Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Neurosci. 2012 Jun;126(3):371–380. doi: 10.1037/a0028453

The Importance of Considering all Attributes of Memory in Behavioral Endophenotyping of Mouse Models of Genetic Disease

Michael R Hunsaker 1
PMCID: PMC3367383  NIHMSID: NIHMS373192  PMID: 22642882

Abstract

To overcome difficulties in evaluating cognitive function in mouse models of genetic disorders, it is critical to take into account the background strain of the mouse and reported phenotypes in the clinical population being studied. Recent studies have evaluated cognitive function across a number of background strains and found that spatial memory assayed by the water maze and contextual fear conditioning often does not provide optimal results. The logical extension to these results is to emphasize not only spatial, but all attributes or domains of memory function in behavioral phenotyping experiments. A careful evaluation of spatial, temporal, sensory/perceptual, affective, response, executive, proto-linguistic, and social behaviors designed to specifically evaluate the cognitive function each mouse model can be performed in a rapid, relatively high throughput manner. Such results would not only provide a more comprehensive snapshot of brain function in mouse disease models than the more common approach that approaches nonspecific spatial memory tasks to evaluate cognition, but also would better model the disorders being studied.

Keywords: Behavioral Endophenotype, Mouse Model, Multiple Memory Systems, Attributes, Behavioral Genetics


In the evaluation of behavioral phenotypes in mouse models of genetic disorders, the background of the mouse provides a major contribution to the interpretation of any behavioral results (Moy, et al., 2008). This is not a trivial point as the FVB/N strain is commonly used for the creation of mouse models of genetic disorders, and the SJL/J mouse has been used in studies evaluating neurotoxicological effects on neurocognition (cf., Berman, et al., 2008; Hornig, et al., 2004). The SJL/J and FVB/N strains, however, are prone to early onset blindness, precluding many researchers from evaluating any behavioral phenotypes of founder generations of novel mouse models (Errijgers, et al., 2007). This is problematic because it is highly impractical for researchers to backcross a newly developed genetic model mouse with C57BL/6J mice until congenic prior to being able to determine if the mouse serves as a valid model for the disorder being studied.

As a step toward ameliorating this difficulty, Farley et al. (2011) applied a collection of behavioral tasks emphasizing nonspatial information processing to evaluate memory in FVB/N mice. Their focus was on the use of common behavioral paradigms that do not require visual function to demonstrate that FVB/N mice do not show poor memory or impaired hippocampal function, just poor vision. The primary strength of this report was that most of the tasks they used can easily be applied by most laboratories set up for behavioral analysis. What remained overlooked, however, was the applicability of the tasks they used as de facto models for human cognitive tasks evaluated in mouse models of disease as well as what specific domains beyond “memory” were being tested by each of the tasks they employed.

Research into mouse models of genetic disease tend to use the water maze, passive/active avoidance, or contextual fear conditioning as the primary index of “learning and memory” (Llano Lopez, et al., 2010; Stewart, Cacucci, & Lever, 2011), even though spatial memory is not always analogous to the learning and memory impairments reported in a number of genetic disorders. Furthermore, inherent in the use of the water maze as a measure of memory is the idea that memory has a direct relationship with hippocampal integrity, which is not true across all domains. In addition to spatial processing, the hippocampus has been implicated in temporal processing in humans and rodents (Chiba, Kesner, & Reynolds, 1994; Hopkins, Kesner, & Goldstein, 1995; Fouquet, Tobin, & Rondi-Reig, 2010). This is important because temporal processing deficits, have been linked to damage or impaired function to a number of brain areas, including (but by no means limited to): hippocampus (Fortin, Agster, & Eichenbaum, 2001; Hunsaker, et al., 2008), anterior thalamus (Wolff, et al., 2006), infralimbic/prelimbic (IL/PL) cortices (Mitchell & Laiacona, 1998), parietal cortex (Marschuetz, 2005), basal ganglia (Coull, Cheng, & Meck, 2011), and cerebellum (Pakaprot, Kim, & Thompson, 2009).

An important consideration in selecting an attribute to study in mouse disease models is to directly model what is being evaluated in human clinical populations. Temporal processing has been evaluated explicitly in a number of disorders, even being shown to be prodromal to full disease onset/progression in a number of disorders. For example, temporal ordering or sequencing deficits have been reported in Alzheimer’s Disease (Bellassen, et al., 2012), Parkinson’s Disease (Sagar, et al., 1988), Huntington’s Disease (Pirogovsky, et al., 2009), fragile X-associated disorders (Johnson-Glenberg, 2008; Simon, 2011), schizophrenia (Davalos, et al., 2003a,b), and autism spectrum disorders (Allman, et al., 2011)--in some cases, in the absence of memory deficits. Behavioral paradigms emphasizing temporal processing, when carefully parameterized, serve as appropriate models for these prodromal deficits seen in clinical populations.

In moving beyond simple memory tests in mice, it is important to evaluate all aspects of brain function in mouse models of genetic disorders, not just simple memory function--as clinical research has moved beyond using solely intelligence testing and into defining collections of quantitative traits that scale with either the severity of disease progression or dosage of the underlying genetic mutation, referred to as “endophenotypes”. Endophenotypes are collections of quantitative traits hypothesized to represent risk for genetic disorders at more biologically (and empirically) tractable levels than the full clinical phenotype which often contains more profound deficits shared across numerous genetic disorders. This behavioral endophenotyping approach this review emphasizes facilitates the identification of behavioral deficits that are specifically associated with both the specific genetic mutation and the pathological features observed in the clinical populations being modeled (cf., Hunsaker, 2012). When designed to evaluate specific disease related hypotheses, behavioral endophenotypes model quantitative patterns of behavioral deficits that scale with the size and/or severity of the genetic mutation (Gottesman & Gould, 2003). Importantly, although this behavioral or cognitive endophenotyping strategy does deviate slightly from the strict definition of an endophenotype as outlined by Gould and Gottesman (2006), the basic process and the underlying theory is the same.

To evaluate behavioral or neurocognitive endophenotypes in mice, it is critical to first define what is known concerning neurocognitive function in the population being studied, preferably through direct interactions with clinical scientists directly studying the disorder in question. The second step is to develop a set of mouse behavioral tasks to explicitly model the pattern of cognitive strengths and weaknesses present in the clinical populations in collaboration or consultation with the clinical scientists. The final step is to continue task refinement to evaluate the mouse model across the maximal number of features present in the clinical population to develop either risk prodrome for later disease progression/onset or biomarkers that can be used as outcome measures for interventional studies (cf., Hunsaker, 2012). The resulting data should provide a quantitative pattern of strengths and weaknesses that scale with the dosage of the mutation in question.

This review expands upon Hunsaker (2012) by moving beyond simply providing an underlying theoretical foundation for behavioral endophenotyping of mouse models of genetic disease. This is important as the review from Hunsaker (2012) proposed the theoretical foundation for behavioral endophenotyping in an abstract sense, but the information therein is difficult to apply to all but the highly experienced behavioral phenotyping laboratory. The present review will explicitly outline an intuitive, practical, and readily applicable methodology that can be applied by experienced behavioral neuroscientists, behavioral phenotyping laboratories, as well as molecular biologists in a relatively straightforward manner.

Approaches to Endophenotyping

Since the tacit acceptance of the water maze, passive/active avoidance and contextual fear conditioning as the standard memory tasks for mouse models of disease (cf., Llano Lopez, et al., 2010; Stewart, Cacucci, & Lever, 2011), the development of behavioral tasks to dissect the role of brain regions affected by the mutation for memory processing has stalled--at least in mice. In contrast, during this same period the research into the neural systems underlying learning and memory processes has reached a boon in rats. Quite recently, an effort has been made to translate the paradigms developed in rats into the mouse disease research (Hunsaker, et al., 2009, 2010; Nakazawa, et al., 2004; Rondi-Reig, et al., 2006).

What has remained elusive in the field of behavioral genetics is a clear theoretical rationale underlying the choice of experiments performed on each given model (i.e., water maze does not test all types of spatial memory, let alone all types of memory). In order to complete the goal of comprehensively evaluating learning and memory processes across all mouse models, it becomes critical to step back and separate learning and memory into component attributes that can be evaluated in turn (cf., Kesner & Rogers, 2004; White & McDonald, 2002). Such an approach allows the murine researcher to evaluate brain function at a level more sophisticated than previously possible using more standard behavioral tasks not developed with any particular cognitive domain in mind (cf., Hunsaker, 2012).

Before moving into a closer analysis of the proposed approach, it is important to mention the pitfalls with the common memory tasks used in mice: the water maze, passive/active avoidance, and contextual fear conditioning. All of these tasks can be useful as a component of a phenotyping approach, but in themselves, do not allow researchers to specifically determine the nature of impaired memory in mouse models. For all these tasks there are confounding factors relating to anxiety and, more importantly, the use of negative reinforcement as the primary motivation for task performance (cf., Barkus, et al., 2010). Additionally, it has been suggested on numerous occasions that the water maze may not be an appropriate task for use in mice, as their performance relative to rats is poorer than would be predicted when compared to performance on non-water based paradigms (Frick, Stillner, & Berger-Sweeney, 2000; cf., Whishaw & Tomie, 1996)--and dry land alternatives are often slow to be adopted (cf., Kesner, Farnsworth, & DiMattia, 1989; Llano Lopez, et al., 2010). Furthermore, when negative reinforcement is used for motivation, especially using assays such as contextual fear conditioning to evaluate spatial memory, models demonstrating disorders in affect (i.e., depression or anxiety disorders) may demonstrate memory deficits for reasons other than impaired spatial processing (cf., Banik & Anand, 2011).

Attributes of Memory Processing

Table 1 outlines the first consideration in developing or choosing behavioral experiments to test mouse disease models, which is to consider what type of memory needs to be tested in the mouse. Briefly, one has to consider if the disorder being studied primarily results in an episodic (Event-Based) memory deficit, Knowledge-Based memory deficits, or executive function (Rule-Based) deficits (Kesner & Hunsaker, 2010; Kesner & Rogers, 2004). Knowledge-based memory is often referred to as semantic memory in the human episodic memory literature. This review will use the term knowledge-based memory because semantic memory has an implicit language component that cannot be evaluated directly in rodents. It is more analogous to the reference memory system proposed by Olton et al. (1979). Once that is determined, then the component memory domains can be identified and tested using experiments designed with each disorder and model in mind (Hunsaker, 2012; Simon, 2007, 2011).

TABLE 1.

Description of the memory systems used in the attribute theory as applicable to research into mouse models of disease

Event-Based Knowledge-Based Rule-Based
Encoding
  • Pattern separation

  • Transient representations

  • Selective attention

  • Associated with permanent memory representations

  • Strategy selection

  • Rule maintenance

  • Short term memory

  • Intermediate term memory

  • Perceptual memory

Retrieval
  • Consolidation

  • Long term memory

  • Short term working memory

  • Pattern completion

  • Retrieval based on flexibility and action

Table 2 outlines a collection of simple tasks based on each component attribute that can be used to test cognitive dysfunction in mouse disease models. Aside from spatial attribute commonly tested, along with the temporal, response, social, and sensory/perceptual attributes tested thoroughly by Farley et al. (2011), it is also critical to evaluate the role of affect, proto-language, and executive functioning attributes in mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders, as these domains are often profoundly affected in these populations (Hunsaker, 2012; Simon, 2007, 2011).

TABLE 2.

Tasks that can be used to evaluate behavioral phenotypes in mice

Attribute Event-Based Knowledge-Based Rule-Based
Spatial
  • Metric processing

  • Topological processing

  • Magnitude estimation

  • Delay match to place with variable interference

  • Biconditional discrimination

  • Delay match to place with variable cues

  • Declarative sequence learning

  • Cheeseboard

  • Covert attention tasks

Temporal
  • Trace conditioning

  • Temporal ordering

  • Sequence learning

  • Sequence completion

  • Duration discrimination

  • 5 choice serial reaction time

  • Peak interval timing

  • Time left task

Sensory Perceptual
  • Delay match to sample with variable interference

  • Biconditional discrimination

Response
  • Ladder walking tasks

  • Acquisition of skilled reaching

  • Working memory for motor movements

  • Delay match to direction

  • Direction discrimination

  • Nondeclarative sequence learning

  • Reversal learning

  • Probabilistic reversal learning

  • Operant conditioning

  • Stop signal task

  • Serial reversal learning

Affect
  • Reward contrast with variable reward value

  • Classical conditioning

  • Trace conditioning

  • Conditioned preference

  • Anticipatory contrast

  • Operant conditioning

  • Gambling Task

  • Latent inhibition

Specific cross-domain tasks important for murine research into neurodevelopmental disorders
Executive Function
  • Contextually cued biconditional discrimination

  • 5 choice serial reaction time task

  • Operant conditioning

  • Covert attention tasks

  • Reversal learning

  • Probabilistic (80/20) reversal learning

  • Serial reversal learning

  • Stop signal task

  • Gambling task

  • Latent inhibition

Social
  • Social transmission of food preference

  • Social novelty detection

Proto- Language
  • Spectrographic analysis of ultrasonic vocalizations

An often overlooked, but critical consideration in choosing behavioral assays is that of the neuropathology associated with any disorder being modeled. It seems an obvious point that one would choose behavioral paradigms that emphasize spatial (and temporal) processing to evaluate disorders with known hippocampal pathology (e.g., Alzheimer’s Disease) and tasks emphasizing response learning in tasks showing clear basal ganglia pathology (e.g., Parkinson’s Disease), but unfortunately this is not consistently taken into consideration in experiments using mouse models of genetic disorders (cf., Taylor, Greene, & Miller, 2010; Wesson, et al., 2011).

Table 3 outlines neuroanatomical substrates underlying each attribute in mice that can be consulted to guide the development or application of behavioral tasks for mouse models of disease. Importantly, although these anatomical structures have been shown to underlie the attributes as mentioned in Table 3, this description is more of a blueprint of structures that are critically involved with these processes (for references pertaining to the structures mentioned in Table 3 cf., Arns, Sauvage, & Steckler, 1999; Dere, et al., 2007; Fischer & Hammerschmidt, 2011; Fukabori, et al., 2012; Harvey, Cohen, & Tank, 2012; Hunsaker and Kesner, 2010; Johansen, et al., 2011; Kargo, Szatmary, & Nitz, 2007; Lalonde & Strazielle, 2003; Madsen, et al., 2012; Matzel & Kolata, 2010; Simpson, Kellendonk, & Kandel, 2010). Stated another way, when one brain region is shown to underlie or be involved in a process, it is more likely than not that a larger network involving the candidate neuroanatomic structure actually underlies the process, and that the contributions of the larger network is more poorly understood than the role for the single structure. An example is the hippocampus: hippocampal ablations result in profound deficits for spatial and temporal processing (cf., Jerman, Kesner, & Hunsaker, 2006; Hunsaker, et al., 2008), but removal of inputs/outputs from the entorhinal cortex and septal nuclei result in qualitatively similar deficits (cf., Hunsaker, Tran, & Kesner, 2008). As such, it can be said the neural networks that include the hippocampus, and not the hippocampus in isolation, subserve spatial and temporal processing.

Table 3.

Neuroanatomical correlates underlying each attribute in mice

Attribute Event-Based Knowledge-Based Rule-Based
Spatial Hippocampus Parietal Cortex IL/PL*
Retrosplenial cortex
Temporal Hippocampus
Basal Ganglia
Anterior Cingulate
IL/PL* cerebellum
Anterior Cingulate
IL/PL*
Sensory/ Perceptual Sensory cortices TE2 Cortex
Perirhinal Cortex
Piriform Cortex
IL/PL*
Response Caudoputamen Precentral Cortex
Cerebellum
Precentral Cortex
Cerebellum
Affect Amygdala Agranular Insula#
Amygdala
Agranular insula#
IL/PL*
Executive Function Basal Ganglia
IL/PL*
IL/PL*
Parietal cortex
IL/PL*
Parietal cortex
Social Underlying neural networks still being elucidated
Proto-Language

Murine homologs of

inferior temporal cortex

*

medial prefrontal cortex

#

orbitofrontal cortex

Application of the Attributes to Behavioral Endophenotyping

Despite the need to move beyond limiting behavioral research to the standard behavioral paradigms (i.e., water maze, contextual fear conditioning), it is by no means necessary to avoid these tasks all together. Rather, it is important to integrate these tasks into more through behavioral analysis necessary for elucidating behavioral endophenotypes.

To begin, it is important to evaluate the basic sensory function in all mice, as any deficits in basic sensation or perception confound interpretations of behavioral results (Crawley, 2007). Sensory deficits do not preclude the behavioral analysis of a mouse model. When a mouse shows sensory deficits, either the model can be bred onto a different background strain over numerous generations--commonly >10 generations backcrossed onto the C57BL/6J strain--or else behavioral tasks can be chosen that minimize the contribution of the particular sensory modality that is not being processed, as Farley et al. (2011) demonstrated in their report emphasizing behaviors not requiring vision in mice that are blind from an early age.

After evaluating basic sensory function in the mouse model, it is critical to determine the pattern of behavioral strengths and weaknesses in the population being modeled by the mouse. With these information from the clinical population, it is important to either create or adopt behavioral tasks to evaluate the same cognitive attributes or domain as tested in the clinical population. For example, if a disorder being modeled shows global memory deficits (measured by intelligence (IQ) and neuropsychological tests) without concomitant impairments for executive function, then the mouse model needs to be tested for memory across a number of domains or attributes evaluated by the neuropsychological tests to better dissect cognitive function in the model. In this example, executive function should also be evaluated, but in this case to verify intact executive function in the model.

More concretely, a general memory deficit may be mediated by an inability to encode new information, consolidate/retrieve encoded information, or an inability to understand the rules required to perform correctly on a given test. All of these factors can be tested in mice, and can further be evaluated across domains: spatial, temporal, and response memory can be specifically evaluated in the mouse, as can the contribution of affect to memory, anxiety, and depressive behaviors. With these data, research into the mouse model may actually serve to inform the clinic as to more specific domains that can be tested in the clinic--emphasizing a direct interaction across the research in the clinic and the behavioral genetics laboratory (cf., Hunsaker, 2012).

A prime example of applying these attributes to develop an appropriate behavioral endophenotype of a mouse model is the elucidation of the BTBR T+tf/J mouse as a putative model of behavioral related to autism spectrum disorders (Moy, et al., 2007, 2008). Autism spectrum disorders are behaviorally diagnosed using on three core criteria: aberrant reciprocal social interactions, deficits in social communication, and repetitive, perseverative behaviors. As can be imagined, the typical battery of behavioral tasks present in behavioral phenotyping labs were initially unable to model these core features, as memory deficits, profound motor dysfunction, and impaired fear processing are not core criteria for an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis.

Recently, however, these three core features were broken down into component processes and each tested in turn to determine how well the mouse models autistic-like features. Reciprocal social interactions were reduced into the social recognition, social anxiety, and sociability domains that can be tested in turn. The evaluation of repetitive, perseverative behaviors did not require breaking the behaviors into component attributes, but rather required the development of novel behavioral paradigms to specifically model the results seen in the tests commonly administered to individuals with autism spectrum disorders. The evaluation of abnormal social communication was more difficult, as it required the careful analysis of vocalizations made by mice using tools from other disciplines. In short, ultrasonic vocalizations have been collected and their rate used as a measure of affect or stress (Moy, et al., 2007, 2008), but it was determined that if the individual vocalizations were analyzed spectrographically, differences among strains were possible, and any systematic differences unique to certain trains may be interpreted as abnormal communication. Altered scent marking behaviors also serve as communication deficits in mice.

To evaluate impaired social interactions, it was not only necessary to evaluate the general sociability of mice, but also to evaluate in more detail the interactions among the mice modeling autistic features and mice that do not. It has been demonstrated that the BTBR T+tf/J mouse shows reduced social approach to other BTBR T+tf/J mice, as well as to C57BL/6J mice, that the BTBR T+tf/J mice show elevated levels of social anxiety, and behaviors analogous to gaze avoidance, a commonly reported behavior in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (Bolivar, Walters, & Phoenix, 2006; Defensor, et al., 2010; McFarlane, et al., 2007; Pobbe, et al., 2010, 2011; Yang, et al., 2012). It has also been demonstrated that the BTBR T+tf/J mouse shows abnormalities in proto-linguistic processes as measured using social and nonsocially evoked ultrasonic vocalizations. These vocalizations are both reduced in quantity, and show abnormal structure when evaluated spectrographically (Scattoni, Ricceri, & Crawley, 2011; Scattoni, et al., 2008). Similar findings have been reported in communicative behaviors involving scent marking, an important method of communication among rodents (Wohr, Roullet, & Crawley, 2011). To evaluate repetitive and stereotyped behaviors as well as perseverative behavior reported in autism spectrum disorders, a number of paradigms were developed. These range from perseverative, restricted search patterns in the holeboard and impaired response reversal learning on a T maze (Moy, et al., 2007, 2008). One recent report of intact reversal learning with predictable reward shifts, but impaired probabilistic reversal with 80/20 reward contingencies directly models results of clinical research into autism spectrum disorders (Amodeo, et al., 2012). Motor and cognitive stereotypies have also been reported in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse (Pearson, et al., 2010).

As another example of the approach to behavioral endophenotyping, one can use the evolution of the research into one of the fragile X-associated disorders, the fragile X premutation. The fragile X premutation is the result of a tandem trinucleotide repeat on the 5′ untranslated region of the FMR1 gene that results in excess FMR1 mRNA and slight reductions in the FMR1 protein (FMRP) levels. Initially, it was thought the carriers of the premutation were cognitively unaffected by the mutation (Hunter, et al., 2008), but more recently a cognitive phenotype has been evaluated that includes altered hippocampal-dependent episodic learning, reduced affect, and spatiotemporal processing that is negatively modulated by increasing CGG repeat length (Goodrich-Hunsaker, et al., 2011a,b,c; Hessl, et al., 2011; Koldewyn, et al., 2008). There are two mouse models of the premutation, the CGG KI mouse (Willemsen, et al., 2003), and the CGG-CCG mouse (Entezam, et al., 2007). Both models have been cognitively evaluated using the traditional approach, using the water maze or passive avoidance to evaluate memory function. Despite identifying cognitive deficits and elevated anxiety in the CGG KI (van Dam, et al., 2005) and CGG-CCG mice (Qin, et al., 2011), the results are difficult to interpret in light of the phenotypes reported in premutation carriers, who do not consistently show memory deficits or anxiety disorders (Hunter, et al., 2010).

More recently, an endophenotyping approach has been applied to the CGG KI mouse, designed to specifically model the spatiotemporal processing results from Goodrich-Hunsaker et al. (2011a,b,c). The approach was to model the modulation of cognitive function seen in carriers of the premutation rather than large-scale deficits, because in these studies there were not consistent group differences between control participants and carriers of the fragile X premutation. The CGG KI mouse shows impairments for spatial processing, temporal processing, and reduced visuomotor function; furthermore, all these impairments worsen as a function of increasing CGG repeat length--suggesting the experimental paradigms may be useful to elucidate a behavioral endophenotype (Diep, et al., 2012; Hunsaker, et al., 2009, 2010, 2011; cf., Gottesman & Gould, 2003; Gould & Gottesman, 2006; Hunsaker, 2012). What remains untested in the mouse models of the premutation are any contribution of altered affect and executive function as has been reported in premutation carriers (Hessl, et al., 2011; Hunter, et al., 2012)

A number of neurodegenerative disorders also have mouse models that have not been throughly characterized beyond the traditional behavioral paradigms. Individuals with Huntington’s Disease do not only show chorea, but also progressive deficits for cognitive function. More precisely executive functioning deficits worsen across time into a subcortical dementia, as do memory deficits and neuropsychiatric sequelae such as anxiety, depression, and blunted affect (cf., Dorsey, et al., 2012). These features have been modeled in the mouse models (Fielding, et al., 2011). However, the neuropathological features associated with Huntington’s Disease, including reduced frontal-striatal connectivity, suggest the patients should show difficulties with temporal ordering and temporal functioning, and this has been proposed to underlie episodic memory deficits (Pirogovsky, et al., 2009). In Huntington’s Disease, deficits in picture sequencing and sequential motor learning have been reported (Feigin, et al., 2006; Foroud, et al., 1995; Ghilardi, et al., 2008; Snowden, et al., 2002), as well as deficits for a temporal ordering paradigm designed initially for rodents (Pirogovsky, et al., 2009). More importantly with the Pirogovsky et al. (2009) report, impaired performance for temporal ordering was correlated with time to symptom onset in individuals with the mutation underlying Huntington’s Disease--suggesting temporal ordering may be an endophenotype or prodromal feature that can be used to characterize the disease. In fact, temporal processing deficits have been reported in Alzheimer’s Disease (Bellassen, et al., 2012), Parkinson’s Disease (Sagar, et al., 1988), Huntington’s Disease (Pirogovsky, et al., 2009), fragile X-associated disorders (Hunsaker, 2012; Johnson-Glenberg, 2008; Simon, 2011), schizophrenia (Davalos, et al., 2003a,b), and autism spectrum disorders (Allman, et al., 2011).

Unfortunately, to date these sequencing/temporal processing features of Huntington’s Disease (among many others) have not been modeled in the respective mouse models. There are a number of behavioral paradigms that have been used in mouse disease models that can be use to test temporal processing in neurodegenerative disease, but they have yet to be widely applied in behavioral genetics research (DeVito, et al., 2009; Hunsaker, 2012; Hunsaker, et al., 2010).

Practical Advice to Apply an Endophenotyping Approach

There are a number of difficult questions that must be answered in the development of a comprehensive behavioral endophenotyping approach, including: how many of these behavioral tests could be conducted on a single cohort of mice? Would multiple tests on the tasks listed lead to potential confounds? What are potential the contributions/confounds of environmental effects such as housing conditions on mouse behavior? These are important factors as small changes in experimental design can make the difference between the successful analysis of a behavioral phenotype and the collection of uninterpretable data.

The question of testing the same group or mice across multiple is sometimes more complicated as it seems, but a solution can be designed if viewed through the attribute model. The most critical aspects that need to be taken into account when performing multiple experiments on the same mice are twofold: any role for negative affect on task performance, and the rule-based memory system succumbing to interference across tasks. It is important to keep in mind the role for negative affect for task performance not only on the present task, but also future tasks performed with the same group of mice. If a mouse is to receive fear conditioning in the middle of an experimental design, it will take a week or so of handling for the mouse to unlearn any associations between the fear conditioning and the experimenter (cf., Rudy & O’Reilly, 2001).

For the rule based memory system, it is important to remember that mice and rats take a significantly longer amount of time to learn and apply rules to guide behavior than humans. As such, if a researcher wants to perform any experiments that require the mouse to learn a rule or set of rules to guide behavior, and are not just exploiting the natural tendency of mice to explore their environment and behaviorally respond to novelty across domains (i.e., novelty detection), then intervening tasks not requiring rule learning/implementation need to be presented prior to the usage of another task taxing the rule-based memory system. Also important in this case is the use of very different apparatus for each rule-based learning task. or else previously learned rules will have to be explicitly extinguished prior to beginning training on a new task (cf., Cohen & O’Reilly, 1996).

As a rule of thumb, it is best to perform at most a set of experiments evaluating basic sensory function in mice (cf., Crawley, 2007), followed by tasks evaluating each attribute in turn, followed potentially by the water maze/avoidance tasks and fear conditioning as the final task to prevent carryover from experiments interfering with subsequent experiments. In this, most likely more than one experiment tasking executive function/complex rule learning can be performed. More than one of these tasks will result in interference that will confound interpretation of subsequent rule-based tasks. Additionally, aside from the water maze and fear conditioning experiments, it is recommended that series of experiments be counterbalanced across animals and groups, preferably using a latin square design to reduce the contribution of task order to any observed effects. Additionally, it is often worth testing the ability of mice to perform the behavioral task battery by evaluating a few wildtype mice of the background strain being used to verify they can perform all the tasks without being overwhelmed by excessive testing.

In addition to genetic background, the treatment of mice prior to and during experimentation is critical. It has been shown a number of times that alterations to the cage environment is an important factor for later behavioral improvement--such that an enriched environment results in better performance on behavioral tasks and increased grey matter and dendritic complexity. As such, mice should preferably be housed in a standard fashion, either with a set number of mice per cage or else singly housed--but it is important to note that mice do not do as well singly housed as rats, they tend to show increased anxiety levels which may affect task performance (cf., Van de Weerd, et al., 2002). As such, multiple housed is recommended unless precise drug dosing or food deprivation is required that precludes group housing. Furthermore, the amount of stimuli available to the mice within each cage should be uniform as well across cages. As such, a standard environment needs to be maintained among all mice during experimentation.

Conclusions

An important consideration in the study of mouse models of any genetic disease is how well the behavior of the mouse serves to actually model the behaviors present in the clinical population being modeled. In order to optimally address this concern is to explicitly develop a behavioral endophenotype of the mouse model in which the mouse is explicitly tested using tasks rigorously designed to explicitly test the cognitive domains affected in the clinical population.

To achieve this goal, it is important to not only apply standard behavioral tasks to mouse models of genetic disorders, but also to directly evaluate brain function across all cognitive domains. Furthermore, it is critical for behavioral genetics labs to interact with clinical research laboratories to develop comprehensive behavioral endophenotypes for the disorder being modeled. The strength of behavioral genetics and the mouse models is the ability to apply behavioral paradigms known to be subserved by known anatomical loci to determine not only the behavioral phenotype of the model, but also to elucidate candidate brain regions affected by the mutation (Robbins, et al., 2012). When research into mouse models of genetic disorders emphasizes patterns of mnemonic strengths and weaknesses across domains (i.e., the behavioral endophenotype), the results will not only directly model the disorder being studied, but may serve both as risk prodrome for disease onset or progression as well as outcome measures that can be applied by the clinic in interventional studies.

Acknowledgments

Funding:

MRH was supported by NINDS RL1 NS062411 awarded to Robert F. Berman. This work was also made possible by a Roadmap Initiative grant (UL1 DE019583) from the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (NIDCR) in support of the NeuroTherapeutics Research Institute (NTRI) consortium. The contents of this manuscript are solely the responsibility of the author and do not represent the official view of NINDS, NIDCR, or NIH.

References

  1. Allman MJ. Deficits in temporal processing associated with autistic disorder. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2011;5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amodeo DA, Jones JH, Sweeney JA, Ragozzino ME. Differences in BTBR T+ tf/J and C57BL/6J mice on probabilistic reversal learning and stereotyped behaviors. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;227(1):64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arns M, Sauvage M, Steckler T. Excitotoxic hippocampal lesions disrupt allocentric spatial learning in mice: effects of strain and task demands. Behavioural Brain Research. 1999;106(1–2):151–64. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00103-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Banik A, Anand A. Loss of learning in mice when exposed to rat odor: a water maze study. Behavioural Brain Research. 2011;216(1):466–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkus C, McHugh SB, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Rawlins JN, Bannerman DM. Hippocampal NMDA receptors and anxiety: at the interface between cognition and emotion. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2010;626(1):49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellassen V, Iglói K, de Souza LC, Dubois B, Rondi-Reig L. Temporal Order Memory Assessed during Spatiotemporal Navigation As a Behavioral Cognitive Marker for Differential Alzheimer’s Disease Diagnosis. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(6):1942–1952. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4556-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berman RF, Pessah IN, Mouton PR, Mav D, Harry J. Low-level neonatal thimerosal exposure: further evaluation of altered neurotoxic potential in SJL mice. Toxicological Science. 2008;101(2):294–309. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfm265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bolivar VJ, Walters SR, Phoenix JL. Assessing autism-like behavior in mice: variations in social interactions among inbred strains. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;176(1):21–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chiba AA, Kesner RP, Reynolds AM. Memory for spatial location as a function of temporal lag in rats: role of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1994;61(2):123–31. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80065-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen JD, O’Reilly RC. A Preliminary Theory of the Interactions Between Prefrontal Cortex and Hippocampus that Contribute to Planning and Prospective Memory. In: Brandimonte M, Einstein GO, McDaniel MA, editors. Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 267–296. [Google Scholar]
  11. Coull JT, Cheng RK, Meck WH. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):3–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Crawley JN. Mouse behavioral assays relevant to the symptoms of autism. Brain Patholology. 2007;17(4):448–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00096.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davalos DB, Kisley MA, Polk SD, Ross RG. Mismatch negativity in detection of interval duration deviation in schizophrenia. Neuroreport. 2003;14(9):1283–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200307010-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Davalos DB, Kisley MA, Ross RG. Effects of interval duration on temporal processing in schizophrenia. Brain and Cognition. 2003;52(3):295–301. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Defensor EB, Pearson BL, Pobbe RL, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. A novel social proximity test suggests patterns of social avoidance and gaze aversion-like behavior in BTBR T+ tf/J mice. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;217(2):302–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. The pharmacology, neuroanatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31(5):673–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. DeVito LM, Koningsberg R, Lykken C, Sauvage MM, Young WS, 3rd, Eichenbaum H. Vasopressin 1b receptor knock-out impairs memory for temporal order. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(9):2676–83. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5488-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diep AA, Hunsaker MR, Kwock R, Kim K, Willemsen R, Berman RF. Female CGG knock-in mice modeling the fragile X premutation are impaired on a skilled forelimb reaching task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2011;97(2):229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dorsey ER Huntington Study Group COHORT Investigators. Characterization of a Large Group of Individuals with Huntington Disease and Their Relatives Enrolled in the COHORT Study. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e29522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Entezam A, Biacsi R, Orrison B, Saha T, Hoffman GE, Grabczyk E, Usdin K. Regional FMRP deficits and large repeat expansions into the full mutation range in a new Fragile X premutation mouse model. Gene. 2007;395(1–2):125–34. doi: 10.1016/j.gene.2007.02.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Errijgers V, Van Dam D, Gantois I, Van Ginneken CJ, Grossman AW, D’Hooge R, Kooy RF. FVB.129P2-Pde6b(+) Tyr(c-ch)/Ant, a sighted variant of the FVB/N mouse strain suitable for behavioral analysis. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2007;6(6):552–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00282.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Farley SJ, McKay BM, Disterhoft JF, Weiss C. Reevaluating hippocampus-dependent learning in FVB/N mice. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;125(6):871–8. doi: 10.1037/a0026033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Feigin A, Ghilardi MF, Huang C, Ma Y, Carbon M, Guttman M, Eidelberg D. Preclinical Huntington’s disease: Compensatory brain responses during learning. Annals of Neurology. 2006;59:53–59. doi: 10.1002/ana.20684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K. Ultrasonic vocalizations in mouse models for speech and socio-cognitive disorders: insights into the evolution of vocal communication. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2011;10(1):17–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00610.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Foroud T, Seimers E, Kleindorfer D, Bill DJ, Hodes ME, Norton JA, Christian JC. Cognitive scores in carriers of Huntington’s disease gene compared to noncarriers. Annals of Neurology. 1995;37:657–664. doi: 10.1002/ana.410370516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5(5):458–62. doi: 10.1038/nn834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fouquet C, Tobin C, Rondi-Reig L. A new approach for modeling episodic memory from rodents to humans: the temporal order memory. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;215(2):172–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Frick KM, Stillner ET, Berger-Sweeney J. Mice are not little rats: species differences in a one-day water maze task. Neuroreport. 2000;11(16):3461–5. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200011090-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fukabori R, Okada K, Nishizawa K, Kai N, Kobayashi K, Uchigashima M, Kobayashi K. Striatal direct pathway modulates response time in execution of visual discrimination. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;35(5):784–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08005.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ghilardi MF, Silvestri G, Feigin A, Mattis P, Zgaljardic D, Moisello C, Eidelberg D. Implicit and explicit aspects of sequence learning in pre-symptomatic Huntington’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 2008;14:457–464. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2007.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Wong LM, McLennan Y, Tassone F, Harvey D, Rivera SM, Simon TJ. Adult Female Fragile X Premutation Carriers Exhibit Age- and CGG Repeat Length-Related Impairments on an Attentionally Based Enumeration Task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;5:63. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Wong LM, McLennan Y, Tassone F, Harvey D, Rivera SM, Simon TJ. Enhanced Manual and Oral Motor Reaction Time in Young Adult Female Fragile X Premutation Carriers. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;21:1–5. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Wong LM, McLennan Y, Srivistava S, Tassone F, Harvey D, Simon TJ. Young adult female fragile X premutation carriers show age-and genetically-modulated cognitive impairments. Brain and Cognition. 2011;75(3):255–60. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636–45. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gould TD, Gottesman II. Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal models. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2006;5:113–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00186.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Harvey CD, Coen P, Tank DW. Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task. Nature. 2012 doi: 10.1038/nature10918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hessl D, Wang JM, Schneider A, Koldewyn K, Le L, Iwahashi C, Rivera SM. Decreased fragile X mental retardation protein expression underlies amygdala dysfunction in carriers of the fragile X premutation. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70(9):859–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hopkins RO, Kesner RP. Goldstein M. Item and order recognition memory in subjects with hypoxic brain injury. Brain and Cognition. 1995;27(2):180–201. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1995.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hornig M, Chian D, Lipkin WI. Neurotoxic effects of postnatal thimerosal are mouse strain dependent. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9(9):833–45. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hunsaker MR. Comprehensive neurocognitive endophenotyping strategies for mouse models of genetic disorders. Progress in Neurobiology. 2012;96(2):220–241. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hunsaker MR, Fieldsted PM, Rosenberg JS, Kesner RP. Dissociating the roles of dorsal and ventral CA1 for the temporal processing of spatial locations, visual objects, and odors. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2008;122(3):643–50. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.3.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hunsaker MR, Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Willemsen R, Berman RF. Temporal ordering deficits in female CGG KI mice heterozygous for the fragile X premutation. Behavioral Brain Research. 2010;213(2):263–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hunsaker MR, Wenzel HJ, Willemsen R, Berman RF. Progressive spatial processing deficits in a mouse model of the fragile X premutation. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;123(6):1315–24. doi: 10.1037/a0017616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hunsaker MR, Tran GT, Kesner RP. A double dissociation of subcortical hippocampal efferents for encoding and consolidation/retrieval of spatial information. Hippocampus. 2008;18:699–709. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hunsaker MR, von Leden RE, Ta BT, Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Arque G, Kim K, Berman RF. Motor deficits on a ladder rung task in male and female adolescent and adult CGG knock-in mice. Behavioural Brain Research. 2011;222(1):117–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hunter JE, Allen EG, Abramowitz A, Rusin M, Leslie M, Novak G, Sherman SL. No evidence for a difference in neuropsychological profile among carriers and noncarriers of the FMR1 premutation in adults under the age of 50. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2008;83(6):692–702. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2008.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hunter JE, Rohr JK, Sherman SL. Co-occurring diagnoses among FMR1 premutation allele carriers. Clinical Genetics. 2010;77(4):374–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2009.01317.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hunter JE, Sherman S, Grigsby J, Kogan C, Cornish K. Capturing the fragile X premutation phenotypes: A collaborative effort across multiple cohorts. Neuropsychology. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0026799. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Jerman TS, Kesner RP, Hunsaker MR. Disconnection analysis of CA3 and DG in mediating encoding but not retrieval in a spatial maze learning task. Learning and Memory. 2006;13(4):458–464. doi: 10.1101/lm.246906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell. 2011;147(3):509–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Johnson-Glenberg MC. Fragile X syndrome: Neural network models of sequencing and memory. Cognitive Systems Research. 2008;9(4):274–292. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kargo WJ, Szatmary B, Nitz DA. Adaptation of prefrontal cortical firing patterns and their fidelity to changes in action-reward contingencies. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(13):3548–59. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3604-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kesner RP, Farnsworth G, DiMattia BV. Double dissociation of egocentric and allocentric space following medial prefrontal and parietal cortex lesions in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1989;103(5):956–961. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.5.956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kesner RP, Hunsaker MR. The temporal attributes of episodic memory. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;215(2):299–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kesner RP, Rogers JR. An analysis of independence and interactions of brain substrates that subserve multiple attributes, memory systems, and underlying processes. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2004;82(3):199–215. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Koldewyn K, Hessl D, Adams J, Tassone F, Hagerman PJ, Hagerman RJ, Rivera SM. Reduced Hippocampal Activation During Recall is Associated with Elevated FMR1 mRNA and Psychiatric Symptoms in Men with the Fragile X Premutation. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2008;2(2):105–116. doi: 10.1007/s11682-008-9020-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lalonde R, Strazielle C. The effects of cerebellar damage on maze learning in animals. Cerebellum. 2003;2(4):300–9. doi: 10.1080/14734220310017456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Llano Lopez L, Hauser J, Feldon J, Gargiulo PA, Yee BK. Evaluating spatial memory function in mice: a within-subjects comparison between the water maze test and its adaptation to dry land. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;209(1):85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Madsen H, Brown R, Short J, Lawrence A. Investigation of the neuroanatomical substrates of reward-seeking following protracted abstinence in mice. Journal of Physiology. 2012 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.225219. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Marshuetz C. Order information in working memory: an integrative review of evidence from brain and behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(3):323–39. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Matzel LD, Kolata S. Selective attention, working memory, and animal intelligence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(1):23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. McFarlane HG, Kusek GK, Yang M, Phoenix JL, Bolivar VJ, Crawley JN. Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2008;7(2):152–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00330.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mitchell JB, Laiacona J. The medial frontal cortex and temporal memory: tests using spontaneous exploratory behaviour in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research. 1998;97(1–2):107–13. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00032-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Poe MD, Nonneman RJ, Young NB, Koller BH, Bodfish JW. Development of a mouse test for repetitive, restricted behaviors: relevance to autism. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;188(1):178–94. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.10.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Young NB, Nonneman RJ, Segall SK, Andrade GM, Magnuson TR. Social approach and repetitive behavior in eleven inbred mouse strains. Behavioural Brain Research. 2008;191(1):118–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nakazawa K, McHugh TJ, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. NMDA receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5(5):361–72. doi: 10.1038/nrn1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Olton DS, Becker JT, Handelmann GE. Hippocampus, space, and memory. Brain and Behavioral Science. 1979;2:313–365. [Google Scholar]
  68. Pakaprot N, Kim S, Thompson RF. The role of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus in short and long term memory for trace eyeblink conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;123(1):54–61. doi: 10.1037/a0014263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pearson BL, Pobbe RL, Defensor EB, Oasay L, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Motor and cognitive stereotypies in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2011;10(2):228–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00659.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pirogovsky E, Goldstein J, Peavy G, Jacobson MW, Corey-Bloom J, Gilbert PE. Temporal order memory deficits prior to clinical diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2009;15(5):662–70. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Pobbe RL, Defensor EB, Pearson BL, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. General and social anxiety in the BTBR T+ tf/J mouse strain. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;216(1):446–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pobbe RL, Pearson BL, Defensor EB, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Expression of social behaviors of C57BL/6J versus BTBR inbred mouse strains in the visible burrow system. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;214(2):443–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Qin M, Entezam A, Usdin K, Huang T, Liu ZH, Hoffman GE, Smith CB. A mouse model of the fragile X premutation: effects on behavior, dendrite morphology, and regional rates of cerebral protein synthesis. Neurobiology of Disease. 2011;42(1):85–98. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Robbins TW, Gillan CM, Smith DG, de Wit S, Ersche KD. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2012;16(1):81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rondi-Reig L, Petit GH, Tobin C, Tonegawa S, Mariani J, Berthoz A. Impaired sequential egocentric and allocentric memories in forebrain-specific-NMDA receptor knock-out mice during a new task dissociating strategies of navigation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(15):4071–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rudy JW, O’Reilly RC. Conjunctive Representations, the Hippocampus, and Contextual Fear Conditioning. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;1:66–82. doi: 10.3758/cabn.1.1.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sagar HJ, Sullivan EV, Gabrieli JD, Corkin S, Growdon JH. Temporal ordering and short-term memory deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1988;111(Pt 3):525–39. doi: 10.1093/brain/111.3.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Scattoni ML, Gandhy SU, Ricceri L, Crawley JN. Unusual repertoire of vocalizations in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. PLoS One. 2008;3(8):e3067. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Scattoni ML, Ricceri L, Crawley JN. Unusual repertoire of vocalizations in adult BTBR T+tf/J mice during three types of social encounters. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2011;10(1):44–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00623.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Simon TJ. Cognitive characteristics of children with genetic syndromes. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2007;16(3):599–616. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Simon TJ. A new account of the neurocognitive foundations of impairments in space, time and number processing in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14(1):52–8. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Simon TJ. Clues to the foundations of numerical cognitive impairments: evidence from genetic disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;36(6):788–805. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Simpson EH, Kellendonk C, Kandel E. A possible role for the striatum in the pathogenesis of the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia. Neuron. 2010;65(5):585–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Snowden JS, Crawford D, Thompson J, Neary D. Psychomotor, executive, and memory function in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002;24:133–145. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.2.133.998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Stewart S, Cacucci F, Lever C. Which memory task for my mouse? A systematic review of spatial memory performance in the Tg2576 Alzheimer’s mouse model. Journal of Alzheimers Disease. 2011;26(1):105–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-101827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Taylor TN, Greene JG, Miller GW. Behavioral phenotyping of mouse models of Parkinson’s disease. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;211(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. van Dam D, Errijgers V, Kooy RF, Willemsen R, Mientjes E, Oostra BA, De Deyn PP. Cognitive decline, neuromotor and behavioural disturbances in a mouse model for fragile-X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS) Behavioural Brain Research. 2005;162(2):233–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Van de Weerd HA, Aarsen EL, Mulder A, Kruitwagen CLJJ, Hendricksen CF, Baumans V. Effects of Environmental Enrichment for Mice: Variation in Experimental Results. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 2002;5(2):87–109. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0502_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wesson DW, Nixon RA, Levy E, Wilson DA. Mechanisms of neural and behavioral dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Molecular Neurobiology. 2011;43(3):163–79. doi: 10.1007/s12035-011-8177-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Whishaw IQ, Tomie J. Of mice and mazes: similarities between mice and rats on dry land but not water mazes. Physiology and Behavior. 1996;60(5):1191–7. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00176-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. White NM, McDonald RJ. Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2002;77(2):125–84. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2001.4008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Willemsen R, Hoogeveen-Westerveld M, Reis S, Holstege J, Severijnen LA, Nieuwenhuizen IM, Oostra BA. The FMR1 CGG repeat mouse displays ubiquitin-positive intranuclear neuronal inclusions; implications for the cerebellar tremor/ataxia syndrome. Human Molecular Genetice. 2003;12(9):949–59. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddg114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Wöhr M, Roullet FI, Crawley JN. Reduced scent marking and ultrasonic vocalizations in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2011;10(1):35–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00582.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Wolff M, Gibb SJ, Dalrymple-Alford JC. Beyond spatial memory: the anterior thalamus and memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odor cues. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(11):2907–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5481-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Yang M, Abrams DN, Zhang JY, Weber MD, Katz AM, Clarke AM, Crawley JN. Low sociability in BTBR T+tf/J mice is independent of partner strain. Physiology and Behavior. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.025. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES