Abstract
A conspicuous feature of cortical organization is the wide diversity of inhibitory interneurons; their differential computational functions remain unclear. Here we propose a local cortical circuit in which three major subtypes of interneurons play distinct roles. In a model designed for spatial working memory, stimulus tuning of persistent activity arises from the concerted action of widespread inhibition mediated by perisoma-targeting (parvalbumin-containing) interneurons and localized disinhibition of pyramidal cells via interneuron-targeting (calretinin-containing) interneurons. Moreover, resistance against distracting stimuli (a fundamental property of working memory) is dynamically controlled by dendrite-targeting (calbindin-containing) interneurons. The experimental observation of inverted tuning curves of monkey prefrontal neurons recorded during working memory supports a key model prediction. This work suggests a framework for understanding the division of labor and cooperation among different inhibitory cell types in a recurrent cortical circuit.
Synaptic inhibition is of paramount importance to cortical recurrent dynamics, sensory processing, and memory function. The complex inhibitory operation is likely to be accomplished by coordinated action of many subtypes of GABAergic (GABA, γ-aminobutyric acid) interneurons present in the cortex. Recent years have witnessed a dramatic accumulation of our knowledge about these inhibitory cells, their morphology, physiology, chemical markers, synaptic connections, short-term plasticity, and molecular characteristics (1–6). On the other hand, little is known about specific computations by the diverse interneuron subtypes in animal behavior.
To elucidate distinct operations performed by diverse interneurons, we have investigated a cortical microcircuit model that incorporates three interneuron subpopulations. Specifically, we report here a recurrent network model for working memory in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). PFC is a brain system critical to working memory, the ability to hold information actively in the mind for a short period of time (7, 8). Understanding the cellular and circuit mechanisms of stimulus-selective persistent activity associated with working memory is a subject of intense current experimental and computational research (9, 10). More generally, persistent activity is believed to be a hallmark of strong recurrency in a cortical microcircuit, therefore modeling a working memory circuit represents a testbed for our investigation of cortical organization and functions.
Methods
Model Architecture. The network model represents a local circuit of dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex in monkey. There are four cell populations: pyramidal (P) neurons and three subpopulations of inhibitory cells. Perisoma-targeting, dendrite-targeting, and interneuron-targeting interneurons are assumed to express parvalbumin (PV), calbindin (CB), and calretinin (CR) calcium-binding proteins, respectively (Fig. 1). P cells are four times more numerous than interneurons, and half of the interneurons are of the CR type in prefrontal cortex (11, 12). For the sake of computational efficiency, simulations reported here were done with NP = 512, NCR = 64, NCB = 32, and NPV = 32. Neurons are spatially distributed in a ring according to the preferred cues (0–360°). The strength of the recurrent connections between neurons in the network depends on the difference between their preferred cues. The conductance between neurons i and j is gsyn,ij = GsynW(θi – θj), where W(θi – θj) is the normalized connectivity profile. The W for excitatory projections (pyramid-to-interneurons and pyramid-to-pyramid) is chosen as W(θi – θj) = J– + (J+ – J–) exp (–(θi – θj)2/2σ2) (13). Our reference parameters are: , σE→E = 18°, σE→CR = 36° for the tuned projections. Excitatory projections to PV and CB are unstructured , except for Fig. 3. Inhibitory projections are modeled as W(θi – θj) = A exp(–(θi – θj)2/(4π2σ2)), where A is a normalization constant. Inhibitory projections from CR and CB are narrow (σCB→P = σCR→CB = 9°), whereas projections from the PV neurons are unstructured. The recurrent excitatory N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) synaptic conductances are (in mS/cm2): GE→E = 1.63, GE→PV = 1.09, GE→CB = 1.12, GE→CR = 0.94. GABAergic synaptic conductances are: GPV→E = 0.93, GPV→PV = 0.65, GPV→CR = 0.98, GCB→E = 0.75, and GCR→CB = 2.81 (in mS/cm2).
All neurons receive unspecified external excitatory inputs mediated by α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors. This external input is modeled as Poisson spike trains to each neuron at a rate of νext = 1,000 Hz per cell, where the synaptic conductances are: gext,E = 0.2, gext,PV = 0.04, gext,CB = 0.025, and gext,CR = 0.055.
Neuron Models. We used Hodgkin-Huxley-type conductance-based models for single pyramidal cells and interneurons, which were calibrated by in vitro physiological measurements. Pyramidal neurons have three compartments, representing a soma/initial axonal segment(s) and a proximal (d1) and a distal (d2) dendrite. The neuronal input–output relation and the shape of the somatic and dendritic action potential were tuned to cortical slice data. Several ion conductances that have been identified in prefrontal pyramidal neurons are included in the model [see Tegnér et al. (14) and Supporting Text, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site]. The somatic compartment contains spike-generating currents (INa and IK), a high-threshold calcium current ICa, and a slow calcium-dependent cationic current ICan. The proximal dendritic compartment has a persistent sodium current INaP and a slowly inactivating potassium current IKS. The distal dendritic compartment has an ICa and a transient A type potassium current IA. The somatic voltage Vs, proximal dendritic voltage Vd1, and distal dendritic voltage Vd2 obey the following membrane equations: CmdVs/dt = –INa – IK – ICa – IL – ICan – gc1 (Vs – Vd1)/p1 – Isyn, CmdVd1/dt =–INaP – IKS – IL – gc1(Vd1 – Vs)/p2 – gc2(Vd1 – Vd2)/p2 – Isyn and CmdVd2/dt = –IA – ICa – IL – gc2(Vd2 – Vd1)/(1 – p1 – p2) – Isyn.
Perisoma-targeting (PV) interneurons are modeled as CmdV/dt = – INa – IK – IL – Isyn, hence they include only spike-generating sodium and potassium currents and show tonic fast-spiking behavior. Dendritic targeting CB interneurons are modeled as CmdV/dt = – INa – IK – Ih – ICa – IKCa – IL – Isyn. These cells show spike-frequency adaptation due to ICa and IKCa as well as postinhibitory rebound due to the hyperpolarization-activated current Ih (3). CR interneurons follow CmdV/dt = –INa – IK – ICa – IKCa – ICa,T – INaP – IL – Isyn. They contain a low-threshold calcium current ICa,T, which can generate a transient burst response, adaptation currents ICa and IKCa, and a persistent sodium current INaP. The interplay between these currents produces irregular firing patterns as observed experimentally (4).
A complete description of neuron models, with details of ion channel kinetics and conductance parameters, can be found in Supporting Text.
Synapses. Synaptic currents are modeled according to Isyn = gsyns(V – Esyn), where gsyn represents the maximal synaptic conductance, and the synaptic reversal potential Esyn (in mV) is 0 for excitation and –75 for inhibition. The gating variable s models the fraction of open synaptic ion channels and follows first-order kinetics for α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) and (GABA) transmission; ds/dt = αsF(Vpre)(1 – s) – s/τs; with αs = 12 (1/ms). The synaptic channels decay as τs = 2 (ms) (AMPA) and τs = 10 (ms) (GABA). The normalized concentration of the postsynaptic transmitter–receptor complex, F(Vpre), is 1/(1 + exp(–(Vpre)/2)). The dynamics of the voltage-dependent N-methyl-d-aspartate channel is modeled by second-order kinetics as: dx/dt = αxF(Vpre)(1 – x) – x/τx: ds/dt = αsx(1 – s) – s/τs; αx = 10, τx = 2 ms, αs = 0.5, and τs = 100 ms. Due to the voltage-dependent magnesium block, an additional multiplicative factor is introduced in the current equation 1/(1+[Mg2+] exp(–0.062V)/3.57), [Mg2+] = 1.0 mM.
Simulation Protocol. The simulation protocol used here follows the experimental procedure of a delayed oculomotor response task (15). The details were described previously (13, 14, 16).
Numerical Integration. The model was programmed in C and was integrated by using a fourth-order Runge–Kutta algorithm.
Physiological Experiments. Neurons were recorded from two male rhesus monkeys Macaca mulatta during a delayed oculomotor response task. The behavioral paradigm and recording methods have been described in detail (17). Neurons were classified as fast- and regular-spiking, as reported previously, according to our narrow classification criterion (17). In the present study, we identified neurons with inverted tuning curves as those that exhibited firing rates significantly lower in the delay than the baseline fixation period (0.5 s before cue presentation) and spatial tuning evidenced by significantly different delay-period firing rates across the eight target locations (ANOVA test, P < 0.05).
Results
Our model (Fig. 1) is designed to describe a local prefrontal microcircuit, which is endowed with extensive horizontal connections between pyramidal neurons within layers 2 and 3 (9, 18, 19). In the model, P neurons are labeled according to their preferred stimulus cues: synaptic connection strength between P neurons decreases with the difference of their preferred cues. The model also includes three subpopulations of inhibitory cells: perisoma-, dendrite-, and interneuron-targeting interneurons. We assume they express, respectively, PV, CB, and CR calcium-binding proteins. The three interneuron classes contribute to recurrent synaptic inhibition in specific ways (Fig. 1). PV interneurons, presumably of the large basket-cell type, provide widespread perisomatic inhibition to P cells (20). By contrast, CB and CR interneurons have narrow dendritic and axonal arbors and subserve local inhibition. CB interneurons project onto the dendritic sites of P cells (2, 5), whereas CR interneurons target other interneurons, preferentially CB cells (21–24). When CR interneurons are activated by elevated activity of local P cells, they strongly inhibit neighboring CB cells, leading to a feedback disinhibition of the same P neurons.
Spatially Tuned Persistent Activity. In our model, cells and diverse interneuron subtypes display distinct spike-firing patterns (Fig. 2a). PV cells are fast-spiking (25), CB cells show spike-frequency adaptation (26), and CR cells are characterized by irregular spiking patterns (4). Fig. 2b shows a network model simulation of the oculomotor delayed-response experiment (15). The network is initially in a resting state, with P neurons firing at a low rate (0.5–2 Hz) due to background inputs. A transient spatial cue triggers an increase of spike discharges in a subset of P cells (those for which the cue is the preferred stimulus): this activity persists after the stimulus offset due to excitatory reverberation in the recurrent network. The elevated firing (typically 30 Hz) remains spatially confined during the delay period. The visual cue is encoded and maintained in active memory by the peak location of the spatially tuned persistent activity pattern or bump attractor. At the end of the delay, a transient excitation terminates delay-period activity and switches the network back into the spontaneous-activity state. Therefore, our biophysically based model captures the main firing characteristics of PFC neurons described during a delayed oculomotor experiment.
To gain insight into how a spatially tuned mnemonic firing pattern is generated, we examined the coordinated activity across the four types of neurons (P, PV, CR, CB) (Fig. 3a). During the resting state, all four neuronal populations exhibit untuned activity with low firing rates. A cue stimulus produces a localized increase of activity in a subset of P neurons, which in turn recruit CR neurons with similar preferred cues (Fig. 3a). Activation of CR neurons then increases inhibition transmitted to CB neurons likewise tuned to the similar preferred cues, thereby reducing CB inhibition on P cells during the delay period (Fig. 3a). On the other hand, on the flanks of the bump, the enhanced activity of PV interneurons suppresses CR cells, consequently CB neurons receive reduced inhibition from CR cells (possibly also increased excitation from P neurons), hence they fire at higher rates during the delay period (Fig. 3a). The end result is that CB interneurons send enhanced inhibition to those P cells that are selective to other stimuli. Lower delay activity of P cells after a nonpreferred cue, compared to spontaneous activity, is consistent with prefrontal recordings from behaving monkeys (15) and in line with our network model of working memory (9, 13, 14).
On another trial, a different stimulus triggers a bump attractor of the same shape but peaks at a new cue location. Using different stimuli across trials, a given model neuron would display stimulus-selective persistent activity, with a tuning curve that can be compared with the experimental data (Fig. 3b). In conformity with a previous model (13), fast-spiking PV cells typically show higher spontaneous firing rates during the resting state (dashed horizontal line, Fig. 3b) and broader tuning of delay-period activity (solid line, Fig. 3b) compared with P cells. Moreover, CB interneurons (Fig. 3b) have relatively high spontaneous activity and inverted tuning curve (defined by a lower firing rate during delay than spontaneous activity, for some cues).
Inverted Tuning Curves. The model predicts inverted tuning curves as an indication of the proposed disinhibition mechanism for tuned persistent activity. Moreover, we expect only a small fraction of PFC neurons to exhibit inverted tuning curves, because the overall number of CB neurons is small in PFC (≈5%) (12). To test this prediction, we examined data from monkey PFC neurons recorded during an oculomotor delayed response task. In the previous work (17), putative regular-spiking P cells and fast-spiking inhibitory interneurons were differentiated according to the widths of their action potentials and firing rates. The tuning curve of delay-period activity is reproduced here for each of the two cell types (Fig. 3c Left and Center). Note that interneurons show higher spontaneous activity levels (horizontal line) and broader tuning curves, than P cells (Fig. 3c), in agreement with model predictions [(13), Fig. 3b]. Motivated by the present modeling results, we reanalyzed our database of 526 neurons that showed significant task-related activity. We found that 24 neurons (≈4.5%) displayed an inverted tuning curve of delay-period activity. An example is shown in Fig. 3c Right. As is evident in the rasters of the same neuron (Fig. 4a), delay-period activity is about the same as in (or slightly larger than) the spontaneous state for some stimuli and much lower for other stimuli. Fig. 4b shows the population poststimulus time histogram, averaging responses from all 24 neurons with inverted tuning. These neurons typically exhibit broad spatial tuning. The average baseline fixation firing rate of neurons with inverted tuning curves was 20.5 spikes per second. This is virtually identical to the baseline firing of fast-spiking (FS) neurons (19.9 spikes per second), as opposed to the firing rate of regular-spiking (RS) neurons (4.0 spikes per second). Interestingly, the spike widths of these cells fell between the distributions of the FS and RS neurons. The mean spike width of neurons with inverted tuning curves is 551 μs compared with the average FS (460) and RS (658) spike widths (Fig. 4c). This observation can be taken as evidence that they may be CB interneurons, because intracellular recording in vitro showed that, in rat frontal cortex, CB-containing interneurons have an average spike width at half-amplitude (≈1 msec) intermediate between those of fast-spiking interneurons (0.6 ms) and P cells (1.3 ms) (26). The existence of inverted tuning curves from putative CB cells in the prefrontal cortex of monkeys performing working memory tasks thus provides strong experimental support of a key prediction of our model and the model itself.
Mexican Hat. Is this disinhibition mechanism important for the generation of tuned persistent activity? We investigated this question by focusing on the Mexican-hat-type network connectivity (with local excitation and lateral inhibition) underlying the generation of spatially confined activity patterns (27). Specifically, we calculated the average synaptic currents from different cell types feeding into the P cell population (Fig. 5a). Recurrent collaterals between P cells produce a Gaussian profile of excitatory current (P, Fig. 5a), whereas CB neurons produce a spatial profile of inhibitory current with a minimum at the peak of the bump state (CB, Fig. 5a). Surprisingly, although the PV-to-P axonal projection is widespread, the resulting inhibitory postsynaptic current on the P cell population turns out to be narrowly tuned and maximal at the peak of the bump (PV, Fig. 5a). As a result, the sum of IP and IPV yields an almost flat spatial profile (Fig. 5b). However, when combined with ICB (which shows a dip at the peak activity), the total recurrent synaptic input does show localized excitation and more widespread inhibition, like a Mexican hat (Fig. 5c). Therefore, dendritic disinhibition plays an important role in the generation of bump-activity patterns in our model.
The spatial tuning of the PV-to-P inhibition originates from the tuned driving force of the inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSC). Namely, if the PV-to-P IPSC is IPV = gPV(VP – EGABAA), then unstructured projection means that gPV is the same for all P cells. However, the membrane potential VP of a P cell increases with its firing rate (28). In the example of Fig. 5a, 〈VP〉 = –56 mV at the peak of the bump, whereas 〈VP〉 = –64 mV on the flanks (Fig. 5d). As a result, for a shunting inhibition, VP – EGABAA is about twice as large at the peak of the bump as on the flanks. Interestingly, this driving-force effect is not present in a network model of integrate- and-fire neurons (13). In that model, the average voltage actually decreases with increasing firing rate of a postsynaptic cell, due to the voltage reset after a spike, leading to minimum (rather than maximum) inhibition at the peak of the bump. Disinhibition, through the other (CB and CR) interneuron types, helps to produce stable bump states in a network model of Hodgkin–Huxley-type neurons.
Dendritic Inhibition and Resistance Against Distractors. For a neural network to maintain memory storage, it should not be easily interrupted by behaviorally irrelevant stimuli (distractors). We examined the network's ability to filter out distractor stimuli, a characteristic of robust working memory in PFC (29). We analyzed how resistance against distractors depends on the relative inhibitory contributions from the CB and PV populations to P cells. A distractor stimulus was introduced during the delay period at a location different from the initial cue (13, 30). We fixed its location and determined the intensity threshold above which it is powerful enough to distract the network from the stored initial cue (Fig. 6 a and b). We found that dendritic inhibition mediated by CB cells is more efficient than perisomatic inhibition mediated by PV cells in filtering out distractor stimuli. For the simulations shown in Fig. 6, when dendritic inhibition is insignificant (as in the classical two-population model), a bump state can be activated but is not stable and shows systematic drifts over time (14). In this case, a distractor of a strength comparable to that of the initial cue is able to disrupt the working memory (Fig. 6c). Increasing the relative inhibition from the CB population increases dramatically the threshold above which a distractor can disrupt the memory of the initial cue, therefore the network's ability to filter out distractors is greatly enhanced (Fig. 6c).
CB cells enhance resistance against distractors, because their inputs target the same dendritic sites as the external stimuli (such as a distractor) hence are well positioned to reduce the efficacy of external inputs. Furthermore, away from the bump's peak activity, CB interneurons show higher firing rates during the delay period than in the spontaneous state, therefore they provide stronger dendritic inhibition to those P cells not storing the initial cue. To quantitatively show this effect, we considered a single P cell whose preferred cue is opposite to the initial cue. As shown in Fig. 6 d–f, given the same external input, the cell's response is significantly reduced, with a higher level of dendritic inhibition (as in the delay period) compared to the case with a lower dendritic inhibition (in the resting state). Therefore, our results show that a working memory network endowed with synaptic inhibition is more robust against distractors when compared with a conventional two-population model.
Discussion
Widespread lateral inhibition is a general assumption in recurrent neural models with a Mexican-hat architecture (13, 16, 27, 31, 32). However, whether, in a cortical microcircuit, inhibition is broader than excitation is still a matter of debate (33). Moreover, here we showed that, even if axonal projection from interneurons is broad, the resulting distribution of inhibitory current in P cells can be narrowly tuned due to the driving force effect. Most previous models used the leaky integrate-and-fire neuron, for which this effect is absent. In a Hodgkin–Huxley model of spatial working memory, persistent activity generally shows systematic drifts in time, thus appears to be dynamically unstable (14). In a recent paper (34), bump attractors were reported in a two-population model of Hodgkin–Huxley-type neurons. However, in that study, the average membrane potential of a model neuron is basically independent of the firing rate, unlike real P cells (28). Further analysis will be worthwhile to clarify under what conditions, or whether, bump attractors can be generated with only one inhibitory population in a biologically realistic model. Regardless, we propose that, in addition to broad inhibition, local disinhibition contributes critically to the robust realization of Mexican-hat input distribution, thus stable spatially tuned persistent activity.
We emphasize that the three interneuron types in our model should be more appropriately interpreted according to their synaptic targets rather than calcium-binding protein expressions. For example, PV cells display a variety of axonal arbors, among which the large basket cells (20) are likely candidates for our widely projecting cells. Similarly, CB interneurons show a high degree of heterogeneity, but some of them (such as double-bouquet cells) are known to act locally and preferentially target dendritic spines and shafts of P cells (2, 5). Finally, although many CR interneurons do project to P cells (2), a subset of CR cells appear to avoid P cells (22), at least in the same cortical layer (23), and preferentially target CB interneurons (21). Electrophysiological evidence is presently lacking of the preferred innervations of CR interneurons onto GABAergic cells; progress in this direction would be most desirable.
Because Mexican-hat connectivity is an architecture widely used in recurrent cortical microcircuit models (13, 16, 27, 31, 32), our four-population model is of general relevance. At the same time, there could also be significant differences among cortical areas serving different functions. It would be interesting to compare quantitatively the distributions of interneuron subtypes in association vs. sensory cortices (21, 35–38). In particular, experiments are needed to test the hypothesis that our proposed disinhibition mechanism is especially prominent in those cortical regions capable of persistent activity.
According to the disinhibition mechanism, dendritic inhibition is reduced locally in activated P cells and increased in those P cells not engaged in encoding the shown stimulus. Our results suggest that, in a working memory network, this mechanism mediated by CB interneurons could serve to filter out distracting stimuli, thereby rendering memory storage robust. We showed that this mechanism is enhanced with a larger dendritic/somatic inhibition ratio (Fig. 6), which could be hard-wired or dynamically controlled by neuromodulation. Interestingly, recent work suggests that dopamine D1 receptor activation precisely increases the ratio of dendritic/somatic inhibition onto P cells in the prefrontal cortex (39). Dopamine was found to reduce the efficacy of inhibitory synapses onto the perisomatic domains of a P cell, mediated by fast-spiking interneurons, whereas it enhances inhibition at synapses from accommodating or low-threshold spiking interneurons that target the dendritic domains of a P cell (39). Our model predicts a specific function for such a dual dopamine action, namely it could boost the ability of a working memory network to filter out behaviorally irrelevant distracting stimuli. By the same token, the model also suggests one possible scenario for how impaired dopamine modulation of PFC could lead to working memory deficits and abnormal distractability in schizophrenia (8).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank G. Elston and B. Lambolez for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute of Mental Health (X.J.W. and P.G.-R.); the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation and the Swartz Foundation (X.J.W.); the McDonnell Foundation (C.C.); and the Silvio O. Conte Center Grant (P.G.-R. and C.C.). J.T. thanks the Wennergren Foundation for a fellowship grant, the Royal Academy of Science, Fernstroms Stiftelse, and the Swedish Society for Medical Research for research grants.
This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.
Abbreviations: PFC, prefrontal cortex; PV, parvalbumin; CB, calbindin; CR, calretinin; P, pyramidal; GABA, γ-aminobutyric acid.
References
- 1.Freund, T. F. & Buzsaki, G. (1996) Hippocampus 6, 347–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.DeFelipe, J. (1997) J. Chem. Neuroanat. 14, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kawaguchi, Y. & Kubota, Y. (1997) Cereb. Cortex 7, 476–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cauli, B., Audinat, E., Lambolez, B., Angulo, M. C., Ropert, N., Tsuzuki, K., Hestrin, S. & Rossier, J. (1997) J. Neurosci. 17, 3894–3906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Somogyi, P., Tamas, G., Lujan, R. & Buhl, E. H. (1998) Brain Res. Rev. 26, 113–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gupta, A., Wang, Y. & Markram, H. (2000) Science 287, 273–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fuster, J. M. (1988) The Prefrontal Cortex (Raven, New York), 2nd Ed.
- 8.Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987) in Handbook of Physiology, The Nervous System (Am. Physiol. Soc., Bethesda), pp. 373–417.
- 9.Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995) Neuron 14, 477–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wang, X.-J. (2001) Trends Neurosci. 24, 455–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Conde, F., Lund, J. S., Jacobowitz, D. M., Baimbridge, K. G. & Lewis, D. A. (1994) J. Comp. Neurol. 341, 95–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gabbott, P. L. A. & Bacon, S. J. (1996) J. Comp. Neurol. 364, 609–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S. & Wang, X.-J. (2000) Cereb. Cortex 10, 910–923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tegnér, J., Compte, A. & Wang, X.-J. (2002) Biol. Cybern. 87, 471–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989) J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Camperi, M. & Wang, X.-J. (1998) J. Comp. Neurosci. 5, 383–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Constantinidis, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2002) J. Neurophysiol. 88, 3487–3497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Levitt, B., Lewis, D. A., Yoshioka, T. & Lund, J. (1993) J. Comp. Neurol. 338, 360–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kritzer, M. F. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995) J. Comp. Neurol. 359, 131–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kisvarday, Z. F., Ferecska, A. S., Kovaics, K., Buzas, P., Budd, J. M. L. & Eysel, U. T. (2003) J. Neurocytol. 31, 255–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.DeFelipe, J., Gonzalez-Albo, M. C., Del Rio, M. R. & Elston, G. N. (1999) J. Comp. Neurol. 412, 515–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gulyás, A. I., Hájos, N. & Freund, T. (1996) J. Neurosci. 16, 3397–3411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meskenaite, V. (1997) J. Comp. Neurol. 379, 113–132. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gonchar, Y. & Burkhalter, A. (1999) Cereb. Cortex 9, 683–696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Connors, B. W. & Gutnick, M. J. (1990) Trends Neurosci. 13, 99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kawaguchi, Y. & Kubota, Y. (1993) J. Neurophysiol. 70, 387–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Amari, S. (1977) Biol. Cybern. 27, 77–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Anderson, J. S., Lampl, I., Gillespie, D. C. & Ferster, D. (2002) Science 290, 1968–1972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. (1996) J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brunel, N. & Wang, X.-J. (2001) J. Comput. Neurosci. 11, 63–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ben-Yishai, R., Lev Bar-Or, R. & Sompolinsky, H. (1995) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 3844–3848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Somers, D. C., Nelson, S. B. & Sur, M. (1995) J. Neurosci. 15, 5448–5465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kang, K., Shelley, M. & Sompolinsky, H. (2003) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 2848–2853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Shriki, O., Hansel, D. & Sompolinsky, H. (2003) Neural Comput. 15, 1809–1841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kondo, H., Tanaka, K., Hashikawa, T. & Jones, E. G. (1999) Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 4197–4203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dombrowski, S. M., Hilgetag, C. C. & Barbas, H. (2001) Cereb. Cortex 11, 975–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Elston, G. N. & Gonzalez-Albo, M. C. (2003) Brain Behav. Evol. 62, 19–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lewis, D. A., Melchitzky, D. S. & Gonzalez-Burgos, G. (2003) J. Neurocytol. 31, 265–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gao, W.-J., Wang, Y. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2003) J. Neurosci. 23, 1622–1630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.