Skip to main content
The British Journal of General Practice logoLink to The British Journal of General Practice
. 2012 May 28;62(599):e446–e450. doi: 10.3399/bjgp12X649142

Comparing performance among male and female candidates in sex-specific clinical knowledge in the MRCGP

A Niroshan Siriwardena 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Bill Irish 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Zahid B Asghar 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Hilton Dixon 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Paul Milne 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Catherine Neden 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Jo Richardson 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, Carol Blow 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8
PMCID: PMC3361125  PMID: 22687238

Abstract

Background

Patients often seek doctors of the same sex, particularly for sex-specific complaints and also because of a perception that doctors have greater knowledge of complaints relating to their own sex. Few studies have investigated differences in knowledge by sex of candidate on sex-specific questions in medical examinations.

Aim

The aim was to compare the performance of males and females in sex-specific questions in a 200-item computer-based applied knowledge test for licensing UK GPs.

Design and setting

A cross-sectional design using routinely collected performance and demographic data from the first three versions of the Applied Knowledge Test, MRCGP, UK.

Method

Questions were classified as female specific, male specific, or sex neutral. The performance of males and females was analysed using multiple analysis of covariance after adjusting for sex-neutral score and demographic confounders.

Results

Data were included from 3627 candidates. After adjusting for sex-neutral score, age, time since qualification, year of speciality training, ethnicity, and country of primary medical qualification, there were differences in performance in sex-specific questions. Males performed worse than females on female-specific questions (–4.2%, 95% confidence interval [CI] = –5.7 to –2.6) but did not perform significantly better than females on male-specific questions (0.3%, 95% CI = –2.6 to 3.2%.

Conclusion

There was evidence of better performance by females in female-specific questions but this was small relative to the size of the test. Differential performance of males and females in sex-specific questions in a licensing examination may have implications for vocational and post-qualification general practice training.

Keywords: assessment, general practice, learning, medical education, primary health care, sex

INTRODUCTION

Patients seeking medical advice in a primary care setting can usually elect to consult any available GP working at their practice. Studies have shown that approximately a third of patients in general practice would rather see a doctor of their own sex, while the remainder have no preference.1 The term ‘sex’ is used rather than ‘gender’ in this article because ‘sex’ refers to the biological and physiological characteristics that define males and females, whereas ‘gender’ refers to the socially constructed roles, behaviours, activities, and attributes that a given society considers appropriate for males and females.2

Sex preferences in relation to choice of doctor — that is, preference for a male or female doctor — are more evident in specialties such as general practice, or those involving intimate or psychosocial problems,3,4 than in more technical specialities such as anaesthetics or surgery.5 However, a minority of patients express a preference for a doctor of the same sex, whether they are seeking care in technical specialities6,7 or in general practice, which extends beyond the management of sex-specific problems.8

The reasons for predilection for a doctor of the same sex are likely to be complex: as well as the perceived practical ease of being examined by someone of the same sex, these may include beliefs about, and preferences for, knowledge, competence, and interpersonal skills.9,10 Stereotyping of physician attributes by patients may extend to the belief that a doctor of the same sex as the patient will have a significantly greater knowledge of clinical problems specific to that sex. Female patients commonly consult their GP with sex-specific issues, such as breast or gynaecological disease, and male patients may consult with problems such as erectile dysfunction or prostatic disease. Although female doctors are known to perform better overall in high-stakes medical examinations11,12 and in sex-specific domains, for example in obstetrics and gynaecology,13 or other areas such as paediatrics,14,15 there is limited published evidence showing differences between male and female GPs in knowledge of sex-specific clinical areas at licensure. Such differences may have important implications for general practice training.

The Membership of the Royal College of General Practitioners (MRCGP)16 is the licensing examination for UK-trained family doctors that certifies their fitness for independent practice. This provides a Certificate of Completion of Training (CCT) in general practice and entry to the General Practice Register of the General Medical Council.17 The assessment includes three components — the Applied Knowledge Test (AKT), a clinical skills assessment, and workplace-based assessments — which, together, assess the curriculum for specialty training for general practice.18

How this fits in

Males and females show differences in performance in medical examinations, including tests that assess sex-specific issues such as obstetrics and gynaecology. This study demonstrates that there was significantly better performance in females compared with males regarding knowledge on female health in a general practice licensing examination. Factors that determine variations in performance for different question types deserve further investigation.

The AKT is a 3-hour, 200-item multiple-choice test, which assesses knowledge of clinical medicine (80%), evidence-based medicine (10%), and administrative issues (10%) relevant to UK general practice; it uses the single-best answer and extended modified-question formats. The test is constructed by a group of GPs with expertise in item writing; individual question performance is analysed using classical test theory, which enables refinement of poorly performing questions by this group.19 Each AKT examination includes a number of sex-specific questions, that is, items on female and male health, which sample across the curriculum.

The aim of this study was to compare differences in the knowledge of male and female candidates with regard to sex-specific questions tested in the AKT.

METHOD

A cross-sectional design was employed using routinely collected performance and demographic data from the first three versions of the AKT. Questions testing knowledge in the first three AKT papers (AKT1, AKT2, and AKT3) taken by candidates over a 1-year period were classified independently by each of the authors as female specific (that is, relating to female health problems), male specific, or sex neutral. Box 1 gives an example of a female-specific question. Inter-rater agreement was assessed using Cohen's kappa: the value of kappa for any two sets of coders was ≥0.75. In the case of disagreement, the item was assigned according to the majority view. The three examinations each comprising 200 questions included seven, seven, and six male-specific questions and 19, 14, and 20 female-specific questions respectively.

Box 1. Example of a female-specific question.

The investigation of infertility

A 25-year-old woman has been infertile for 3 years. She has had recurrent pelvic pain due to endometriosis. Investigations in primary care are reported as follows:

  • Full blood count: normal

  • Thyroid function: normal

  • Sex hormone profile: normal

  • Rubella serology: immune

  • Day 21 progesterone: normal

  • Chlamydia swab: negative

Her partner's semen analysis is reported as normal. A referral to secondary care is agreed and she asks what is likely to happen next.

According to current guidelines, which is the SINGLE MOST appropriate NEXT investigation? Select ONE option only

  1. Cervical cytology

  2. Hysterosalpingogram

  3. Hysteroscopy

  4. Laparoscopy

  5. Post-coital test

Examination scores and demographic data, including candidate sex, age, year of qualification, stage of training, ethnic group, and place of primary medical qualification, were obtained from the MRCGP examination department for each examination candidate. Ethical approval was granted by the University of Lincoln Research Ethics Committee. All data were anonymised.

The association of candidate sex with scores for sex-specific questions was determined using multivariate analysis of covariance, correcting for other confounders including sex-neutral test score, years since qualification, stage of general practice specialty training, ethnicity, and country of primary medical qualification.

Categorical variables were created from continuous variables using cut-off points created as close as possible to quartiles but with whole years. Categories were established for age (25–29 years, 30–31 years, 32–35 years, and 36–70 years), years since qualification (0–4 years, 5–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–39 years), stage of general practice specialty training (Speciality Year 1, Speciality Year 2, Speciality Year 3, fully trained, or summative assessment), ethnicity (white, Asian, African-Caribbean, mixed, other, and unknown), and country of primary medical qualification (UK; European Union and Russia; US, Canada and Oceania; Africa; and South Asia).

A quantile-quantile (Q-Q) plot of residuals was performed to validate model fitness. All interactions were fitted and non-significant effects dropped after applying a Bonferroni correction because of multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

The analysis included examination data from 3627 candidates, 45.5% of whom were males and 54.5% of whom were females. Candidates’ characteristics are detailed in Table 1. The mean female-specific score was higher for females (83.2%; standard deviation [SD] 9.8%) compared with males (77.1%; SD 11.7%; P < 0.001). The mean male-specific score was not significantly different for males (64.1%; SD 20.2%) and females (65.0%; SD 19.6%; P = 0.19) (Table 2).

Table 1.

Summary characteristics of 3627 female and male candidates

Female, n (%) Male, n (%) Total
Age group, years
 25–29 840 (46.1) 490 (32.3) 1330
 30–31 592 (32.5) 594 (39.1) 1186
 32–35 231 (12.7) 214 (14.1) 445
 36–70 158 (8.7) 221 (14.6) 379
 Total 1821 (100.0) 1519 (100.0) 3340

Years since qualification
 0–4 14 (0.9) 9 (0.7) 23
 5–6 1125 (72.5) 752 (61.3) 1877
 7–8 279 (18.0) 305 (24.9) 584
 9–39 134 (8.6) 161 (13.1) 295
 Total 1552 (100.0) 1227 (100.0) 2779

Stage of training
 Speciality Year 1 30 (1.6) 26 (1.7) 56
 Speciality Year 2 282 (14.9) 236 (15.1) 518
 Speciality Year 3 1266 (66.7) 966 (61.8) 2232
 Fully trained 320 (16.9) 335 (21.4) 655
 Total 1898 (100.0) 1563 (100.0) 3461

Ethnic group
 White 1077 (56.4) 534 (33.6) 1611
 Asian 679 (35.6) 863 (54.4) 1542
 African-Caribbean 62 (3.3) 85 (5.4) 147
 Mixed 31 (1.6) 27 (1.7) 58
 Other 58 (3.0) 78 (4.9) 136
 Unknown 49 (2.6) 31 (2.0) 80
 Total 1907 (100.0) 1587 (100.0) 3494

Place of primary medical qualification
 Africa 56 (2.8) 88 (5.4) 144
 EU and Russia 135 (6.8) 114 (6.9) 249
 South Asia 410 (20.7) 571 (34.8) 981
 UK 1263 (63.7) 763 (46.4) 2026
 US, Canada, and Oceania 37 (1.9) 34 (2.1) 71
 Other 83 (4.2) 73 (4.4) 156
 Total 1984 (100.1) 1643 (100.0) 3627

Totals less than 3627 indicate missing data for these variables. EU = European Union.

Table 2.

Performance of female and male candidates in sex-related questions

Females (%) Males (%) Total

n Mean SD n Mean SD n
Total score 1984 76.4 7.7 1643 73.4 8.1 3627

Sex-neutral score 1984 74.0 8.3 1643 70.8 8.7 3627

Male-specific score 1984 65.0 19.6 1643 64.1 20.2 3627

Female-specific score 1984 83.2 9.8 1643 77.1 11.7 3627

SD = standard deviation.

Males performed worse than females on female-specific questions (–4.2%, 95% CI = –5.7 to –2.6) after correcting for younger age, white ethnicity, later stage of training, and overall performance expressed as sex-neutral score that were all significant predictors for higher scores (Table 3). The Q-Q plot suggested a good statistical model fit (Figure 1). Plots of P-values showed significantly better female-specific examination scores for female compared to male candidates after applying a Bonferroni correction. For male-specific question scores, males did not perform significantly better than females (0.3%, 95% CI = –2.6 to 3.2%) after correcting for sex-neutral score and demographic factors (Table 4).

Table 3.

Regression analysis (ANCOVA) for variables predicting female-specific examination score

Female-specific question score: demographic and background factors Coefficient Standard error P-value (95% CI)
Sex –4.2 0.8 <0.001 (–5.7 to –2.6)

Age –0.3 0.1 <0.001 (–0.6 to –0.1)

Ethnicity 0.6 0.1 <0.001 (0.3 to 0.9)

Sex-neutral score –2.4 0.1 <0.001 (–2.6 to –2.1)

Stage of training 2.0 0.3 <0.001 (1.4 to 2.5)

ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The quantile-quantile plot of residuals of Ancova model.

Table 4.

Regression analysis (ANCOVA) for variables predicting male-specific examination score

Male-specific question score: demographic and background factors Coefficient Standard error P-value (95% CI)
Sex 0.3 1.5 0.8 (−2.6 to 3.2)

Age 0.0 0.2 1.0 (–0.4 to 0.4)

Ethnicity 1.1 0.3 <0.001 (0.5 to 1.6)

Sex-neutral score –2.9 0.2 <0.001 (–3.3 to –2.5)

Stage of training 2.3 0.5 <0.001 (1.3 to 3.4)

ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

DISCUSSION

Summary

This is the first study investigating differences in knowledge by sex of candidate on sex-specific questions in a medical licensing examination for general practice. These results showed that, on average, males scored worse than females on female-specific questions after controlling for other significant predictors for differences in scores. The actual difference in test performance was small due to the small number of female sex-specific questions, which comprised only 9% of questions across the three examinations. There were even fewer male-specific questions compared with female-specific questions over the three tests (20/600 [3%] versus 53/600 [9%]) which may have partly accounted for the lack of difference in male doctor performance for male-specific questions.

In contrast, males did not perform significantly differently from females in male sex-specific questions; this showed that females were just as knowledgeable about male health issues as males.

Strengths and limitations

A large number of candidates were included and the recording rates of demographic variables were high. This investigation was limited to a single knowledge test format in general practice in one developed country and showed differences in cognitive knowledge, rather than competence, performance, or psychomotor skills; the results, therefore, cannot be extrapolated to knowledge tests in other specialties or other countries. Potential confounders in the analysis were accounted for. An alternative means of investigating sex bias in responses would have been to conduct a differential item functioning analysis using item response theory.20

Comparison with existing literature

This study focused on knowledge rather than performance or clinical behaviour. Previous studies of examination performance have shown differences according to sex of candidate at different stages in trainees’ medical education.21 This contrasts with some other studies, however, that have not found sex differences in specific skills and behaviours, such as sharing management options.22

These differences, when they are present, may be partly due to the different educational experiences of male and female doctors. For example, during training, women doctors are significant more likely than men to see patients of the same sex, whereas the converse is not true,23 particularly for female problems or intimate examinations24 — these differences in experience are likely to be increased when they have a preceptor of the same sex.25 This greater exposure to sex-specific problems, rather than simply a greater interest in problems relating to their own sex, may increase motivation among female trainees to learn more about female health issues.

Differences in knowledge may partly explain sex differences in clinical behaviour, for example in test ordering,26 sexual-history taking,27 or specific healthcare interventions, such as screening.14 However, an important finding from previous research is that performance is related to clinical skills rather than a doctor's sex per se — male doctors providing routine gynaecology care28 or working in centres of excellence for female health also received high ratings from female patients.29

Implications for practice and research

In assessing clinical knowledge, it is important that sex-specific knowledge is tested. It is also important to consider the appropriate educational response to differences, albeit small, in sex-specific clinical knowledge between males and female doctors; this has been demonstrated in this study.

Increased awareness of disparities in knowledge enables these to be identified and addressed through curriculum planning and organising learning experiences appropriately. Although this is already being done in some settings in relation to practical skills, such as intimate examinations,30 and may go some way to reducing sex bias in the clinical behaviour of doctors,31 the current findings raise issues around the assimilation of sex-specific knowledge in clinical practice. For example, opportunities for trainees to spend some time with trainers of the opposite sex may be helpful.25

Current trends suggest that female doctors are seeing increasing numbers of female patients,32 which will mean that many are likely to be more proficient than male doctors in dealing with female issues during independent clinical practice Although it might be expected that males doctors are more likely to see male patients for problems such as erectile dysfunction and prostatic disease, this is not the case for trainees, a finding which may be partly explained by the low consultation rates for these conditions.23

Despite these changing working patterns and patterns of consulting behaviour among patients, training should ensure that patients consulting doctors of the opposite sex for sex-specific problems will not be disadvantaged in terms of the quality of care provided.

It is important to note that patient preferences for the sex of their doctor are more often related to interpersonal skills, clinical behaviour, and expertise, rather than sex alone.10 These behaviours and competencies may become an important area for professional development when it comes to extending future general practice training.33 Further evaluation of performance in sex-specific questions in other generalist or specialist examinations and the reasons for such differences warrants further study.

Acknowledgments

We thank the members of the AKT Development Group and MRCGP Research Reference Group who advised and commented on this study.

Ethical approval

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Lincoln School of Health and Social Care at the University of Lincoln.

Provenance

Freely submitted; externally peer reviewed.

Competing interests

All authors, aside from Bill Irish and Zahid Asghar, are members of the panel of examiners of the Royal College of General Practitioners and members of the Applied Knowledge Test (AKT) Development Group. The authors state there are no other competing interests.

Discuss this article

Contribute and read comments about this article on the Discussion Forum: http://www.rcgp.org.uk/bjgp-discuss

REFERENCES

  • 1.Fennema K, Meyer DL, Owen N. Sex of physician: patients’ preferences and stereotypes. J Fam Pract. 1990;30(4):441–446. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.World Health Organization. What do we mean by ‘sex’ and ‘gender’? in Gender, Women and Health. Geneva: WHO; 2011. http://www.who.int/gender/whatisgender/en/ (accessed 28 Mar 2012) [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Haar E, Halitsky V, Stricker G. Factors related to the preference for a female gynecologist. Med Care. 1975;13(9):782–790. doi: 10.1097/00005650-197509000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Carrejo MH, Balla DJ, Tan RS. Preference for gender of health care provider in management of erectile dysfunction. Int J Impot Res. 2007;19(5):474–479. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijir.3901553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kerssens JJ, Bensing JM, Andela MG. Patient preference for genders of health professionals. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44(10):1531–1540. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00272-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Menees SB, Inadomi JM, Korsnes S, Elta GH. Women patients’ preference for women physicians is a barrier to colon cancer screening. Gastrointest Endosc. 2005;62(2):219–223. doi: 10.1016/s0016-5107(05)00540-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tempest HV, Vowler S, Simpson A. Patients’ preference for gender of urologist. Int J Clin Pract. 2005;59(5):526–528. doi: 10.1111/j.1368-5031.2005.00465.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Graffy J. Patient choice in a practice with men and women general practitioners. Br J Gen Pract. 1990;40(330):13–15. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shah R, Ogden J. ‘What's in a face?’ The role of doctor ethnicity, age and gender in the formation of patients’ judgements: an experimental study. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;60(2):136–141. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mavis B, Vasilenko P, Schnuth R, et al. Female patients’ preferences related to interpersonal communications, clinical competence, and gender when selecting a physician. Acad Med. 2005;80(12):1159–1165. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200512000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wiskin CM, Allan TF, Skelton JR. Gender as a variable in the assessment of final year degree-level communication skills. Med Educ. 2004;38(2):129–137. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2004.01746.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cuddy MM, Swanson DB, Dillon GF, et al. A multilevel analysis of the relationships between selected examinee characteristics and United States Medical Licensing Examination Step 2 Clinical Knowledge performance: revisiting old findings and asking new questions. Acad Med. 2006;81(10 Suppl):S103–S107. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200610001-00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bienstock JL, Martin S, Tzou W, Fox HE. Medical students’ gender is a predictor of success in the obstetrics and gynecology basic clerkship. Teach Learn Med. 2002;14(4):240–243. doi: 10.1207/S15328015TLM1404_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Henderson JT, Weisman CS. Physician gender effects on preventive screening and counseling: an analysis of male and female patients’ health care experiences. Med Care. 2001;39(12):1281–1292. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200112000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McDonough CM, Horgan A, Codd MB, Casey PR. Gender differences in the results of the final medical examination at University College Dublin. Med Educ. 2000;34(1):30–34. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2923.2000.00456.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Royal College of General Practitioners. Examination for membership (MRCGP) regulations for 2008. London: RCGP; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Royal College of General Practitioners. nMRCGP: the CCT and new Membership Assessment. London: RCGP; 2008. http://www.rcgp-curriculum.org.uk/nmrgcp.aspx (accessed 28 Mar 2012) [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Riley B, Haynes J, Field S. The condensed curriculum guide for GP training and the new MRCGP. London: RCGP; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Haertal EH. Reliability. In: Brennan RL, editor. Educational measurement. 4th edn. Westport, CN: American Council on Education/Praeger; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Camilli G. Test fairness. In: Brennan RL, editor. Educational measurement. 4th edn. Westport, CN: American Council on Education/Praeger; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pritchard DJ. Effects of sex and alphabetical listing on examination performance of medical students. Med Educ. 1988;22(3):205–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.1988.tb00008.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Siriwardena AN, Edwards AG, Campion P, et al. Involve the patient and pass the MRCGP: investigating shared decision making in a consulting skills examination using a validated instrument. Br J Gen Pract. 2006;56(532):857–862. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.De Jong J, Visser MRM, Mohrs J, Waard MW. Opening the black box: the patient mix of GP trainees. Br J Gen Pract. 2011;61(591):e650–e657. doi: 10.3399/bjgp11X601361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wollstadt LJ, Gravdal J, Glasser M. Gender and the educational experience in a primary care training setting. Fam Med. 1990;22(3):210–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Levy BT, Merchant ML. Differences in clinical experiences based on gender of third-year medical students in a required family medicine preceptorship. Acad Med. 2002;77(12 pt 1):1241–1246. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200212000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sood R, Sood A, Ghosh AK. Non-evidence-based variables affecting physicians’ test-ordering tendencies: a systematic review. Neth J Med. 2007;65(5):167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Burd ID, Nevadunsky N, Bachmann G. Impact of physician gender on sexual history taking in a multispecialty practice. J Sex Med. 2006;3(2):194–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2005.00168.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bean-Mayberry BA, Chang CC, McNeil MA, Scholle SH. Ensuring high-quality primary care for women: predictors of success. Womens Health Issues. 2006;16(1):22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.whi.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Henderson JT, Hudson SS, Weisman CS, Anderson RT. The role of physician gender in the evaluation of the National Centers of Excellence in Women's Health: test of an alternate hypothesis. Womens Health Issues. 2004;14(4):130–139. doi: 10.1016/j.whi.2004.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hendrickx K, De Winter BY, Wyndaele JJ, et al. Intimate examination teaching with volunteers: implementation and assessment at the University of Antwerp. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;63(1–2):47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hamberg K, Risberg G, Johansson EE. Male and female physicians show different patterns of gender bias: a paper-case study of management of irritable bowel syndrome. Scand J Public Health. 2004;32(2):144–152. doi: 10.1080/14034940310015401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fang MC, McCarthy EP, Singer DE. Are patients more likely to see physicians of the same sex? Recent national trends in primary care medicine. Am J Med. 2004;117(8):575–581. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2004.03.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kramer AW, Düsman H, Tan LH, et al. Effect of extension of postgraduate training in general practice on the acquisition of knowledge of trainees. Fam Pract. 2003;20(2):207–212. doi: 10.1093/fampra/20.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The British Journal of General Practice are provided here courtesy of Royal College of General Practitioners

RESOURCES