Abstract
Human area V1 offers an excellent opportunity to study, using functional MRI, a range of properties in a specific cortical visual area, whose borders are defined objectively and convergently by retinotopic criteria. The retinotopy in V1 (also known as primary visual cortex, striate cortex, or Brodmann’s area 17) was defined in each subject by using both stationary and phase-encoded polar coordinate stimuli. Data from V1 and neighboring retinotopic areas were displayed on flattened cortical maps. In additional tests we revealed the paired cortical representations of the monocular “blind spot.” We also activated area V1 preferentially (relative to other extrastriate areas) by presenting radial gratings alternating between 6% and 100% contrast. Finally, we showed evidence for orientation selectivity in V1 by measuring transient functional MRI increases produced at the change in response to gratings of differing orientations. By systematically varying the orientations presented, we were able to measure the bandwidth of the orientation “transients” (45°).
Why Primary Visual Cortex?
The increasing sophistication of modern neuroimaging methods has made it possible to analyze multiple features of the functional architecture in specific cortical areas. Similar studies have been done in experimental animals by using different techniques (e.g., optical recording, deoxyglucose labeling, and single unit mapping). The human neuroimaging studies have greatly clarified the functional organization of presumptive human areas such as MT (1–7), and the retinotopy of multiple cortical visual areas (8–15). Here we combine these retinotopic and functional mapping approaches in primary visual cortex, whose borders can also be defined in the same individuals.
Area V1 is the human visual cortical area with the most well-defined anatomical boundaries, agreed on by virtually all previous studies, both historical (e.g., refs. 16–18) and more recent (e.g., refs. 19–23). In primates, area V1 plays a critical role in visual information processing, because most visual information ultimately reaching the rest of visual cortex is first funneled through V1 (24). This anatomically complicated routing task is presumably why “striate” cortex is so markedly laminated (striated); in cortex, ascending and descending inputs and outputs are anatomically segregated in different layers. In fact, primate striate cortex has been subdivided into 11 identifiable laminar divisions (1, 2, 3, 4A, 4B, 4Ca, 4Cb, 5A, 5B, 6A, and 6B) (25, 26) rather than the customary six layers described in most cortical areas.
This “gatekeeper” role of V1 in the primate cortical hierarchy may also influence the number of neurons in the area—V1 is the largest known visual cortical area, and perhaps the largest cortical area, at least in macaques, where multiple cortical area boundaries are best known (24). In human V1, ocular dominance columns have been demonstrated by using multiple anatomical stains (27–30), and in one report, functional labeling (31). Human V1 was also the focus of several early functional studies, including electrically induced phosphenes (e.g., ref. 32), retinotopic lesion defects (e.g., ref. 21), and positron-emission tomography retinotopy (33). In macaques, V1 is probably the most thoroughly studied area in visual cortex—thus furnishing very detailed predictions about what should be found in human V1.
Therefore human V1 appeared to be an ideal location to test for addition functional features within a well defined, well accepted cortical area, by using functional MRI (fMRI). We did these studies by using fMRI techniques described elsewhere (e.g., ref. 12), except that some of the data here were collected with a 3-T scanner (General Electric/Advanced NMR). This high field scanner yields activity data of somewhat higher signal/noise ratio, but otherwise similar magnetic resonance signals.
Retinotopy
As in multiple extrastriate cortical areas (5, 8–15), striate cortex can be accurately mapped in vivo on the basis of its fMRI retinotopy. In fact, retinotopic maps are one of the most certain criteria for defining a cortical visual area (24). Although it is well known that V1 normally extends over the depth and lips of the calcarine fissure, there is significant variability in the size, location, and shape of V1, and even more variability in the shape of the calcarine fissure (e.g., ref. 23). Thus in V1, as elsewhere in human visual cortex, the area boundaries need to be mapped in each individual subject—they cannot be safely generalized from other subjects or other studies, or even from the contralateral hemisphere of the same subject.
One way to map retinotopic area boundaries is to measure and calculate the field sign. Roughly, the “field sign” indicates whether the geometry of the cortical representation matches the (magnified) map polarity of the actual visual field, or whether it is inverted to that. Field sign analysis is based on paired scans revealing cortical activation to the two polar retinotopic coordinates: (i) eccentricity, and (ii) polar angle (12, 13). Fig. 1 A and B shows the location of V1 and surrounding retinotopic areas, based on field sign analysis, on left and right hemispheres of the cortical surface in its normal configuration, showing the medial bank. The location of V1 and surrounding retinotopic areas is clarified in 1C and 1D, which shows the same data in “inflated” cortical surfaces, viewed from a posterior inferior viewpoint. In this subject, V1 extends further beyond the lips of the calcarine fissure, and onto the lateral surface, compared with most subjects.
In a previous study using the deoxyglucose labeling technique, the internal retinotopy of macaque V1 was revealed by stimulating with stationary, flickering, retinotopically specific check stimuli based on polar geometry (rays and rings) (34, 35). To judge more accurately the degree of similarity between humans and macaques, here we did an analogous experiment in human V1, using fMRI.
The present experiment was redesigned slightly to match two differences of the fMRI technique: (i) poorer spatial resolution, but (ii) an unlimited number of activity maps, from each fMRI subject. Here we stimulated with spatially alternating flickering check stimuli grouped into one of the following: (i) isopolar angle “wedges”, (ii) isoeccentricity “rings”, or (iii) circles of equal polar-angle diameter.
These stimuli, and the results of this stimulation, are shown in Fig. 2. Data from three scans are shown. For each scan the stimuli are shown in S1–2, S3–4, and S5–6, and the corresponding activity is shown in A plus B, C plus D, and E plus F, respectively. The activities produced by the first and second retinotopic stimuli (in each scan) are shown in red and green (respectively) in the activity maps. The activity maps are illustrated from the same hemispheres shown in Fig. 1, but now are rendered on fully flattened portions of the cortical surface.
As one might expect from a roughly polar retinotopy in macaque V1: (i) the isopolar-angle wedges produce roughly equal width, roughly parallel stripes in cortex; (ii) the isoeccentric rings of radially varying width produce stripes of roughly equal width, oriented approximately orthogonal to those in i; and finally (iii) the circles of radially varying stimulus diameter produced circular activity patches, of roughly equal cortical width. The retinotopic patterns do not extend far into extrastriate cortex—they are mostly in V1 and V2, with minor encroachment into V3/VP. Other fMRI evidence (12) suggests that this distribution reflects the progressive decrease in retinotopic resolution from V1 through V2, V3, VP, V3A, V4v, etc.
Mapping the Blind Spot
It is well known that the retinal array is nonuniform. Receptor photopigment, size, density, and rod/cone type all vary widely across the retina. Because of the high degree of retinotopic precision in V1, it should be possible to trace how these retinal inhomogenieties are reflected (or filled in) within V1 and subsequent retinotopic areas.
The blind spot is one of these retinal nonuniformities. In each retina, a sizable gap is formed in the receptor array due to the bundling of optic nerves from a common region exiting the eye. The blind spot is roughly 5° in diameter, centered about 15° medial to the fovea, just slightly above the horizontal meridian (see Fig. 3). During normal viewing, the blind spot is functionally “filled in” by monocular input from the opposing eye. However the blind region is not small, and its effects can be demonstrated if one closes one eye and tests for vision in the appropriate visual field region. In histological tissue from V1 of macaques (35, 36) and humans (29, 30), the blind spot is represented in the input layer 4 as an elliptical, monocular region, about four ocular dominance columns wide. However, it has not been demonstrated functionally in humans by using neuroimaging techniques, to our knowledge.
The representation of the blind spot was straightforward to demonstrate in V1, by treating it as an extra-large ocular dominance column. Subjects were instructed to fixate the center of a field of scaled, black-and-white flickering checks. In this experiment, the checks extended over the entire stimulus field, throughout the experiment. When shown binocularly, this stimulus produced no significant differences in activity. However, when subjects viewed the stimulus monocularly, using either left or right eye in alternating 16-sec epochs, the representation of the monocular “blind” spot was clearly revealed (see yellow–red vs. blue–cyan spots near the middle of the left vs. right hemispheres, respectively, in Fig. 4). The monocular crescent was beyond the limits of the stimulus, so it was not activated in these experiments.
As one would predict from its location in the retina, the blind spot is located just inferior (superior in the visual field) to the cortical representation of the horizontal meridian, centered at the represented eccentricity near 15°. Although the exact size of the blind spot activation depends partly on the activity threshold, it is generally consistent with the human cortical magnification in V1 (9, 10, 13), and with the width of the blind spot representation in human histological material (29, 30). The fMRI time courses extracted from voxels in the blind spot representation (Fig. 4A) are quite similar to the fMRI time courses reported for conventional ocular dominance columns (31).
Contrast Sensitivity
Another major way to distinguish cortical areas is based on differences in global functional properties. Such global differences include the manipulation of visual motion to distinguish area MT from surrounding cortical areas (2, 3, 5, 11), and color to distinguish an area in the fusiform gyrus (“V4,” according to refs. 2 and 3) (37, 38). Here we manipulate luminance contrast to distinguish V1 from other cortical visual areas.
In previous studies, the fMRI contrast response was measured from voxels in three cortical areas (5). In V1, the contrast response varied continuously and monotonically over contrasts greater than ≈6%. (5, 39). However, the contrast response in MT and V3 was quite different, essentially saturated at contrasts higher than ≈6% (5) (see Fig. 5A).
Because the contrast response functions appeared so different in striate vs. extrastriate areas, one obvious question arises: are the contrast response functions in the additional cortical visual areas relatively low in sensitivity, like those in V1? Alternatively, do they saturate quickly with high sensitivity, like those in MT and V3?
The data in Fig. 5A suggest that one could selectively activate V1 (relative to V3/MT, at least) by presenting two carefully chosen, alternating stimulus contrast values (6% and 95%). This presentation should produce near-maximal modulation of fMRI signals in V1, but saturated (unmodulated) fMRI signals in high-gain extrastriate areas such as (at least) V3 and MT. In addition to confirming the earlier time course information from V1 and MT in an activity map format, this experiment was thus designed to give an overall view of the contrast response functions of the many additional extrastriate areas.
Moving square wave gratings (0.7 cycle/°, 6°/sec) of 6% and 95% contrast were presented in 16-sec epochs, alternating throughout the scan (duration: 4 min, 16 sec). To minimize optokinetic nystagmus, the gratings were presented in a radial rather than a one-dimensional configuration, and the direction of motion was reversed every 2 sec to prevent motion aftereffect (6).
The 6–95% contrast modulation activated a large region in and surrounding the calcarine fissure (Fig. 5C). For comparison, retinotopic area boundaries (Fig. 5B) defined previously in this same subject by the field sign calculations (see Fig. 1) have been transposed onto the same hemisphere. The area activated by the retinotopically invariant contrast modulation (Fig. 5C) was confined approximately to V1, as defined by entirely different, retinotopic criteria. In control scans, we presented gratings spanning a lower contrast range (0 vs. 6%). This stimulus alternation did not activate V1, but it did activate MT and V3/V3A (not shown), as predicted from Fig. 5A. This finding strongly suggests that V1 has lower contrast sensitivity than many or all extrastriate cortical areas. It also suggests that contrast sensitivity increases by probability summation in the progressively larger receptive fields of extrastriate cortex, as suggested by some models (40).
Orientation Sensitivity
The typical fMRI experiment tests for a difference in response amplitude during several tens of seconds of stimulation with one stimulus, compared with a subsequent epoch of equal length, using a control stimulus. Unfortunately, some stimulus dimensions do not lend themselves to this approach. For instance, how would one test for the presence or absence of orientation selectivity in a given area? In animal experiments, electrophysiological studies suggest that approximately equal numbers of neurons will respond at essentially equal levels to stimuli of differing orientations (e.g., refs. 41–43). If the same is true in humans, sustained stimulation with gratings of one orientation should produce levels of activation that are equal to (i.e., no different from) that produced by control stimulation with gratings of a different orientation. Thus, tests to identify such dimensions in human visual cortex require a different approach.
One way to do this is to use high-resolution fMRI to directly visualize cortical columns that respond selectively to different orientations or directions (e.g., ref. 31). However, there is no guarantee that all interesting functional distinctions are anatomically segregated into systematic cortical columns, and it is not technically trivial to do high-resolution fMRI.
Here we describe an alternative approach, applied to demonstrating and measuring orientation sensitivity in human V1. It exploits the fact that prolonged neural responses to a given orientation will adapt slightly over time, so that useful information can be gained by looking at the postadaptation fMRI response. This approach was distilled from an earlier study in which differential fMRI responses were obtained after adaptation to different stimulus directions in human area MT (6).
In the present experiments, subjects fixated the center of a black/white grating during fMRI scanning. Every 0.4 sec, each stripe within the grating was varied randomly in width (range = 0.1–2°) and in position (“phase,” if it had been a square-wave grating). These frequent changes in the grating configuration were designed to prevent retinal aftereffects or cortical habituation.
In the first experiment (Fig. 6 A and B), grating orientation changed 90° every 40 sec, from one oblique orientation to the other. Both grating orientations were thus 45° to vertical/horizontal, but maximally different from each other in terms of orientation.
Fig. 6A shows the averaged time course from V1. Fig. 6B shows the averaged “change-of-orientation transient,” from the data in Fig. 6A. About 7 sec after the actual change in stimulus orientation, the averaged MR signal shows a transient increase. The 7-sec delay is consistent with the known hemodynamic delay in fMRI experiments. The 90° changes in grating orientation produce a transient positive response in fMRI signals.
In subsequent experiments we demonstrated that smaller angular changes in orientation produce correspondingly smaller fMRI “transients.” By systematically varying the orientation (Fig. 6C), we were able to measure the bandwidth of this fMRI orientation selectivity (≈45°, half-width at half-height; Fig. 6D) in V1. This bandwidth is essentially equal to that found in a previous study measuring the human orientation bandwidth by using visual evoked potentials (44), but it is arguably wider than that predicted by single unit reports in cats and monkeys.
In preliminary tests using the flattened cortical format and stimuli that were specific for both retinotopy and orientation, we also found that such signals appear to be retinotopically specific, and preferentially located in areas V1 and V2, and to a lesser extent V3 and VP (not shown). This is similar to the topography of orientation selectivity in macaque visual cortex, as measured by single units (e.g., ref. 45).
Conclusions
This study benefited greatly from the wealth of previous data in area V1 of humans and other mammalian species. In addition to telling us where V1 is usually located, prior studies from V1 in macaques and humans had already suggested a high degree of retinotopic precision and a polar organization of the V1 retinotopy ((e.g., refs. 9, 10, 13, 34, 35, 46, 47), monocular dominance in the blind spot representation (29, 30, 35, 36), orientation selectivity (e.g., refs. 41–43), and even the averaged contrast gain functions (5, 48). In this sense the present study has value as a “calibration” or “confirmation” study.
However, some other aspects of the present study are more novel. The tests for orientation selectivity based on transient fMRI signals are an approach that could easily be generalized to tests for similarly coded stimulus dimensions, such as visual motion, color, etc. The tests for the blind spot representation could likewise be generalized to trace where and when other retinal inhomogenieties (e.g., rod/cone ratio changes with eccentricity, etc.) are “filled in” in cortex. For instance, it is interesting that the monocular blind spot representation does not appear in V2 (Fig. 4), although a binocular stimulus of almost equal dimensions is represented in V2 (Fig. 2). Because we are largely unconscious of these retinal variations, this issue also bears on the question of which cortical visual areas, and what functional aspects of visual processing, have access to conscious perception (e.g., ref. 49). Finally, the maps of contrast sensitivity (Fig. 5) suggest that contrast gain may be better accounted for by considering models of probability summation within progressively larger receptive fields (e.g., ref. 40), rather than in terms of predominant input from either magnocellular or parvocellular “streams” (e.g., refs. 5 and 48).
Acknowledgments
We thank Mary Foley, Terrance Campbell, William Kennedy, Bruce Rosen, and Thomas Brady for invaluable assistance during the course of this project. We are especially grateful to Dr. Stephan Brandt for helping to acquire some of the data. This work was supported by grants from the Human Frontiers Science Program and the National Eye Institute to R.B.H.T., the Swiss Fonds National de la Recherche Scientifique to N.K.H., and the McDonnell–Pew Foundation to J.D.M.
ABBREVIATIONS
- MR
magnetic resonance
- MRI
MR imaging
- fMRI
functional MRI
References
- 1.Lueck C J, Zeki S, Friston K J, Dieber M-P, Cope P, Cunningham V J, Lammertsma A A, Kennard C, Frackowiak R S J. Nature (London) 1989;340:386–389. doi: 10.1038/340386a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zeki S, Watson J D G, Lueck C J, Friston K J, Kennard C, Frackowiak R S J. J Neurosci. 1991;11:641–649. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-03-00641.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Watson J D G, Myers R, Frackowiak R S J, Hajnal J V, Woods R P, Mazziota J C, Shipp S, Zeki S. Cereb Cortex. 1993;3:79–94. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.2.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dupont P, Orban G A, DeBruyn B, Verbruggen A, Mortelmans L. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1420–1424. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.3.1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tootell R B H, Reppas J B, Kwong K K, Malach R, Born R T, Brady T J, Rosen B R, Belliveau J W. J Neurosci. 1995;15:3215–3230. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-03215.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tootell R B H, Reppas J B, Dale A M, Look R B, Malach R, Jiang H-J, Brady T J, Rosen B R. Nature (London) 1995;375:139–141. doi: 10.1038/375139a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McCarthy G, Spicer M, Adrignolo A, Luby M, Gore J, Allison T. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995;2:234–243. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schneider W, Noll D C, Cohen J D. Nature (London) 1993;365:150–153. doi: 10.1038/365150a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Engel S A, Rumelhart D E, Wandell B A, Lee A T, Glover G H E, Chichilnisky J, Shadlen M N. Nature (London) 1994;370:106. doi: 10.1038/369525a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Engel S A, Glover G H, Wandell B A. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:181–192. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tootell R B H, Dale A M, Sereno M I, Malach R. Trends Neurosci. 1966;19:481–489. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10053-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tootell R B H, Mendola J D, Hadjikhani N K, Ledden P J, Liu A K, Reppas J B, Sereno M I, Dale A M. J Neurosci. 1997;17:7060–7078. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07060.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sereno M I, Dale A M, Reppas J B, Kwong K K, Belliveau J W, Brady T J, Rosen B R, Tootell R B H. Science. 1995;268:889–893. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.DeYoe E A, Carman G J, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:2382–2386. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.DeYoe E A, Bandettini P, Neitz J, Miller D, Winans P. J Neurosci Methods. 1994;54:171–187. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(94)90191-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gennari F. Francisci Gennari Parmensis Medicinae Doctoris Collegiati de Peculiari Structura Cerebri Nonnullisque Eius Morbis—Paucae Aliae Anatom. Observat. Accedunt. Parma, Italy: Regio Typographeo; 1782. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vic-D’Azyr F. Traité d’Anatomie et de Physiologie; Vol 1, Anatomie et Physiologie du Cerveau; Vol 2, Planches Anatomiques. Paris: Didot; 1786–89. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baillarger J-P. Mem Acad Med Paris. 1840;8:148. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Brodmann K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig, Germany: Barth; 1909. . Second printing 1925. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Von Economo C, Koskinas G N. Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde des erwachsenen Menschen. Vienna: Springer; 1925. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Holmes G, Lister W T. Brain. 1916;39:34. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Polyak S. J Comp Neurol. 1933;57:541. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Stensaas S S, Eddington D K, Dobelle W H. J Neurosurg. 1974;40:747–751. doi: 10.3171/jns.1974.40.6.0747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Felleman D J, Van Essen D C. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lund J S. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1988;11:253–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hendry S H, Huntsman M M, Vinuela A, Mohler H, de Blas A L, Jones E G. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2383–2401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-02383.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Haseltine E C, DeBruyn E J, Casagrande V A. Brain Res. 1979;176:153–158. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90878-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hitchcock P F, Hickey T L. Brain Res. 1980;182:176–179. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90841-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Horton J C, Hedley-Whyte E T. Phil Trans R Soc London B. 1984;304:255–272. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1984.0022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Horton J C, Dagi L R, McCrane E P, DeMonasterio F M. Arch Ophthalmol. 1990;108:1025–1031. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1990.01070090127054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Menon R, Ogawa S, Strupp J P, Ugurbil K. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:2780–2787. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Penfield W, Jaspers H, McNaughton F. Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain. Brown, Boston: Little; 1954. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fox P T, Miezin F M, Allman J M, Van Essen D C, Raichle M E. J Neurosci. 1987;7:913–922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-03-00913.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tootell R B H, Silverman M, Switkes E, DeValois R L. Science. 1982;218:902–904. doi: 10.1126/science.7134981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tootell R B H, Silverman M S, Hamilton S L, Switkes E. J Neurosci. 1988;8:1531–1568. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-05-01531.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Horton J C. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 1984;304:199–253. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1984.0021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sakai K, Watanabe E, Onodera Y, Uchida I, Kato H, Yamamoto E, Koizumi H, Miyashita Y. Proc R Soc London B. 1995;261:89–98. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kleinschmidt A, Lee B B, Requardt M, Frahm J. Exp Brain Res. 1996;110:279–288. doi: 10.1007/BF00228558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Boynton G M, Engel S A, Glover G H, Heeger D J. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4207–4221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04207.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Watson A B. Vis Neurosci. 1992;8:65–76. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800006507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hubel D H, Wiesel T N. J Comp Neurol. 1974;158:267–294. doi: 10.1002/cne.901580304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bonhoffer T, Grinvald A. J Neurosci. 1993;13:4157–4180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-10-04157.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hubel D H, Wiesel T N. J Physiol (London) 1962;160:106–154. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jakobsson P. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenhagen) 1985;63:183–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-3768.1985.tb01532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zeki S. J Physiol (London) 1978;277:273–290. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Daniel P M, Whitteridge D. J Physiol (London) 1961;159:203–221. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hubel D H, Wiesel T N. J Comp Neurol. 1974;158:295–306. doi: 10.1002/cne.901580305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sclar G, Maunsell J H R, Lennie P. Vision Res. 1990;30:1–10. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Crick F, Koch C. Nature (London) 1995;375:121–123. doi: 10.1038/375121a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]