Abstract
Emotional maturity and social awareness are important for adolescents, particularly college students beginning to face the challenges and risks of the adult world. However, there has been relatively little research into personality maturation and psychological development during late adolescence and the neural changes underlying this development. We investigated the correlation between psychological properties (neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety, and depression) and age among late adolescents (n = 25, from 18 years and 1 month to 22 years and 8 months). The results revealed that late adolescents became less neurotic, less anxious, less depressive and more extraverted as they aged. Participants then observed video clips depicting hand movements with and without a risk of harm (risk-taking or safe actions) during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The results revealed that risk-taking actions elicited significantly stronger activation in the bilateral inferior parietal lobule, temporal visual regions (superior/middle temporal areas), and parieto-occipital visual areas (cuneus, middle occipital gyri, precuneus). We found positive correlations of age and extraversion with neural activation in the insula, middle temporal gyrus, lingual gyrus, and precuneus. We also found a negative correlation of age and anxiety with activation in the angular gyrus, precentral gyrus, and red nucleus/substantia nigra. Moreover, we found that insula activation mediated the relationship between age and extraversion. Overall, our results indicate that late adolescents become less anxious and more extraverted with age, a process involving functional neural changes in brain networks related to social cognition and emotional processing. The possible neural mechanisms of psychological and social maturation during late adolescence are discussed.
Introduction
Late adolescence is a unique and important period for human development. Erikson (1994) examined the concept of identity in relation to late adolescence [1], [2]. Although Erikson considered identity formation to be a life-long process, he emphasized late adolescence as a key stage in his developmental theory, constituting a particular critical development period when a sense of personal and social identity becomes integrated through ‘identity crisis’. Newman and Newman (2007, 2009) redefined Erikson’s criteria regarding developmental stages, dividing adolescence into early and late adolescence, such that late adolescence (18–24) is distinguished from adolescence (12–18) [3], [4]. Erikson theorized that identity develops when young people are given a psycho-social ‘moratorium’, referring to an opportunity in which they can experiment with different social roles before making permanent commitments to an occupation, to intimate relationships, to social groups and communities, and to a philosophy of life. This ‘moratorium’ period [5] closely corresponds to ‘college age’; attending college provides students with consciousness-raising experiences to learn about themselves and others through exposure to diverse perspectives, opinions, and ways of living [6], [7], [8]. Examining psychological changes in college-age late adolescents is valuable in elucidating the ongoing process of human identity integration or maturity [9], [10], [11].
Understanding the late adolescence period is also of particular importance because dynamic psychological changes, such as human identity integration or maturity, continue throughout this period, as social and affective instabilities are overcome. Contrary to a long-held assumption that the brain is largely mature by the end of childhood, recent neuroimaging studies have provided increasing evidence that adolescence involves profound brain growth and change [12], [13]. For example, increases in white matter volume have been reported throughout childhood and adolescence, particularly in the prefrontal and parietal cortices (e.g., [14], [15], [16], [17], [18], [19], [20]. In addition, grey matter volume has been reported to increase in the prefrontal and parietal cortices during the preadolescent stage, followed by a steady decline during late adolescence [14], [21]. These findings indicate that brain development in adolescence is not linear, and that the brain volume of a typical early adolescent is measurably different to that of a typical late adolescent. In addition, these findings suggest that brain regions involved in planning, decision-making, cognitive reasoning, or controlling impulses and emotions undergo refinement through adolescence at least into a person’s twenties (i.e., late adolescence).
In addition to the morphometric studies discussed above, behavioral techniques have been used to examine brain development during late and post adolescence. For example, a behavioral study using a mentalizing task requiring theory of mind and executive function reported that social abilities like ‘theory of mind’ continue to improve from adolescence to adulthood [22], further suggesting that developmental changes continue throughout the late adolescent phase. Another study using a gambling task reported that the rate of risky choices did not significantly change between early (12–15 y.o.) and mid (15–18 y.o.) adolescence, but was significantly reduced in adulthood (25–35 y.o.) [23]. These findings indicate that a profound ‘shift’ in cognitive or emotional regulation ability during late adolescence may occur in the transition from adolescence to adulthood. Functional neuroimaging studies of mental-state attribution have reported decreases in frontal cortex activity between adolescence and adulthood [12], providing further evidence that a developmental shift occurs during late adolescence, i.e., from adolescence to adulthood. A similar discrepancy between adolescence and adulthood has been observed in the neural correlates of emotional processing. For example, in processing fearful facial expressions, adolescents were found to exhibit a strong reliance on the emotional network in the brain, while adults tended to rely more on an attentional network [24]. In addition, adults, compared with adolescents, exhibited decreased activity in the hippocampus during the encoding of negative images [25]. In accord with the studies discussed earlier, this evidence indicates that late adolescence is important as a transitional period from adolescence to adulthood, involving the maturation of emotional regulation and cognitive processing in social situations.
Several studies have reported the usefulness of a five-factor model that describes five distinct personality traits for parsing personality constructs in late adolescents [26], [27], [28]. Of the five factors, neuroticism and extraversion are of particular interest, as they are believed to be crucial for the development of healthy social interactions and to exert an over-arching influence on affect and mood. Extraversion is characterized by an increased tendency to be optimistic, and to experience positive emotions and enhanced sociability. Conversely, neuroticism is defined as an increased tendency to worry and to experience psychological distress, accompanied by negative affect and over-sensitivity to negative cues. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that task-evoked brain activity varies with neuroticism and extraversion scores in the prefrontal cortex and cingulate cortex [29], [30], [31], [32], [33]. Thus, these two personality traits are strongly associated with emotional experience and may modulate emotion-evoked brain activity [29].
Development of personality traits occurs not only in adulthood, but also in childhood and adolescence [34]. Neuroticism and extraversion are consistently included in personality models, including 3-factor and 5-factor models [35], [36], [37]. In addition, the two dimensions seem to be most related to age, educational level, and positive/negative life events from late adolescence to young adulthood [38], [39], [40]. A longitudinal study of college students reported that an increase in positive life events with age was associated with extraversion, while an increase in negative events was associated with neuroticism [40]. Another longitudinal study showed that extraversion in high school students predicted their experience of more positive life events over 4 years later in their college- or work-life, while their neuroticism predicted the experience of more negative life events [39]. These studies highlight the importance of understanding extraversion and neuroticism during late adolescence.
Moreover, a meta-analysis of 92 longitudinal studies revealed that the largest changes in personality traits occurred between ages 18 and 30, and, specifically, that late adolescents typically become more socially dominant (a facet of extraversion) and less neurotic [41]. The study indicates that late adolescence is a critical period for the development of personality traits, showing that extraversion and neuroticism are influential for late adolescents in adapting to society as they mature.
Recently, extraversion and neuroticism were also shown to impact on structural features of the prefrontal cortex in adult and elderly populations [42], [43], suggesting that extraversion and neuroticism are related to structural brain development in the earlier stages of life. Low extraversion and high neuroticism are also associated with depression, anxiety [44], and phobia [45], which are all related to psychological and psychiatric problems frequently occurring in late adolescence [45], [46]. Neuroticism also predisposes individuals to develop chronic functional pain/pain disorders [47] and mood disorders [48], [49], which are also common problems among late adolescents [50], [51]. The way in which adolescents become extraverted and less neurotic with regard to the challenging external environment during the late adolescent period is a developmental issue that has not been adequately addressed. Moreover, the neural correlates of this process are currently unclear.
In a review study examining social and emotional development during late adolescence, depressive and anxiety symptoms were found to be predictive of changes in psychosocial functioning [52]. For example, rejection sensitivity (the tendency to anxiously expect, readily perceive, and intensely react to rejection) appears to be particularly salient in late adolescence as anxiety or angry expectation [53], and was linked to a relative increase in adolescent depressive and anxiety symptoms [54]. Similarly, social anxiety was predictive of physical/psychological ‘dating aggression’ among late adolescents [55]. Moreover, healthy adolescents between 12 and 21 years old, who engaged in more extracurricular activities (i.e., participation in organized sports teams, clubs, etc.) and experienced higher quality family relationships, presented with significantly less depressive symptoms [56]. Since late adolescents face increasingly complex social situations, symptoms of depression and anxiety may be particularly damaging for the development of social competence.
In the current study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure neural responses elicited by the observation of actions associated with a certain risk. Moreover, we investigated the influence of developmental differences of psychological properties among late adolescents. We administered psychological questionnaires to measure anxiety, depression, neuroticism, and extraversion, all of which play a crucial developmental role in the establishment of identity during late adolescence. During fMRI scanning, participants viewed hand movements associated with a risk of harm (risk-taking actions) or no risk of harm (safe actions). This task was designed to represent common situations involving potential risks in an everyday environment, providing an index of how late adolescents are likely to cope with potential risks in their social lives in the future. Late adolescence is a challenging period characterized by pervasive social role changes across many domains [57], [58]. Salient tasks of late adolescence include goals relating to friendship, academic success, and social conduct, giving way to occupational and romantic goals as late adolescents move into young adulthood [9]. The large number of changes faced in late adolescence make it an unstable time, but also reflects the explorations that take place during the late adolescent years. Many of the changes made by late adolescents are for the purpose of some new period of exploration, in love, work, or education. In accord with this notion, it is possible that a late adolescent’s level of tolerance of risk-taking actions may become entrenched as they are frequently confronted with challenging social situations during late adolescence. Here, we postulated that action observation of risk-taking would be a developmental indicator of motivation to overcome a broad range of difficulties in the world. Although teenagers are generally regarded as engaging in more ‘risky behavior’, such as binge drinking, cigarette smoking, having casual sex partners, violence and other criminal behavior etc [59], [60], [61], it should be noted that in this study we do not use the term ‘risk-taking’ to refer to a tendency to such a ‘risky behavior’. Rather, we use ‘risk-taking’ to refer to a more positive concept, whereby adolescents confront and manage the difficulties facing them.
Although this is an exploratory analysis, we hypothesized that late adolescents will become less anxious, less neurotic, less depressive, and more extraverted as they age, measured by the correlation between age and questionnaire scores, and that neural responses to the observation of risk-taking actions will be modified as adolescents become tolerant of risks in the external environment. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the developmental aspects of their tolerance to the external environment (measured as the correlation between questionnaire scores and age) will be mediated by changes of neural circuitry. These changes may involve the limbic or paralimbic systems (e.g., insula) or brainstem, which are central to the processing of affective information. In addition, the changes may also affect the prefrontal areas, which are important for emotional regulation. Activity in the anterior insula has been found to be associated with empathic maturity during the observation of emotional expressions among children [62]. This finding supports the notion that the insula is relevant to social functioning in everyday life.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-five participants in their late adolescence took part in this experiment (12 females and 13 males, from 18 years and 1 month to 22 years and 8 months, M ± SD years: 20.60±1.09). All participants were undergraduate students and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants were right-handed (lateralization quotient for the right side of more than 90%) as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [62]. Written informed consent was given before participation in the study, which was specifically approved by the Institutional Ethical Review Board of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Japan. All participants were screened to rule out head trauma, the use of medication, history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and other serious medical conditions.
Image Acquisition
Images were acquired using a 1.5 T Magnetom Vision plus MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). We acquired a unique high-resolution structural image (T1-weigthed anatomical images; 3D MP-RAGE sequence, repetition time; TR = 11.4 ms, echo time; TE = 4.4 ms, flip angle = 15°, 256×256 matrix, slice thickness 1.25 mm) with 144 sagittal slices after the functional runs. Each functional run involved the acquisition functional echo-planar imaging (EPI) volumes (gradient-echo, TR = 3,000 ms, TE = 40 ms, field of view; FOV = 192 mm, flip angle = 90°, 64×64 matrix, slice thickness 3.5 mm), each with 36 interleaved slices approximately parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line. Stimuli were displayed on a screen positioned at the rear of the scanner, which the participant could comfortably see through a mirror mounted on the standard head coil.
Psychological Measures
Prior to the fMRI session, participants completed the Maudsley Personality Inventory (MPI) [63], [64], Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [65], [66], and the Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS) [67], [68]. The MPI consists of 80 items, each assessing a constellation of traits, also providing a measure of personality along the neuroticism and extraversion scales. The STAI-trait is a self-report instrument of the longstanding quality of trait anxiety. The STAI-trait consists of 20 items, and high total scores indicate more trait anxiety. The SDS was developed as a self-administered measure of depression severity, with higher scores indicating more severe depression. The 20 items of the scale address each of the four most commonly found characteristics of depression: pervasive effects, physiological equivalence, other disturbances, and psychomotor activities.
Action Observation Stimuli and Procedure
The action observation experiment involved two types of video clips, ‘risk-taking’ (video of a person’s hand performing an action with clear potential for causing harm to oneself) and ‘safe’ (showing a person’s hand performing an action with no clear danger) (Figure 1). The stimuli are described in detail in Table 1. It should be noted that the video clips were not created to convey the meaning of problematic behaviors among adolescents, such as using drugs or alcohol, driving drunk, smoking, unprotected sex, or other offensive or criminal activities. Rather, the video clips presented participants with situations involving common risks that most people are exposed to in everyday life. Each functional run began and ended with the presentation of a white fixation dot for 9 s. Between these two fixation periods, video clips from the two conditions were presented in alternating 21 s blocks. Each block consisted of a 21 s video clip, and each baseline consisted of a 9 s fixation period. Each block contained videos depicting three different risk-taking actions, or three different safe actions. Half of the blocks showed people performing actions from the right of the screen, and half from the left. Participants completed 10 blocks of each condition during a single scan. The order of presentation of the stimuli was determined according to an optimized random sequence for each block. The brightness of the screen, the intensity of contrast (luminance contrast and texture contrast), the velocity of hand actions, and the representation of objects were equalized for all task/control video clips. The total duration of the risk-taking video clip equaled the duration of the safe video clip (the length of each video clip was 7.0 s). Three functional runs, lasting 10 minutes each, were collected for each participant. The hand movements of participants were monitored by direct visual inspection and video-monitoring from the back of the fMRI tunnel. No visible movements were noted during the presentation of the experimental stimuli.
Table 1. Experimental conditions and Visual Analog Scale (VAS) score (100-0) for each condition.
Action description | Risk-taking-Safety | Anxiety-Comfort |
Risk-taking actions | ||
Grasping and placing the blade of the box-cutter knife | 80.2±17.9 | 83.4±10.3 |
Grasping and placing the clipper blade of the scissors | 80.3±9.0 | 76.5±12.0 |
Grasping and placing the brimming cup | 39.2±18.7 | 31.0±25.1 |
Safe actions | ||
Grasping and placing the handle of the box-cutter knife | 36.7±25.1 | 36.6±24.5 |
Grasping and placing the scissors | 35.0±21.0 | 36.1±20.6 |
Grasping and placing the pots | 21.7±21.4 | 20.8±18.5 |
Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation.
Behavioral Measures
After the scanning procedure, each participant was shown the video clips again, and asked to answer specific questions related to them. To test subjective evaluations of the degree of risk, participants were asked to rate their subjective experience of the video clips using a visual analog scale (VAS) (1) from feelings of risk to safety (i.e. to what extent did you feel alarmed while watching the video clip?) and (2) from feelings of anxiety to comfort (i.e. how anxious did you feel while watching the video clip?).
fMRI Data Analysis
Image processing was carried out using statistical parametric mapping software (SPM8, the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). The functional time series was motion corrected, slice timing corrected and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum. The corresponding high-resolution structural image (the T1 image as the source image) was registered to the first EPI image as the reference image. The co-registered structural image was then transformed into standard anatomical space using the Montreal Neurological Institute structural template (MNI 152). These parameters were used to normalize all functional images. Following preprocessing, ‘Risk-taking’ and ‘Safe’ condition, and motion parameters (six realignment parameters) were entered as regressors. A high-pass filter (hpf) of 128 sec was also applied as regressors. The risk-taking and safe blocks were convolved with a hemodynamic response function without derivatives, and modeled as 21 s boxcar regressors. The 9 s fixation periods were modeled as implicit baseline blocks. Next, a first fixed level of analysis was computed subject-wise using the general linear model. The following T-contrasts were estimated: risk-taking, safe, risk-taking vs. safe, and safe vs. risk-taking.
Neural Response to Observation of Risk-taking versus Safe Actions
To test our hypothesis that activation in areas related to action observation would be significantly enhanced when actions involved risk-taking, we compared activation in each condition using linear contrasts (risk-taking versus safe and safe versus risk-taking). The resulting set of voxel values for each contrast constituted a statistical parametric map of the t statistic SPM(t). Anatomical localization was performed in MNI coordinates. Talairach coordinates (Talairach Daemon, www.talairach.org/daemon.html) were used for anatomical localization to be compared with Brodmann areas [69]. Significant activations were defined using a lenient height-threshold of p<0.001, uncorrected, and an extent threshold of k = 10 (voxels), to reduce the risk of false negatives. Our use of cluster size thresholding combined with uncorrected p values was intended to adequately control for the prevalence of false positives [70]. This threshold suffices to eliminate speculation that effects observed in the primary parametric analysis are an artifact due to non-specific reductions in BOLD signal.
Neural Activity Associated with Age and Psychological Measures
This analysis aimed to reveal the brain regions in which activity mediates the age-related change of psychological properties that are essential in individual maturity during late adolescence. In a second-level random-effect analysis, participant’s imaging data were regressed with psychological scores (neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety and depression) and age with a multiple regression analysis. The correlation map of neural responses to risk-taking action (the main SPM(t) contrast of risk-taking vs. safe actions) with age and the correlation map of the same neural response with each of the psychological scores were calculated separately. A conjunction analysis was then performed to show overlapping areas of the two correlational maps: an age-related activation map and a personality-related map.
Parameter estimates were extracted from the regions surviving the conjunction analyses that tested for statistical mediation using the INDIRECT macro for SPSS (http://www.afhayes.com/) [71], [72], [73], [74]. According to Baron and Kenny (1986), four steps are required to establish that neural activity in a particular region mediates the relation between age and psychological properties: (1) showing that age is associated with psychological properties; (2) showing that age is associated with neural activity in the region; (3) showing that neural activity in the region predicts psychological properties when controlling for age; and (4) showing that the relation between age and psychological properties is reduced when controlling for neural activity in the region. For a sample of 20–80 participants, statisticians recommend the use of bootstrapping methods for testing the statistical significance of mediation (rather than the Sobel test, which is appropriate for larger samples; [71], [72]. The current study used the bootstrapping approach outlined by Shrout and Bolger (2002), which provides a mean estimate of the indirect effect (i.e., the path through the mediator) and the associated 95% confidence interval. A confidence interval that does not contain zero indicates statistically significant mediation (p<0.05). Cook’s distance metric was used to test whether data from a few individuals unduly influenced the strength of the bivariate relationships. A value (age, psychological property, and neural activity) greater than 1 for a data point represents a statistical outlier [75]. No point had a Cook’s distance greater than 0.5, indicating that none of the correlations were dependent on statistical outliers.
Results
Behavioral Measures
Table 2 shows descriptive features of psychological measurements (neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety-trait, and depression scores) and the correlation coefficients between these scores and age. The scores of neuroticism, anxiety, and depression were negatively correlated with age (r (23) = −0.49, −0.56, and −0.54, respectively), and extraversion scores were positively correlated with age (r (23) = 0.44), suggesting that the late adolescents became less neurotic, less anxious, less depressive, and more extraverted with age. These scores were used for the multiple regression analysis of neural responses, regressed with age and psychological variables.
Table 2. Psychological assessment scores and correlation coefficients with age for each score.
Psychologicalmeasurement | Score | Correlationcoefficient with age |
Maudsley Personality Inventory (MPI) | ||
Neuroticism | 22.7±11.1 | −0.49* |
Extraversion | 33. 6±10.3 | 0.44* |
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-trait) | ||
Anxiety-trait | 44.9±10.6 | −0.56** |
Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS) | ||
Depression | 37.7±9.1 | −0.54** |
Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation.
Spearman’s ρ: *p<0.05; **p<0.01.
Table 3 shows the scores of subjective ratings using the visual analog scale for each video clip. To test the efficiency of the categorization of the stimuli in terms of risks, the scores of 1) participants’ subjective levels of risk-taking and 2) the extent to which they felt anxious when observing the stimuli were compared between the two within-participant categories (risk-taking and safe actions in video clips). The results indicated that participants experienced significantly stronger feelings of risk-taking and anxiety during the observation of risk-taking compared with safe actions (F (1, 24) = 151.16, p<0.001, and F (1, 24) = 93.73, p<0.001, respectively, using repeated-measures ANOVA). To validate the video clip task used in this study, we calculated the correlation coefficients of the subjective ratings of the feeling (“risky” and “anxious”) induced by the actions in the video clips with psychological assessments (correlation coefficients r are provided in the Table 3). The VAS scores of feeling “risky” about Risk-taking actions were positively correlated with anxiety (r (23) = 0.41), and negatively correlated with extraversion (r (23) = −0.42). VAS scores for feeling “anxiety” about Risk-taking actions were positively correlated with neuroticism and anxiety (r (23) = 0.44 and 0.44, respectively), and negatively correlated with extraversion (r (23) = −0.40). In contrast, VAS ratings for safe actions were not correlated with any psychological assessment. These results indicate that the video task in this study (the observation of risk-taking action) provides a suitable measure of the psychological factors of interest in this study, and can be used to examine important developmental processes in the late adolescence period.
Table 3. Correlation coefficients between psychological assessments and subjective ratings (“risky” and “anxiety”) about the actions in video clips.
Neuroticism | Extraversion | Anxiety | Depression | |
Risk-taking actions | ||||
“risky” | 0.32 | −0.42* | 0.41* | 0.35 |
“anxiety” | 0.44* | −0.40* | 0.44* | 0.36 |
Safe actions | ||||
“risky” | 0.03 | −0.27 | 0.19 | 0.30 |
“anxiety” | 0.10 | −0.09 | 0.14 | 0.35 |
Bold type*: p<0.05.
Neural Response to Risk-taking versus Safe Actions
We compared the neural activation elicited by observing risk-taking versus safe actions across the whole brain (Figure 2, Table S1). The results revealed that risk-taking actions elicited significantly stronger activation, mainly in the bilateral middle frontal gyrus (BA9/10), superior frontal gyrus/frontal pole (BA8/10), supramarginal gyrus (BA39/40), inferior parietal lobule (BA40), superior temporal gyri (BA22/39), middle occipital gyri (BA18/19), and cuneus (including the calcarine sulcus) (BA17/18/19) compared with safe actions. Additional areas of significant activation were also found in the left middle temporal gyrus (BA21/22), medial frontal gyri (supplementary motor area) (BA6), superior parietal gyrus/lobule (BA7), precentral gyrus (BA6), posterior cingulate (BA23), fusiform gyrus (BA37), lingual gyrus (BA17), insula (BA13), and declive. In the right hemisphere, we observed significant activation in the precuneus (BA7) and postcentral gyrus (BA2). No regions exhibited greater activation while viewing safe actions compared with risk-taking actions associated with risk.
Neural Responses Mediating Relationship between Age and Psychological Properties
Finally, we performed a conjunction analysis to reveal common brain activity that correlated both with age and psychological scores (neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety, and depression) (Table 4). A positive correlation between age and neural response to observation of risk-taking action [r age(+)(23), p<.001] and a positive correlation with extraversion [r e(+)(23), p<.001] were found to overlap in the insula [BA13, r age(+) = 0.70, r e(+) = 0.76], middle temporal gyrus [BA22, r age(+) = 0.68, r e(+) = 0.70, ], and precuneus [BA19, r age(+) = 0.67, r e(+) = 0.67]. A positive correlation with age [r age(+)(23), p<.001] and a negative correlation with anxiety [r anx(-)(23), p<.001] were found to overlap in the angular gyrus/supramarginal gyrus [BA39, r age(+) = 0.68, r anx(-) = −0.79, ], precentral gyrus [BA6, r age(+) = 0.67, r anx(-) = −0.73, ], and red nucleus/substantia nigra [r age(+) = 0.66, r anx(-) = −0.79]. Parameter estimates were extracted from the regions surviving the conjunction analysis. The parameter estimates were used in a series of analyses testing for statistical mediation.
Table 4. Brain regions mediating association between psychological measurement and age.
MNI | Cluster | |||||
Anatomical region | BA | x | y | z | T | k |
Neuroticism-Age | ||||||
no mediation | ||||||
Extraversion-Age | ||||||
Positive correlation with age | ||||||
Insula | 13 | −40 | −10 | 20 | 4.55 | 32 |
Middle temporal gyrus | 22 | −58 | −34 | −2 | 4.45 | 23 |
Precuneus | 19 | 36 | −74 | 36 | 4.27 | 19 |
Negative correlation with age | ||||||
no mediation | ||||||
Anxiety-Age | ||||||
Positive correlation with age | ||||||
no mediation | ||||||
Negative correlation with age | ||||||
Angular gyrus/Supramarginal gyrus | 40 | −62 | 34 | 4.39 | 23 | |
Precentral gyrus | 6 | 16 | −20 | 64 | 4.23 | 18 |
Red nucleus, Substantia nigra | 8 | −22 | −8 | 4.14 | 16 | |
Depression-Age | ||||||
no mediation |
Statistical threshold: p<0.001 uncorrected, k = 10.
MNI refers to Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates; BA refers to putative Brodmann Area; L and R refer to left and right hemispheres.
As shown in Figure 3, insula activation (BA13) mediated the relation between age and extraversion. This insula activation was positively associated with age and extraversion (age: β = 0.44; extraversion controlling for age: β = 0.41). The relationship between age and extraversion (β = 0.28) was reduced when controlling for activity in the insula (β = 0.22). Bootstrapping revealed that the insula significantly mediated the relation between age and extraversion (mean indirect effect = 5.35, 95% confidence interval ranging from 0.37 to 11.78). The results indicate that, as late adolescents’ age, their neural responses to the observation of risk-taking actions increase in the insula and middle temporal, lingual, and precuneus areas. These changes were related to developmental changes of the participants’ psychological properties, such as increased extraversion. In particular, the insula significantly mediated the relation between age and extraversion. Also, age-related increases of neural activation in the angular gyrus, precentral gyrus, and red nucleus (and substantia nigra) were found to contribute to decreasing anxiety with age.
Discussion
The primary question motivating the present study was whether late adolescents become more extraverted, less neurotic, and less anxious as they age, and whether such changes might reflect increased tolerance to the challenges of the adult world. In addition, we also sought to test how brain function reflects developmental changes occurring in late adolescent psychology.
The results revealed several major findings. First, we observed significant correlations between age and scores on psychological parameters that have been hypothesized to play central roles in the development of the late adolescent mind: younger people tended to become more extraverted and less neurotic, less anxious and less depressive with age during late adolescence, even within the small age range in our sample. Levels of neuroticism have been previously reported to decline with age until around age 80 [76]. However, extroversion has also been found to decline with age [77]. Reports of the correlation of age with depression and anxiety are, however, inconsistent. While some studies have reported no age-related difference on the SDS and STAI among students aged 18–28 [78], another study reported correlations of anxiety and depression with age in a population with a mean age of 33.0 years [79]. To date, there has been no study reporting detailed changes of these psychological properties within a small age range in late adolescence. The robust association of age with neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety, and depression in our study indicates that the late adolescent mind undergoes dramatic changes within a short period of time. These changes appear to be unique and distinct from psychological changes that occur across the longer lifespan. Future studies with larger samples will be necessary for elucidating the nature of these dramatic changes in late adolescence.
Analysis of the “risk vs. safe” contrast revealed neural activation in areas broadly associated with action recognition. These results indicate that observing risk-taking compared with safe actions in our task elicited neural activation in; 1) occipital visual areas including the calcarine sulcus and fusiform gyrus, which are related to lower-level processing of visual information and sending output information to the action recognition network; 2) the bilateral superior and inferior parietal regions, which have been implicated in action recognition and representation [80]; 3) the posterior middle temporal gyrus, which is located in the middle of the ventral pathway and serves as a central node in the association of actions and meanings [81]; 4) the supramarginal gyrus or inferior parietal area, widely considered to be homologous to the monkey parietal mirror neuron system, which is critical for encoding and recognition of gestures such as object–related postures and movements [81]; 5) the posterior cingulate area associated with human awareness, self-reflection [82], and memory retrieval [83], [84] etc. and 6) the frontal pole (superior frontal gyrus), which has been implicated in retrospective monitoring of observed actions that affect one’s future actions [85], [86]. Therefore the results suggest that the risk content of the observed action in the video clips enhanced the broad spectrum of visually-guided action recognition processing; the network of visual input and its processing appear to encode the meaning of the observed action and even the reflective or retrospective monitoring of the action’s outcomes. As a result, the risk-related content of the action enhanced a broad network associated with action recognition. This finding indicates that risk-taking situations may increase cognitive load in the entire action recognition system, commanding more attention to the actions shown in the video clips. One possible explanation for the result is that risky actions are relatively unusual and involve novelty, which may cause more brain activation. In the present study, however, it is unlikely that factors related to novelty exerted a substantial effect on brain activation, because the situations depicted in the video clips were not unusual. Rather, the videos depicted common situations that often occur in everyday life. In addition, the degree of novelty of the experimental stimuli was controlled using control video clips. Although we did not observe strong activation in affect-related brain areas, another explanation is that the present results reflect some affective impact on brain activation related to risk-taking context. For example, activation in the mirror neuron system while observing action can be enhanced by motivational and affective aspects of the observed action, (e.g., [87]). Thus the action recognition system may be modulated by the affective context of the risk-taking actions of observed actions.
Alternatively, processing risky actions may require more cognitive resources, involving the estimation and monitoring of possible outcomes of observed actions (see [86]. This notion is in accord with our current finding that the frontopolar region was more engaged while observing risk-taking compared with safe actions. In addition, a previous study reported that the superior part of the frontal polar area exhibited stronger activation when participants thought about the future compared with when they thought about the past [85]. Moreover, human lesion studies have indicated that the frontal polar area may be involved in generating insights into one’s future [88], [89], [90]. On the basis of the current findings, taken together with previous evidence, we hypothesize that risk-taking actions require more cognitive resources to process, involving the estimation of action outcomes resulting in stronger activation in the frontopolar cortex.
The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) was also activated by risk-taking vs. safe actions. This region may be another center for risk-related brain activity, which has been suggested by previous animal studies. For example, it has been reported that PCC activation in monkeys is sensitive to risk in decision making tasks [91]. In addition, the PCC is reported to exhibit activation when monkeys make risky choices, and to become more active with greater perceived risk [92]. These reports are consistent with the present finding that the observation of risk-taking actions (compared to safe ones) activated the PCC. The PCC is reciprocally connected to parietal areas (action recognition network) and receives feedback input from the prefrontal cortex (see the review in [93]), which is involved in estimating the possible outcomes of action. Activation in this area may have exhibited risk-related sensitivity, together with the other regions listed above.
The main finding in the current study was that neural activation during the observation of risk-taking (compared with safe) actions was correlated with age and some psychological measures, especially, extraversion and anxiety. These results indicate that these psychological properties have important developmental components during late adolescence, and that these developmental changes are represented by brain activation related to the observation of risk-taking actions in the insula and the parietal-temporal-occipital association area (middle temporal gyrus, lingual gyrus, and precuneus). Furthermore, we found that the insula significantly ‘mediated’ the relationship between age and extraversion.
The insula is a multifunctional cortical region involved in emotional processing [94], [95], [96], [97], [98], [99], speech-motor function [100], [101], [102], [103], [104], [105], aversive experience [106], both physical (i.e. visceral and somatic pain) and emotional (i.e. affect and mood) experience [94], [96], [107], conscious awareness (interoceptive awareness) [108], and emotional awareness [109]. The insula also plays a critical role in the processing of risk-taking during decision-making [110], suggesting that insula activity may also be modified by the risk-taking context of the observed action in our study.
The present finding of a correlation between insula activity and extraversion is consistent with the results of a previous positron emission tomography (PET) study [30] showing that the blood flow of the insula cortex was correlated with extraversion. Introversion (low extraversion) is associated with anxiety through increased limbic activation (in the insular cortex and amygdala), and is affected by genetic factors [111]. A morphometric study also revealed that extraversion correlated positively with gray matter volume of the insula [112]. Importantly, a previous fMRI study [113] revealed that extraversion correlates with neural responses to positive word stimuli in the bilateral insula. It is possible that, although the insula response to observing risk-taking actions may reflect an elevated alertness to the risk of harm in the environment, the participants did not experience substantial negative affect, as would be the case if they suffered from severe neuroticism or anxiety/depression. Rather, participants may have been receptive to the new challenges evoked by the task, which could result in the relationship between insula activity and a personality trait relating to a more positive aspect of affect, i.e., extraversion. Although insula activity reflects the processing of arousal or novelty [114], [115], this may be accompanied by positive affect to some extent.
While insula activity is related to extraversion, the insula has also been found to exhibit developmental changes [116], [117]. A neuroimaging study revealed increased activation in the insula with age in response to a risk-taking task [116]. Studies of individuals with clinical or developmental disorders consistently show insular morphometric changes, such as gyrification [117] and reduction of the insular volume in Williams syndrome [117], which further suggests developmental changes in the insula. The current finding of an age-related increase in insula activation may also support the notion that the level of affective (particularly positively-valenced) engagement in risk-taking action increases with age in late adolescence. Overall, the current results, which show a link between age and insula activity as well as a link between insula activity and extraversion, suggest that insula activity may ‘mediate’ the development of extraversion.
Our results also revealed that activity in the posteromedial parietal cortex (including the precuneus) correlated positively with extraversion. Gamma et al., (2000) report a similar positive correlation between extraversion and rCBF in the precuneus, consistent with the current findings [118]. Extraversion is related to the active seeking of social or interpersonal engagement, and the precuneus is also related to processing social information and estimating interpersonal relationships. For example, the precuneus is activated during ‘forgivability’ judgments in social scenarios [119] and in the attribution of emotions to the self and others [120]. Moreover, a number of studies have identified that the precuneus is modulated by agency and intentions in action/movement recognition [121], [122], and moral judgments [123]. The precuneus, in parallel with its social functions, shows age-related changes of brain activity during theory of mind tasks [124], [125]. The correlation between extraversion and activity in the precuneus, with its age-related changes, suggests that increased precuneus activation may mediate the process of late adolescents becoming increasingly extraverted and exhibiting improved social functioning with age.
In the current study, as late adolescents aged, activity in the angular gyrus and precentral gyrus increased, and was negatively correlated with anxiety. In a previous fMRI study [126], participants were told that an electrodermal stimulation could occur at any time (“threat”) or that no stimulation would occurs (“safe”). The results revealed stronger activity in the “safe” condition than in the “threat” condition in the angular gyrus and precentral gyrus. Thus, these regions may reflect enhanced perceptions of safety, consistent with the current finding of a negative correlation between the activity in these two regions and the level of anxiety. Simmons et al. [127] also reported reduced activity in the posterior superior temporal cortex adjacent to the angular gyrus in anxiety-prone participants compared with control participants during the observation of aversive images. Moreover, the more repetitively the emotional facial pictures were presented the stronger the neural activity was in the angular/posterior superior temporal gyrus and precentral gyrus, such that the activation in these areas correlated with participants becoming habituated to the emotional stimuli and becoming less anxious [128]. The precentral gyrus has dense connections with the angular gyrus [129], and the angular gyrus and precentral gyrus have been reported to exhibit coactivation during a range of cognitive tasks, including lexical [130], [131] and calculation tasks [132]. These findings suggest the existence of a network involving these two areas. Moreover, both the angular gyrus and precentral gyrus are included in the default-mode network (DMN), a prominent large-scale brain network that exhibits strong activity during the resting state and deactivation during cognitively demanding tasks [133], [134]. We propose that maturation of a network including the angular gyrus and precentral gyrus may play an important role in stabilizing affective states during late adolescence.
Several limitations of the current study should be considered. First, the sample size was relatively small for studying personality-related factors. This may have reduced the statistical power of our analysis, potentially influencing the results. Additional studies with larger sample sizes and more detailed longitudinal behavioral and cognitive testing focusing on the developmental aspects of personality are required to verify these novel findings. Second, our mediation analysis design, consisting of three variables, may have omitted many other variables that influence both insula response and extraversion in the same direction, potentially resulting in a positive bias in the results. For example, it has been reported that empathic ability is correlated with insula activity [135]. In addition, some evidence suggests that empathic individuals are more likely to be extraverted [136]. Moreover, an independent variable can have multiple mediators, which would have been omitted in this design. These potential confounds are likely to affect mediators and dependent variables in the same way. Future studies should take into account other potential mediators. Third, the conjunction analysis shown in Supplementary Table 1 may contain false positives, meaning that the results should be corrected for multiple comparisons (e.g. Bonferroni correction). Applying the Bonferroni correction would increase the likelihood of false negatives, however. Since the current study is an exploratory examination, it may be appropriate to consider the statistical significances of conjunction analyses as preliminary values at this point. Additional studies using the Bonferroni correction will be needed to more rigorously test our hypothesis. Fourth, we found a negative correlation of age and anxiety with activation in the red nucleus/substantia nigra, but no significant effect of the main contrast (risk-taking vs. safe; in Supplementary Table 1). The substantia nigra contains dopamine-containing neurons [137], [138]. Moreover, the sequence planning and timing-related motor functions in the substantia nigra indicate dopaminergic gating of motor sequences [139], [140]. Future studies will be required to investigate this issue in more detail. Fifth, as shown in Table 3, multiple correlation analyses were computed between psychological measurements. This may have led to significant effects due to chance in each correlation analysis, such as correlation coefficients between the subjective ratings of the two kinds of video and the four psychological measurements. Adopting a more conservative corrected alpha level, however, would increase the likelihood of false negative results. In the current study, we adopted a thresholding method (p<0.001, uncorrected, with 10 contiguous voxels) that was initially proposed more than a decade ago [70] and has been used in many fMRI studies. A number of alternative methods of correction for multiple comparisons currently exist (e.g., [141], [142]), and the thresholding method in our study is not the only one available. It is important to note that the present correlational analysis constitutes an exploratory finding. Future studies will be required to test whether the current data can be replicated.
Overall, our findings indicate that late adolescents become less neurotic, less anxious, less depressive, and more extraverted as they age. These changes are associated with activity in brain regions related to social cognition and emotional processing.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to acknowledge the editorial assistance of Benjamin Knight of Edanz Editing in the preparation of this manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This research was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B), KAKENHI (21770269) (http://www.jsps.go.jp/english/e-grants/index.html). This study was supported in part by a Research Grant for a Health Science Grant from the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare of Japan, and the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Erikson E. Identity and the life cycle. New York: Norton. 1994.
- 2.Erikson E. Identity: youth and crisis. New York: Norton. 1994.
- 3.Newman BM, Newman PR. Theories of human development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 2007.
- 4.Newman BM, Newman PR. Development through life: a psychosocial approach. Belmont, California: Wadsworth. 2009.
- 5.Okonogi K. Human Moratorium Period [Japanese Edition]. Tokyo: New Company. 1981.
- 6.Azmitia M, Syed M, Radmacher K. Azmitia M, Syed M, Radmacher K, editors. On the intersection of personal and social identities: Introduction and evidence from a longitudinal study of emerging adults. 2008. pp. 1–16. The Intersections of Personal and Social Identities: New Derections for Child and Adolescent Development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 7.French SE, Seidman E, Allen L. Racial/ethnic identity, congruence with social context, and the transition to high school. J Adolesc Res. 2000;15:587–602. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hurtado A. Voicing Chicana Feminisms: Young Women Speak Out on Sexuality and Identity. New York: New York University Press. 2003.
- 9.Arnett JJ. Emerging Adulthood: The Winding Road from the Late Teens through the Twenties. New York: Oxford University Press. 2004.
- 10.Eccles J, Tmpleton J, Barber B, Stone M. Bornstein MH, Davidson L, Keyes CLM, Moore KA, editors. Adolescence and emerging adulthood: The critical passage ways to adulthood. 2003. pp. 383–406. Well-Being: Positive Development Across the Life Course. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- 11.Shaver P, Furman W, Buhrmester D. Duck S, Perlman D, editors. Transition to college: Network changes, social skills, and loneliness. 1985. pp. 193–219. Understanding Personal Relationships: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE publications.
- 12.Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Blakemore SJ. The developing social brain: implications for education. Neuron. 2010;65:744–747. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:551–560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.4.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Paus T, Zijdenbos A, Worsley K, Collins DL, Blumenthal J, et al. Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science. 1999;283:1908–1911. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5409.1908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, et al. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol. 1994;51:874–887. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1994.00540210046012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Reiss AL, Abrams MT, Singer HS, Ross JL, Denckla MB. Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. Brain 119 (Pt. 1996;5):1763–1774. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, et al. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Batth R, Jernigan TL, et al. Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage. 1999;9:587–597. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dumontheil I, Apperly IA, Blakemore SJ. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Dev Sci. 2010;13:331–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Burnett S, Bault N, Coricelli G, Blakemore SJ. Adolescents’ heightened risk-seeking in a probabilistic gambling task. Cogn Dev. 2010;25:183–196. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Monk CS, McClure EB, Nelson EE, Zarahn E, Bilder RM, et al. Adolescent immaturity in attention-related brain engagement to emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage. 2003;20:420–428. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00355-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Addis DR, Leclerc CM, Muscatell KA, Kensinger EA. There are age-related changes in neural connectivity during the encoding of positive, but not negative, information. Cortex. 2010;46:425–433. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Costa PT, McCrae RR. The revised NEO personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) professional manual. Odessa, FL: PAR Inc. 1992.
- 27.Eysenk HJ. Biological dimensions of personality. 1990. Pervin LA, editor. New York: Guilford Press.
- 28.John OP, Srivastava S. The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. 1999. Pervin LA, John OP, editors. New York: Guilford Press.
- 29.Canli T, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Kang E, Gross J, et al. An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:33–42. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Johnson DL, Wiebe JS, Gold SM, Andreasen NC, Hichwa RD, et al. Cerebral blood flow and personality: a positron emission tomography study. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:252–257. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.2.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kim SH, Hwang JH, Park HS, Kim SE. Resting brain metabolic correlates of neuroticism and extraversion in young men. Neuroreport. 2008;19:883–886. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328300080f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Haas BW, Omura K, Constable RT, Canli T. Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121:249–256. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Branje SJT, van Lieshout CFM, Gerris JRM. Big Five Personality Development in Adolescence and Adulthood. Eur J Pers. 2007;21:45–62. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Matthews G, Gilliland K. The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: a comparative review. Pers Individ Dif. 1999;26:583–626. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bienvenu OJ, Hettema JM, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Low extraversion and high neuroticism as indices of genetic and environmental risk for social phobia, agoraphobia, and animal phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1714–1721. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Eysenk HJ. The biological basis of personality. Springville IL: Charles C Thomas Pub Ltd. 1967.
- 38.Poropat AE. The Eysenckian personality factors and their correlations with academic performance. Br J Educ Psychol. 2011;81:41–58. doi: 10.1348/000709910X497671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ludtke O, Roberts BW, Trautwein U, Nagy G. A random walk down university avenue: life paths, life events, and personality trait change at the transition to university life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;101:620–637. doi: 10.1037/a0023743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vaidya JG, Gray EK, Haig J, Watson D. On the temporal stability of personality: evidence for differential stability and the role of life experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83:1469–1484. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132:1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wright CI, Feczko E, Dickerson B, Williams D. Neuroanatomical correlates of personality in the elderly. Neuroimage. 2007;35:263–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wright CI, Williams D, Feczko E, Barrett LF, Dickerson BC, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1809–1819. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jylha P, Isometsa E. The relationship of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23:281–289. doi: 10.1002/da.20167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Beesdo K, Bittner A, Pine DS, Stein MB, Hofler M, et al. Incidence of social anxiety disorder and the consistent risk for secondary depression in the first three decades of life. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:903–912. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stein MB, Fuetsch M, Muller N, Hofler M, Lieb R, et al. Social anxiety disorder and the risk of depression: a prospective community study of adolescents and young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:251–256. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tanum L, Malt UF. Personality and physical symptoms in nonpsychiatric patients with functional gastrointestinal disorder. J Psychosom Res. 2001;50:139–146. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(00)00219-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bienvenu OJ, Nestadt G, Samuels JF, Costa PT, Howard WT, et al. Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189:154–161. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200103000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Goodwin RD, Gotlib IH. Gender differences in depression: the role of personality factors. Psychiatry Res. 2004;126:135–142. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lieb R, Meinlschmidt G, Araya R. Epidemiology of the association between somatoform disorders and anxiety and depressive disorders: an update. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:860–863. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b0103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lieb R, Pfister H, Mastaler M, Wittchen HU. Somatoform syndromes and disorders in a representative population sample of adolescents and young adults: prevalence, comorbidity and impairments. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2000;101:194–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Keltner D, Kring AM. Emotion, Social Function, and Psychopathology. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2:320–342. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Harper MS, Dickson JW, Welsh DP. Self-Silencing and Rejection Sensitivity in Adolescent Romantic Relationships. J Youth Adolesc. 2006;35:459–467. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Marston EG, Hare A, Allen JP. Rejection Sensitivity in Late Adolescence: Social and Emotional Sequelae. J Res Adolesc. 2010;20:959–982. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00675.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hanby MS, Fales J, Nangle DW, Serwik AK, Hedrich UJ. Social Anxiety as a Predictor of Dating Aggression. J Interpers Violence. 2012. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 56.Mason MJ, Schmidt C, Abraham A, Walker L, Tercyak K. Adolescents’ social environment and depression: social networks, extracurricular activity, and family relationship influences. J Clin Psychol Med Settings. 2009;16:346–354. doi: 10.1007/s10880-009-9169-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am Psychol. 2000;55:469–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Schulenberg JE, Bryant AL, O’Malley PM. Taking hold of some kind of life: how developmental tasks relate to trajectories of well-being during the transition to adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16:1119–1140. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Steinberg L. A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Adolescent Risk-Taking. Dev Rev. 2008;28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Blum RW, Nelson-Mmari K. The health of young people in a global context. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35:402–418. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Williams PG, Holmbeck GN, Greenley RN. Adolescent health psychology. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70:828–842. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Eysenk HJ. Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory. London: University of London Press. 1959.
- 64.Iwawaki S, Oyama T, Kikuchi M, Sugiyana Y, Teraoka T, et al. Maudsley Personality Inventory (in Japanese). Tokyo: Seishinshobo. 1969.
- 65.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. 1970.
- 66.Mizuguchi T, Shimonaka Y, Nakazato K. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form X) (in Japanese). Kyoto: Sankyobo. 1982.
- 67.Zung WW, Richards CB, Short MJ. Self-rating depression scale in an outpatient clinic. Further validation of the SDS. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1965;13:508–515. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01730060026004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Fukuda K, Kobayashi S. SDS Self-rating Depression Scale (in Japanese). Kyoto: Sankyobo. 1983.
- 69.Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar Stereoaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York: George Thieme Verlag. 1988.
- 70.Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, et al. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med. 1995;33:636–647. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Tabachnik SE, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 2006.
- 76.Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Brant LJ, Costa PT., Jr Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.McCrae RR, Costa PT, Jr, Pedroso de Lima M, Simoes A, Ostendorf F, et al. Age differences in personality across the adult life span: parallels in five cultures. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:466–477. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.2.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Tanaka E, Sakamoto S, Kijima N, Kitamura T. Different personalities between depression and anxiety. J Clin Psychol. 1998;54:1043–1051. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4679(199812)54:8<1043::aid-jclp4>3.0.co;2-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.De Moor MH, Beem AL, Stubbe JH, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJ. Regular exercise, anxiety, depression and personality: a population-based study. Prev Med. 2006;42:273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Kilner JM. More than one pathway to action understanding. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:352–357. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Johnson MK, Raye CL, Mitchell KJ, Touryan SR, Greene EJ, et al. Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1:56–64. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR. Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2:435–443. doi: 10.1093/cercor/2.6.435-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Remembering familiar people: the posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroscience. 2001;104:667–676. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00108-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Okuda J, Fujii T, Ohtake H, Tsukiura T, Tanji K, et al. Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1369–1380. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00179-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Tsujimoto S, Genovesio A, Wise SP. Frontal pole cortex: encoding ends at the end of the endbrain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Cheng Y, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. Motivation modulates the activity of the human mirror-neuron system. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1979–1986. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Burgess PW, Veitch E, de Lacy Costello A, Shallice T. The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:848–863. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123 (Pt. 2000;11):2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:215–225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Watson KK. Evolution, risk, and neural representation. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1128:8–12. doi: 10.1196/annals.1399.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.McCoy AN, Platt ML. Risk-sensitive neurons in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1220–1227. doi: 10.1038/nn1523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Pearson JM, Heilbronner SR, Barack DL, Hayden BY, Platt ML. Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, et al. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:500–505. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Mayer EA, Naliboff BD, Craig AD. Neuroimaging of the brain-gut axis: from basic understanding to treatment of functional GI disorders. Gastroenterology. 2006;131:1925–1942. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2006.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Rauch SL, Savage CR, Alpert NM, Miguel EC, Baer L, et al. A positron emission tomographic study of simple phobic symptom provocation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:20–28. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950130020003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:5497–5502. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0935845100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Phillips ML, Williams LM, Heining M, Herba CM, Russell T, et al. Differential neural responses to overt and covert presentations of facial expressions of fear and disgust. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1484–1496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Braun AR, Varga M, Stager S, Schulz G, Selbie S, et al. Altered patterns of cerebral activity during speech and language production in developmental stuttering. An H2(15)O positron emission tomography study. Brain 120 (Pt. 1997;5):761–784. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.5.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Corfield DR, Murphy K, Josephs O, Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, et al. Cortical and subcortical control of tongue movement in humans: a functional neuroimaging study using fMRI. J Appl Physiol. 1999;86:1468–1477. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1999.86.5.1468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Dronkers NF. A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature. 1996;384:159–161. doi: 10.1038/384159a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Price CJ. The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging. J Anat 197 Pt. 2000;3:335–359. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19730335.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Fox PT, Huang A, Parsons LM, Xiong JH, Zamarippa F, et al. Location-probability profiles for the mouth region of human primary motor-sensory cortex: model and validation. Neuroimage. 2001;13:196–209. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Brown S, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Laird AR, Fox PT. Stuttered and fluent speech production: an ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:105–117. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Zald DH, Pardo JV. The neural correlates of aversive auditory stimulation. Neuroimage. 2002;16:746–753. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Paulus MP, Rogalsky C, Simmons A, Feinstein JS, Stein MB. Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1439–1448. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00251-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Smoller JW, Paulus MP, Fagerness JA, Purcell S, Yamaki LH, et al. Influence of RGS2 on anxiety-related temperament, personality, and brain function. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:298–308. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Sollberger M, Stanley CM, Wilson SM, Gyurak A, Beckman V, et al. Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2812–2827. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Omura K, Aron A, Canli T. Variance maps as a novel tool for localizing regions of interest in imaging studies of individual differences. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2005;5:252–261. doi: 10.3758/cabn.5.2.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:615–620. doi: 10.1152/jn.00636.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Moriguchi Y, Negreira A, Weierich M, Dautoff R, Dickerson BC, et al. Differential hemodynamic response in affective circuitry with aging: an FMRI study of novelty, valence, and arousal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:1027–1041. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Lee TM, Leung AW, Fox PT, Gao JH, Chan CC. Age-related differences in neural activities during risk taking as revealed by functional MRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:7–15. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Cohen JD, Mock JR, Nichols T, Zadina J, Corey DM, et al. Morphometry of human insular cortex and insular volume reduction in Williams syndrome. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44:81–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Gamma A, Buck A, Berthold T, Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX. 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) modulates cortical and limbic brain activity as measured by [H(2)(15)O]-PET in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:388–395. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00130-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Farrow TF, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JF, et al. Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2433–2438. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Ochsner KN, Knierim K, Ludlow DH, Hanelin J, Ramachandran T, et al. Reflecting upon feelings: an fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:1746–1772. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Farrer C, Franck N, Georgieff N, Frith CD, Decety J, et al. Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage. 2003;18:324–333. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00041-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.den Ouden HE, Frith U, Frith C, Blakemore SJ. Thinking about intentions. Neuroimage. 2005;28:787–796. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Berthoz S, Grezes J, Armony JL, Passingham RE, Dolan RJ. Affective response to one’s own moral violations. Neuroimage. 2006;31:945–950. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E. Children’s and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1522–1532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Pfeifer JH, Lieberman MD, Dapretto M. “I know you are but what am I?!”: neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1323–1337. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Butler T, Pan H, Tuescher O, Engelien A, Goldstein M, et al. Human fear-related motor neurocircuitry. Neuroscience. 2007;150:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Simmons A, Strigo I, Matthews SC, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Anticipation of aversive visual stimuli is associated with increased insula activation in anxiety-prone subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:402–409. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Feinstein JS, Goldin PR, Stein MB, Brown GG, Paulus MP. Habituation of attentional networks during emotion processing. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1255–1258. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200207190-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Amin H, Rykhlevskaia E, Nguyen DA, et al. Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2636–2646. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Sakurai Y, Asami M, Mannen T. Alexia and agraphia with lesions of the angular and supramarginal gyri: evidence for the disruption of sequential processing. J Neurol Sci. 2010;288:25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Buchweitz A, Mason RA, Tomitch LMB, Just MA. Brain activation for reading and listening comprehension: an fMRI study of modality effects and individual differences in language comprehension. Psychology & Neuroscience. 2009;2:111–123. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Davis N, Cannistraci CJ, Rogers BP, Gatenby JC, Fuchs LS, et al. The neural correlates of calculation ability in children: an fMRI study. Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;27:1187–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2009.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, et al. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:648–663. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. J Adolesc. 2006;29:589–611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Damier P, Hirsch EC, Agid Y, Graybiel AM. The substantia nigra of the human brain. I. Nigrosomes and the nigral matrix, a compartmental organization based on calbindin D(28K) immunohistochemistry. Brain 122 (Pt. 1999;8):1421–1436. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.8.1421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Damier P, Hirsch EC, Agid Y, Graybiel AM. The substantia nigra of the human brain. II. Patterns of loss of dopamine-containing neurons in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 122 (Pt. 1999;8):1437–1448. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.8.1437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Boecker H, Jankowski J, Ditter P, Scheef L. A role of the basal ganglia and midbrain nuclei for initiation of motor sequences. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1356–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Watkins KE, Smith SM, Davis S, Howell P. Structural and functional abnormalities of the motor system in developmental stuttering. Brain. 2008;131:50–59. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Chumbley JR, Friston KJ. False discovery rate revisited: FDR and topological inference using Gaussian random fields. Neuroimage. 2009;44:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Chumbley J, Worsley K, Flandin G, Friston K. Topological FDR for neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 2010;49:3057–3064. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.