Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jun 4.
Published in final edited form as: Psychosom Med. 2010 Dec 10;73(1):29–43. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318200a54b

Mediation Analysis in Psychosomatic Medicine Research

Ginger Lockhart 1, David P MacKinnon 1, Vanessa Ohlrich 1
PMCID: PMC3366636  NIHMSID: NIHMS375650  PMID: 21148809

Abstract

This article presents an overview of statistical mediation analysis and its application to psychosomatic medicine research. The article begins with a description of the major approaches to mediation analysis and an evaluation of the strengths and limits of each. Emphasis is placed on longitudinal mediation models, and an application using latent growth modeling is presented. The article concludes with a description of recent developments in mediation analysis and suggestions for the use of mediation for future work in psychosomatic medicine research.

Keywords: statistical mediation, mediation analysis, mechanism, indirect effect

INTRODUCTION

Mediation analysis is a valuable tool for researchers because it can help explain the transmission of effects between two variables. In psychosomatic medicine research, the relation between psychosocial and physiological processes is often more clearly explained by mediating variables, such as health behaviors or interpersonal relationships (1,2). As a result, mediating variables are central to many questions in psychosomatic medicine:

  • Does socioeconomic status affect cortisol levels through psychosocial distress (3)?

  • Does an intervention designed to decrease anxiety result in lower mortality rates (4)?

  • Does childhood intelligence quotient predict locus of control, and does locus of control predict adult overweight and obesity risk (5)?

Questions like these suggest that psychosocial and biological variables may exist in an ordered chain of relations, in which one variable is thought to cause changes in a second variable, which then causes changes in a third variable. Mediation analysis is one way to investigate these processes.

Within the psychosomatic medicine literature, the importance of identifying mediating processes was expressed nearly 50 years ago, “… the question as to the mediating mechanisms remains one of the greatest puzzles of medicine” (6) and continues today, “prospective studies are needed to examine the behavioral mechanisms mediating the relationship between psychosocial factors and HIV disease progression” (1). In response, mediation studies have become more common. In this journal, more than 60 empirical articles tested some form of mediation since 1999, compared with the 23 articles of the prior decade. This surge in popularity has coincided with increasingly advanced methods to test mediating effects, which can complicate substantive researchers’ analysis decisions. This article is a guide to the major mediation approaches with a specific application to psychosomatic medicine research. First, we present statistical definitions of mediation, along with applications of the single mediator model and multiple mediator model. Second, we build on these basic models with a description of longitudinal mediation models and applications to experimental research. Finally, we discuss new methods for categorical data, person-centered approaches, and experimental approaches, and we conclude with limitations and future directions of mediation analysis.

Model-Building Considerations and Definitions

Like any statistical analysis, a significant mediation effect in a sample of data may or may not reflect a true underlying mediation relation. Thus, mediation analysis must be combined with research literature and sound theory to provide incremental evidence for mediation relations. This foundation is critical in the selection of mediating variables and their hypothesized relations to outcomes. Consider, for example, the relationship between exposure to neighborhood violence and overweight in adolescents. The links between exposure to neighborhood violence and elevation of blood pressure (7) and between blood pressure and body fat (8) suggest that blood pressure may be a mediator in the relationship between neighborhood violence and overweight. Researchers also use theory to guide their selection of mediators. For example, social cognitive theory can help generate a mediation model for a program to prevent falls in older populations (9). Self-efficacy, a central component of social cognitive theory, is thought to increase the likelihood that an individual will confidently perform a given task, such as physical exercise. Thus, a program designed to increase mobility among older adults could specifically target self-efficacy to increase mobility and reduce falls. In this case, self-efficacy is the mediator between the program and mobility.

In its simplest form, mediation is the extent to which a variable, M, is intermediate in a causal relationship between an independent variable X and dependent variable Y. The addition of this third variable suggests a causal chain, in which X causes M and M causes Y: X→M→Y. Mediation is conceptually distinct from other types of third variable relationships (10). For example, a third variable, Z, could cause both X and Y, such that excluding Z would lead to an incorrect inference of the relationship between X and Y. In this case, Z is a confounding variable. Confounding variables are independent from the causal process and can include stable characteristics (e.g., gender) or malleable variables (e.g., dietary behaviors) (11). Mediation and confounding effects are statistically equivalent yet conceptually clearly distinct (12). Thus, it is critical that researchers consider the potential for confounding effects by thoughtfully integrating theory, design, and prior research to construct a model representing a causal sequence.

Another third variable relationship that is different from mediation is a covariate, in which the information about variable Z improves the ability of X to predict Y but does not substantially change the relationship between X and Y. Finally, a third variable, Z, can moderate the relationship between X and Y, such that the relationship of X to Y differs at different values of Z. Unlike mediators, moderators, confounders, and covariates are not part of a causal sequence. The distinction between mediation and moderation has been the subject of much discussion and research (1316). There are more detailed distinctions among several of these third variable measures (17), but these major distinctions are useful for organizing most applications of third variable effects.

In mediation models, the X variable can be continuous or categorical. Gale and colleagues (5) found that a continuous measure of childhood intelligence quotient (X) predicted internal locus of control (M), which predicted health outcomes. Chesney and associates (18) randomly assigned patients with human immunodeficiency virus to a treatment or control condition (X), which predicted changes in coping efficacy (M), which then predicted psychological distress (Y). Randomized treatment studies, in which the X variable represents treatment or control conditions, are advantageous because the researcher manipulates the timing of the X variable, such that X precedes M and Y, making causal interpretation of results clearer. However, the use of X variables that do not represent randomization to conditions is often a necessity in psychosomatic research, for example, socioeconomic status, religiosity, or stressful life events. In these situations, a body of research and evidence from many sources must be brought to bear to claim mediation relations.

Mediation Regression Equations and Statistical Mediation Analysis

The following three regression equations provide the information for the most commonly used approaches to statistical mediation analysis:

Y=i1+cX+e1 (1)
Y=i2+cX+bM+e2 (2)
M=i3+aX+e3 (3)

where i1 and i2 and i3 are intercepts, e1, e2, and e3 are residuals, Y is the dependent variable, X is the independent variable, M is the mediator, and c is the coefficient relating the independent variable and the dependent variable in the first equation. Equations 2 and 3 are represented in a diagram of the mediation model in Figure 1, where c′ is the coefficient relating the independent variable to the dependent variable adjusted for the mediator, b is the coefficient relating the mediator to the dependent variable adjusted for independent variable, and a is the coefficient relating the independent variable to the mediator. Because the mediating variable, M, is both a dependent variable (Eq. 3) and an independent variable (Eq. 2), identifying mediation relations are often challenging.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Basic mediation model.

A review of empirical articles in Psychosomatic Medicine from the years 1999 to 2008 revealed 66 studies, which tested for mediation (Appendix). Of these articles, over half (n = 36) used the causal steps approach outlined in the classic work of Baron and Kenny (13,19) and Judd and Kenny (20,21). With this approach, four requirements must be met to establish mediation. First, the independent variable must be significantly related to the dependent variable in Equation 1. Second, a significant relation of the independent variable to the hypothesized mediating variable is required in Equation 3. Third, the mediating variable must be significantly related to the dependent variable when both the independent variable and mediating variable are predictors of the dependent variable in Equation 2. Fourth, the coefficient relating the independent variable to the dependent variable must be larger than the coefficient relating the independent variable to the dependent variable in the regression model with both the independent variable and the mediating variable predicting the dependent variable. The requirement of a significant X to Y relationship substantially reduces the power to find a true mediated effect (22,23). For example, the Baron and Kenny (13) causal steps test requires >20,000 subjects to detect mediation with 0.8 power when the effects of the a and b paths in the model are small and X→Y is completely mediated through M (a typical finding in psychology data) (23). Low statistical power is important in practice, because it leads to Type 2 errors, or the failure to find true mediated effects.

Appendix.

Mediation Articles, 1999–Present: Psychosomatic Medicine

Article Model Method
(77) Bosch et al., 2005 emotionality→catecholamines→NK mobilization Baron & Kenny
(78) Brummett et al., 2005 social support→health bx→mortality % reduction in IV coefficient
(79) Carney et al., 2007 depression→heart rate turbulence→time of death (survival) Kraemer
(80) Chen et al., 2003 SES→stress/beliefs→cytokine Stone
(18) Chesney et al., 2003 treatment→self-efficacy→psychological distress/well being Baron & Kenny
(81) Christensen et al., 2004 SES→health behavior→BMI % reduction
(82) Ciechanowski et al., 2004 attachment style→patient-provider relationship→diabetes adherence Baron & Kenny
(83) Cohen et al., 1999 stress→IL-6→disease expression Baron & Kenny
(84) Cohen et al., 2001 stress→health practices→antibody responses Baron & Kenny, Stone
(85) Cohen et al., 2005 large structural equation model with multiple mediators path analysis; unspecified
(3) Cohen et al., 2006 ed/income→psychosocial mediators→cortisol Percent reduction in IV coefficient
(86) Creed et al., 2005 sex abuse→somatization→pain Baron & Kenny
(87) D. Cruess et al., 2000 group assignment→change in BFS scores→Time2 cortisol Baron & Kenny
(69) S. Cruess et al., 2000 CBSM →social support → HSV-2 IgG levels Baron & Kenny
(88) Devriese et al., 2000
  1. intervention→negative affectivity→selective conditioning

  2. intervention→negative affectivity→generalization effect

unspecified
(89) Deykin et al., 2001 PTSD→depression symptoms/medical conditions→health services utilization Path analyses
(90) Ditzen et al., 2008 intimacy→affect quality→cortisol Sobel; MacKinnon’s critical values
(91) DuHamel et al., 2004 large structural equation model with multiple mediators Holmbeck; SEM
(92) Favaro et al., 2008 obstetric complications→harm avoidance→eating disorder Baron & Kenny; Sobel; nonparametric bootstrapping procedure
(4) Frasure-Smith et al., 2002 treatment→anxiety→survival Baron & Kenny
(93) Friedberg et al., 2007 alexithymia→anxiety→fatigue Baron & Kenny
(5) Gale et al., 2008 childhood IQ→locus of control→health outcomes % reduction in direct effect
(94) Gallo et al., 2001 education → HDL-C/smoking/waist-to-hip ratio → coronary calcification Baron & Kenny, Reduction of unstandardized regression coefficient between IV and DV
(95) Gallo et al., 2003 marital grouping→pulse pressure→atherosclerosis Baron & Kenny
(96) Garcia-Linares, 2004 abuse→psychological distress→immunity Baron & Kenny
(97) Germain et al., 2003 stress exposure→psychophysiological reactivity→REM sleep parameters Baron & Kenny
(98) Glei et al., 2007 chronic stressors→perceived stress→physiological dysregulation Baron & Kenny
(99) Grewen et al., 2004 depression→norepinephrine→blood pressure Baron & Kenny
(100) Gump et al., 2001 social interaction→talking→DBP Baron & Kenny
(101) Hall et al., 1998 intrusive thoughts/avoidance behaviors → sleep → natural killer cell # and function Baron & Kenny
(102) Janicki-Deverts et al., 2007 SES→psychosocial factors→urinary catecholamines % reduction in effect
(103) Johnston-Brooks et al., 1998 density→cardiovascular reactivity→number of Days Ill Path analysis, significant indirect effect (unspecified) (Pedhazur quoted)
(104) Johnston-Brooks et al., 2002 self variables → self-care → HbA1c Baron & Kenny
(105) Keogh et al., 2006 negative expectations→anxiety→fear Baron & Kenny
(106) Kivimäki et al., 2002 stressful life events → increased psychological problems and behaviors involving risk →health Path analysis
(107) Kivimaki et al., 2008 organizational justice→blood pressure→CHD Kraemer
(108) Kullowatz et al., 2008 stress→airway inflammation→lung function Baron & Kenny
(109) Labus et al., 2007 psych distress→gastrointestinal-specific anxiety→gastrointestinal symptom severity Bias-corrected bootstrap
(110) Lackner et al., 2004 depression→catastrophizing→pain severity Baron & Kenny
(111) Leucken et al., 2005 family relationships→self regulation→SBP reactivity/recovery SEM; presence of nonsignificant direct paths
(112) Lustman et al., 2005 depression→diabetes care→hyperglycemia Baron & Kenny; Kraemer; Babyak
(113) Lutgendorf et al., 1998 intervention→cognitive coping→total mood disturbance intervention→social support→total mood disturbance Baron & Kenny
(114) Mancuso, et al., 2004 pregnancy-specific anxiety→CRH→gestational age Baron & Kenny
(115) Manne et al., 2001 intrusions→avoidance→later distress (simplified description of complex structural equation model) Path analysis
(116) Matthews et al., 2000 SES →reduced serotonergic function → health Unspecified
(117) Miller et al., 1999 depression → physical activity → proliferative responses % reduction in variance
(118) Miller et al., 2004 stress→cortisol/health practices→antibody response Sobel; MacKinnon’s critical values
(70) Miller et al., 2007 SES→stress disparities→respiratory problems Unspecified
(119) Mohr et al., 2003 PTSD→sleep disturbance→somatic symptoms Baron & Kenny
(120) Moore et al., 2002 Income → sleep quality → psychological distress education → income → health Baron & Kenny
(121) Morin et al., 2003 stress→sleep arousal→sleep Baron & Kenny
(122) Mykletun et al., 2007 depression→health behavior→mortality % reduction of direct effect
(123) Niaura et al., 2000 Ho → BMI, WHR, and fasting insulin → TRG, HDL-C, SBP, and DBP Path analysis
(124) Peters et al., 1999 (mental) effort effect → heart rate/SBP/DBP →CD8+ and CD16+ cells (percentage and absolute numbers) and NKCA Baron & Kenny
(125) Raikkonen et al., 2004 trait anger→metabolic syndrome→atherosclerosis Path analysis; fit only
(126) Rodriguez et al., 2007 personal attributes→perceived harm→adult smoking Model indirect (Mplus; delta method)
(127) Rojo et al., 2006 stress→comorbidity→eating disorder AMOS indirect r-square
(128) Stansfeld et al., 1998 psychosocial factors→health-realted behaviors/biochemical measures→decrements in functioning Odds ratio adjustment
(129) Steffen et al., 2001 religious coping/ethnicity → satisfaction with support/BDI/alcohol Consumption → BP Baron & Kenny
(130) Sullivan et al., 2001 depression→positive affect→physical health Decrease in correlation b/n IV and DV by mediating variable
(131) Thomas et al., 2006 ethnicity→discrimination→diastolic pressor response Baron & Kenny
(132) Thomsen et al., 2004 rumination→immunity→health care utilization Baron & Kenny
(133) Thurston et al., 2006 educational attainment→depression/anxiety→CHD Cox proportional hazards; 1-log(HRadjusted)/log(HRunadjusted).
(134) Van Tilburg et al., 2001 BDI→SMBG frequency→HbA1c Baron & Kenny (weakened the effect of DV on IV)
(135) Vitaliano et al., 2005 caregiving→mental health→gingival symptoms Baron & Kenny
(136) Waldinger et al., 2006 childhood trauma→fearful attachment→somatization Baron & Kenny; Kraemer
(137) Whiteman et al., 2000 complicated SEM model Path analysis
(138) Wilson et al., 2005 personality→behavior patterns→mortality % reduction
(139) Wulsin et al., 1999 depression→chronic physical illness/smoking/alcohol abuse/suicide and related accidents→early death (4 mediators) Various

NK = natural killer; bx = —; IV = independent variable; SES = socioeconomic status; BMI = body mass index; IL = interleukin; BFS = benefit finding scale; HSV-2 = herpes simplex virus Type 2; IgG = immunoglobulin G; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; SEM = structural equation modeling; IQ = intelligence quotient; HDL-C = high-density lipoprotein cholesterol; DV = dependent variable; REM = rapid eye movement; DBP = diastolic blood pressure; HbA1c = glycated hemoglobin; CHD = coronary heart disease; SBP = systolic blood pressure; CRH = corticotropin-releasing hormone; Ho = cook-medley hostility scalescore; WHR = waist/hip ratio; TRG = triglycerides; NKCA = natural killer cell activity; AMOS = statistical program; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; SMBG = self-monitored blood glucose; HR = heart rate.

The mediated effect in the single mediator model (Fig. 1) may be calculated as either a product (âb̂) or a difference (ĉĉ′) between coefficients (24) if both M and Y are continuous variables. The product of coefficients method involves estimating Equations 2 and 3 and computing the product of â and , âb̂, to form the mediated or indirect effect (25). The rationale behind this method is that mediation depends on the extent to which X changes the mediator (M), represented by the coefficient a, and the extent to which the mediator affects the outcome variable, the coefficient b. The mediated or indirect effect is the change in Y when X is fixed and M is changed to the level it would have had when X is increased by one unit. To test for significance, the product is then divided by the standard error of the product, and the ratio is compared with a standard normal distribution. Many studies only require ĉ′ to be less than ĉ when testing mediation, but the significance of this drop should be tested to assess whether the reduction of ĉ to ĉ′ is consistent with chance.

The difference in the coefficients, ĉĉ′, from Equations 1 and 2, is the reduction in the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable adjusted for the effect of the mediator. To test for significance, the difference is then divided by the standard error of the difference, and the ratio is compared with a standard normal distribution. The âb̂ and ĉĉ′ measures of mediation are algebraically equivalent (26) for normal theory ordinary least squares and maximum likelihood estimation of the mediation equations. This equivalence does not always hold for logistic or probit regression; transformation is required to achieve similar results (24).

In the mediation approaches described above, a mediation effect is obtained by dividing the effect by its standard error and comparing the resulting ratio to a table of the normal distribution (27,28). If the absolute value of the observed Z statistic is greater than 1.96, then it is concluded that the effect would occur 5% of the time or less by chance alone. However, in the mediation model, statistical power and Type I error rates are too low for most tests of significance based on the normal distribution (22), and confidence limits are imbalanced (22,26). An explanation for these findings is that the product of two normally distributed random variables, such as the ab product estimator of the mediated effect, is not itself normally distributed (29,30). Asymmetric confidence limits for the mediated effect based on the distribution of the product (31) are more accurate than those based on the normal distribution. A new program, PRODCLIN (MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams, and Lockwood, 2007; program available at http://www.public.asu.edu/~davidpm/ripl/Prodclin/), estimates critical values of the distribution of the product and computes confidence limits for the mediated effect.

Computer intensive methods, such as bootstrapping and other resampling methods, make fewer assumptions and may be more accurate in this case because of the nonnormal distribution of the product. Fritz and MacKinnon (23) compared single-sample methods (such as the distribution of the product) to several computer intensive methods to test mediation in the single mediator model. This work showed that bootstrapping and distribution of the product were most accurate. Further work (32) has shown similar results for more complex mediation models that incorporate multiple independent and dependent variables.

Model Assumptions

Mediation analysis rests on several statistical and design assumptions that extend beyond the usual assumptions of regression models (10). First, the model assumes inclusion of relevant variables and reliable and valid measures of constructs (3335). For example, it is assumed that measurement error in any of the measures is not large enough to affect results. Measurement error in the mediator, for example, is likely to reduce estimates of the mediated effect (36). Second, the residuals in Equations 2 and 3 are assumed to be independent and that M and the residual in Equation 2 are assumed to be independent for the mediated effect, âb̂ (35,37). Third, it is assumed that the functional form-relating variables is correct. Most applications of mediation relations assume linear relations among variables but nonlinear relations among variables may be also be valid. Fourth, the model assumes no XM interaction, although the presence of such an interaction can be tested. Finally, it is assumed that the model is correctly specified with regard to causal ordering and direction of effects (e.g., X→M→Y rather than Y→M→X without bidirectional effects). There are several complicating aspects of this assumption that form most new developments in mediation analysis. For example, the model is considered self-contained with no outside or omitted influences that would alter the relations in the model. In many situations, it is likely that there are other influences, so researchers often include covariates and other variables to form more comprehensive mediation models. Additionally, the role of the mediator as both a dependent and independent variable creates ambiguities in the causal interpretation of mediation relations. There are several new approaches that attempt to distinguish types of mediation relations, such as those that are at fixed values of variables versus those relations obtained by controlling for variables (38). Most of these new approaches are based on counterfactual consideration of all possible conditions any one participant may serve in, as well as the actual condition in which the participant served. This ambiguity of interpretation of the M→Y relation in even the simplest mediation model highlights the need to evaluate carefully this relation in many forms of study, including experimental research designs, qualitative data, and replication studies. To date, these models have focused on the three-variable mediation model described above and have not yet incorporated longitudinal data. The specification of mediation relations with longitudinal data seeks to clarify the temporal precedence of variables in the mediation model, consistent with changes in X preceding changes in M, and changes in M preceding changes in Y (10,39). In light of the challenges of identifying true mediation relations, it is best to focus on building evidence for a mediation relation with a variety of qualitative, statistical, theoretical, and empirical evidence that is tested in new designs and different contexts (10).

Multiple Mediator Model

For psychosomatic medicine, models with multiple mediators likely provide a more accurate assessment of mediation effects. For example, a cardiovascular disease program may target multiple mediators, such as diet, exercise, and stress management. Models with more than one mediator are straightforward extensions of the single mediator case (40). Several standard error formulas for comparing different mediated effects are given by MacKinnon (10,40).

Effect Size Measures

Effect size measures of the mediated effect provide information about the size and meaningfulness of the mediated effect. These measures can be applied to individual paths (e.g., X→M) or for the entire mediated effect (âb̂). Details of effect size measures of individual paths are documented elsewhere (10); here we describe the most common effect size measures of the mediated effect. To illustrate, we use a study (41) from ATHENA (Athletes Targeting Healthy Exercise and Nutrition Alternatives), a preventive intervention designed to promote healthy eating and exercise habits among 1,668 adolescent female athletes. This program targeted several mediators and social norms (i.e., beliefs about the extent to which magazine advertisements are true), which were expected to be positively associated with intentions to engage in unhealthy weight control behaviors, such as diet pills or vomiting.

Proportion Mediated and Ratio Measures

The proportion of the total effect that is mediated (âb̂/ĉ) is one of the most common methods for assessing the effect size of the mediated effect. One mediation test in the ATHENA analysis showed that social norms mediated the relationship between treatment condition and intentions to use unhealthy weight control behaviors, with a proportion mediated of 0.45. Thus, the mediated effect explains 45% of the total effect of the program on unhealthy weight control intentions. One limitation of this method is its lack of stability for sample sizes less than 500 (26,42). Additionally, inconsistent mediation models, in which the mediated effect and the direct effect have opposite signs, may result in nonsensical values, such as a proportion greater than one or a negative value (12). For the inconsistent mediation model case, the absolute values of the coefficients can be used (25).

The ratio effect size measure, (âb̂/ĉ′), compares the mediated effect to the direct effect. To take the ATHENA example, the mediated effect through social norms was (0.219 × 0.234)/(0.006) = 8.5 times larger than the direct effect of program condition on intentions. This method is also limited by instability in sample sizes less than 500 (26).

R2 Measures

R2 measures divide the observed variance in the dependent variable into the part attributable to the direct effect, to the mediated effect, and the part that is not explained by either effect. For example, one measure represents the amount of variance in Y explained by M specific to the mediated effect. R2 measures have shown minimal bias for smaller sample sizes (14,43,44).

Longitudinal Mediation Models

The complex relationship between biological and psychological processes often calls for research questions that not only examine mediating mechanisms at a given time point but that also document changes in these mechanisms over time. Longitudinal data provide a closer representation of the temporal precedence assumption of the mediation model. It should be emphasized, however, that longitudinal data do not remove the need for models that are constructed based on theory and/or prior research; mediation relations may be missed due to a discrepancy in the timing of measurement and the timing of the mediated effect. Models with 3 or more waves, although they are complicated and may require large sample sizes, are often preferable because they likely increase the accuracy of the estimated relations among variables. For the longitudinal models described below, we turn away from the regression procedures discussed thus far and present mediation models that require a covariance structure program, such as SAS, Mplus (45), or LISREL (46).

Autoregressive Models

Autoregressive mediation models are path models in which contemporaneous and longitudinal relations among X, M, and Y across 3 or more time points can be tested. Large coefficients indicate high stability in the rank order among subjects; individual differences in growth are not modeled with this method. The basic three-wave autoregressive mediation model is a path model in which relations among variables one lag (wave) apart are considered, the stability of measures is assessed over time, and only longitudinal relations consistent with longitudinal mediation are considered (47,48). Figure 2 shows the basic autoregressive model. The arrows with sX, sM, and sY are the stability of the measures; a1 and a2 represent the two longitudinal a paths in the X to M relation; b1 and b2 are the two longitudinal b paths in the M to Y relations; and c′1 and c2 are the two longitudinal direct effects in the X to Y relations.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Basic autoregressive mediation model.

A second autoregressive model (Fig. 3) includes both longitudinal mediation and contemporaneous mediation (mediation within the same wave) for the last two time points. Because of issues concerning the timing of measurement versus the timing of the mediated effect, results of contemporaneous mediation may be more accurate than longitudinal mediation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Autoregressive mediation model with longitudinal and contemporaneous mediation.

A third type of autoregressive mediation model allows the relations between X, M, and Y to vary freely, in which paths opposite to the traditional direction (i.e., Y to M and X or M to X) are estimated (Fig. 4). These relations (indicated in bold) violate the temporal precedence of the mediation model but may be more reasonable than assuming these relations are zero; there may be a theoretical reason that such a cross-lag relation could exist. One example is involvement in an online weight loss program (X), designed to increase social support (M), which reduces weight (Y). Weight loss at Time 1 could increase the likelihood both of remaining involved in the online program and obtaining social support at Time 2. Again, theory and prior empirical evidence should guide these modeling decisions.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Autoregressive model with all paths free to vary (bold paths violate the temporal precedence of X→M→Y).

Autoregressive mediation models provide one flexible way for examining the extent to which a sample shows similar rank ordering among individuals for X, M, and Y for both cross-sectional and longitudinal relations. The most notable limitations of these models are that small sample sizes tend to yield biased standard errors, and cross-lag coefficients may be inaccurate because of sampling error (49). It is also important to note that autoregressive models are intended to measure stability, or lack of a sample’s movement in the levels of X, M, and Y. Thus, these models may be useful for data that are not expected to show large amounts of change over time. For research questions concerning the extent of individual change or “trajectories” of variables and the mediation relations among these trajectories, latent growth modeling may be an appropriate choice.

Latent Growth Models

Latent growth modeling (LGM), or parallel process modeling, has become an increasingly common method of analyzing longitudinal data (50). In the LGM framework, change in an observed continuous variable over time (i.e., slope) and the starting point (i.e., intercept) are treated as latent variables, which can either predict or be predicted by other latent variables. Average slopes within an LGM analysis can be either linear or nonlinear, adding further flexibility for more complex tests. Mediation LGMs in which X is continuous consist of three latent growth trajectories, one each for X, M, and Y (Fig. 5). The relations in mediation LGM are similar to those for a nonlatent mediation model, such that the relation between X and Y is explained by both the indirect effect through M and the direct effect. In the LGM case, the growth of M mediates the relation between the growth of X and the growth of Y.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Latent growth curve mediation model. I = intercept; S = slope.

Figure 5 shows a parallel process LGM of linear growth. The α path is the relation between the slope (change) of X and the slope of M. This path corresponds to the a path in the single mediator model. A significant α coefficient would indicate that a change in the trajectory of X predicted a change in the trajectory of the mediator. The β path is the relation between the slope of the mediator and the slope of the Y variable; this path corresponds to the b path in the single mediator model. The product of α and β represents the mediated effect. LGM can also be applied to models for which X is a categorical treatment condition. In this case, the α path is the mean difference in the growth rate of the mediator between the treatment and control group. A significant α coefficient in the expected direction would indicate that the prevention or treatment program was effective in changing the trajectory of the mediator. Significance testing of the mediated effect can be performed, using the bootstrap or distribution of the product methods described earlier.

To illustrate the parallel process LGM, we present findings from Adolescents Training and Learning to Avoid Steroids, a preventive intervention for male high school athletes (39). One of the goals of the program (X) was to improve athletes’ dietary habits (Y) through the perceived importance of information from their peer team leaders (M). Participants received either a team-based intervention program (X = 1) or a pamphlet addressing health behaviors (X = 0) and were measured four times at 6-month intervals. Figure 6 shows the findings of the linear growth model. The αβ mediated effect was 0.859 × 0.891 = 0.765 and the standard error (σab) of the mediated effect was 0.210. Significance testing, using first-order solution proposed by Sobel (51), indicates a significant mediation effect, (zαβ = 3.643; p < .001); the same is true for the test of asymmetric confidence intervals (95% confidence interval, 3.72, 1.20).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Results from parallel process growth model. Only values relevant to the final mediation analysis are shown. For more details, see Cheong et al (39). Program = prevention program condition (X); Nutrit = nutrition behaviors (Y); Perleader = team leaders as an information source (M); IM = initial status factor of PerLeader; SM = growth rate factor of Perleader; IY = initial status factor of Nutrit; SY = growth rate factor of Nutrit.

Thus, the program altered significantly the trajectory of importance of peers as an information source, which, in turn, altered the trajectory of nutrition behaviors.

In the difference score approach to longitudinal mediation, differences between the mediator and dependent variables scores are taken, as is the independent variable if it does not reflect assignment to treatment condition. These difference scores are then analyzed, using the same equations as for cross-sectional models. The latent difference score (LDS) model can also be applied to 3 or more waves, using a latent framework (5254). In the LDS model, fixed parameters and latent variables are used to specify latent difference scores, such that the model represents differences between waves as dynamic change. The LDS model can be especially useful in situations where it is expected there will be different predictors at different measurement occasions. For example, treatment effects may be significant for the first two waves of a study but not the final wave.

Timing Considerations

A central task for any longitudinal study is to minimize the discrepancy between the timing of measurements and the timing of observable changes in the variables under study. Measurement timing decisions are not easily made and are even more complicated for longitudinal mediation models because there are at least two sequences (X→M and M→Y) to consider. It has been suggested that longitudinal mediation analyses require a minimum of three time points to measure longitudinal relations for both the a and b paths (48). Consider a case, however, in which a mediated effect does not exist for a three-wave autoregressive model with a randomized X variable (Fig. 7a). This finding may actually reflect a measurement timing failure in which a mediated effect has been “missed.” For example, X-linked changes in the mediator may not be observable until a later time point (Fig. 7b). In this case, a two-wave model is favored because it represents more accurately the theoretical model for change.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Importance of the match between theoretical model for change and measurement in a longitudinal mediation model.

Categorical Data

Categorical outcomes, such as the presence versus absence of a given disease or substance abuse diagnostic category, are common in psychosomatic medicine research. Logistic regression is the method of choice for categorical outcomes because it transforms the expected values of the outcome so that these values can be interpreted in the same manner as ordinary least squares (55). This transformation is necessary to prevent the prediction of impossible values, such as numbers <0 or >1 in the binary case.

If logistic regression is used to analyze data for mediation analysis in Equations 1 to 3, the ab and c–c′ estimates are not equivalent because of the fixing of logistic regression error terms across equations (24). As a result, c will differ from c′ because of the mediated effect but also because the error term is fixed across equations, unlike ordinary least squares regression where the error term is allowed to differ across equations. Specifically, the ab mediated effect increases appropriately as the value of b increases, whereas the c–c′ method increases initially and then levels off and begins to decline, even though the true mediated effect continues to increase (56). Therefore, the c–c′ method of assessing mediation is not accurate for logistic regression or any method that fixes error variance to a certain value. One solution to this issue is to use a computer program that simultaneously estimates all regression equations so that the standardization is equivalent across regression equations (57). The Mplus computer program, for example, can be used to accomplish this goal (45). Similarly, variables can be specified as Poisson and other types of distributions.

Person-Centered Approaches

Each of the methods described thus far can be characterized as “variable-driven”; they measure the extent to which the relations between X, M, and Y (and consequently the mediated effect) are different from zero. Person-centered approaches provide a way of summarizing the data according to similarities in levels of or relations between variables. For the purpose of this article, we present approaches that have the common goal of identifying groups of people who are similar with regard to the type or intensity of mediation. We also discuss a hybrid mediation model, which combines variable-and person-centered analysis in a single model.

Collins and others (58) have proposed a method for identifying people with mediation “patterns” in treatment studies with three waves and binary X, M, and Y. To qualify for mediation with this method, individuals in the treatment group must have higher likelihoods of acquiring the expected level of the mediator (e.g., performing vigorous exercise for 20 minutes each day) and, subsequently, the expected level of the outcome (e.g., absence of major depressive disorder) than individuals in the control group. Specifically, three conditions must be met in order for individuals to meet requirements for mediation. First, assuming that individuals begin at a state that is absent of the mediator (i.e., they are not exercising for 20 minutes each day) and absent of the outcome (i.e., they have a diagnosis of major depressive disorder), the probability of being in both the mediator and outcome stage is greater in the treatment group than in the control group. Second, the probability of transitioning into the mediator stage from the no mediator stage is greater in the treatment group than in the control group. Using the example above, individuals who transition begin the study by exercising less than 20 minutes per day but, after exposure to treatment, exercise 20 minutes or more per day. This condition is congruent to a significant a path in Equation 3. Finally, for individuals not already in the outcome stage, being in the mediator stage increases the probability of transitioning into the outcome stage for both the treatment in control groups. Thus, membership in the state of exercising at least 20 minutes per day increases the probability that depressed individuals will transition into the “nondepressed” state. This condition applies regardless of treatment condition because the M→Y relation is not manipulated by the researcher.

The person-centered approach proposed by Collins et al. (58) can be useful in determining the extent to which a program is relatively effective for certain types of people, but the model assumes that individuals have neither the mediator nor the outcome at baseline measurement, which may be unrealistic for psychosomatic medicine researchers. As a result, this strategy may have more use in studies of very specific populations (e.g., depressed individuals who do not exercise). A second challenge with this approach is that it is only applicable to binary independent variables, mediators, and outcomes, resulting in reduced power to detect real effects (59).

MacKinnon (10) suggested that a person-oriented approach may be applied to continuous data by classifying individuals according to an underlying mediation process. Recently, von Eye et al. (60) proposed an application of configural frequency analysis to mediation. This method tests whether there are subgroups who differ from one another with regard to no, partial, or full mediation and can be applied to continuous X, M, and Y variables. The categories of mediation were constructed according to the conventions of Baron and Kenny (13), which may present considerable power issues because the sample is split into groups (23). The statistical properties of the configural frequency approach to mediation have not been tested in simulation studies.

A third application of person-oriented methods of mediation analysis identifies classes of individuals on the basis of shared, unobserved distributions, or mixtures, within a sample (61,62). Mixtures can represent latent (i.e., unobserved) classes at one point in time, latent growth classes, or regression classes. This framework also accommodates either categorical or continuous indicator variables. Although there is no established method for integrating mediation analysis into a mixture modeling framework, hybrid models testing the relation between class membership and an outcome can be used to determine indirect effects. Consider the latent growth example described earlier of the longitudinal effects of a preventive intervention on high school male athletes’ dietary habits. If there were a theoretical or empirical reason to believe that the sample contains unobserved groups with different growth shapes of the mediator (importance of peer leaders’ information), a growth mixture model (GMM) can be tested to deter- mine the number of growth classes. Specifically, it may be reasonable to expect that some individuals show an increase in importance of peer leaders’ information, whereas others show no change. Yet, another group may show an increase but not until a later wave. Figure 8 shows a hypothetical model in which growth mixture class, denoted as CM, mediates the relation between treatment condition and athletes’ dietary habits. CM is a categorical latent trajectory class variable. In this example, the latent trajectory class of the mediator predicts the outcome dietary behaviors. Interpreting the full model, treatment condition predicts individuals’ membership in a given trajectory class, which, in turn, predicts levels of dietary behaviors. The indirect effect from X to Y and the bootstrapped standard errors can be obtained in the Mplus software program (45).

Figure 8.

Figure 8

Hypothetical growth mixture model in which peer leader’s information growth mixture class (CM) is a mediator between treatment condition and outcome (dietary habits).

Because there are very few well-developed guidelines for assessing mixture models, it is important to use a systematic model-building procedure in addition to incorporating substantive theory. The statistical procedure for testing hybrid models should follow three general steps. First, determine the number of latent classes for the latent class variable. To begin, test the quality of a one-class model versus a two-class model and continue to increase the number of classes until the optimum number of classes is reached. A variety of fit indices and information criteria are available for determining the number of classes. For latent class analysis, the Bayesian information criterion (smaller numbers indicate more accurate models) and the Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test identify the correct model most consistently (63). Additionally, Tofighi and Enders (64) found that the sample size-adjusted Bayesian information criterion (65) and the Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test (66) provide the most consistent results for GMMs. The second step in testing hybrid models is to test the effect of covariates on class membership. In the mediation GMM example described above, treatment condition (X) is considered a covariate. Thus, a logistic regression analysis of the growth mixture class of the mediator (CM) on treatment condition (X) is the second step in this particular model. The final step is to test whether latent class membership predicts distal outcomes. This CM to Y relation is congruent to the b path in the simple mediation model described earlier. The significance of the indirect effect can then be calculated once this model-building procedure is complete. It is important to note that these models have strong assumptions concerning normally distributed repeated measures and that the introduction of covariates into a mixture model can affect the number of latent classes (67,68).

Experimental Approaches to Assessing Mediation

Because mediation entails causal relationships between variables, randomization of the predictor variables is necessary to establish causality. Many experiments within psychosomatic medicine have randomized the treatment or intervention (X), which allows the differences in means of groups on the mediator (M) and outcome (Y) variables to be attributed to the experimental manipulation. Because the mediator often cannot be directly manipulated, however, the M→Y (b) path is correlational. Experimental designs can help address this issue by manipulating the hypothesized mediator as directly as possible. For example, a blockage design uses an experimental manipulation to block the mediation process. If the resulting relation is removed, then there is evidence for mediation. Consider a study in which social support mediates the relationship between a cognitive behavioral stress management intervention (CBSM) and immunoglobulin G antibody titers to herpes simplex virus Type 2 (69). A theoretical blockage design would assign randomly those receiving the CBSM treatment to either a group which limits associations with friends or a group where no specification about social interaction is given (control). If social support mediates the relation between CBSM and immunoglobulin antibody titers, then herpes simplex virus Type 2 immunoglobulin G titers should be reduced in the control group but not in the group for which social support was limited.

A second experimental design is an enhancement design. This design is similar to a blockage study, but elevated levels of the mediator are used to enhance the mediation process. Miller, Cohen, and Herbert (116) described a mediation relation where depression reduced lymphocyte proliferation through physical activity. An enhancement design of this process would assign randomly those with depression to groups with varying levels of physical activity or a group that is given no instruction (control). If depression reduced lymphocyte proliferation through physical activity, then reduced lymphocyte proliferation should be seen in all physical activity groups, with elevated results for the groups with the highest levels of physical activity. For both the blockage and enhancement designs, the effect of assignment to the treatment (blocked versus not or enhanced versus not) group can be found by testing the equality of the a and b paths across the groups (10,71).

Limitations of the Mediation Model

Several limitations of the mediation model warrant discussion. Holland (33) first outlined how the usual structural equation modeling defined by Equations 2 and 3 does not clearly lend itself to causal effects even when X represents a randomized intervention. When X represents a randomized intervention, the a and c relations represent causal effects but the b relation does not, as it is a self-selected treatment. Several methods have been proposed to improve the interpretation of this relation as a causal effect. In principal stratification (72), independent strata of individuals’ mediator to outcome relation are hypothesized to obtain more accurate estimates. In marginal structural modeling, the influence of covariates is used to create propensity score approaches toward improving inference for this relation (17,73). Although these models attempt to address the complex task of identifying mediating variables, it remains unclear whether these more complicated models improve causal inference for real data (74).

A second issue related to causal inference is the problem of collider bias, which results from omitting one or more relevant covariates. Failure to incorporate covariates leads to inaccurate estimations of the M→Y relation. It is not possible to know definitively whether all relevant variables are included in the model, but theory and previous literature can guide these decisions.

Finally, because the mediation model is longitudinal by design, it may be of limited use in cross-sectional studies. Cole and Maxwell (48) and Maxwell and Cole (75) emphasized criticisms by Gollob and Reichardt (47) of cross-sectional mediation models, which do not account for the temporal ordering among the three variables. Two-wave mediation models allow for a longitudinal relation of the a path, but three-wave designs may provide a more convincing result in some cases, given that the variables are measured in concert with the true change process.

Summary and Recommendations

The purpose of this article was to outline the major approaches to testing mediation relations and provide an introduction to recently developed methods. Mediation models are natural tools for assessing causal processes in psychosomatic medicine because they help explain the often complex relations between the mental and physical domains of human functioning. The increase in the number of mediation studies has given rise to a wide variety of options for testing mediating processes, each with its own strengths and limitations. Baron and Kenny’s (13) causal steps approach has been the most widely used method in this journal over the past 10 years, but now newer more accurate methods are available. One challenge with this method is its reduced power due to the requirement that the relation between X and Y is significant (23). Additionally, other commonly used methods that use the standard normal distribution to test mediated effects are un-derpowered (22). Bootstrapping and confidence limits based on an asymmetric distribution produce more accurate results (22). Effect size measures, such as the proportion mediated (for samples with at least 500 cases) and R2 measures, can provide additional information on the size and meaning of the mediated effect (14,43,44). None of these methods, however, can replace theory and prior research-driven decisions of model testing.

Because the mediation model assumes specific temporal ordering of links among variables, more studies testing mediated effects over time are encouraged. Tests of longitudinal mediation, although complex, provide a more convincing case for temporal precedence than cross-sectional designs (10,48,75). Autoregressive models can estimate longitudinal mediated effects of 2 or more waves, but the interpretability of paths can sometimes be difficult (49,76). Rogosa (49) suggested addressing this issue with latent growth models. Parallel process latent growth models are useful in explaining mediated relations of growth among variables (39). LDS tests can be applied to models in which X does not represent a randomized treatment condition. Each of these and other tests of longitudinal mediation require careful attention to capturing the mediated effect as it actually occurs and to measuring variables as accurately as possible.

New approaches to mediation analysis, including experimental approaches, longitudinal mediation models, person-centered methods, and causal inference strategies may provide additional information about the links between bodily and psychological processes. Causal inference and experimental mediation designs may strengthen the evidence for causality because they introduce further manipulation into the M→Y relationship. Person-centered approaches provide a way of identifying groups that have similar meditational process patterns. Longitudinal models directly address temporal precedence among variables in the mediation process. These methods, still under development, represent promising tools for the difficult task of identifying mediating mechanisms in psychosocial and physiological processes.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01DA009757-09; MacKinnon) and the National Institute of Mental Health (P30 MH066247; Lockhart).

Glossary

ATHENA

Athletes Targeting Healthy Exercise and Nutrition Alternatives

CBSM

cognitive behavioral stress management intervention

References

  • 1.Gore-Felton C, Koopman C. Behavioral mediation of the relationship between psychosocial factors and HIV disease progression. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:569–74. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318177353e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Levenstein S. The very model of a modern etiology: a biopsychosocial view of peptic ulcer. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:176–85. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200003000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cohen S, Schwartz JE, Epel E, Kirschbaum C, Sidney S, Seeman T. Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the coronary artery risk development in young adults (CARDIA) study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:41–50. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195967.51768.ea. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Frasure-Smith N, Lespérance F, Gravel G, Masson A, Juneau M, Bourassa MG. Long-term survival differences among low-anxious, high-anxious and repressive copers enrolled in the Montreal heart attack readjustment trial. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:571–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000021950.04969.f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gale CR, Batty D, Deary IJ. Locus of control at age 10 years and health outcomes and behaviors at age 30 years: the 1970 British cohort study. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:397–403. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816a719e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ruesch J. Psychosomatic medicine in the behavioral sciences. Psychosom Med. 1961;23:277–86. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196107000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Murali R, Chen E. Exposure to violence and cardiovascular and neuroendocrine measures in adolescents. Ann Behav Med. 2005;30:155–63. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3002_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Epel ES, McEwen B, Seeman T, Matthews K, Castellazzo G, Brownell KD, Bell J, Ickovics JR. Stress and body shape: stress-induced cortisol secretion is consistently greater among women with central fat. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:623–32. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200009000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bandura A. Human agency in social cognitive theory. Am Psychol. 1989;44:1175–84. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.44.9.1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.MacKinnon DP. Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Last JM. A Dictionary of Epidemiology. 2. New York: Oxford University; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000;1:173–81. doi: 10.1023/a:1026595011371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173–82. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fairchild AJ, MacKinnon DP, Taborga M, Taylor AB. R2 effect-size measures for mediation analysis. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:486–98. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.2.486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Holmbeck GN. Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65:599–610. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kraemer HC, Stice E, Kazdin A, Offord D, Kupfer D. How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:848–56. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.6.848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Robins JM, Greenland S. Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology. 1992;3:143–55. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199203000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chesney MA, Chambers DB, Taylor JM, Johnson LM, Folkman S. Coping effectiveness training for men living with HIV: results from a randomized clinical trial testing a group-based intervention. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:1038–46. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097344.78697.ed. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N. Data analysis in social psychology. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology. 4. Vol. 1. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Judd CM, Kenny DA. Estimating the Effects of Social Interventions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review. 1981;5:602–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behav Res. 2004;39:99–128. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:233–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.MacKinnon DP, Dwyer JH. Estimating mediated effects in prevention studies. Evaluation Review. 1993;17:144–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Alwin DF, Hauser RM. The decomposition of effects in path analysis. Am Sociol Rev. 1975;40:37–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.MacKinnon DP, Warsi G, Dwyer JH. A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behav Res. 1995;30:41–62. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3001_3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bollen KA, Stine RA. Direct and indirect effects: classical and bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociological Methodology. 1990;20:115–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wolchik SA, West SG, Westover S, Sandler IN, Martin A, Lustig J, Tein J-Y, Fisher J. The children of divorce parenting intervention: outcome evaluation of an empirically based program. Am J Community Psychol. 1993;21:293–331. doi: 10.1007/BF00941505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Craig CC. On the frequency function of xy. Ann Math Statis. 1936;7:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Springer MD, Thompson WE. The distribution of products of independent random variables. SIAM J Appl Math. 1966;14:511–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Meeker WQ, Jr, Cornwell LW, Aroian LA. The product of two normally distributed random variables. In: Kennedy WJ, Odeh RE, editors. Selected Tables in Mathematical Statistics. VII. Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Williams J, MacKinnon DP. Resampling and distribution of the product methods for testing indirect effects in complex models. Struct Equ Modeling. 2008;15:23–51. doi: 10.1080/10705510701758166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Holland PW. Causal inference, path analysis, and recursive structural equation models. Sociological Methodology. 1988;18:449–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.James LR, Brett JM. Mediators, moderators, and tests for mediation. J Appl Psychol. 1984;69:307–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.McDonald RP. Haldane’s lungs: a case study in path analysis. Multivariate Behav Res. 1997;32:1–38. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3201_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hoyle RH, Kenny DA. Sample size, reliability, and tests of statistical mediation. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Statistical Strategies for Small Sample Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Merrill RM. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Tempe, AZ: Arizona State University; 1994. Treatment effect evaluation in nonadditive mediation models. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Emsley R, Dunn G, White IR. Mediation and moderation of treatment effects in randomised controlled trials of complex interventions. Stat Methods Med Res. 2010;19:237–70. doi: 10.1177/0962280209105014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cheong J, MacKinnon DP, Khoo ST. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Struct Equ Modeling. 2003;10:238–62. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1002_5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.MacKinnon DP. Contrasts in multiple mediator models. In: Rose JS, Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, editors. Multivariate Applications in Substance Use Research: New Methods for New Questions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ranby KW, Aiken LS, Mackinnon DP, Elliot DL, Moe EL, McGinnis W, Goldberg L. A mediation analysis of the ATHENA intervention for female athletes: prevention of athletic-enhancing substance use and unhealthy weight loss behaviors. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34:1069–83. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Freedman LS. Confidence intervals and statistical power of the ‘vali dation’ ratio for surrogate or intermediate endpoints. J Statist Plann Inference. 2001;96:143–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Taborga MP. Unpublished Master’s thesis. Tempe, AZ: Arizona State University; 2000. Effect size in mediation models. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Taborga MP, MacKinnon DP, Krull JL. A simulation study of effect size measures in mediation models. Poster presented at the 7th annual meeting of the Society for Prevention Research; New Orleans, LA. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus 3.0: User’s Guide. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén;; 1998–2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D. LISREL (Version 8.5) [Computer Software] Chicago: Scientific Software International; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gollob HF, Reichardt CS. Interpreting and estimating indirect effects assuming time lags really matter. In: Collins LM, Horn JL, editors. Best Methods for the Analysis of Change: Recent Advances, Unanswered Questions, Future Directions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112:558–77. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rogosa DR. Myths about longitudinal research. In: Schaie KW, Campbell RT, Meredith WM, Rawlings SC, editors. Methodological Issues in Aging Research. New York: Springer; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Llabre MM, Spitzer S, Sieger S, Saab PG, Schneiderman N. Applying latent growth curve modeling to the investigation of individual differences in cardiovascular recovery from stress. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:29–41. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000107886.51781.9c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology. 1982;13:290–312. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ferrer E, McArdle JJ. Alternative structural models for multivariate longitudinal data analysis. Struct Equ Modeling. 2003;10:493–524. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.McArdle JJ. A latent difference score approach to longitudinal dynamic structural analysis. In: Cudeck R, du Toit S, Sörbom D, editors. Structural Equation Modeling: Present and Future. A Festschrift in Honor of Karl Jöreskog. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McArdle JJ, Nesselroade JR. Growth curve analysis in contemporary psychological research. In: Schinka J, Velicer W, editors. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology. II. New York: Pergamon Press; 2003. Research Methods in Psychology. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Winship C, Mare RD. Structural equations and path analysis for discrete data. Am J Sociol. 1983;89:54–110. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Collins LM, Graham JW, Flaherty BP. An alternative framework for defining mediation. Multivariate Behav Res. 1998;33:295–312. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fairchild AJ, MacKinnon DP. A general model for testing mediation and moderation effects. Prev Sci. 2009;10:87–99. doi: 10.1007/s11121-008-0109-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.von Eye A, Mun EY, Mair P. What carries a mediation process? Configural frequency analysis of mediation. Integr Psychol Behav Sci. 2009;43:228–47. doi: 10.1007/s12124-009-9088-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.McCutcheon AC. Latent Class Analysis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Muthén B, Asparouhov T. Growth mixture analysis: models with non-Gaussian random effects. In: Fitzmaurice G, Davidian M, Verbeke G, Molenbergs G, editors. Advances in Longitudinal Data Analysis. Virginia Beach, VA: Chapman & Hall/CRC Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14:535–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tofighi D, Enders CK. Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. In: Hancock GR, Samuelsen KM, editors. Advances in Latent Variable Mixture Models. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sclove LS. Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika. 1987;52:333–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001;88:767–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Distributional assumptions of growth mixture models: implications for overextraction of latent trajectory classes. Psychol Methods. 2003;8:338–63. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Muthén B. Latent variable analysis: growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In: Kaplan D, editor. Handbook of Quantitative Methodology for the Social Sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Cruess S, Antoni M, Cruess D, Fletcher MA, Ironson G, Kumar M, Lutgendorf S, Hayes A, Klimas N, Schneiderman N. Reductions in herpes simplex virus type 2 antibody titers after cognitive behavioral stress management and relationships with neuroendocrine function, relaxation skills, and social support in HIV-positive men. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:828–37. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Miller G, Chen E. Unfavorable socioeconomic conditions in early life presage expression of proinflammatory phenotype in adolescence. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:402–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318068fcf9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Mark MM. Validity typologies and the logic and practice of quasi-experimentation. In: Trochim WMK, editor. Advances in Quasi-Experimental Design and Analysis. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Frangakis CE, Rubin DB. Principal stratification in causal inference. Biometrics. 2002;58:21–9. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2002.00021.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Greenland S. Causal analysis in the health sciences. J Am Stat Assoc. 2000;35:286–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pearl J. Causality: Models, Reasoning, and Inference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Maxwell SE, Cole DA. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:23–44. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Dwyer JH. Statistical Models for the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York: Oxford; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Bosch JA, Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT, Marucha PT. Differential mobilization of functionality distinct natural killer subsets during acute psychologic stress. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:366–75. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000160469.00312.8e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Brummett BH, Mark DB, Siegler IC, Williams RB, Babyak MA, Clapp-Channing NE, Barefoot JC. Perceived social support as a predictor of mortality in coronary patients: effects of smoking, sedentary behavior, and depressive symptoms. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:40–5. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000149257.74854.b7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Carney RM, Howells WB, Blumenthal JA, Freeland KE, Stein PK, Berkman LF, Watkins LL, Czajkowski SM, Steinmeyer B, Hayano J, Domitrovich PP, Burg MM, Jaffe AS. Heart rate turbulence, depression, and survival after acute myocardial infarction. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:4–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000249733.33811.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Chen E, Fisher EB, Barcharier LB, Strunk RC. Socioeconomic status, stress and immune markers in adolescents with asthma. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:984–92. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097340.54195.3c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Christensen U, Lund R, Damsgaard MT, Holstein BE, Ditlevsen S, Diderichsen F, Due P, Iversen L, Lynch J. Cynical hostility, socioeconomic position, health behaviors, and symptom load: a cross-sectional analysis in a Danish population-based study. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:572–7. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000126206.35683.d1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Ciechanowski P, Russo J, Katon W, Von Korff M, Ludman E, Lin E, Simon G, Bush T. Influence of patient attachment style on self-care and outcomes in diabetes. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:720–8. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000138125.59122.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Cohen M, Mansoor D, Langut H, Lorber A. Quality of life, depressed mood, and self-esteem in adolescents with heart disease. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:i313–i318. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318051542c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cohen M, Pollack S. Mothers with breast cancer and their adult daughters: the relationship between mothers’ reaction to breast cancer and their daughters’ emotional and neuroimmune status. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:64–71. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000151746.36540.6e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP. Psychological stress, cytokine production, and severity of upper respiratory illness. Psychosom Med. 1999;61:175–80. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199903000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Creed F, Guthrie E, Ratcliffe J, Fernandes L, Rigby C, Tomenson B, Read N, Thompson DG. Reported sexual abuse predicts impaired functioning but a good response to psychological treatments in patients with severe irritable bowel. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:490–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000163457.32382.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Cruess DG, Antoni MH, McGregor BA, Kilbourn KM, Boyers AE, Alferi SM, Carver CS, Kumar M. Cognitive-behavioral stress management reduces serum cortisol by enhancing benefit finding among women being treated for early stage breast cancer. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:304–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200005000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Devriese S, Winters W, Stegen K, Van Diest I, Veulemans H, Nemery B, Eelen P, Van de Woestijne K, Van den Bergh O. Generalization of acquired somatic symptoms in response to odors: a pavlovian perspective on multiple chemical sensitivity. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:751–9. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Deykin EY, Keane TM, Kaloupek D, Fincke G, Rothendler J, Siegfried M, Creamer K. Posttraumatic stress disorder and the use of health services. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:835–41. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200109000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Ditzen B, Hoppman C, Klumb P. Positive couple interactions and daily cortisol: on the stress-protecting role of intimacy. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:883–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318185c4fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.DuHamel KN, Manne S, Nereo N, Ostroff J, Martini R, Parsons S, Williams S, Mee L, Sexson S, Wu L, Winkel G, Bouland F, Redd WH. Cognitive processing among mothers of children undergoing bone marrow/stem cell research. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:92–103. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000108104.23738.04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Favaro A, Tenconi E, Santonastaso P. The relationship between obstetric complications and temperament in eating disorders: a mediation hypothesis. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:372–7. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318164604e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Friedburg F, Quick J. Alexithymia in chronic fatigue syndrome: associations with momentary recall and retrospective measures of somatic complaints and emotions. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:54–60. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802b873e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Gallo LC, Matthews KA, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Edmundowicz D. Educational attainment and coronary and aortic calcification in postmenopausal women. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:925–35. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200111000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Gallo LC, Troxel WM, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Edmundowicz D, Matthews KA. Marital status, marital equality, and atherosclerotic burden in postmenopausal women. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:952–62. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097350.95305.fe. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Garcia-Linares IM, Sanchez-Lorente S, Coe CL, Martinez M. Intimate male partner violence impairs immune control over herpes simplex virus type 1 in physically and psychologically abused women. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:965–72. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000145820.90041.c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Germain A, Buysse DJ, Ombao H, Kupper DJ, Hall M. Psychophysiological reactivity and coping styles influence the effects of acute stress exposure on rapid eye movement sleep. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:857–64. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000079376.87711.b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Glei DA, Goldman N, Chuang Y, Weinstein M. Do chronic stressors lead to physiological dysregulation? Testing the theory of allostatic load. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:769–76. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318157cba6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Grewen KM, Girdler SS, Hinderliter A, Light KC. Depressive symptoms are related to higher ambulatory blood pressure in people with a family history of hypertension. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:9–16. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000106881.60228.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gump BB, Polk DE, Kamarck TW, Shiffman SM. Partner interactions are associated with reduced blood pressure in the natural environment: ambulatory monitoring evidence from a healthy, multiethnic adult sample. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:423–33. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200105000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Hall M, Baum A, Buysse DJ, Prigerson HG, Kupfer DJ, Reynolds CF. Sleep as a mediator of the stress-immune relationship. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:48–51. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199801000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Janicki-Deverts D, Cohen S, Alder NE, Schwartz JE, Matthews KA, Seeman TE. Socioeconomic status is related to urinary catecholamines in the coronary artery risk development in young adults (CARDIA) study. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:514–20. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3180f60645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Johnston-Brooks CH, Lewis MA, Garg S. Self-efficacy impacts self-care and HbA1c in young adults with type I diabetes. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:43–51. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200201000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Johnston-Brooks CH, Lewis MA, Evans GW, Whalen CK. Chronic stress and illness in children: the role of allostatic load. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:597–603. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199809000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Keogh E, Hughes S, Ellery D, Daniel C, Holdcroft A. Psychosocial influences on women’s experience of planned elective cesarean section. Psychosom Med. 2005;68:167–74. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000197742.50988.9e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Kivimäki M, Vahtera J, Elovainio M, Lillrank B, Devin MV. Death or illness of a family member, violence, interpersonal conflict, and financial difficulties as predictors of sickness absence: longitudinal cohort study on psychological and behavioral links. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:817–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Kivimäki M, Ferrie JE, Shipley M, Gimeno D, Elovainio M, de Vogli R, Vahtera J, Marmot MG, Head J. Effects on blood pressure do not explain the association between organizational justice and coronary heart disease in the Whitehall II study. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:1–6. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815aaca3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Kullowatz A, Rosenfield D, Dahme B, Magnussen H, Kanniess F, Ritz T. Stress effects on lung function in asthma are mediated by changes in airway inflammation. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:468–75. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816f9c2f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Labus JS, Mayer EA, Chang L, Bolus R, Naliboff BD. The central role of gastrointestinal-specific anxiety in irritable bowel syndrome: further validation of the Visceral Sensitivity Index. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:89–98. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802e2f24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Lackner JM, Quigley BM, Blanchard EB. Depression and abdominal pain in IBS patients: the mediating role of catastrophizing. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:435–41. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000126195.82317.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Luecken LJ, Rodriguez AP, Appelhans BM. Cardiovascular stress responses in young adulthood associated with family-of-origin relationship experiences. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:514–21. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000160466.10397.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Lustman PJ, Clouse RE, Ciechanowski PS, Hirsch IB, Freedland KE. Depression-related hyperglycemia in type I diabetes: a mediational approach. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:195–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000155670.88919.ad. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Fletcher MA, Schneiderman N. Changes in cognitive coping skills and social support during cognitive behavioral stress management intervention and distress outcomes in symptomatic human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)-seropositive gay men. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:204–14. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199803000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Mancuso RA, Schetter CD, Rini CM, Roesch SC, Hobel CJ. Maternal prenatal anxiety and corticotrophin-releasing hormone associated with timing of delivery. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:762–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000138284.70670.d5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Manne SL, Glassman M, Du Hamel K. Intrusion, avoidance, and psychological distress among individuals with cancer. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:658–67. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Matthews KA, Flory JD, Muldoon MF, Manuck SB. Does socioeconomic status relate to central serotonergic responsivity in healthy adults? Psychosom Med. 2000;62:231–7. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200003000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Miller GE, Cohen S, Herbert TB. Pathways linking major depression and immunity in ambulatory female patients. Psychosom Med. 1999;61:850–60. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199911000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Miller GE, Cohen S, Pressman S, Barkin A, Rabin BS, Treanor JJ. Psychological stress and antibody response to influenza vaccination: when is the critical period for stress, and how does it get inside the body? Psychosom Med. 2004;66:215–23. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000116718.54414.9e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Mohr D, Vedantham K, Neylan T, Metzler TJ, Best S, Marmar CR. The mediating effects of sleep in the relationship between traumatic stress and health symptoms in urban police officers. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:485–9. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000041404.96597.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Moore PJ, Adler NE, Williams DR, Jackson JS. Socioeconomic status and health: the role of sleep. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:337–44. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200203000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Morin CM, Rodrigue S, Ivers H. Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:259–67. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000030391.09558.a3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Mykletun A, Bjerkeset O, Dewey M, Prince M, Overland S, Stewart R. Anxiety, depression, and cause-specific mortality: the HUNT Study. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:323–31. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31803cb862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Niaura R, Banks SM, Ward KD, Stoney CM, Spiro A, 3rd, Aldwin CM, Landsberg L, Weiss ST. Hostility and the metabolic syndrome in older males: the normative aging study. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:7–16. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200001000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Peters ML, Godaert GL, Ballieux RE, Brosschot JF, Sweep FC, Swinkels LM, van Vliet M, Heijnen CJ. Immune responses to experimental stress: effects of mental effort and uncontrollability. Psychosom Med. 1999;61:513–24. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199907000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Raikkonen K, Matthews KA, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kuller LH. Trait anger and the metabolic syndrome predict progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:903–8. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143638.31297.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Rodriguez D, Romer D, Audrain-McGovern J. Beliefs about the risks of smoking mediate the relationship between exposure to smoking and smoking. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:106–13. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802e0f0e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Rojo L, Convesa L, Bermudez O, Livianos L. Influence of stress in the onset of eating disorders: data from a two-stage epidemiologic controlled study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:628–35. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000227749.58726.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Stansfeld SA, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Marmot MG. Psychosocial work characteristics and social support as predictors of SF-36 health functioning: the Whitehall II study. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:247–55. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199805000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Steffen PR, Hinderliter AL, Blumenthal JA, Sherwood A. Religious coping, ethnicity, and ambulatory blood pressure. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:523–30. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Sullivan MD, LaCroix AZ, Russo JE, Walker EA. Depression and self-reported physical health in patients with coronary disease: mediating and moderating factors. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:248–56. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200103000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Thomas KS, Nelesen RA, Malcarne VL, Ziegler MG, Dimsdale JE. Ethnicity, perceived discrimination, and vascular reactivity to phenylephrine. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:692–7. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000238214.80871.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Thomsen DK, Mehlsen MY, Hokland M, Viidik A, Olesen F, Avlund K, Munk K, Zachariae R. Negative thoughts and health: associations among rumination, immunity, and health care utilization in a young elderly sample. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:363–71. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000127688.44363.fb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Thurston RC, Kubzansky LD, Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Do depression and anxiety mediate the link between educational attainment and CHD? Psychosom Med. 2006;68:25–32. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195883.68888.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Van Tilburg MAL, McCaskill CC, Lane JD, Edwards CL, Bethel A, Feinglos MN, Surwit RS. Depressed mood is a factor in glycemic control in type I diabetes. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:551–5. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Vitaliano PP, Persson R, Kiyak A, Saini H, Echeverria D. Caregiving and gingival symptom reports: psychophysiological mediators. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:930–8. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000188485.65153.7b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Waldinger RJ, Schulz MS, Barsky AJ, Ahern DK. Mapping the road from childhood trauma to adult somatization: the role of attachment. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:129–35. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195834.37094.a4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Whiteman MC, Deary IJ, Fowkes FGR. Personality and social predictors of atherosclerotic progression: Edinburgh artery study. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:703–14. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200009000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Gu L, Bienias JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in a defined population of older persons. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:841–5. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000190615.20656.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Wulsin LR, Vaillant GE, Wells VE. A systematic review of the mortality of depression. Psychosom Med. 1999;61:6–17. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199901000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES