Abstract
Recent studies have revealed asymmetries between the left and right sides of the brain in invertebrate species. Here we present a review of a series of recent studies from our laboratories, aimed at tracing asymmetries at different stages along the honeybee’s (Apis mellifera) olfactory pathway. These include estimates of the number of sensilla present on the two antennae, obtained by scanning electron microscopy, as well as electroantennography recordings of the left and right antennal responses to odorants. We describe investigative studies of the antennal lobes, where multi-photon microscopy was used to search for possible morphological asymmetries between the two brain sides. Moreover, we report on recently published results obtained by two-photon calcium imaging for functional mapping of the antennal lobe aimed at comparing patterns of activity evoked by different odours. Finally, possible links to the results of behavioural tests, measuring asymmetries in single-sided olfactory memory recall, are discussed.
Keywords: Apis mellifera, Antennal lobe, Olfactory learning, Brain asymmetry, PER, SEM, EAG, Two-photon microscopy
Introduction
The brain of the honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) (Hymenoptera: Apidae), is an important model for small neural networks. In spite of the fact that it contains less than 1 million neurons (Menzel and Giurfa 2001), it shows a remarkably rich variety of cognitive performances. The olfactory pathway within the brain is of special interest to a large community of neuroscientists, since it provides for revealing experimental tests of information coding, learning, and memory models (Giurfa 2007). Over the years, various techniques have been developed to investigate the single odour processing steps from the periphery, the antennae, via the central nervous system (CNS), to the behavioural response.
Functional probing of the peripheral olfactory system became possible after the development of electroantennography (EAG) (Schneider 1957), which allowed for the first time to record the sum of the olfactory receptor responses to odour stimuli presented to the antenna. Detailed morphological information about peripheral olfactory sensitive structures became instead accessible thanks to innovative high-resolution imaging techniques like scanning electron microscopy (SEM), by which sensilla have been identified as carriers of odour receptors (Dietz and Humphreys 1971).
Early investigation of activity in the primary olfactory centres within the insect brain, the antennal lobes (ALs), was performed by means of extracellular recording (Boeckh 1974). In this respect, the major breakthrough was provided by the combination of intracellular recording with morphological imaging (Matsumoto and Hildebrand 1981), giving for the first time the ability to perform systematic analysis of specific neurons.
Pioneering work directed at optically imaging the antennal lobe activity in the honeybee brain was performed by Lieke (1993), and thanks to the development of calcium-sensitive dyes (Grynkiewicz et al. 1985) this soon became one of the most successful techniques for investigating the odour-evoked activity of the AL’s functional subunits, the so-called glomeruli (Galizia and Vetter 2004). Each glomerulus connected to a single olfactory receptor family was categorised in four classes, T1–T4, of which T1 is projecting via the lateral-axonal tract (l-ACT), T3–T4 via the median axonal tract (m-ACT) into the mushroom body and lateral horn (Galizia et al. 1999b), the higher centre of the brain for multisensory integration. Here it undergoes further processing steps before the outcome of the olfactory processing network is finally projected out of the central nervous system. Motor neurons carry impulses to the muscles like those of the bee’s proboscis. Thus responses to odour stimuli can be studied by the Proboscis Extension Reflex (PER) paradigm, an experimental procedure based on Pavlovian conditioning (Pavlov 1927). PER was for the first time experimentally conditioned in honeybees by Kuwabara (1957) and has now become the most utilised procedure for exploring associative learning and memory recall in bees (Menzel and Müller 1996). In this paper we resume some experiments that combine the aforementioned techniques to investigate left-right asymmetries in the honeybee olfactory circuit.
The phenomenon of functional asymmetries was first reported in the human brain 150 years ago (Broca 1861). After that, lateralised behaviours were discovered also in vertebrates in the 1970s (Nottebohm 1970; Rogers and Anson 1979); they were recognised by the time to be widespread among all taxonomic groups (Vallortigara and Rogers 2005; MacNeilage et al. 2009). But only recently, evidence of their presence in invertebrates has been demonstrated (e.g. Kells and Goulson 2001; Ades and Ramires 2002; Hobert et al. 2002).
In the honeybee, side-specificity of odour learning was observed in behavioural studies (Sandoz and Menzel 2001; Letzkus et al. 2006; Rogers and Vallortigara 2008) as well as in morphological studies at the level of the antennae (Letzkus et al. 2006). On the contrary, from investigations of the primary olfactory centres in the brain so far only symmetric results have been reported regarding morphology (Winnington et al. 1996) as well as the activity pattern (Galizia et al. 1998).
The approach of the experimental studies carried out by our research groups and summarised here was to systematically trace asymmetries along the olfactory pathway using both established techniques as well as new imaging methods to acquire morpho-physiological, functional, and behavioural data. The focus of this review was placed in particular on the experimental methods and their relative advantages and disadvantages.
Scanning electron microscopy
In Frasnelli et al.’s (2010a) anatomical differences in the number of sensilla between the right and the left antennae of the honeybee were investigated by SEM. Compared to Letzkus et al. (2006), the whole antenna was imaged, various types of sensilla were considered, and the study was based on a larger sample (Fig. 1). The antennae were cut at the base of the scape and attached to a circular stub by double-sided conductive tape (TAAB Laboratories Equipment). All samples were gold-coated, guaranteeing electrical conductivity during imaging. Antennae were imaged using a XL 30 Field Emission Environmental Scanning Electron Microscope (FEI-Philips) in high-vacuum mode, from four different viewpoints, covering almost the complete surface (for details see Frasnelli et al. 2010a). Segments 3–9 were scanned longitudinally at a magnification of 600×, the tenth smaller apical segment with a magnification of 800×; segments 1 and 2 were omitted, since they don’t carry olfactory receptors.
Eight sensillum types were tagged and counted on all acquired images by using image analysis software (UT-HSCSA ImageTool Version 3.0). In particular, the putative olfactory sensilla were identified: s. placodea, s. trichodea type a and b, and s. basiconica thick and tapered; as well as the sensilla with non-olfactory functions: s. coeloconica, involved in perception of temperature, carbon dioxide and humidity; s. campaniformia, considered to be hydro- and thermoreceptors and/or mechanoreceptors; and s. chaetica, sensitive to mechanical or gustatory stimuli (Dietz and Humphreys 1971; Whitehead and Larsen 1976).
The SEM images of left and right antennae were analysed (N = 14), comparing the number of olfactory and non-olfactory sensilla per antenna segment; the results are summarised in Fig. 2. A significant asymmetry of the number of olfactory sensilla (Fig. 2a) was detected throughout all segments in favour of the right antenna (ANOVA analysis of variance; F(1,13) = 5.56; P < 0.05). For non-olfactory sensilla (Fig. 2b), there was evidence of asymmetry in segments 3–9, but in the opposite direction, with more non-olfactory sensilla on the left than on the right antenna [ANOVA analysis of variance; F(1,13) = 6.07; P < 0.05].
These data confirm and complete the results of Letzkus et al. (2006), suggesting that an asymmetry in the olfactory pathway may partially arise from a significant difference in the number of olfactory sensilla in favour of the right antenna. However, as we discuss below, there is evidence of changes in the role of the left and right antenna during memory consolidation (Rogers and Vallortigara 2008) that cannot be explained by sensilla asymmetries only.
Electroantennography
Anfora et al. (2010) measured via electroantennography the overall depolarisation potential of the antenna’s olfactory receptor neurons after odour presentation. The experimental setup is depicted in Fig. 3a: single antennae were cut at the level of the scape and brought in contact with the microcapillaries by means of two joystick micromanipulators (MN-151, Narishige). Microcapillary glass electrodes were pulled to a tip size matching the antenna diameter of ~200μm (PP-83, Narishige) and filled with Kaissling saline solution (Kaissling and Thorson 1980). The signal was picked up by a high impedance probe followed by a fixed gain (5×) preamplifier and a high pass filter (INR-5, Syntech) suppressing 50 Hz noise. The output was recorded by an acquisition module (IDAC-232, Syntech), connected to a computer, which was synchronised with a stimulus controller (CS-55, Syntech). For each subject, EAG peak responses (mV) (Fig. 3b) were measured from both the right and the left antenna.
Five samples of either isoamyl acetate (Sigma-Aldrich, >99.7% purity), component of the honeybee alarm pheromone (Boch et al. 1962), or (−)-linalool (Sigma-Aldrich, >98.5% purity), a common floral odour, dissolved in hexane at concentrations ranging from 10−2 to 102 μg/μl were prepared. Aliquots of the test solutions (25 μl each), absorbed by pieces of filter paper (1 cm2), were inserted into individual Pasteur pipettes, while pure hexane and empty pipettes served as control. Odour puffs of 500 ms, created by the stimulus controller, were injected into an air stream in ascending concentration order. The total air flux was kept constant at ~30 ml/s to avoid mechanical stimuli. Odour puffs were emitted in 60-s intervals. The results (Fig. 4 and Anfora et al. 2010) revealed a similar pattern as the SEM experiments: the EAG responses, elicited by both tested compounds on the right antenna, were significantly higher than those on the left one (ANOVA: F(1,15) = 5.12; P < 0.05]. As expected, a significant increase in their EAG responses with increasing doses of isoamyl acetate and (−)-linalool was observed, with the latter showing saturation for the highest concentration.)
The electrophysiological measurements of Anfora et al. (2010) on single antennae confirm the morphological studies discussed above (Letzkus et al. 2006; Frasnelli et al. 2010a), showing the presence of a significant response asymmetry favouring the right antenna (Anfora et al. 2010).
However, as noted above, behavioural experiments evidenced changes in lateralisation associated with memory consolidation (Rogers and Vallortigara 2008) with a dominance of the right antenna at short time scales and a dominance of the left antenna at long (>3 h) time scales. This suggests a contribution of the CNS to lateralisation, motivating new research efforts aimed at looking for lateralisation in the primary olfactory centres of the honeybee brain, the antennal lobes.
Two-photon microscopy
Two-photon microscopy is a well-established imaging technique that offers unprecedented optical sectioning capabilities thanks to its enhanced penetration depth and axial resolution (Denk et al. 1990). Haase et al. (2011) introduced this technique to the field of honeybee neuro-imaging through the implementation of an in vivo imaging platform allowing to obtain both morphological and functional data from the whole antennal lobe of the honeybee. This imaging tool provided the capability to conduct studies aimed at finding the presence of volumetric lateralisation of the glomeruli, as well as analysing the glomerular activity for asymmetries in their odour response maps. Given the novelty of these procedures, they will be described here in some detail.
Animal preparation
For the two-photon microscopy experiments reported here (Haase et al. 2011; Rigosi et al. 2011), bees were prepared according to a well-established protocol (Galizia and Vetter 2004). After chilling to immobility, bees were fixed to a custom-made imaging stage using dental waxes (Kerr; Siladent). A window was cut into the head’s cuticula above antennal lobes and mushroom body, and glands and trachea were gently removed. A pond for the objective immersion liquid was formed above the imaging region using a plastic cover slip, sealed with silicon (Kwik-Sil, WPI). This protocol was followed closely, which should also guaranty a comparability of the imaging results with previous experiments.
Morphological imaging
In Rigosi et al. (2011) bees’ ALs were morphologically stained with a membrane-selective dye. First, the neural sheath was digested with a 1% solution of Protease Type XIV (Sigma-Aldrich). Then the bee brain was homogeneously stained by bath application of the cell-permeant dye RH795 (50μM, Invitrogen). This preparation method was chosen by the authors as the least invasive technique, avoiding artefacts due to brain extraction, fixation, and dehydration (Bucher et al. 2000).
The imaging setup is sketched in Fig. 5a. It consists of a two-photon microscope (Ultima IV, Prairie Technologies) combined with an ultra-short pulsed laser (Mai Tai Deep See HP, Spectra-Physics). The excitation wavelength was 1,040 nm for two-photon excitation of RH795; detection was centred around 607 nm. The beam is strongly focussed onto the sample by a water immersion objective (40×, NA 0.8, Olympus).
A point-spread-function measurement verified the microscope’s resolution to be diffraction limited to a Gaussian width σx,y = 230 nm in the plane and σz = 1.1 μm axially (for the 40× lens at 800 nm excitation wavelength). For later volume reconstruction, image stacks of 3 μm axial distance were taken. Dye diffusion limited the imaging depth to ~150 μm. Post-processing for 3D reconstruction, image segmentation, and volumetric measurements was performed using the software Amira (Visage Imaging). As a test sample five primary glomeruli were chosen in the study by Rigosi et al. (2011), which allow easy identification and exhibit very diverse response profiles (Galizia et al. 1999a; Haase et al. 2011). The latter would cover the possibility of an odour-specific lateralisation (see Rigosi et al. 2011).
The volume measurements of the reconstructed glomeruli gave results about twice the size of those from dehydrated samples investigated in previous studies (e.g. Winnington et al. 1996; Hourcade et al. 2009). This confirmed that impressively strong shrinkage effects were avoided by this imaging method.
The determined relative volume imbalance between left (VL) and right (VR) ALs was quantified by the lateralisation index L = VR/(VR + VL). The results reported by Rigosi et al. (2011) showed no evidence of significant morphological asymmetry between sides; only the expected significant difference in volume among different glomerular classes was observed.
Thus, this experiment not only confirmed the findings of Winnington et al. (1996), the only previously published study comparing AL volumes between the two sides of the brain, but it also strengthened these results, improving the axial resolution of the AL images by almost an order of magnitude with respect to the one by Winnington et al., which was based on histological slices taken at distances of 25 μm. Moreover, possible artefacts due to anisometric shrinkage and diffraction-index mismatch (Bucher et al. 2000) could be avoided, representing a clear advantage over the common method in morphological imaging of the bee brain, which implied extraction, fixation, and clearing of the brain sample (e.g. Galizia et al. 1999b; Hourcade et al. 2009).
The findings of Rigosi et al. (2011) do not necessarily conflict with the morphological asymmetries found in the peripheral part of the olfactory pathway, the antennae, since there are several possible effects that might superimpose or compensate for this peripheral imbalance. Firstly, there is no linear correlation of sensilla number to glomerular volume, since the single glomeruli represent a convergent site for all odour receptor neurons of one type (Galizia and Szyszka 2008), and even within a receptor class the number and type of neurons per sensillum can vary drastically (Kelber et al. 2006). Therefore, the peripheral lateralisation might cause only minor asymmetries between the AL, too small to be distinguished from statistical variations. Besides that, there are reports about enormous odour-experience-dependent volumetric plasticity in specific glomeruli (Winnington et al. 1996; Sigg et al. 1997; Hourcade et al. 2009), which again might cause statistical variations, overshadowing a minor lateralisation effect, since the odour experience of the bees imaged by Rigosi et al. (2011) could not be controlled. In future morphological studies, efforts have to be made to control the subjects’ odour experience, e.g. by uniform conditioning before the experiments. Moreover, in order to look for an effect corresponding to the lateral shift from short-term memory (STM) to long-term memory (LTM) in the antennal lobe, the time span between conditioning and imaging should be varied systematically.
Functional imaging
Apart from morphological measurements, first prove-of-principle experiments in functional imaging using two-photon microscopy were reported by Haase et al. (2011), aimed to further decipher the olfactory code, to study the principles of odour learning and memory, and to search for possible asymmetries in the functional signals along the olfactory pathway.
For in vivo functional imaging, the projection neurons were selectively stained by the backfill method (Gelperin and Flores 1997) using a solution of the calcium-sensitive dye fura2-dextran (Invitrogen) and 2% bovine serum albumin (Sigma-Aldrich). For dye injection, glass capillaries (ID 1.15 mm, OD 1.65 mm, Garner Glass Co.) were pulled to tip sizes of ~5 μm (PP-83, Narishige), coated by rolling them in the viscous dye solution, then tips were broken, leaving a final tip diameter of ~20 μm. Dye-coated micro-tips were carefully injected into the antenno-cerebralis tracts below the α-lobe. Then the cuticula was reclosed and the animals were stored for 20 h in a dark, cool, and humid place in order for the dye to diffuse into the AL. Before the imaging session, the cuticula, the glands, and the trachea above the AL were removed, and a pond was formed and filled with Ringer’s solution (Galizia and Vetter 2004).
The imaging setup in Haase et al. (2011) was similar to that for the morphological imaging described above, except for the excitation wavelength of 800 nm for resonant two-photon excitation of the free fluorophore fura-2, the detection channel, which was filtered by a 70-nm bandpass around 525 nm, and for the time-optimised laser scanning technique. To record the spatio-temporal response pattern with the highest possible temporal resolution, the excitation laser was scanned within an arbitrary horizontal plane along a one-dimensional custom-defined trace (Fig. 5b). These line scan traces were chosen to cross all glomeruli of interest and allowed scanning frequencies as high as 70 Hz. An optimal signal-to-noise ratio was achieved with laser powers of about 10 mW at the sample surface, without observing any induced photo-damage during the measurement. The temperature of the experimental environment was stabilised to 29°C (Franke 2009).
Functional imaging was performed measuring the intra-neuronal calcium concentration, which increases with enhanced neuronal activity. When the Ca2+-sensitive fluorophore binds calcium, its absorption spectrum is shifted away from the laser excitation wavelength. This causes a drop in the measured fluorescence intensity, manifesting themselves in dark bands in the scanline-overtime maps at the positions of the active glomeruli (Fig. 6).
We see changes in the staining quality between single subjects causing variations in the absolute fluorescence intensity, but, since fura-2 is a ratiometric calcium marker, the relative shift due to the Ca2+ concentration change remains constant. This was confirmed in our experiments.
A stimulus controller (CS-55, Syntech) delivered odour stimuli at constant total air flux (30 ml/s). The odour puffs came from Pasteur pipettes in which 10 μl of an odour solution (1:10 in mineral oil) was deposited on filter paper. All command signals and acquisitions were controlled by a common gate, which allowed precise synchronisation of the involved pulses.
The experimental cycle began by starting the image acquisition. After 3 s the stimulus generator received a trigger releasing an odour puff of 2 s length. The exact arrival time of the odour at the bee antenna was measured and found to be stable within 10 ms, which allowed for accurate measurements of the neuronal response delay. These measurements were performed by optically monitoring a cantilever at the position of the antenna before the functional imaging. To assess concentration fluctuation during the odour response measurements, a photo-ionisation detector would have to be added to the setup. After 9 s, image acquisition stopped and data evaluation was automatically performed via Matlab (Mathworks) scripts.
All acquired data were corrected for photo-bleaching, but this turned out to be negligible, as the fitted exponentials were mostly flat. A 2D running average filtering was used to reduce the noise level. Spatial averaging was performed over 30 pixels corresponding to 17.4 μm (the minimum glomerular size). Temporal averaging was applied over five pixels corresponding to 88 ms, preserving all main dynamic features of the data. The spatio-temporal response pattern of each imaging experiment was then analysed. Thanks to the reduced photo-damage of two-photon microscopy (Denk and Svoboda 1997) due to the limited absorption volume in the sample, photo-bleaching was strongly suppressed, allowing for repeated recordings. To measure the total response intensity, each odour was applied three times consecutively, and data sets were averaged. This improved the signal-to-noise ratio, which became especially important at greater imaging depths where random fluctuations increased.
The reduced photo-damage allowed also to extend the imaging sessions up to 5 h before an essential drop in the brain activity could be noticed.
The resulting spatio-temporal response patterns of the AL are presented in Fig. 6. The calcium concentration changes were recorded along the line traces indicated in Fig. 5b. To compare the performance of two-photon microscopy with conventional methods, the applied odour stimuli were chosen to be three well-studied plant volatiles: 1-hexanol, 1-octanol, and 1-nonanol (Galizia et al. 1999a; Peele et al. 2006). The odour response signals detected by Haase et al. (2011) went up to 20% direct fluorescence intensity change (Fig. 7), which represents more than a fourfold improvement with respect to comparable experiments using full-field imaging. The recorded odour response maps (Fig. 6) reproduced features that had already been observed by conventional single-photon fluorescence microscopy, such as the very strong response of glomerulus T1-28 to all tested odours decreasing with the odour’s increasing carbon chain length, and those of T1-33 increasing with the odour’s carbon chain length (Sachse et al. 1999; Peele et al. 2006). Likewise, 1-hexanol was found to produce the broadest response spectrum of the tested odours. Strikingly different from previously published data obtained with full-field microscopy (Peele et al. 2006) were the quite strong responses of glomeruli T1–29 and T1–37 for both 1-hexanol and 1-octanol, which might be due to the enhanced sensitivity of the imaging system.
As in wide-field microscopy experiments (e.g. Galizia et al. 1999a, b), we see a variance in the total responses strength to the same odour among different preparations. We attribute this mainly to differences in age and odour experience, which we were unable to control in our outdoor hives. For an improved quantitative analysis, odour conditioning before the imaging might be a solution.
The high temporal resolution of the study is demonstrated in Fig. 7, where response curves of glomerulus T1-33 to all tested odours are shown at an imaging depth of 25 μm. The time resolution and the precise stimulus synchronisation predestine this method for a systematic investigation of response delays and their dependence on the odour stimuli, as well as the study of a possible specificity of the observed temporal variations of the response signal. Both response latency (Müller et al. 2002) and synchronised oscillations (Laurent 2002), previously observed in electrophysiological measurements, are hypothesised to be part of a general odour code. The larger penetration depth due to infrared excitation together with the backfill staining method allowed Haase et al. to optically access the whole antennal lobe. Figure 8a shows an image stack down to 270 μm, where the deepest glomeruli are located.
A volumetric reconstruction of single glomeruli was performed down to a depth of 200 μm, the limit to which they were so far able to identify glomeruli with certainty. Figure 8b shows volume images of these glomeruli along all principle axes. The reconstructed glomeruli have the following colour code: green ones belong to the T1 sensory group projecting into the l-ACT axonal tract, while blue and red coloured ones belong to the deeper laying T2 and T3 groups, respectively, both projecting into the m-ACT.
These results manifest how two-photon microscopy led to improvements in honeybee neuroimaging in five respects: The enhanced signal amplitude of 20% fluorescence change together with the increased temporal resolution might allow to resolve features that have so far been hidden to conventional full-field microscopy, e.g. possible lateralisation in the activity pattern, which could not be revealed in a previous study (Galizia et al. 1998). Increased penetration depth allowed optical access also to profound regions within the AL, which so far could not be systematically investigated. Higher axial resolution will allow to extend the odour response maps (Galizia et al. 1999a) into a third spatial dimension, including deeper lying glomerular classes (T2–T4). This is of great interest since intracellular recording (Müller et al. 2002; Krofczik et al. 2009) and imaging of their axon terminal boutons in the mushroom body (Yamagata et al. 2009) predicted a fundamentally different role for this glomerular classes in olfactory coding and memory. Lateralisation effects in short- and long-term memory recall observed in behavioural studies (Rogers and Vallortigara 2008) might be caused by plasticity in these regions. Furthermore, for the first time a convenient temporal resolution could be achieved by an optical method, giving hope to add a fourth, temporal dimension to the glomerular response maps. The temporally resolved activity pattern will allow investigating the role of response latencies, already observed by intracellular recording (Müller et al. 2002), as a possible odour coding parameter, which was confirmed in other species (Junek et al. 2010). The observed temporal variations of the response signal should be checked for specificity, which would support dynamic odour coding theories (Laurent 2002). So there is great potential to verify whether several proposed coding models apply to the bee’s olfactory pathway and whether a possible asymmetry that could not be detected in the morphology of T1 glomeruli might be found in these complex codes. The fundamental question whether odour learning manifests itself in functional plasticity within ALs has not been answered satisfyingly yet, since a series of experiments came to very different results. Various studies (Faber et al. 1999; Sandoz et al. 2003; Fernandez et al. 2009) have found plasticity in the calcium response signals of the olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs), while others could not confirm this effect in either the ORNs (Hourcade et al. 2009) or in the projection neurons (Peele et al. 2006; Roussel et al. 2010). Possible explanations for these differing results are different time intervals between odour conditioning and imaging, which should be investigated systematically. This could be realised thanks to the reduced photo-damage of two-photon microscopy, which permitted Haase et al. to extend imaging sessions up to 5 h, which may allow in the future to study plasticity in the AL in real time over extended periods. The latter would also be extremely interesting regarding the search for an effect in the AL, corresponding to the lateral shift from the right to the left brain side, which was observed in behavioural experiments between the 1st and 6th h after odour conditioning (Rogers and Vallortigara 2008) and was hypothesised to be associated with memory consolidation from STM to LTM. But this experiment will be quite ambitious since the restriction that in one sample only one AL can be prepared requires a big statistical basis to detect lateralisation on top of the response fluctuations between different individuals. Nevertheless, considering the limited knowledge about physiological correlates of lateralised behaviour, it might be a challenging and intriguing question to address. In honeybees, in fact, the only evidence at the central level about lateralisation has been recently revealed; Biswas and colleagues in fact demonstrated a difference between sides in the distribution of neuroligin-1, a protein involved in learning and memory when honeybees were only left or right antennae amputated (Biswas et al. 2010).
Behavioural experiments
The last stage of the olfactory pathway, the behavioural response, was analysed in Rigosi et al. (2011). The experiments were linked to the morphological imaging study by choosing odour stimuli that strongly involved the imaged glomerular subset, as shown in previous recorded response maps (Galizia et al. 1999a, Peele et al. 2006; Haase et al. 2011), searching for a possible odour dependence of lateralisation.
Rigosi et al. (2011) cooled bees until immobility and mounted them into customised holders (Bitterman et al. 1983; Rogers and Vallortigara 2008). The subjects were assigned randomly to groups for the occlusion of one antenna. The bees in one group had their left antenna coated with silicone (Silagum-Mono, DMG), those in the second group had their right antenna coated, while both the antennae of the bees belonging to the third group were left uncoated. One hour later each bee groups was subdivided into three groups to be trained to three different odours, involving odour compounds from floral parts like (−)-linalool (Sigma-Aldrich, purity >98.5%), but also volatiles released by green organs of the plants like 1-octanol and 2-octanone (both Fluka, purity >95%). These odours served as a positive conditioned stimulus (CS+) together with 1 M sucrose solution as a food reward (unconditioned stimulus, US); 10 μl of each odour compound was dissolved in 3 ml of the sucrose solution. A saturated saline solution served as negative conditioned stimulus (CS−).
Three trials spaced 6 min apart were performed, where a droplet of the odour sugar solution at the end of a 23-gauge needle was held 1 cm above the bee’s antennae. After 5 s the antennae were touched, which led to PER. The bee was then allowed to ingest the drop of the CS+/US solution. The procedure was repeated with the saline solution, which did not trigger PER but avoidance by moving the antennae away from the droplet. STM recall was tested 1 h later by presenting the odour dissolved in distilled water or the saline solution and holding the droplet 1 cm above the antennae without touching it. These CS+ and CS− stimuli were presented for 5 s. Each bee was tested a total of ten paired trials; the CS+ and CS− were given in a random order with 60 s between each odour presentation. The PER responses were recorded, and the percentage of success was scored as extensions of the proboscis to odours and no extension to saturated salt solution [(PER(CS+)−PER(CS−)]/(#paired trials).
The results of the conditioning and retrieval tests by Rigosi et al. (2011) can be summarised as follows: the right antenna seemed to show better recall than the left antenna in odour STM retrieval, with a strong odour dependence. However this effect was strongly modulated by the odour type. While the authors revealed no significant lateralisation for 1-octanol (Kruskal-Wallis test; χ2 = 1.02; P = 0.6, N = 70) and 2-octanone (Kruskal-Wallis test; χ2 = 0.97; P = 0.62, N = 61), bees trained with (−)-linalool showed a significant effect of the antenna in use (Kruskal-Wallis test; χ2 = 9.91; P < 0.01, N = 63) favouring the right antenna as was previously demonstrated elsewhere (Frasnelli et al. 2010a).
These results confirm that olfactory learning asymmetry in bees is indeed odour-dependent. In previous studies documenting lateralisation (Letzkus et al. 2006; Rogers and Vallortigara 2008; Frasnelli et al. 2010a, b; Anfora et al. 2010), tested odours were pheromones or volatiles from plant’s blooms, all of great importance to forager bees. In the experiments of Rigosi et al. (2011) also unspecific compounds (1-octanol and 2-octanone) released by the green organs of the plants and being of minor importance in pollinator plant interaction were tested. These showed no significant olfactory lateralisation effect, while (−)-linalool, one of the most common derivate of floral scents, which was shown to be a key compound in the identification of odour mixtures (Reinhard et al. 2010), produced significant olfactory lateralisation, confirming the findings of Anfora et al. (2010). This suggests that the occurrence of lateralisation might depend on the odours’ importance to pollinators, which means lateralisation of olfactory information might be advantageous only if the odour is biologically relevant to the bees. Being specialised just in one side for specific odour memory might represent an important advantage in terms of leaving the other hemisphere free to perform a different parallel task (for an example in vertebrates, see Rogers et al. 2004). Moreover, this difference between sides in naive individuals apparently does not impair sensory sensitivity as for instance olfactory asymmetries have been shown in odour tracking in Drosophila melanogaster (Duistermars et al. 2009).
This assumption has to be confirmed by further experiments using odour compounds of different origin and significance for honeybees, and by repeating the memory retrieval tests at different time points, ruling out that the experiments were accidentally performed at the point where the lateral shift from STM to LTM temporarily causes symmetric response efficiencies.
This aspect of possible distinct memory mechanisms for more or less relevant odours is extremely interesting also for future optical imaging experiments, where the search for possible lateralisation could be focused on glomerular subsets with narrow response spectra to the relevant odour classes.
Acknowledgments
This work has been realised also thanks to the support from the Provincia Autonoma di Trento and the Fondazione Cassa di Risparmio di Trento e Rovereto. C.V. acknowledges Provincia Autonoma di Trento (project COMFI) and National Institutes of Health grant RO1EB006432.
Footnotes
Special Issue: SIBPA 2011 Meeting.
Contributor Information
Albrecht Haase, Email: albrecht.haase@unitn.it, Physics Department and Biotech Research Centre, University of Trento, Via Sommarive 14, 38050 Povo, TN, Italy.
Elisa Rigosi, CIMeC, Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy. Sustainable Agro-ecosystems and Bioresources Department, IASMA Research and Innovation Centre, Fondazione Edmund Mach, Via E. Mach 1, 38010 San Michele all’Adige, TN, Italy.
Elisa Frasnelli, Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, Adolf Lorenz Gasse 2, 3422 Altenberg, Austria.
Federica Trona, Sustainable Agro-ecosystems and Bioresources Department, IASMA Research and Innovation Centre, Fondazione Edmund Mach, Via E. Mach 1, 38010 San Michele all’Adige, TN, Italy.
Francesco Tessarolo, Physics Department and Biotech Research Centre, University of Trento, Via Sommarive 14, 38050 Povo, TN, Italy.
Claudio Vinegoni, Center for Systems Biology and Center for Molecular Imaging Research, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 185 Cambridge Street, Boston, MA 02114, USA.
Gianfranco Anfora, Sustainable Agro-ecosystems and Bioresources Department, IASMA Research and Innovation Centre, Fondazione Edmund Mach, Via E. Mach 1, 38010 San Michele all’Adige, TN, Italy.
Giorgio Vallortigara, CIMeC, Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy.
Renzo Antolini, Physics Department and Biotech Research Centre, University of Trento, Via Sommarive 14, 38050 Povo, TN, Italy.
References
- Ades C, Ramires EN. Asymmetry of leg use during prey handling in the spider Scytodes globula (Scytodidae) J Insect Behav. 2002;15:563–570. doi: 10.1023/A:1016337418472. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Anfora G, Frasnelli E, Maccagnani B, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Behavioural and electrophysiological lateralization in a social (Apis mellifera) but not in a non-social (Osmia cornuta) species of bee. Behav Brain Res. 2010;206:236–239. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr. 2009.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswas S, Reinhard J, Oakeshott J, Russell R, Srinivasan MV, et al. Sensory regulation of neuroligins and neurexin i in the honeybee brain. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(2):e9133. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schäfer S. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera) J Comp Psychol. 1983;97:107–119. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.97.2.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boch R, Shearer DA, Stone BC. Identification of isoamyl acetate as an active component in the sting pheromone of the honey bee. Nature. 1962;195:1018–1020. doi: 10.1038/1951018b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boeckh J. Die Reaktion olfaktorischer Neurone im Deutocerebrum von Insekten im Vergleich zu den Antwortmustern der Geruchssinneszellen. J Comp Physiol A. 1974;90:183–205. doi: 10.1007/BF00694484. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Broca P. Perte de la parole, ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe antérieur gauche du cerveau. Bull Soc Anthropol. 1861;2:235–238. [Google Scholar]
- Bucher D, Scholz M, Stetter M, Obermayer K, Pfluger HJ. Correction methods for three-dimensional reconstructions from confocal images: I. tissue shrinking and axial scaling. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;100:135–143. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(00)00245-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denk W, Svoboda K. Photon upmanship: why multiphoton imaging is more than a gimmick. Neuron. 1997;18:351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81237-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denk W, Strickler JH, Webb WW. Two-photon laser scanning fluorescence microscopy. Science. 1990;248:73–76. doi: 10.1126/science.2321027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietz A, Humphreys J. Scanning electron microscopic studies of antennal receptors of the worker honey bee, including Sensilla campaniformia. Ann Entomol Soc Amer. 1971;64:919–925. [Google Scholar]
- Duistermars BJ, Chow DM, Frye Ma. Flies require bilateral sensory input to track odor gradients in flight. Curr Biol. 2009;19(15):1301–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faber T, Joerges J, Menzel R. Associative learning modifies neural representation of odours in the insect brain. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:74–78. doi: 10.1038/4576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez PC, Locatelli FF, Person-Rennell N, Deleo G, Smith BH. Associative Conditioning tunes transient dynamics of early olfactory processing. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10191–10202. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franke T. PhD thesis. Freien Universität; Berlin: 2009. In vivo 2-photon calcium imaging of olfactory interneurons in the honeybee antennal lobe. [Google Scholar]
- Frasnelli E, Anfora G, Trona F, Tessarolo F, Vallortigara G. Morpho-functional asymmetry of the olfactory receptors of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) Behav Brain Res. 2010a;209:221–225. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frasnelli E, Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Response competition associated with right-left antennal asymmetries of new and old olfactory memory traces in honeybees. Behav Brain Res. 2010b;209:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Szyszka P. Olfactory coding in the insect brain: molecular receptive ranges, spatial and temporal coding. Entomol Exp Appl. 2008;128(1):81–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.2007.00661.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Vetter R. Optical methods for analyzing odor-evoked activity in the insect brain. In: Christensen TA, editor. Advances in Insect Sensory Neuroscience. CRC press; Boca Raton: 2004. pp. 349–392. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Nägler K, Hölldobler B, Menzel R. Odour coding is bilaterally symmetrical in the antennal lobes of honeybees (Apis mellifera) Europ J Neurosci. 1998;10:2964–2974. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1998.00303.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Sachse S, Rappert A, Menzel R. The glomerular code for odor representation is species specific in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Nat Neurosci. 1999a;2:473–478. doi: 10.1038/8144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, McIlwrath SL, Menzel R. A digital three-dimensional atlas of the honeybee antennal lobe based on optical sections acquired by confocal microscopy. Cell Tissue Res. 1999b;295:383–394. doi: 10.1007/s004410051245. http://neuro.unikonstanz.de/23beemorph/default.html. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelperin A, Flores J. Vital staining from dye-coated microprobes identifies new olfactory interneurons for optical and electrical recording. J Neurosci Methods. 1997;72:97–108. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(96)02169-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M. Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the honeybee: a taste from the magic well. J Comp Physiol A. 2007;193:801–824. doi: 10.1007/s00359-007-0235-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grynkiewicz G, Poenie M, Tsien RY. A New Generation of Ca2+ Indicators with Greatly Improved Fluorescence Properties. J Biol Chem. 1985;260:3440–3450. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haase A, Rigosi E, Trona F, Anfora G, Vallortigara G, Antolini R, Vinegoni C. In vivo two-photon imaging of the honey bee antennal lobe. Biomed Opt Express. 2011;2:131–138. doi: 10.1364/BOE.2.000131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobert O, Johnston RJ, Jr, Chang S. Left–right asymmetry in the nervous system: the Caenorhabditis elegans model. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:629–640. doi: 10.1038/nrn897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hourcade B, Perisse E, Devaud JM, Sandoz JC. Long-term memory shapes the primary olfactory center of an insect brain. Learn Mem. 2009;16:607–615. doi: 10.1101/lm.1445609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Junek S, Kludt E, Wolf F, Schild D. Olfactory Coding with Patterns of Response Latencies. Neuron. 2010;67:872–884. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaissling KE, Thorson J. Insect olfactory sensilla: structural, chemical and electrical aspects of the functional organization. In: Sattelle DB, Hall LM, Hildebrand JG, editors. Receptors for Neurotransmitters. Hormones, and Pheromones in Insects. Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press; 1980. pp. 261–282. [Google Scholar]
- Kelber C, Rössler W, Kleineidam CJ. Multiple olfactory receptor neurons and their axonal projections in the antennal lobe of the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2006;496:395–405. doi: 10.1002/cne.20930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kells AR, Goulson D. Evidence for handedness in bumblebees. J Insect Behav. 2001;14:47–55. doi: 10.1023/A:1007897512570. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krofczik S, Menzel R, Nawrot MP. Rapid odor processing in the honeybee antennal lobe network. Front Comput Neurosci. 2009 doi: 10.3389/neuro.10.009.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuwabara M. Bildung des bedingten Reflexes von Pavlovs Typus bei der Honigbiene, Apis mellifica. J Fac Sci Hokkaido Univ Zool. 1957;13:458–464. [Google Scholar]
- Laurent G. Olfactory network dynamics and the coding of multidimensional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:884–895. doi: 10.1038/nrn964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letzkus P, Ribi WA, Wood JT, Zhu H, Zhang SW, Srinivasan MV. Lateralization of Olfaction in the Honeybee Apis mellifera. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1471–1476. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieke EE. Optical recording of neuronal activity in the insect central nervous system: odorant coding by the antennal lobes of honeybees. Eur J Neurosci. 1993;5:49–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1993.tb00204.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacNeilage PF, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Origins of the left and right brain. Sci Am. 2009;301:60–67. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0709-60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto SG, Hildebrand JG. Olfactory Mechanisms in the Moth Manduca sexta: Response Characteristics and Morphology of Central Neurons in the Antennal Lobes. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1981;213:249–277. [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R, Giurfa M. Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:62–71. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R, Müller U. Learning and memory in honeybees: From behavior to neural substrates. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1996;19:379–404. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.002115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller D, Abel R, Brandt R, Zöckler M, Menzel R. Differential parallel processing of olfactory information in the honeybee, Apis mellifera L. J Comp Physiol A. 2002;188:359–370. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0310-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F. Ontogeny of bird song. Science. 1970;167:950–956. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3920.950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Oxford University Press; London: 1927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peele P, Ditzen M, Menzel R, Galizia CG. Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee antennal lobe neurons. J Comp Physiol A. 2006;192:1083–1103. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0152-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhard J, Sinclair M, Srinivasan MV, Claudianos C. Honeybees learn odour mixtures via a selection of key odorants. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e9110. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigosi E, Frasnelli E, Vinegoni C, Antolini R, Anfora G, Vallortigara G, Haase A. Searching for anatomical correlates of olfactory lateralization in the honeybee antennal lobes: a morphological and behavioural study. Behav Brain Res. 2011;221:290–294. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers LJ, Anson JM. Lateralisation of function in the chicken forebrain. Pharmacol Biochem Be. 1979;10:679–686. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(79)90320-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. From antenna to antenna: lateral shift of olfactory memory recall by honeybees. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(6):e2340. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers LJ, Zucca P, Vallortigara G. Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proc Biol Sci. 2004;271(Suppl 6):S420–S422. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Searching for learning-dependent changes in the antennal lobe: simultaneous recording of neural activity and aversive olfactory learning in honeybees. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:155. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Rappert A, Galizia CG. The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: steps towards the olfactory code. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:3970–3982. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00826.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz JC, Menzel R. Side-specificity of olfactory learning in the honeybee: generalization between odors and sides. Learn Mem. 2001;8:286–294. doi: 10.1101/lm.41401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz JC, Galizia CG, Menzel R. Side-specific olfactory conditioning leads to more specific odor representation between sides but not within sides in the honeybee antennal lobes. Neuroscience. 2003;120:1137–1148. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(03) 00384-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider D. Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen von Chemo- und Mechanorezeptoren der Antenne des Seidenspinners Bombyx mori L. J Comp Physiol A. 1957 doi: 10.1007/BF00298148. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sigg D, Thompson CM, Mercer AR. Activity-dependent changes to the brain and behaviour of the honey bee. Apis mellifera (L.) J Neurosci. 1997;17:7148–7156. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07148.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:575–633. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehead AT, Larsen JR. Ultrastructure of the contact chemoreceptors of Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera : Apidae) Int J Insect Morphol Embryol. 1976;5:301–315. doi: 10.1016/0020-7322(76)90030-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Winnington AP, Napper RM, Mercer AR. Structural plasticity of identified glomeruli in the antennal lobes of adult worker honey bee. J Comp Neurol. 1996;365:479–490. doi: 10.1002/(SICI) 1096-9861(19960212)365:3< 479:AID-CNE10>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamagata N, Schmuker M, Szyszka P, Mizunami M, Menzel R. Differential odor processing in two olfactory pathways in the honeybee. Front Syst Neurosci. 2009;3:16. doi: 10.3389/neuro. 06.016.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]