Abstract
Aim
A biased attributional style, in which negative events are attributed to external and personal causes, is associated with paranoid delusions in schizophrenia. It is not known whether this biased attributional style also characterizes individuals at clinical risk for psychosis, or if it is associated with their emergent paranoia.
Methods
33 clinical high-risk patients and 15 age- and gender-similar controls were assessed with the Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire for externalizing and personalizing attributional biases and for potential correlates with suspiciousness and other symptoms.
Results
Both patients and controls had a similar external-personalizing attributional style, which was unrelated to symptoms, including suspiciousness.
Conclusions
Consistent with other studies, a biased attributional style was not associated with subthreshold paranoia. Therefore, a biased attributional style is likely not a trait that contributes to emergent paranoid delusions, but instead a state-dependent correlate of paranoid delusions.
Keywords: attribution, high risk, psychosis, schizophrenia, social cognition
Introduction
Attributional style refers to the characteristic ways in which individuals explain the causation of events. Studies have examined attribution to external versus internal causes, showing that healthy people have a self-serving bias, and tend to attribute negative events to external causes and attribute positive events to themselves.1,2 Further, healthy people preferentially attribute their own negative behavior to situational factors, constituting an external-situational attributional style,3 but attribute the negative behavior of others to personal factors, constituting an external-personal attributional style.4 This normal self-serving bias, is attenuated in clinical cohorts with depression and anxiety,5 but is exaggerated in individuals with paranoia, across diagnoses.6,7,8
Attributional bias was initially measured primarily using the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ),9 a 12-item self-report measure in which respondents read about a situation, write a causal statement about the event, and then rate that statement on dimensions of internality, stability, and globality, which refers to whether attributions can apply to a broad or narrow range of situations.10 The Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ) was then developed to further distinguish between attributions of causation to other individuals (external-personal) or to situational factors (external-situational).11 Initial studies with the IPSAQ found an external-personalizing bias in individuals with paranoia, in which causation for negative events is disproportionately attributed to others.12 However, later studies with the IPSAQ found no difference between schizophrenia patients and controls,13,14 or an attenuated self-serving bias comparable to but less severe than that seen in depression.15
The exaggerated self-serving bias may be state-specific in schizophrenia patients, seen only in the context of acute paranoia16,17 with concurrent grandiose delusions.18 As yet, it is unclear if an exaggerated self-serving bias would be observed with suspiciousness, or with subthreshold paranoia, which might suggest it is a risk factor for paranoid delusions. Studies using the IPSAQ in nonclinical cohorts suggest no difference in attributional style among college students with subclinical paranoia,14,19 in adults (ages 36–65) who endorse psychotic-like experiences in population surveys, or in first-degree adult relatives of people with non-affective psychosis.20 However, to the best of our knowledge, attributional style has not yet been evaluated in a clinical risk cohort of youths at appreciable risk for schizophrenia (~30%).21 Herein, we characterize causal attribution in a CHR cohort and evaluate its relationship to concurrent symptoms, specifically subthreshold paranoia.
Method
Participants
This study was conducted at the Center of Prevention and Evaluation (COPE), a psychosis-risk clinical research program at the New York State Psychiatric Institute at Columbia University Medical Center. Patients were help-seeking youths considered at clinical high-risk for non-affective psychosis, generally referred from schools and clinicians, or self-referred from the program website (www.copeclinic.com). Participants met criteria for at least one of three prodromal syndromes, as assessed with the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes: 1) attenuated positive symptoms syndrome; 2) genetic risk and deterioration syndrome; or 3) brief intermittent psychotic syndrome.22 Healthy controls were recruited from the same source community using mailings and internet postings. All participants provided written informed consent, or if under the age of 18, written assent with provision of written informed consent by a parent. Data were collected between April, 2005 and July, 2009. This study was approved by the New York State Psychiatric Institute’s Institutional Review Board.
Measures
All measures, including demographics, symptoms, and attributional style, were administered at baseline upon enrollment into the longitudinal cohort study.
Demographic data including age, sex and ethnicity, were reported by the participants.
Attributional style was measured using the Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire,11 in which the respondent rates positive and negative events as being caused by themselves (internal), other people (external-personal), or the situation (external-situational). Scores for attribution biases were coded according to their guidelines: externalizing bias is coded from −16 to 16, with positive numbers signifying greater external attributions for negative events; personalizing bias is coded from 0 to 1, with scores greater than .5 signifying more external-personal than external-situational attributions for negative events. The IPSAQ scales have adequate reliability (externalizing bias alpha = .7189, personalizing bias alpha = .7609).11
Subthreshold psychotic symptoms and modified global assessment of function were rated using the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes/ Scale of Prodromal Symptoms (SIPS/SOPS) by clinicians certified in its administration by Barbara Walsh at Yale University. Interrater reliability is excellent to near-excellent for individual items.22 Depression and anxiety were evaluated using the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression23 and Hamilton Rating Scale for Anxiety,24 respectively.
Statistics
The effects of demographic variables on attributional style measures were tested in the combined sample of patients and controls using independent samples t-tests and Spearman’s rank order correlation; effect modification was also evaluated. Independent samples t-tests and chi-square tests were used to compare CHR patients and healthy controls for demographics, attribution, and symptoms. Between-group comparisons for attributional style were repeated using analysis of variance with the inclusion of sex as a potential confounder. For the CHR group, Spearman’s rank order correlations were calculated for attribution scores with symptoms and function. Linear regression analyses were also conducted with sex and symptoms as predictors and IPSAQ measures as response variables. Alpha was set at .05 for differences between patients and controls for attribution and symptoms, and for tests of association between attributional style and suspiciousness or subthreshold paranoia. Exploratory analyses were done to examine any differences in attributional style among CHR participants who later made a transition to psychosis.
Results
There were 33 CHR patients and 15 healthy controls, who were comparable in age, ethnicity, and gender (Table 1). In the combined group of patients and controls (N=48), age and ethnicity bore no association to attribution, but females had a greater externalizing bias than males, t(45) = 2.194, p=.03; there was no age by gender interaction effects on attributional style. CHR patients and healthy controls had nearly identical attributional styles, with both externalizing and personalizing biases (Table 1). Inclusion of gender in the model also did not yield any group effect on attribution. As expected, CHR patients differed from healthy controls in symptoms and function (Table 1). Attributional bias also had no association with subthreshold paranoia (i.e. suspiciousness) or other positive symptoms, or with function, depression or anxiety (Table 2), even in models including gender as a covariate. Attribution was comparable among those CHR patients who did (n=7; mean externalizing score was 1.4, SD 3.0, mean personalizing score was .51, SD .34) and did not (n=26; mean externalizing score was 2.2, SD 4.9, mean personalizing score was .67, SD .26) make a later transition to psychosis (externalizing: t(31) = −.378, p=.71; personalizing: t(31) = −1.284, p=.21).
Table 1.
Descriptive statistics and between-group comparisons for CHR patients and controls on demographic, attribution, and symptom measures.
CHR n=33 | Controls n=15 | Statistics | |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 18.7 (3.4) | 19.9 (3.7) | t (46) = −1.032, p=.31 |
Sex (% female) | 18 | 50 | χ2(1, n=47) = 3.510, p=.06 |
Race (% Caucasian) | 48 | 69 | χ2(1, n=46) = .890, p=.35 |
IPSAQ: | |||
Externalizing score | 2.0 (4.7) | 2.1 (4.3) | t (46) = −.073, p=.94 |
Personalizing score | .64 (.28) | .61 (.29) | t (46) = .314, p=.76 |
Scale of Prodromal Symptoms: | |||
Suspiciousness | 2.7 (1.4) | 0.4 (.7) | t (44.3) = 7.631, p<.001 * |
Unusual Thought Content | 3.5 (1.2) | .21 (.43) | t (43.8) = 13.177, p<.001 * |
Grandiosity | 1.7 (1.5) | 0 (0) | t (32.0) = 6.600, p<.001 * |
Perceptual Disturbances | 2.6 (1.5) | .14 (.36) | t (39.4) = 8.626, p<.001 * |
Conceptual | 1.9 (1.3) | .14 (.36) | t (41.4) = 6.918, p<.001 * |
Disorganization | |||
Total positive | 12.4 (4.4) | 0.9 (.9) | t (35.3) = 13.924, p<.001 * |
Total negative | 13.0 (5.7) | 1.8 (2.0) | t (44.0) = 9.991, p<.001 * |
Global Function | 44.5 (7.1) | 77.4 (6.5) | t (45) = −14.9, p<.001 * |
Hamilton: | |||
Depression | 12.1 (6.8) | 1.5 (1.7) | t (33.2) = 7.824, p<.001 * |
Anxiety | 10.9 (6.8) | 1.5 (1.5) | t (32.2) = 7.018, p<.001 * |
All data listed are means, with standard deviations in parentheses. Alpha=.05 for all t-tests and χ2, with degrees of freedom in parentheses.
p<.001
Table 2.
Correlations between measures of attributional bias and measures of symptoms and functioning among the CHR sample.
N | IPSAQ: Externalizing | IPSAQ: Personalizing | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
rho | p | rho | p | ||
Age | 33 | 0.01 | 0.96 | 0.21 | 0.25 |
Sex | 33 | 0.385 | .027 * | 0.34 | 0.06 |
Race | 33 | −0.19 | 0.30 | −0.04 | 0.82 |
Scale of Prodromal Symptoms: | |||||
Suspiciousness | 33 | −0.15 | 0.41 | 0.00 | 0.99 |
Unusual thought content | 33 | 0.14 | 0.45 | −0.22 | 0.21 |
Grandiosity | 33 | 0.14 | 0.43 | −0.12 | 0.52 |
Perceptual disturbances | 33 | −0.17 | 0.34 | −0.33 | 0.06 |
Conceptual disorganization | 33 | 0.09 | 0.62 | 0.20 | 0.27 |
Total positive | 33 | 0.07 | 0.71 | −0.01 | 0.95 |
Total negative | 33 | −0.01 | 0.94 | −0.25 | 0.17 |
GAF-m | 33 | 0.04 | 0.81 | 0.25 | 0.17 |
Hamilton | |||||
Depression | 28 | −0.18 | 0.35 | −0.02 | 0.92 |
Anxiety | 28 | −0.09 | 0.65 | 0.01 | 0.98 |
Spearman’s rank correlations,
p<.05
Discussion
This initial study is the first to examine causal attributional style in a clinical high risk cohort. We found that the attributional style of the clinical high risk patients was comparable to that of the healthy controls, even accounting for gender differences between the groups. The exaggerated self-serving bias associated with paranoia was not evident in this CHR cohort. It is unlikely that a finding of comparability in externalizing is a consequence of the small sample size and potential Type 2 error, as an N of 2,618 would be necessary to detect a statistically significant difference (calculated using the means and standard deviations yielded herein). Also, among CHR patients, there was no association of attribution with suspiciousness or subthreshold paranoia (or any clinical measures), or eventual transition to psychosis. Of note, the cohort is similar to other North American CHR cohorts in terms of age, high prevalence of attenuated positive symptom syndrome, and mean positive and negative symptom scores.25
Most studies of paranoia or schizophrenia have measured attribution with either the ASQ or the IPSAQ, facilitating comparison of results across studies. The IPSAQ was used here due to its better reliability and ability to distinguish between personal and situational attributions.11 The externalizing and personalizing scores in our study are comparable to those found in previous studies (mean externalizing scores: controls=2.6, range 1.9 to 3.1, patients=2.2, range 1.7 to 3; mean personalizing bias: controls=.56, range .40 to .65, patients=.70, range .64 to .75; across 4 studies).11,12,13,19
Although widely used, the IPSAQ and ASQ have been criticized as having low ecological validity and being difficult to understand for some symptomatic patients, leading to a preference among some investigators for qualitative assessments of attribution6,17,26 using the Leeds Attribution Coding System.27 Qualitative studies have provided evidence that attributional biases are state-based17 and vary depending on whether the respondent sees the delusion as the cause or the outcome of the event.26 These aspects of attribution are difficult to elicit using self-report measures that present hypothetical events, such as the IPSAQ and ASQ. Future qualitative research with CHR samples may allow the identification of associations between attributional style and subthreshold symptoms that were not observed in the present study. Future studies can also evaluate the association of attributional style with IQ, which was not measured in this study.
Our data suggest attributional bias is not a promising trait-like candidate risk factor for the development of paranoid delusions. This is consistent with studies in other “risk” cohorts, including first-degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia, and adults and college students who endorse psychotic-like experiences when queried,14,19,20 and with evidence that attributional biases are state-specific in schizophrenia.16,17 However, given that attributional bias is associated with paranoia in clinical cohorts, future studies in larger cohorts should examine the evolution of attributional patterns and paranoid symptoms over time to better understand their relationship.
Acknowledgments
This study was carried out at the Center of Prevention and Evaluation at New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY 10032, USA.
The present study was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health - 5R21MH086125-02 (C.M.C.) and K23MH066279 (C.M.C.), the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD, C.M.C.), the Sackler Foundation (C.M.C.), the Florence T. Irving Award (C.M.C.), and the Lieber Schizophrenia Research Program (C.M.C.).
References
- 1.Miller DT, Ross M. Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: fact or fiction? Pscyhol Bull. 1975;82:213–225. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zuckerman M. Attribution of success and failure revisited, or: the motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. J Pers. 1979;47:245–287. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jones E, Nisbett RE. The actor and the observer: divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning Press; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jones EE, Harris VA. The attribution of attitudes. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1967;3:1–24. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mezulis AH, Abramson LY, Hyde JS, Hankin BL. Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:711–747. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Craig JS, Hatton C, Craig FB, Bentall RP. Persecutory beliefs, attributions and theory of mind: comparison of patients with paranoid delusions, Asperger’s syndrome and healthy controls. Schizophr Res. 2004;69:29–33. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00154-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kinderman P, Kaney S, Morley S, Bentall RP. Paranoia and the defensive attributional style: deluded and depressed patients’ attributions about their own attributions. Br J Med Psychol. 1992;65:371–383. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1992.tb01718.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Candido CL, Romney DM. Attributional style in paranoid vs. depressed patients. Br J Med Psychol. 1990;63:355–363. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1990.tb01630.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Peterson C, Semmel A, von Baeyer C, Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Seligman MEP. The attributional style questionnaire. Cogn Ther Res. 1982;6:287–300. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Abramson LY, Seligman MEP, Teasdale JD. Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reforumlation. J Abnorm Psychol. 1978;87:49–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kinderman P, Bentall RP. A new measure of causal locus: the internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Person Indiv Diff. 1996;20:261–264. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kinderman P, Bentall RP. Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: internal, personal, and situational attributions for negative events. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:341–345. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Martin JA, Penn DL. Attributional style in schizophrenia: an investigation in outpatients with and without persecutory delusions. Schizophr Bull. 2002;28:131–141. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M. Paranoia, persecutory delusions and attributional biases. Psychi Res. 2005;136:233–245. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Moritz S, Woodward TS, Burlon M, Braus DF, Andresen B. Attributional style in schizophrenia: evidence for a decreased sense of self-causation in currently paranoid patients. Cogn Ther Res. 2007;31:371–383. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lincoln TM, Mehl S, Exner C, Lindenmeyer J, Rief W. Attributional style and persecutory delusions. Evidence for an event independent and state specific external-personal attribution bias for social situations. Cogn Ther Res. 2010;34:297–302. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aakre JM, Seghers JP, St-Hilaire A, Docherty N. Attributional style in delusional patients: a comparison of remitted paranoid, remitted nonparanoid, and current paranoid patients with nonpsychiatric controls. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:994–1002. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jolley S, Garety P, Bebbington P, et al. Attributional style in psychosis - the role of affect and belief type. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:1597–1607. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Martin JA, Penn DL. Social cognition and subclinical paranoid ideation. Br J Clin Psychol. 2001;40:261–265. doi: 10.1348/014466501163670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Janssen I, Versmissen D, Campo JA, Myin-Germeys I, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Attribution style and psychosis: evidence for an externalizing bias in patients but not in individuals at high risk. Psychol Med. 2006;36:771–778. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schobel SA, Lewandowski NM, Corcoran CM, et al. Differential targeting of the CA1 subfield of the hippocampal formation by schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:938–946. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al. Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:703–715. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hamilton M. NIMH. Hamilton Anxiety Scale. In: Guy W, editor. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. Rockville, MD: NIMH; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cannon TD, Cadenhead K, Cornblatt B, et al. Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:28–37. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Beese AG, Stratton P. Causal attributions in delusional thinking: an investigation using qualitative methods. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43:267–283. doi: 10.1348/0144665031752961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stratton P, Heard D, Hanks HG, Munton AG, Brewin C, Davidson I. Coding causal beliefs in natural discourse. Br J Soc Psychol. 1986;25:299–313. [Google Scholar]