stke.2522004re14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Costigan M, Scholz J, Woolf CJ. Neuropathic pain: a maladaptive response of the nervous system to damage. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Nassar MA, Levato A, Stirling LC, Wood JN. Neuropathic pain develops normally in mice lacking both Na(v)1.7 and Na(v)1.8. Mol Pain. 2005;1:24. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-1-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nassar MA, Stirling LC, Forlani G, Baker MD, Matthews EA, et al. Nociceptor-specific gene deletion reveals a major role for Nav1.7 (PN1) in acute and inflammatory pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:12706–12711. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0404915101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Edelman GM, Gally JA. Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:13763–13768. doi: 10.1073/pnas.231499798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mogil JS. Animal models of pain: progress and challenges. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:283–294. doi: 10.1038/nrn2606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Campbell JN, Basbaum AI, Dray A, Dubner R, Dworkin RH, et al., editors. Emerging Strategies for the Treatment of Neuropathic Pain. Seattle: IASP Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Strogatz SH. Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos: With Applications to Physics, Biology, Chemistry, and Engineering. Don Mills, ON: Addison-Wesley; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Prescott SA, De Koninck Y, Sejnowski TJ. Biophysical basis for three distinct dynamical mechanisms of action potential initiation. PLoS ComputBiol. 2008;4:e1000198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dorval AD, White JA. Channel noise is essential for perithreshold oscillations in entorhinal stellate neurons. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10025–10028. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3557-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Golomb D, Donner K, Shacham L, Shlosberg D, Amitai Y, et al. Mechanisms of firing patterns in fast-spiking cortical interneurons. PLoS ComputBiol. 2007;3:e156. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kovalsky Y, Amir R, Devor M. Simulation in sensory neurons reveals a key role for delayed Na+ current in subthreshold oscillations and ectopic discharge: implications for neuropathic pain. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:1430–1442. doi: 10.1152/jn.00005.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Choi JS, Waxman SG. Physiological interactions between Nav1.7 and Nav1.8 sodium channels: A computer simulation study. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:3173–3184. doi: 10.1152/jn.00100.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Prescott SA, De Koninck Y. Gain control of firing rate by shunting inhibition: roles of synaptic noise and dendritic saturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:2076–2081. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337591100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rinzel J, Ermentrout GB. Analysis of neural excitability and oscillations. In: Koch C, Segev I, editors. Methods in Neuronal Modeling: From Ions to Networks. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1998. pp. 251–291. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Borisyuk A, Rinzel J. Understanding neuronal dynamics by geometrical dissection of minimal models. In: Chow C, Gutkin B, Hansel D, Meunier C, Dalibard J, editors. Methods and Models in Neurophysics. Proc Les Houches Summer School: Elsevier; 2005. pp. 19–72. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Amir R, Michaelis M, Devor M. Burst discharge in primary sensory neurons: triggered by subthreshold oscillations, maintained by depolarizing afterpotentials. J Neurosci. 2002;22:1187–1198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-01187.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Prescott SA, Ratté S, De Koninck Y, Sejnowski TJ. Pyramidal neurons switch from integrators in vitro to resonators under in vivo-like conditions. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:3030–3042. doi: 10.1152/jn.90634.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Izhikevich EM. Dynamical Systems in Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wang JA, Lin W, Morris T, Banderali U, Juranka PF, et al. Membrane trauma and Na+ leak from Nav1.6 channels. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol. 2009;297:C823–834. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.00505.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Emery EC, Young GT, Berrocoso EM, Chen L, McNaughton PA. HCN2 ion channels play a central role in inflammatory and neuropathic pain. Science. 2011;333:1462–1466. doi: 10.1126/science.1206243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Choi JS, Cheng X, Foster E, Leffler A, Tyrrell L, et al. Alternative splicing may contribute to time-dependent manifestation of inherited erythromelalgia. Brain. 2010;133:1823–1835. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Costigan M, Befort K, Karchewski L, Griffin RS, D'Urso D, et al. Replicate high-density rat genome oligonucleotide microarrays reveal hundreds of regulated genes in the dorsal root ganglion after peripheral nerve injury. BMC Neurosci. 2002;3:16. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-3-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kepler TB, Abbott LF, Marder E. Reduction of conductance-based neuron models. Biol Cybern. 1992;66:381–387. doi: 10.1007/BF00197717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Waxman SG, Kocsis JD, Black JA. Type III sodium channel mRNA is expressed in embryonic but not adult spinal sensory neurons, and is reexpressed following axotomy. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:466–470. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.1.466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Yang EK, Takimoto K, Hayashi Y, de Groat WC, Yoshimura N. Altered expression of potassium channel subunit mRNA and alpha-dendrotoxin sensitivity of potassium currents in rat dorsal root ganglion neurons after axotomy. Neuroscience. 2004;123:867–874. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Marder E. Variability, compensation, and modulation in neurons and circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(Suppl 3):15542–15548. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010674108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rush AM, Dib-Hajj SD, Liu S, Cummins TR, Black JA, et al. A single sodium channel mutation produces hyper- or hypoexcitability in different types of neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:8245–8250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602813103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Baccei ML, Kocsis JD. Voltage-gated calcium currents in axotomized adult rat cutaneous afferent neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2227–2238. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.4.2227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sutton KG, Martin DJ, Pinnock RD, Lee K, Scott RH. Gabapentin inhibits high-threshold calcium channel currents in cultured rat dorsal root ganglion neurones. Br J Pharmacol. 2002;135:257–265. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0704439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bauer CS, Tran-Van-Minh A, Kadurin I, Dolphin AC. A new look at calcium channel alpha2delta subunits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:563–571. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Schulz DJ, Goaillard JM, Marder E. Variable channel expression in identified single and electrically coupled neurons in different animals. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:356–362. doi: 10.1038/nn1639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Golowasch J, Goldman MS, Abbott LF, Marder E. Failure of averaging in the construction of a conductance-based neuron model. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1129–1131. doi: 10.1152/jn.00412.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Swensen AM, Bean BP. Robustness of burst firing in dissociated purkinje neurons with acute or long-term reductions in sodium conductance. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3509–3520. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3929-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cantrell AR, Catterall WA. Neuromodulation of Na+ channels: an unexpected form of cellular plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:397–407. doi: 10.1038/35077553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wang G, Thompson SM. Maladaptive homeostatic plasticity in a rodent model of central pain syndrome: thalamic hyperexcitability after spinothalamic tract lesions. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11959–11969. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3296-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Morris C, Lecar H. Voltage oscillations in the barnacle giant muscle fiber. Biophys J. 1981;35:193–213. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(81)84782-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Prescott SA, Sejnowski TJ. Spike-rate coding and spike-time coding are affected oppositely by different adaptation mechanisms. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13649–13661. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1792-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ermentrout B. Simulating, Analyzing, and Animating Dynamical Systems: A Guide to XPPAUT for Researchers and Students. Philadelphia, PA: SIAM; 2002. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.