Abstract
Synthesizing information on test performance metrics such as sensitivity, specificity, predictive values and likelihood ratios is often an important part of a systematic review of a medical test. Because many metrics of test performance are of interest, the meta-analysis of medical tests is more complex than the meta-analysis of interventions or associations. Sometimes, a helpful way to summarize medical test studies is to provide a “summary point”, a summary sensitivity and a summary specificity. Other times, when the sensitivity or specificity estimates vary widely or when the test threshold varies, it is more helpful to synthesize data using a “summary line” that describes how the average sensitivity changes with the average specificity. Choosing the most helpful summary is subjective, and in some cases both summaries provide meaningful and complementary information. Because sensitivity and specificity are not independent across studies, the meta-analysis of medical tests is fundamentaly a multivariate problem, and should be addressed with multivariate methods. More complex analyses are needed if studies report results at multiple thresholds for positive tests. At the same time, quantitative analyses are used to explore and explain any observed dissimilarity (heterogeneity) in the results of the examined studies. This can be performed in the context of proper (multivariate) meta-regressions.
KEY WORDS: gold standard, test performance, meta-analysis
INTRODUCTION
The series of papers in this supplement of the journal highlights common challenges in systematic reviews of medical tests and outlines their mitigation, as perceived by researchers partaking in the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Effective Healthcare Program. Generic by their very nature, these challenges and their discussion apply to the larger set of systematic reviews of medical tests, and are not specific to AHRQ’s program.
This paper focuses on choosing strategies for meta-analysis of test “accuracy”, or more preferably, test performance. Meta-analysis is not required for a systematic review, but when appropriate, it should be undertaken with a dual goal: to provide summary estimates for key quantities, and to explore and explain any observed dissimilarity (heterogeneity) in the results of the examined studies.
“Summing-up” information on test performance metrics such as sensitivity, specificity, and predictive values is rarely the most informative part of a systematic review of a medical test.1–4 Key clinical questions driving the evidence synthesis (e.g., is this test alone or in combination with a test-and-treat strategy likely to improve decision-making and patient outcomes?) are only indirectly related to test performance per se. Formulating an effective evaluation approach requires careful consideration of the context in which the test will be used. These framing issues are addressed in other papers in this issue of the journal.5–7 Further, in this paper we assume that medical test performance has been measured against a “gold standard”, that is a reference standard that is considered adequate in defining the presence or absence of the condition of interest. Another paper in this supplement discusses ways to summarize medical tests when such a reference standard does not exist.8
Syntheses of medical test data often focus on test performance, and much of the attention to statistical issues relevant to synthesizing medical test evidence focuses on summarizing test performance data; thus their meta-analysis was chosen to be the focus of this paper. We will assume that the decision to perform meta-analyses of test performance data is justified and taken, and will explore two central challenges, namely how do we quantitatively summarize medical test performance when: 1) the sensitivity and specificity estimates of various studies do not vary widely, or 2) the sensitivity and specificity of various studies vary over a large range.
Briefly, it may be helpful to use a “summary point” (a summary sensitivity and summary specificity pair) to obtain summary test performance when sensitivity and specificity estimates do not vary widely across studies. This could happen in meta-analyses where all studies have the same explicit test positivity threshold (a threshold for categorizing the results of testing as positive or negative) since if studies have different explicit thresholds, the clinical interpretation of a summary point is less obvious, and perhaps less helpful. However, an explicit common threshold is neither sufficient nor necessary for opting to synthesize data with a “summary point”; a summary point can be appropriate whenever sensitivity and specificity estimates do not vary widely across studies.
When the sensitivity and specificity of various studies vary over a large range, rather than using a “summary point”, it may be more helpful to describe how the average sensitivity and average specificity relate by means of a “summary line”. This oft-encountered situation can be secondary to explicit or implicit variation in the threshold for a “positive” test result, heterogeneity in populations, reference standards, or the index tests, study design, chance, or bias.
Of note, in many applications it may be informative to present syntheses in both ways, as they convey complementary information.
Deciding whether a “summary point” or a “summary line” is more helpful as a synthesis is subjective, and no hard-and-fast rules exist. We briefly outline common approaches for meta-analyzing medical tests, and discuss principles for choosing between them. However, a detailed presentation of methods or their practical application is outside the scope of this work. In addition, it is expected that readers are versed in clinical research methodology, and familiar with methodological issues pertinent to the study of medical tests. We also assume familiarity with the common measures of medical test performance (reviewed in the Appendix, and in excellent introductory papers).9 For example, we do not review challenges posed by methodological or reporting shortcomings of test performance studies.10 The Standards for Reporting of Diagnostic accuracy (STARD) initiative published a 25-item checklist that aims to improve reporting of medical tests studies.10 We refer readers to other papers in this issue11 and to several methodological and empirical explorations of bias and heterogeneity in medical test studies.12–14
Nonindependence of sensitivity and specificity across studies and why it matters for meta-analysis
In a typical meta-analysis of test performance, we have estimates of sensitivity and specificity for each study, and seek to provide a meaningful summary across all studies. Within each study sensitivity and specificity are independent, because they are estimated from different patients (sensitivity from those with the condition of interest, and specificity from those without). According to the prevailing reasoning, across studies sensitivity and specificity are likely negatively correlated: as one estimate increases the other is expected to decrease. This is perhaps more obvious when studies have different explicit thresholds for “positive” tests (and thus the term “threshold effect” has been used to describe this negative correlation). For example, the D-dimer concentration threshold for diagnosing an acute coronary event can vary from approximately 200 to over 600 ng/mL.15 It is expected that higher thresholds would correspond to generally lower sensitivity but higher specificity, and the opposite for lower thresholds (though in this example it is not clearly evident; see Fig. 1a). A similar rationale can be invoked to explain between-study variability for tests with more implicit or suggestive thresholds, such as imaging or histological tests.
Negative correlation between sensitivity and specificity across studies may be expected for reasons unrelated to thresholds for positive tests. For example, in a meta-analysis evaluating the ability of serial creatine kinase-MB (CK-MB) measurements to diagnose acute cardiac ischemia in the emergency department,16, 17 the time interval from the onset of symptoms to serial CK-MB measurements (rather than the actual threshold for CK-MB) could explain the relationship between sensitivity and specificity across studies. The larger the time interval, the more CK-MB is released into the bloodstream, affecting the estimated sensitivity and specificity. Unfortunately, the term “threshold effect” is often used rather loosely to describe the relationship between sensitivity and specificity across studies, even when, strictly speaking, there is no direct evidence of variability in study thresholds for positive tests.
Because of the above, the current thinking is that in general, the study estimates of sensitivity and specificity do not vary independently, but jointly, and likely with a negative correlation. Summarizing the two correlated quantities is a multivariate problem, and multivariate methods should be used to address it, as they are more theoretically motivated.18, 19 At the same time there are situations when a multivariate approach is not practically different from separate univariate analyses. We will expand on some of these issues.
PRINCIPLES FOR ADDRESSING THE CHALLENGES
To motivate our suggestions on meta-analyses of medical tests, we invoke two general principles
Principle 1: Favor the most informative way to summarize the data. Here we refer mainly to choosing between a summary point and a summary line, or both.
Principle 2: Explore the variability in study results with graphs and suitable analyses, rather than relying exclusively on “grand means”.
RECOMMENDED APPROACHES
Which metrics to meta-analyze
For each study, the estimates of sensitivity, specificity, predicitive values, likelihood ratios, and prevalence are related through simple formulas (Appendix). However, if one performed a meta-analysis for each of these metrics, the summaries across all studies will generally be inconsistent: the formulas would not be satisfied for the summary estimates. To avoid this, we propose to obtain summaries for sensitivities and specificities via meta-analysis, and to back-calculate the overall predictive values or likelihood ratios from the formulas in the Appendix, for a range of plausible prevalences. Figure 2 illustrates this strategy for a meta-analysis of K studies. We explain the rationale below.
Why it does make sense to directly meta-analyze sensitivity and specificity
Summarizing studies with respect to sensitivity and specificity aligns well with our understanding of the effect of positivity thresholds for diagnostic tests. Further, sensitivity and specificity are often considered independent of the prevalence of the condition under study (though this is an oversimplification that merits deeper discussion).20 The summary sensitivity and specificity obtained by a direct meta-analysis will always be between zero and one. Because these two metrics do not have as intuitive an interpretation as likelihood ratios or predictive values,9 we can use formulas in the Appendix to back-calculate “summary” (overall) predictive values and likelihood ratios that correspond to the summary sensitivity and specificity for a range of plausible prevalence values.
Why it does not make sense to directly meta-analyze positive and negative predictive values or prevalence
Predictive values are dependent on prevalence estimates. Because prevalence is often wide ranging, and because many medical test studies have a case-control design (where prevalence cannot be estimated), it is rarely meaningful to directly combine these across studies. Instead, predictive values can be calculated as mentioned above from the summary sensitivity and specificity for a range of plausible prevalence values.
Why directly meta-analyzing likelihood ratios could be problematic
Positive and negative likelihood ratios could also be combined in the absence of threshold variation, and in fact, many authors give explicit guidance to that effect.21 However, this practice does not guarantee that the summary positive and negative likelihood ratios are “internally consistent”. Specifically, it is possible to get summary likelihood ratios that correspond to impossible “summary” sensitivities or specificities (outside the zero to one interval).22 Back-calculating the “summary” likelihood ratios from summary sensitivities and specificities avoids this complication. Nevertheless, these aberrant cases are not common,23 and calculations of summary likelihood ratios by directly meta-analyzing them or from back calculation of the summary sensitivity and specificity rarely results in different conclusions.23
Directly meta-analyzing diagnostic odds ratios
The synthesis of diagnostic odds ratios is straightforward and follows standard meta-analysis methods.24, 25 The diagnostic odds ratio is closely linked to sensitivity, specificity, and likelihood ratios, and it can be easily included in meta-regression models to explore the impact of explanatory variables on between-study heterogeneity. Apart from challenges in interpreting diagnostic odds ratios, a disadvantage is that it is impossible to weight the true positive and false positive rates separately.
Desired characteristics of meta-analysis methods
Over several decades many methods have been used for meta-analyzing medical test performance data. Based on the above considerations, methods should be motivated by (a) respecting the multivariate nature of test performance metrics (i.e., sensitivity and specificity); (b) allowing for the nonindependence between sensitivity and specificity across studies (“threshold effect”) and (c) allowing for between-study heterogeneity. Table 1 lists commonly used methods for meta-analysis of medical tests. The most theoretically motivated meta-analysis approaches are based on multivariate methods (hierarchical modeling).
Table 1.
Method | Description or comment | Does it have desired characteristics? |
---|---|---|
Summary point | ||
Independent meta-analysis of sensitivity and specificity | Separate meta-analyses per metric | Ignores correlation between sensitivity and specificity |
Within-study variability preferably modeled by the binomial distribution.44 | Underestimates summary sensitivity and specificity and incorrect confidence intervals26 | |
Joint (multivariate) meta-analysis of sensitivity and specificity based on hierarchical modeling | Based on multivariate (joint) modeling of sensitivity and specificity. | The generally preferred method |
Two families of models26, 30 (see text), equivalent when there are no covariates18 | ||
Modeling preferably using binomial likelihood rather than normal approximations30, 37, 45, 46 | ||
Summary line | ||
Moses and Littenberg model | Summary line based on a simple regression of the difference of logit-transformed true and false positive rates versus their average.32–34 | Ignores unexplained variation between-studies (fixed effects) |
Does not account for correlation between sensitivity and specificity | ||
Does not account for variability in the independent variable | ||
Inability to weight studies optimally—yields wrong inferences when covariates are used | ||
Random intercept augmentation of the Moses-Littenberg model | Regression of the difference of logit-transformed true and false positive rates versus their average with random effects to allow for variability across studies35, 36 | Does not account for correlation between sensitivity and specificity |
Does not account for variability in the independent variable | ||
Summary ROC based on hierarchical modeling | Same as for multivariate meta-analysis to obtain a summary point—hierarchical modeling26, 30 | Most theoretically motivated method |
Many ways to obtain a (hierarchical) summary ROC : | Rutter-Gatsonis HSROC recommended in the Cochrane handbook,47 as it is the method with which there is most experience | |
Rutter-Gatsonis (most common)30 | ||
Several alternative curves37, 38 |
We will focus on the case where each study reports a single pair of sensitivity and specificity at a given threshold (although thresholds can differ across studies). Another, more complex situation arises when multiple sensitivity and specificity pairs (at different thresholds) are reported in each study. Statistical models for the latter case exist, but there is less empirical evidence on their use. These will be described briefly, as a special case.
Preferred methods for obtaining a “summary point” (summary sensitivity and specificity): two families of hierarchical models
When a “summary point” is deemed a helpful summary of a collection of studies, one should ideally perform a multivariate meta-analysis of sensitivity and specificity, i.e., a joint analysis of both quantities, rather than separate univariate meta-analyses. This is not only theoretically motivated,26–28 but also corroborated by simulation analyses.1, 27, 29
Multivariate meta-analyses require advanced hierarchical modeling. We can group the commonly used hierarchical models in two families: The so called “bivariate model”26 and the “hierarchical summary ROC” (HSROC) model.30 Both use two levels to model the statistical distributions of data. At the first level, they model the counts of the 2 × 2 table within each study, which accounts for within-study variability. At the second level, they model the between-study variability (heterogeneity), allowing for the theoretically expected nonindependence of sensitivity and specificity across studies. The two families differ in their parameterization at this second level: the bivariate model uses parameters that are transformations of the average sensitivity and specificity—while the HSROC model uses a scale parameter and an accuracy parameter, which are functions of sensitivity and specificity, and define an underlying hierarchical summary ROC curve.
In the absence of covariates, the two families of hierarchical models are mathematically equivalent; one can use simple formulas to relate the fitted parameters of the bivariate model to the HSROC model and vice versa, rendering choices between the two approaches moot.18 The importance of choosing between the two families becomes evident in meta-regression analyses, when covariates are used to explore between-study heterogeneity. The differences in design and conduct of the included diagnostic accuracy studies may affect the choice of the model.18 For example, “spectrum effects,” where the subjects included in a study are not representative of the patients who will receive the test in practice,31 “might be expected to impact test accuracy rather than the threshold, and might therefore be most appropriately investigated using the HSROC approach. Conversely, between-study variation in disease severity will (likely) affect sensitivity but not specificity, leading to a preference for the bivariate approach.”18 When there are covariates in the model, the HSROC model allows direct evaluation of the difference in accuracy or threshold parameters or both, which affect the degree of asymmetry of the SROC curve, and how much higher it is from the diagonal (the line of no diagnostic information).18 Bivariate models, on the other hand, allow for direct evaluation of covariates on sensitivity or specificity or both. Systematic reviewers are encouraged to look at study characteristics and think through how study characteristics could affect the diagnostic accuracy, which in turn might affect the choice of the meta-regression model.
Preferred methods for obtaining a “summary line”
When a summary line is deemed more helpful in summarizing the available studies, we recommend summary lines obtained from hierarchical modeling, instead of several simpler approaches (Table 1).32–36 As mentioned above, when there are no covariates, the parameters of hierarchical summary lines can be calculated from the parameters of the bivariate random effects models using formulas.18, 30, 37 In fact, a whole range of HSROC lines can be constructed using parameters from the fitted bivariate model;37, 38 one proposed by Rutter and Gatsonis30 is an example. The various HSROC curves represent alternative characterizations of the bivariate distribution of sensitivity and specificity, and can thus have different shapes. Briefly, apart from the commonly used Rutter-Gatsonis HSROC curve, alternative curves include those obtained from a regression of logit-transformed true positive rate on logit-transformed false positive rate; logit false positive rate on logit true positive rate; or the major axis regression between logit true and false positive rates.37, 38
When the estimated correlation between sensitivity and specificity is positive (as opposed to the typical negative correlation) the latter three alternative models can generate curves that follow a downward slope from left to right. This is not as rare as once thought37– a downward slope (from left to right) was observed in approximately one out of three meta-analyses in a large empirical exploration of 308 meta-analyses (report under review, Tufts Evidence-based Practice Center). Chappell et al. argued that in meta-analyses with evidence of positive estimated correlation between sensitivity and specificity (e.g., based on the correlation estimate and confidence interval or its posterior distribution) it is meaningless to use an HSROC line to summarize the studies,38 as a “threshold effect” explanation is not possible. Yet, even if the estimated correlation between sensitivity and specificity is positive (i.e., not in the “expected” direction), an HSROC still represents how the summary sensitivity changes with the summary specificity. The difference is that the explanation for the pattern of the studies cannot involve a “threshold effect”; rather, it is likely that an important covariate has not been included in the analysis (see the proposed algorithm below).38
A special case: joint analysis of sensitivity and specificity when studies report multiple thresholds
It is not uncommon for some studies to report multiple sensitivity and specificity pairs at several thresholds for positive tests. One option is to decide on a single threshold from each study and apply the aforementioned methods. To some extent, the setting in which the test is used can guide the selection of the threshold. For example, in some cases, the threshold which gives the highest sensitivity may be appropriate in medical tests to rule-out disease. Another option is to use all available thresholds per study. Specifically, Dukic and Gatsonis extended the HSROC model to analyze sensitivity and specificity data reported at more than one threshold.39 This model represents as extension of the HSROC model discussed above. Further, if each study reports enough data on sensitivity and specificity to construct a ROC curve, Kester and Buntinx40 proposed a little-used method to combine whole ROC curves.
Both models are theoretically motivated. The Dukic and Gatsonis model is more elaborate and more technical in its implementation than the Kester and Buntinx variant. There is no empirical evidence on the performance of either model in a large number of applied examples. Therefore, we refrain from providing a strong recommendation to always perform such analyses. Systematic reviewers are mildly encouraged to perform explorations, including analyses with these models. Should they opt to do so, they should provide adequate description of the employed models and their assumptions, as well as a clear intuitive interpretation of the parameters of interest in the models. At a minimum, we suggest that systematic reviewers perform explorations in a qualitative, graphical depiction of the data in the ROC space (see Algorithm section). This will provide a qualitative summary and highlight similarities and differences among the studies. An example of such a graph is Figure 3, which illustrates the diagnostic performance of early measurements of total serum bilirubin (TSB) to identify post-discharge TSB above the 95th 10- hour-specific percentile in newborns.41
A WORKABLE ALGORITHM
We propose using the following three step algorithm for meta-analyzing studies of medical test performance when there is a “gold standard”. This algorithm should assist meta-analysts in deciding whether a summary point, a summary line, or both are helpful syntheses of the data. When reviewing the three step algorithm, keep these points in mind:
A summary point may be less helpful or interpretable when the studies have different explicit thresholds for positive tests, and when the estimates of sensitivity vary widely along different specificities. In such cases, a summary line may be more informative.
A summary line may not be well estimated when the sensitivities and specificities of the various studies show little variability or when their estimated correlation across studies is small. Further, if there is evidence that the estimated correlation of sensitivity and specificity across studies is positive (rather than negative, which would be more typical), a “threshold effect” is not a plausible explanation for the observed pattern across studies. Rather, it is likely that an important covariate has not been taken into account.
In many applications, a reasonable case can be made for summarizing studies both with a summary point and with a summary line, as these provide alternative perspectives.
Step 1: Start by considering sensitivity and specificity independently
This step is probably self explanatory; it encourages reviewers to familiarize themselves with the pattern of study-level sensitivities and specificities. It is very instructive to create side-by-side forest plots of sensitivity and specificity in which studies are ordered by either sensitivity or specificity. The point of the graphical assessment is to obtain a visual impression of the variability of sensitivity and specificity across studies, as well as an impression of any relationship between sensitivity and specificity across studies, particularly if such a relationship is prominent (Fig. 1 and illustrative examples).
If a summary point is deemed a helpful summary of the data, it is reasonable to first perform separate meta-analyses of sensitivity and specificity. The differences in the point estimates of summary sensitivity and specificity with univariate (separate) versus bivariate (joint) meta-analyses is often small. In an empirical exploration of 308 meta-analyses, differences in the estimates of summary sensitivity and specificity were rarely larger than 5 % (report under review, Tufts Evidence-based Practice Center). The width of the confidence intervals for the summary sensitivity and specificity is also similar between univariate and bivariate analyses. This suggests that practically, univariate and multivariate analyses may yield comparable results. However, our recommendation is to prefer reporting the results from the hierarchical (multivariate) meta-analysis methods because of their better theoretical motivation and because of their natural symmetry with the multivariate methods that yield summary lines.
Step 2: Multivariate meta-analysis (when each study reports a single threshold)
To obtain a summary point, meta-analysts should perform bivariate meta-analyses (preferably using the exact binomial likelihood).
Meta-analysts should obtain summary lines based on multivariate meta-analysis models. The interpretation of the summary line should not automatically be that there are “threshold effects”. This is most obvious when performing meta-analyses with evidence of a positive correlation between sensitivity and specificity, which cannot be attributed to a “threshold effect”, as mentioned above.
If more than one threshold is reported per study and there is no strong a priori rationale to review only results for a specific threshold, meta-analysts should consider incorporating alternative thresholds into the appropriate analyses discussed previously. Tentatively, we encourage both qualitative analysis via graphs and quantitative analyses via one of the multivariate methods mentioned above.
Step 3. Explore between-study heterogeneity
Other than accounting for the presence of a “threshold effect”, the HSROC and bivariate models provide flexible ways to test and explore between-study heterogeneity. The HSROC model allows one to examine whether any covariates (study characteristics) explain the observed heterogeneity in the accuracy and threshold parameters. One can use the same set of covariates for both parameters, but this is not mandatory, and should be judged for the application at hand. On the other hand, bivariate models allow one to use covariates to explain heterogeneity in sensitivity or specificity or both; and again, covariates for each measure can be different. Covariates that reduce the unexplained variability across studies (heterogeneity) may represent important characteristics that should be taken into account when summarizing the studies, or they may represent spurious associations. We refer to other texts for a discussion of the premises and pitfalls of metaregressions.24, 42 Factors reflecting differences in patient populations and methods of patient selection, methods of verification and interpretation of results, clinical setting, and disease severity are common sources of heterogeneity. Investigators are encouraged to use multivariate models to explore heterogeneity, especially when they have chosen these methods for combining studies.
Illustrations
We briefly demonstrate the above with two applied examples. The first example on D-dimer assays for the diagnosis of venous thromboembolism15 shows heterogeneity which could be attributed to a “threshold effect” as discussed by Lijmer et al..43 The second example is from an evidence report on the use of serial creatine kinase-MB measurements for the diagnosis of acute cardiac ischemia,16, 17 and shows heterogeneity for another reason.
D-dimers for diagnosis of venous thromboembolism
D-dimers are fragments specific for fibrin degradation in plasma, and can be used to diagnose venous thromboembolism. Figure 1 presents forest plots of the sensitivity and specificity and the likelihood ratios for the D-dimer example.43 Sensitivity and specificity appear more heterogeneous than the likelihood ratios (this is true by formal testing for heterogeneity). This may be due to threshold variation in these studies (from 120 to 550 ng/mL, when stated; Fig. 1), or due to other reasons.43
Because of the explicit variation in the thresholds for studies of D-dimers, it is probably more helpful to summarize the performance of the test using a HSROC, rather than to provide summary sensitivities and specificities (Fig. 4a). (For simplicity, we select the highest threshold from two studies that report multiple ELISA thresholds.) This test has very good diagnostic ability, and it appropriately focuses on minimizing false negative diagnoses. It is also informative to estimate “summary” negative (or positive) predictive values for this test. As described previously, we can calulate them based on the summary sensitivity and specificity estimates and over a range of plausible values for the prevalence. Figure 4b shows such an example using the summary sensitivity and specificity of the 11 studies of Figure 4a.
Second example: Serial creatine kinase-MB measurements for diagnosing acute cardiac ischemia
An evidence report examined the ability of serial creatine kinase-MB (CK-MB) measurements to diagnose acute cardiac ischemia in the emergency department.16, 17 Figure 5 shows the 14 eligible studies along with how many hours after symptom onset the last measurement was taken. It is evident that there is between-study heterogeneity in the sensitivities, and that sensitivity increases with longer time from symptom onset.
For illustrative purposes, we compare the summary sensitivity and specificity of studies where the last measurement was performed within three hours of symptom onset versus greater than three hours from symptom onset (Table 2). We used a bivariate multilevel model with exact binomial likelihood. In the fixed effects part of the model, we include a variable that codes whether the last measurement was earlier than three hours from symptom onset or not. We allow this variable to have different effects on the summary sensitivity and on the summary specificity. This is essentially a bivariate meta-regression.
Table 2.
Meta-analysis metric | ≤3 hours | >3 hours | p-Value for the comparison across subgroups |
---|---|---|---|
Summary sensitivity (percent) | 80 (64 to 90) | 96 (85 to 99) | 0.036 |
Summary specificity (percent) | 97 (94 to 98) | 97 (95 to 99) | 0.56 |
Results based on a bivariate meta-regression that effectively compared the summary sensitivity and summary specificity according to the timing of the last serial CK-MB measurement for diagnosis of acute cardiac ischemia. The meta-regression is on a variable that takes the value 1 if the time from the onset of symptoms to testing was 3 hours or less, and the value 0, when the respective time interval was more than 3 hours. The bivariate meta-regression model allows for different effects of timing on sensitivity and specificity. To facilitate interpretation, we present the summary sensitivity and specificity in each subgroup, calculated from the parameters of the meta-regression model, which also gave the p-values for the effect of timing on test performance.
Note that properly specified bivariate meta-regressions (or HSROC-based meta-regressions) can be used to compare two or more medical tests. The specification of the meta-regression models will be different when the comparison is indirect (different medical tests are examined in independent studies) or direct (the different medical tests are applied in the same patients in each study).
OVERALL RECOMMENDATIONS
We summarize:
- Consider presenting a “summary point” when sensitivity and specificity do not vary widely across studies, and studies use the same explicit or “implicit threshold”.
- To obtain a summary sensitivity and specificity use the theoretically motivated bivariate meta-analysis models.
- Back-calculate overall positive and negative predictive values from summary estimates of sensitivity and specificity, and for a plausible range of prevalence values rather than meta-analyzing them directly.
- Back-calculate overall positive and negative likelihood ratios from summary estimates of sensitivity and specificity, rather than meta-analyzing them directly.
- If the sensitivity and specificity vary over a large range, it may be more helpful to use a summary line, which best describes the relationship of the average sensitivity and specificity. The summary line approach is also most helpful when different explicit thresholds are used across studies. To obtain a summary line use multivariate meta-analysis methods such as the HSROC model.
- Several SROC lines can be obtained based on multivariate meta-analysis models, and they can have different shapes.
- If there is evidence of a positive correlation, the variability in the studies cannot be secondary to a “threshold effect”; explore for missing important covariates. Arguably, the summary line is a valid description of how average sensitivity relates to average specificity.
If more than one threshold is reported per study, this has to be taken into account in the quantitative analyses. We encourage both qualitative analysis via graphs and quantitative analyses via proper methods.
One should explore the impact of study characteristics on summary results in the context of the primary methodology used to summarize studies using meta-regression-based analyses or subgroup analyses.
Acknowledgment
This manuscript is based on work funded by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ). All authors are members of AHRQ-funded Evidence-based Practice Centers. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not reflect the official position of AHRQ or the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they do not have a conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Lau J, Ioannidis JP, Schmid CH. Summing up evidence: one answer is not always enough. Lancet. 1998;351(9096):123–127. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)08468-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tatsioni A, Zarin DA, Aronson N, Samson DJ, Flamm CR, Schmid C, et al. Challenges in systematic reviews of diagnostic technologies. Ann Intern Med. 2005;142(12 Pt 2):1048–1055. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-142-12_part_2-200506211-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lijmer JG, Leeflang M, Bossuyt PM. Proposals for a phased evaluation of medical tests. Med Decis Making. 2009;29(5):E13–E21. doi: 10.1177/0272989X09336144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lord SJ, Irwig L, Bossuyt PM. Using the principles of randomized controlled trial design to guide test evaluation. Med Decis Making. 2009;29(5):E1–E12. doi: 10.1177/0272989X09340584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Trikalinos TA, Kulasingam S, Lawrence WF. Chapter 10: Deciding Whether to Complement a Systematic Review of Medical Tests with Decision Modeling. J Gen Intern Med 2012. doi:10.1007/s11606-012-2019-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 6.Matchar DB. Introduction to the methods guide for medical test reviews. J Gen Intern Med. 2011. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1798-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 7.Hartmann K. Chapter 6: Assessing applicability of medical test studies in systematic reviews. J Gen Intern Med. 2011. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1961-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 8.Trikalinos TA, Ballion CM. Chapter 9: Options for summarizing medical test performance in the absence of a "gold standard.". J Gen Intern Med. 2012. doi:10.1007/s11606-012-2031-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 9.Loong TW. Understanding sensitivity and specificity with the right side of the brain. BMJ. 2003;327(7417):716–719. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7417.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bossuyt PM, Reitsma JB, Bruns DE, Gatsonis CA, Glasziou PP, Irwig LM, et al. The STARD statement for reporting studies of diagnostic accuracy: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2003;138(1):W1–12. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-138-1-200301070-00012-w1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Santaguida PL, Riley CM, Matchar DB. Chapter 5: Assessing risk of bias as a dojmain of quality in medical test studies. J Gen Intern Med. 2012. doi:10.1007/s11606-012-2030-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 12.Lijmer JG, Mol BW, Heisterkamp S, Bonsel GJ, Prins MH, Meulen JH, et al. Empirical evidence of design-related bias in studies of diagnostic tests. JAMA. 1999;282(11):1061–1066. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.11.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rutjes AW, Reitsma JB, Di NM, Smidt N, Rijn JC, Bossuyt PM. Evidence of bias and variation in diagnostic accuracy studies. CMAJ. 2006;174(4):469–476. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.050090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Whiting P, Rutjes AW, Reitsma JB, Glas AS, Bossuyt PM, Kleijnen J. Sources of variation and bias in studies of diagnostic accuracy: a systematic review. Ann Intern Med. 2004;140(3):189–202. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-140-3-200402030-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Becker DM, Philbrick JT, Bachhuber TL, Humphries JE. D-dimer testing and acute venous thromboembolism. A shortcut to accurate diagnosis? Arch Intern Med. 1996;156(9):939–946. doi: 10.1001/archinte.1996.00440090029004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Balk EM, Ioannidis JP, Salem D, Chew PW, Lau J. Accuracy of biomarkers to diagnose acute cardiac ischemia in the emergency department: a meta-analysis. Ann Emerg Med. 2001;37(5):478–494. doi: 10.1067/mem.2001.114905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lau J, Ioannidis JP, Balk E, Milch C, Chew P, Terrin N et al. Evaluation of technologies for identifying acute cardiac ischemia in emergency departments. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Summ ) 2000;(26):1-4. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18.Harbord RM, Deeks JJ, Egger M, Whiting P, Sterne JA. A unification of models for meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy studies. Biostatistics. 2007;8(2):239–251. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Irwig L, Tosteson AN, Gatsonis C, Lau J, Colditz G, Chalmers TC, et al. Guidelines for meta-analyses evaluating diagnostic tests. Ann Intern Med. 1994;120(8):667–676. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-120-8-199404150-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Leeflang MMG, Bossuyt PM, Irwig L. Diagnostic accuracy may vary with prevalence: Implications for evidence-based diagnosis. J Clin Epidemiol 2009;62(1):5–12. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 21.Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Diagnostic tests 4: likelihood ratios. BMJ. 2004;329(7458):168–169. doi: 10.1136/bmj.329.7458.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Zwinderman AH, Bossuyt PM. We should not pool diagnostic likelihood ratios in systematic reviews. Stat Med. 2008;27(5):687–697. doi: 10.1002/sim.2992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Simel DL, Bossuyt PM. Differences between univariate and bivariate models for summarizing diagnostic accuracy may not be large. J Clin Epidemiol 2009; 62(12):1292–300 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 24.Fu R, Gartlehner G, Grant M, Shamliyan T, Sedrakyan A, Wilt TJ, et al. Conducting quantitative synthesis when comparing medical interventions: AHRQ and the Effective Health Care Program. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(11):1187–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Glas AS, Lijmer JG, Prins MH, Bonsel GJ, Bossuyt PM. The diagnostic odds ratio: a single indicator of test performance. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(11):1129–1135. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(03)00177-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Reitsma JB, Glas AS, Rutjes AW, Scholten RJ, Bossuyt PM, Zwinderman AH. Bivariate analysis of sensitivity and specificity produces informative summary measures in diagnostic reviews. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005;58(10):982–990. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2005.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Riley RD, Abrams KR, Sutton AJ, Lambert PC, Thompson JR. Bivariate random-effects meta-analysis and the estimation of between-study correlation. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2007;7:3. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-7-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Riley RD, Abrams KR, Lambert PC, Sutton AJ, Thompson JR. An evaluation of bivariate random-effects meta-analysis for the joint synthesis of two correlated outcomes. Stat Med. 2007;26(1):78–97. doi: 10.1002/sim.2524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Harbord RM, Whiting P, Sterne JA, Egger M, Deeks JJ, Shang A, et al. An empirical comparison of methods for meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy showed hierarchical models are necessary. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008;61(11):1095–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rutter CM, Gatsonis CA. Regression methods for meta-analysis of diagnostic test data. Acad Radiol. 1995;2(Suppl 1):S48–S56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mulherin SA, Miller WC. Spectrum bias or spectrum effect? Subgroup variation in diagnostic test evaluation. Ann Intern Med. 2002;137(7):598–602. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-137-7-200210010-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kardaun JW, Kardaun OJ. Comparative diagnostic performance of three radiological procedures for the detection of lumbar disk herniation. Methods Inf Med. 1990;29(1):12–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Littenberg B, Moses LE. Estimating diagnostic accuracy from multiple conflicting reports: a new meta-analytic method. Med Decis Making. 1993;13(4):313–321. doi: 10.1177/0272989X9301300408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Moses LE, Shapiro D, Littenberg B. Combining independent studies of a diagnostic test into a summary ROC curve: data-analytic approaches and some additional considerations. Stat Med. 1993;12(14):1293–1316. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780121403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Oei EH, Nikken JJ, Verstijnen AC, Ginai AZ, Myriam Hunink MG. MR imaging of the menisci and cruciate ligaments: a systematic review. Radiology. 2003;226(3):837–848. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2263011892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Visser K, Hunink MG. Peripheral arterial disease: gadolinium-enhanced MR angiography versus color-guided duplex US–a meta-analysis. Radiology. 2000;216(1):67–77. doi: 10.1148/radiology.216.1.r00jl0367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Arends LR, Hamza TH, Houwelingen JC, Heijenbrok-Kal MH, Hunink MG, Stijnen T. Bivariate random effects meta-analysis of ROC curves. Med Decis Making. 2008;28(5):621–638. doi: 10.1177/0272989X08319957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chappell FM, Raab GM, Wardlaw JM. When are summary ROC curves appropriate for diagnostic meta-analyses? Stat Med. 2009;28(21):2653–2668. doi: 10.1002/sim.3631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dukic V, Gatsonis C. Meta-analysis of diagnostic test accuracy assessment studies with varying number of thresholds. Biometrics. 2003;59(4):936–946. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2003.00108.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kester AD, Buntinx F. Meta-analysis of ROC curves. Med Decis Making. 2000;20(4):430–439. doi: 10.1177/0272989X0002000407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Trikalinos TA, Chung M, Lau J, Ip S. Systematic review of screening for bilirubin encephalopathy in neonates. Pediatrics. 2009;124(4):1162–1171. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Thompson SG, Sharp SJ. Explaining heterogeneity in meta-analysis: a comparison of methods. Stat Med. 1999;18(20):2693–2708. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19991030)18:20<2693::AID-SIM235>3.0.CO;2-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lijmer JG, Bossuyt PM, Heisterkamp SH. Exploring sources of heterogeneity in systematic reviews of diagnostic tests. Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1525–1537. doi: 10.1002/sim.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hamza TH, Houwelingen HC, Stijnen T. The binomial distribution of meta-analysis was preferred to model within-study variability. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008;61(1):41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chu H, Cole SR. Bivariate meta-analysis of sensitivity and specificity with sparse data: a generalized linear mixed model approach. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59(12):1331–1332. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hamza TH, Reitsma JB, Stijnen T. Meta-analysis of diagnostic studies: a comparison of random intercept, normal-normal, and binomial-normal bivariate summary ROC approaches. Med Decis Making. 2008;28(5):639–649. doi: 10.1177/0272989X08323917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cochrane Diagnostic Test Accuracy Working Group. Handbook for diagnostic test accuracy reviews. 2011. The Cochrane Collaboration. Ref Type: Report