Skip to main content
Pediatrics logoLink to Pediatrics
. 2011 Aug;128(2):344–355. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1036

Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors for Autism: A Comprehensive Meta-analysis

Hannah Gardener a,, Donna Spiegelman a,b, Stephen L Buka c
PMCID: PMC3387855  PMID: 21746727

Abstract

BACKGROUND:

The etiology of autism is unknown, although perinatal and neonatal exposures have been the focus of epidemiologic research for over 40 years.

OBJECTIVE:

To provide the first review and meta-analysis of the association between perinatal and neonatal factors and autism risk.

METHODS:

PubMed, Embase, and PsycInfo databases were searched for studies that examined the association between perinatal and neonatal factors and autism through March 2007. Forty studies were eligible for the meta-analysis. For each exposure, a summary effect estimate was calculated using a random-effects model. Heterogeneity in effect estimates across studies was examined, and, if found, a meta-regression was conducted to identify measured methodological factors that could explain between-study variability.

RESULTS:

Over 60 perinatal and neonatal factors were examined. Factors associated with autism risk in the meta-analysis were abnormal presentation, umbilical-cord complications, fetal distress, birth injury or trauma, multiple birth, maternal hemorrhage, summer birth, low birth weight, small for gestational age, congenital malformation, low 5-minute Apgar score, feeding difficulties, meconium aspiration, neonatal anemia, ABO or Rh incompatibility, and hyperbilirubinemia. Factors not associated with autism risk included anesthesia, assisted vaginal delivery, postterm birth, high birth weight, and head circumference.

CONCLUSIONS:

There is insufficient evidence to implicate any 1 perinatal or neonatal factor in autism etiology, although there is some evidence to suggest that exposure to a broad class of conditions reflecting general compromises to perinatal and neonatal health may increase the risk. Methodological variations were likely sources of heterogeneity of risk factor effects across studies.

Keywords: autistic disorder, risk factors, etiology, infant, newborn, pregnancy complications


WHAT'S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT:

Autism etiology is unknown, although perinatal and neonatal exposures have been the focus of epidemiologic research for more than 40 years. Although studies show that obstetrical and neonatal complications may increase autism risk, the specific complications and magnitude of effect have been inconsistent.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS:

Our study provides the first review and meta-analysis of all 64 studies of perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism published through March 2007.

The etiology of autism is unknown. Although the estimated 60% to 92% concordance rate in monozygotic twins compared with the 0% to 10% rate in dizygotic twins underscores the importance of genetic influences, the incomplete concordance in monozygotic twins also indicates a role of environmental factors.1,2 It is now believed that the mechanism underlying autism etiology is most likely polygenic and potentially epistatic and that environmental factors may interact with genetic factors to increase risk.3,4

Although the distinctive neuropathology remains elusive, studies3,5 have shown macroscopic, microscopic, and functional brain abnormalities. These various brain abnormalities suggest that the etiologically relevant period may be in utero or possibly in early infancy.3 Obstetrical and delivery factors as well as neonatal exposures have been the focus of a significant amount of epidemiologic research on the possible risk factors for autism. Although many studies support the hypothesis that obstetrical and neonatal complications may increase the risk of autism,6 the specific complications, magnitude of effect, and overall conclusions of these studies have been inconsistent. These inconsistencies may be a result of methodological variations, including diagnostic criteria, comparison groups, sample size, and exposure-assessment methods.

The purpose of this study is to provide a systematic review and meta-analysis of the epidemiologic literature on the relationship between perinatal and neonatal complications and exposures and autism. A review article by Kolevson et al6 discusses 7 studies on this topic, drawn from population-based registers or cohorts with prospectively collected obstetric information. Our study provides the first review and meta-analysis of all 64 studies of perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism published through March 2007. We have previously published a similar review and meta-analysis7 of prenatal exposures in relation to autism, which focused on conditions during pregnancy only. The current report complements our previous article by addressing all variables assessed at approximately the onset of labor through delivery and the neonatal period.

METHODS

Data Sources and Review Methods

The PubMed, Embase, and PsycInfo databases were searched using the key words “autism” in combination with “prenatal” or “perinatal” or “pregnancy” or “neonatal,” limited to peer-reviewed studies published in any language through March 2007. The search identified 698 studies in PubMed, 176 in Embase, and 416 in PsycInfo. The literature search sought to identify all epidemiologic studies that have examined the association of pregnancy and delivery factors and neonatal complications to the risk of autism. On the basis of a review of all abstracts, 83 articles were identified as potentially relevant and were reviewed further. Those studies that were not reviewed included case series, animal studies, autism prevalence studies, medical hypotheses, studies of other psychiatric diseases (eg, schizophrenia), and studies of unrelated exposures (eg, demographics, familial psychiatric diseases, genetics, or infant behaviors). A total of 41 additional potential articles were identified after screening the reference lists of articles. Among 124 studies reviewed, we excluded those that did not include a comparison group (n = 13) or formal statistical analyses (n = 3), did not examine exposures during pregnancy or the first month of life (n = 10), grouped their autism cases with other childhood psychotic disorders (n = 15), and were review or commentary articles (n = 18). The control group had to be nonautistic but could be otherwise abnormal. In total, 65 studies2,871 were eligible for inclusion in the review. Two studies13,28 reporting on the same data set were considered together, resulting in 64 studies for review. This report focuses on the perinatal and neonatal factors because a previous publication7 reviewed the prenatal exposures.

Dr Gardener abstracted each article on 2 separate occasions spaced 1 year apart. For each study, the following information was recorded: (1) study design (cohort or case control); (2) sample size and description (eg, clinic based or population based); (3) comparison group description (eg, matching criteria, sibling control subjects, healthy versus abnormal control subjects, and diagnoses of abnormal control subjects); (4) autism diagnostic criteria and mode of reporting (eg, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Third Edition versus Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition, parental report versus medical record review versus study physician assessment, and diagnostic measures used); (5) risk factors examined and mode of reporting (eg, parental interview and medical record review); (6) covariates in multivariate models; and (7) study results, including indicators of statistical significance, prevalence of exposures among case and control subjects, rates or risks of autism across exposure levels, relative risks (RRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Studies were classified as prospective versus retrospective if exposures were assessed and recorded before or after the onset of autism. For the quantitative review, we counted the number of studies that examined each perinatal and neonatal factor in relation to the risk of autism and the number of null findings, significant positive findings, and significant negative findings.

Statistical Analysis

Meta-analysis

Of 64 studies reviewed, 40847 were appropriate for inclusion in this meta-analysis. A total of 24 studies were excluded because they did not report RRs and CIs or did not provide information needed to calculate them. The statistical methods for this study have been described in detail previously.7 A separate meta-analysis was conducted for each exposure variable that was examined in 2 or more studies. For each exposure, a summary effect estimate was calculated using a random-effects model.72 Because power to detect heterogeneity is low in meta-analyses such as these,73 we took a conservative approach and used random-effects models to form CIs because random-effects models account for any observed heterogeneity regardless of whether the heterogeneity is statistically significant.

If an effect estimate was reported without the corresponding 95% CI, the confidence bounds were derived from the P value provided. If no P value was provided, then a P value of .05 or .50 was assumed for factors that did and did not reach statistical significance. Because of the rarity of many of the exposures and small sample sizes, there were tables in some (<5%) of the meta-analyses with 0 cell counts. In these instances, .5 was added to each cell of the 2 × 2 table.74

Meta-regression

Meta-regression is an analysis of whether study-level characteristics (ie, variations in methodology across studies) can explain heterogeneity in effect estimates reported across studies for the same risk factor–outcome association. It is an important aspect of a meta-analysis to indicate whether the summary effect estimates and 95% confidence bounds are an adequate representation of the results in the literature or whether stratified analyses on the basis of study design differences are more appropriate.

For each risk factor assessed in multiple studies, we examined the heterogeneity in the RRs estimated across studies using the Q statistic.72,75 Because of the limited power of this test,73 a liberal P value of <.10 was used to identify meta-analyses that required additional examination to assess potential sources of heterogeneity. If we found evidence of suggested heterogeneity, a meta-regression76 was conducted.

Meta-regression relates the effect estimate of each study to its design characteristic or methodology, to determine whether variability in study designs across studies may account for differences in reported associations across studies (ie, between-study effect modification) for each given risk factor.76 The study characteristics that were examined included the following: diagnostic criteria (inclusion of spectrum disorders: yes versus no); exposure information quality (0 = retrospective exposure assessment, 1 = mix of retrospective and prospective exposure assessment, and 2 = prospective exposure assessment); control for confounding (0 = univariate analysis, 1 = control for select demographic factors, birth order, or IQ and 2 = full multivariate analysis or matching with sibling control subjects); normal versus abnormal control subjects; and case selection (clinic based versus population based). A total of 19 studies used broad diagnostic criteria, of which 9 were clinic based and 10 were population based, whereas 45 studies used narrow diagnostic criteria (autism only), of which 29 were clinic based and 16 were population based. If effect modification was suggested for a given study characteristic (P < .10), then a stratified analysis was performed.

Publication bias was assessed for each factor by conducting tests for funnel plot asymmetry.77 Two statistical approaches were used to examine the association between study size and the effect of the exposure: the Begg test78 and the Egger test.79

RESULTS

Table 1 lists the perinatal and neonatal factors that were not included in the meta-analysis because of the unavailability of 2 or more effect estimates and 95% CIs, as well as an indication of whether they were associated with autism in the studies in which they were examined. Table 2 lists the perinatal and neonatal factors included in the meta-analysis, as well the number of null findings, significant positive findings, and significant negative findings (reduced risk). For each factor examined in the meta-analysis, Table 2 reports the summary effect estimate and 95% CI from the random-effects model and the P value for the test of heterogeneity.

TABLE 1.

Review of Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors not Eligible for Meta-analysis

Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors Examined in Only 1 Study
No association with autism Premature rupture of membranes, delayed labor, loss of amniotic fluid on the day before delivery, analgesia during labor, green amniotic fluid, acidosis pH <7.2 in cord blood, shoulder dystocia, cephalopelvic disproportion, near dead at birth, blue baby, blood t4 level (thyroxine), hypoglycemia, hypocalcemia, polycythemia, hyponatremia, infantile vomiting, intracranial hemorrhage, elevated serum IgM, macrocephaly, hydrocephaly, abnormal fetal cardiac activity, blood poisoning, and incubator use
Positive association with autism No labor, stimulated breathing at birth, asphyxia, baby held back, Apgar score ≤6, colic, quiet and sleepy, encephalopathy, and hemolytic disease
Negative association with autism Infected amniotic fluid
Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors Examined in Multiple Studiesa
Perinatal/neonatal factor (number of studies) Results across studiesb
Labor complications (3) 3, null results
Delivery complications (5) 3, null results, 2, significant positive results, P < .05
Birth length (3) 3, null results
Clinical dysmaturity (2) 1, null results, 1, significant positive results, P < .05
Poor condition at birth (2) 1, null results, 1, significant positive results, P < .05
Apgar <9 (2) 2, null results
Difficulty regulating temperature (2) 2, null results
Septicemia or meningitis (3) 3, null results
a

Although these factors were examined in multiple studies, effect estimates and CIs were available for fewer than 2 studies.

b

Total number of studies included in the review: 64.

TABLE 2.

Meta-analysis of Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors for Autism

Perinatal and Neonatal Factors (number of studies) Results Across Studiesa Summary Effect Estimate (95% CI)b Heterogeneity (P)
Labor
    Prolonged labor (9) 5−, 3↑, 1 ↓ 1.77 (0.76–4.14) <.001
    Induced or augmented labor (8) 6−, 2↑ 1.21 (0.90–1.62) .01
    Precipitous labor (5) 4−, 1↑ 1.52 (0.59–3.90) .002
    Premature rupture of membranes (7) 7− 1.30 (0.96–1.76) .43
Delivery procedures
    Anesthesia (7) 6−, 1↑ 1.32 (0.84–2.07) .17
        General anesthesia (3) 1.36 (0.87–2.13) .81
    Cesarean section (12) 8−, 3↑, 1↓ 1.26 (0.99–1.60) .06
        Emergency Cesarean (4) 1.04 (0.60–1.78) .03
        Elective Cesarean (2) 1.40 (0.75–2.62) .04
    Vacuum, forceps, or assisted vaginal (14) 13−, 1↑ 1.12 (0.95–1.32) .37
        Forceps (7) 1.16 (0.94–1.43) .94
        Vacuum extraction (2) 0.85 (0.43–1.69) .77
Presentation
    Abnormal presentation (15) 10−, 5↑ 1.44 (1.07–1.94) .08
        Breech (4) 1.81 (1.21–2.71) .22
    Other perinatal factors
    Cord complications (14) 13−, 1↑ 1.50 (1.00–2.24) .15
    Amniotic fluid not clear or meconium staining (6) 3−, 2↑, 1↓ 0.82 (0.25–2.69) .06
    Fetal distress (4) 3−, 1↑ 1.52 (1.09–2.12) .32
    Birth injury or trauma (6) 6− 4.90 (1.41–16.94) .31
    Twins or multiple birth (10) 7−, 3↑ 1.77 (1.23–2.55) .01
    Maternal hemorrhage (4) 3−, 1↑ 2.39 (1.35–4.21) .58
    Timing of birth
        Season and month of birth (12) 6−, 6↑
            January through March (4) 1.07 (0.90–1.27) .27
            April through June (4) 1.14 (0.80–1.63) .003
            July through September (4) 1.06 (0.95–1.17) .67
            October through December (4) 0.80 (0.58–1.10) .03
            Fall (3) 0.84 (0.62–1.14) .06
            Winter (3) 0.92 (0.69–1.24) .07
            Spring (3) 1.14 (0.71–1.83) <.001
            Summer (3) 1.14 (1.02–1.26) .68
Gestational length
    Weeks' gestation (decreased) (10) 5−, 3↑, 2↓
        Postterm (14) 7−, 6↑, 1↓ 1.14 (0.58–2.24) <.001
        Preterm (17) 9−, 7↑, 1↓ 1.16 (0.83–1.62) <.001
        ≥4 weeks preterm (2) 2.51 (0.77–8.23) .17
Birth weight and size
    Total birth weight (decreased) (15) 12−, 2↑, 1↓
    Low birth weight (<2500 g) (15) 8−, 7↑ 1.63 (1.19–2.33) <.001
    Birth weight <2000 g (2) 1−, 1↑ 2.20 (0.53–9.12) .003
    Birth weight <1500 g (3) 2−, 1↑ 3.00 (1.73–5.20) .27
    High birth weight (>4000 g) (6) 6− 1.13 (0.95–1.35) .70
    Small for gestational age (10) 7−, 3↑ 1.35 (1.14–1.61) .81
    Large for gestational age (4) 4− 1.11 (0.85–1.46) .20
    Small head circumference (5) 5− 1.17 (0.91–1.50) .68
    Large head circumference (5) 5− 0.88 (0.70–1.11) .85
Clinical impression
    Congenital malformation (11) 4−, 7↑ 1.80 (1.42–2.82) .87
    Irritable, floppy infant, convulsions, soft neurological signs (4) 4− 1.49 (0.64–3.47) .84
    Apgar score
        1-minute Apgar score (decreased) (5) 4−, 1↑
        Low 1-minute Apgar score (6) 4−, 1↑, 1↓ 1.08 (0.71–1.64) .01
        5-minute Apgar score (decreased) (6) 4−, 2↑
        Low 5-minute Apgar score (8) 6−, 2↑ 1.67 (1.24–2.26) .48
Neonatal Status
    Feeding difficulties (3) 2↑, 1− 3.35 (1.33–8.40) .22
    Weak or no crying after birth or slow to cry (3) 2−, 1↑ 1.84 (0.19–17.58) <.001
    Meconium aspirated (3) 2−, 1↑ 7.34 (2.30–23.47) .73
    Respiratory distress or no breathing after birth (13) 9−, 4↑ 1.84 (0.80–4.21) <.001
Neonatal medical conditions
    Anemia (4) 3−, 1↑ 7.87 (1.43–43.36) .50
    Neonatal infection (2) 2− 0.35 (0.09–1.44) .57
    Elevated temperature (2) 1−, 1↑ 2.84 (0.52–15.64) .01
    ABO or Rh incompatible (5) 4−, 1↑ 3.70 (1.90–7.23) .83
    Hyperbilirubinemia or jaundice (14) 11−, 3↑ 1.39 (0.97–1.97) .001
        Jaundice (4) 1.06 (0.80–1.40) .52
        Hyperbilirubinemia (6) 1.87 (1.01–3.47) .001
        Phototherapy (2) 1.65 (0.86–3.19) .09
    Seizures (5) 4−, 1↓ 2.72 (0.70–10.53) .43
    Microcephaly (3) 3− 1.40 (0.19–10.35) .36
    Heart defect (2) 2− 1.59 (0.86–2.93) .39
Medical treatment
    Medical intervention in the first month (7) 1−, 4↑, 2↓ 1.34 (0.81–2.21) .003
    Oxygen treatment or resuscitation (10) 8−, 2↑ 1.38 (0.65–2.94) <.001

Null results: −; significant positive results (P < .05): ↑; significant negative results (P < .05): ↓.

a

Total number of studies included in the review: 64.

b

Total number of studies included in the meta-analysis: 40.

The meta-analysis identified several perinatal and neonatal factors that were associated with an increased risk for autism. These included abnormal presentation in general (RR: 1.44, P = .02), breech presentation (RR: 1.81, P = .004), umbilical-cord complications (eg, prolapsed cord, cord wrapped around the neck; RR: 1.50, P = .05), fetal distress (RR: 1.52, P = .01), birth injury or trauma (RR: 4.90, P = .01), multiple birth (RR: 1.77, P = .002), maternal hemorrhage (RR: 2.39, P = .003), summer birth (RR: 1.14, P = .02), low birth weight (<2500 g; RR: 1.63, P = .002), very low birth weight (<1500 g; RR: 3.00, P < .001), small for gestational age (RR: 1.35, P = .001), congenital malformations (RR: 1.80, P < .001), low 5-minute Apgar score (RR: 1.67, P = .001), meconium aspirated (RR: 7.34, P = .001), feeding difficulties (RR: 3.35, P = .01), neonatal anemia (RR: 7.87, P = .02), ABO or Rh incompatibility (RR: 3.70, P < .001), and hyperbilirubinemia (RR: 1.87, P = .05). Cesarean delivery was associated with a 26% increased risk of autism that did not reach statistical significance (P = .06).

Heterogeneity (P < .10) in effect estimates across studies was observed for many factors including prolonged labor, induced or augmented labor, precipitous labor, Cesarean delivery, abnormal presentation, amniotic fluid not clear or meconium staining, multiple births, season of birth, preterm birth, postterm birth, low birth weight, low 1-minute Apgar score, weak or no crying after birth, respiratory distress, elevated temperature, hyperbilirubinemia, medical intervention during the neonatal period, and oxygen treatment or resuscitation. Table 3 shows the results of the regression analyses that examined the potential between-study sources of heterogeneity for those risk factors that showed heterogeneity in effect estimates across studies (P < .10). Heterogeneity in effect estimates across studies for prolonged labor, Cesarean delivery, and multiple births could not be explained by between-study variability in any of the methodological characteristics examined.

TABLE 3.

Analysis of Effect Modification by Study Characteristics: Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors With Heterogeneity, P < .10

Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors with Heterogeneity, P < .10 Significant Sources of Between-Study Heterogeneity: Study Characteristics, P < .10a Summary Effect Estimate (95% CI)
Prolonged labor None 1.77 (0.76–4.14)
Induced/augmented labor 1.21 (0.90–1.62)
Population based (P = .03)
4 Studies: population based 0.99 (0.72–1.35)
3 Studies: clinic based 1.72 (1.17–2.54)
Precipitous labor 1.52 (0.59–3.90)
Population based (P < .001)
2 Studies: population based 0.71 (0.45–1.11)
3 Studies: clinic based 4.10 (1.95–8.62)
Exposure data collection (P < .001)
4 Studies: prospective 0.78 (0.52–1.16)
1 Study: mix of retrospective and prospective 5.33 (2.12–13.40)
Cesarean delivery None 1.26 (0.99–1.60)
Abnormal presentation 1.44 (1.07–1.94)
Exposure data collection (P = .03)
9 Studies: prospective 1.56 (1.17–2.08)
3 Studies: retrospective and mixed 0.75 (0.32–1.76)
Amniotic fluid not clear or meconium staining 0.82 (0.25–2.69)
Abnormal and multivariate versus univariate analysis (P = .03)
3 Studies: normal control subjects and multivariate analysis or sibling control subjects 1.10 (0.50–2.39)
1 Study: abnormal comparison group and univariate analysis 0.08 (0.01–0.72)
Twins or multiple birth None 1.77 (1.23–2.55)
April–June birth 1.14 (0.80–1.63)
Population based (P < .001)
2 Studies: population based 0.86 (0.77–0.97)
2 Studies: clinic based 1.69 (1.20–2.38)
October–December birth None 0.80 (0.58–1.10)
Fall birth 0.84 (0.62–1.14)
Population based or multivariate versus univariate analysis (P = .03)
2 Studies: population based and univariate analysis 0.99 (0.88–1.11)
1 Study: clinic based and sibling control subjects 0.52 (0.30–0.91)
Winter birth None 0.92 (0.69–1.24)
Spring birth None 1.14 (0.71–1.83)
Postterm birth 1.14 (0.58–2.24)
Abnormal (P = .02)
9 Studies: normal control subjects 1.40 (0.77–2.56)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.25 (0.14–0.44)
Preterm birth 1.16 (0.83–1.62)
Abnormal (P = .003)
14 Studies: normal control subjects 1.30 (0.99–1.71)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.31 (0.17–0.57)
Exposure data collection (P = .02)
10 Studies: prospective 1.54 (1.14–2.06)
5 Studies: retrospective and mixed 0.74 (0.41–1.35)
Diagnostic criteria (P < .001)
10 Studies: narrow diagnostic criteria 0.81 (0.55–1.20)
5 Studies: broad diagnostic criteria 1.76 (1.44–2.16)
Low birth weight (<2500 g) 1.61 (1.18–2.19)
Exposure data collection (P = .01)
10 Studies: prospective 2.03 (1.39–2.96)
4 Studies: retrospective and mixed 0.86 (0.58–1.29)
Birth weight < 2000 g None 2.20 (0.53–9.12)
Low 1–minute Apgar score 1.08 (0.71–1.64)
Abnormal (P = .003)
5 Studies: normal control subjects 1.25 (0.93–1.67)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.21 (0.07–0.62)
Weak or no crying after birth or slow to cry 1.84 (0.19–17.58)
Abnormal (P < .001)
2 Studies: normal control subjects 5.93 (2.75–12.80)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.34 (0.17–0.68)
Respiratory distress or no breathing after birth 1.84 (0.84–4.21)
Abnormal (P = .04)
7 Studies: normal control subjects 2.95 (1.14–7.61)
2 Studies: abnormal control subjects 0.48 (0.24–0.94)
Hyperbilirubinemia or jaundice 1.39 (0.97–1.97)
Diagnostic criteria (P = .05)
8 Studies: narrow diagnostic criteria 1.63 (1.12–2.37)
2 Studies: broad diagnostic criteria 0.86 (0.53–1.40)
Medical intervention in the first month 1.34 (0.81–2.21)
Abnormal (P = .01)
5 Studies: normal control subjects 1.58 (1.09–2.30)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.18 (0.05–0.67)
Oxygen treatment or resuscitation 1.38 (0.65–2.94)
Abnormal (P = .001)
6 Studies: normal control subjects 1.89 (1.06–3.37)
1 Study: abnormal control subjects 0.22 (0.11–0.46)
a

Exposure data collection refers to the effect modification by exposure measurement (prospective versus retrospective). Diagnostic criteria refers to the effect modification by diagnostic criteria (narrow versus broad). Multivariate versus univariate analysis refers to effect modification by the degree of control for covariates. Population based refers to the effect modification by population-based versus clinic-based samples. Abnormal refers to the effect modification by use of a normal comparison group versus an abnormal comparison group. None refers to no effect modification (P < .10) by any of the above study characteristics.

The observed relationship between the induction of labor and risk of autism was significantly different for clinic-based compared with population-based studies. Specifically, a significant 72% increased risk of autism was observed in relation to induced labor among the 3 clinic-based studies, with no association observed in the population-based studies. It is interesting to note that a significant elevated risk also was observed in relation to precipitous labor in the clinic-based studies but not in the 2 population-based studies. Potential recall bias in 1 retrospective study30 also may have driven the summary effect estimate above the null for precipitous labor.

The timing of exposure assessment may have influenced the effect estimates for abnormal presentation. Although a significant 56% increased risk was observed in relation to abnormal fetal presentation in 9 prospective studies, no association was observed in 3 studies that assessed exposure in some or all participants retrospectively.

The meta-regression analysis suggested that the use of an abnormal (eg, Down's syndrome, physical handicaps) versus normal comparison group may account for the heterogeneity in effect estimates for several neonatal factors, including preterm birth, postterm birth, low 1-minute Apgar score, weak or no crying after birth, respiratory distress, resuscitation, medical intervention in the first month, as well as meconium staining. For all of these factors, the use of a normal comparison group (versus abnormal control subjects) resulted in a larger effect estimate, indicating a more positive association between these neonatal complications and the risk of autism. In fact, among the studies that used normal comparison groups, weak or no crying after birth, respiratory distress, and oxygen treatment or resuscitation were all significantly associated with an elevated risk of autism.

Overall, preterm birth was not associated with the risk of autism, although there was significant heterogeneity across studies. In 5 studies with broad diagnostic criteria, preterm birth was associated with a 76% increased risk, and in 10 prospective studies preterm birth conferred a 54% increase in risk. The positive association between low birth weight and risk of autism was also driven by 10 of 14 studies that assessed birth weight prospectively, suggesting that differential recall may have resulted in bias toward the null. Finally, the use of narrow diagnostic criteria and a clinic-based case sample were methodological characteristics that may have accounted for some of the heterogeneity in effect estimates for hyperbilirubinemia and were associated with an elevation in the effect estimate.

Publication bias was assessed for factors examined in 3 or more studies. No significant publication bias was observed using Begg's test.78 Using Egger's test,79 publication bias (P < .05) was suggested for high birth weight (>4000 g), meconium aspiration, and October to December birth. Although both tests for publication bias lacked sufficient power because of the small number of studies included in each meta-analysis, Egger's test is slightly higher powered. In the meta-analysis, high birth weight was assessed in 6 studies, quarter of birth in 4 studies, and meconium aspiration in only 3 studies. Although Egger's test indicates that publication bias may have impacted the summary effect estimates observed, ∼4 significant results would be expected as a result of chance alone because 76 tests of publication bias were conducted in this meta-analysis study of prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal risk factors for autism.

Several studies examined composite indices reflecting compromised perinatal and neonatal health in general in relation to the risk of autism, although an insufficient number of studies provided the data necessary for a formal meta-analysis. Perinatal and neonatal optimality scales were used to assess the number of perinatal and neonatal complications, respectively, experienced by case and control subjects. Various scales have been developed for this line of research, and the Gillberg optimality scale53 was used most often. The use of these scales helps solve the problem of low statistical power attributed to rare perinatal and neonatal complications. In addition, the use of optimality scores allows investigators to examine the hypothesis that general compromises to fetal and neonatal development, rather than specific exposures, increase the risk of autism. Of 8 studies that used perinatal optimality scales, only 1 reported reduced perinatal optimality among autism cases. However, reduced neonatal optimality was associated with an increased risk of autism in 8 of 11 studies that examined neonatal optimality scores.

DISCUSSION

The current study is the first meta-analysis of the relationship between perinatal and neonatal factors and risk of autism. Over 60 perinatal and neonatal factors have been studied in relation to autism in 64 epidemiologic studies, of which 40 studies were eligible for meta-analysis. Few of these factors have been examined in multiple well-designed studies. Therefore, attempted replication in methodologically rigorous studies remains necessary for many perinatal and neonatal variables. Most perinatal and neonatal factors examined in multiple studies have shown inconsistent results, and the preponderance of findings overall have not been statistically significant. The factors with the strongest evidence for an association with autism risk included abnormal fetal presentation, umbilical-cord complications, fetal distress, birth injury or trauma, multiple birth, maternal hemorrhage, summer birth, low birth weight, small for gestational age, congenital malformation, low 5-minute Apgar score, feeding difficulties, meconium aspiration, neonatal anemia, ABO or Rh incompatibility, and hyperbilirubinemia. In contrast, the factors with the strongest evidence against a role in autism risk included anesthesia use during delivery, assisted vaginal delivery, postterm birth, high birth weight, and head circumference.

Heterogeneity in effect estimates across studies was observed for several of the risk factors examined, and variability in study design characteristics may have accounted for the observed heterogeneity for many of these variables. There were some potentially important early risk factors for autism that were not found in most individual studies but are highlighted in these stratified analyses, including oxygen resuscitation and respiratory distress at birth.

Although there is insufficient evidence to implicate any 1 perinatal or neonatal factor in autism etiology, the studies using optimality scales provide some evidence to suggest that exposure to multiple neonatal complications may increase autism risk. It also is important to note that the observed association between perinatal and neonatal complications and risk of autism may actually reflect the consequences of previous prenatal complications. As mentioned, we previously published a meta-analysis of prenatal risk factors for autism, in which we found that few prenatal risk factors were associated with the risk of autism. The strongest prenatal factors included advanced maternal and paternal age at birth, maternal gestational bleeding, gestational diabetes, being first born versus third born or later, maternal prenatal medication use, and maternal birth abroad.7 The perinatal and neonatal complications identified in the current analysis may be the result of previous prenatal complications and/or may operate in combination with prior prenatal complications to impact autism risk. Additional research that considers the joint and independent effects of adverse conditions during these various time periods is required to address these possibilities.

This study suggests that several perinatal and neonatal complications may be related to autism risk, either alone, in combination, or perhaps only in those who are genetically vulnerable. However, the correlated occurrence of many of these complications limits the ability to determine which factors, if any, are independently associated with autism. For example, Cesarean deliveries are more common in pregnancies with abnormal fetal presentation, fetal distress, and multiple birth.80,81 Congenital malformations, low birth weight, abnormal presentation, and low Apgar score also are interrelated.36 Most studies did not use multivariate analyses to simultaneously control for all obstetrical factors examined, and a different set of factors was examined in each study. It is possible that increasing rates of some obstetrical factors, such as Cesarean delivery, low birth weight, multiple birth, and neonatal resuscitation, may be contributing factors to the rising prevalence of autism.81

The obstetrical complications that have emerged as significant risk factors for autism in the current meta-analysis suggest a possible role of fetal and neonatal hypoxia. In particular, growth retardation, fetal distress, umbilical-cord wrapping around the neck, low Apgar score, respiratory distress, resuscitation, meconium aspiration, and Cesarean delivery are all potential risk factors that also may be associated with an increased risk of hypoxia.6,24,26,36,38,82 Although some brain abnormalities observed in individuals with autism may reflect a potential role of oxygen deprivation during development, this possibility requires additional examination. Hypoxia also has been shown to increase dopaminergic activity, and there is evidence for dopamine overactivation in autism.83

The current meta-analysis indicates that low birth weight and being small for gestational age are significantly associated with an increased risk of autism. The association with low birth weight was particularly strong among prospective studies. Birth weight is commonly studied as an indicator of fetal growth and development and is affected by many prenatal factors, only some of which may be etiologically relevant. In addition to autism, low birth weight has been associated with a variety of psychiatric, cognitive, and behavioral problems.8493

Season or month of birth was significantly related to the risk of autism in 6 of 12 studies. Although the seasonal trends varied across studies, March10,23,60,64 and August10 were both suggested as birth months associated with an elevated risk of autism. The meta-analysis of season of birth only included a small subset of the studies that examined this association, and it indicated a potential elevated risk of autism associated with summer birth. Although the biological mechanism underlying this association is unclear, a relationship may be caused by seasonal variation in viral or other infections, nutritional factors, or vitamin deficiencies. Therefore, maternal infections and vitamin and nutrient consumption during pregnancy should be examined further in future prospective cohort studies.

Several investigators have questioned the causal nature of the observed relationship between perinatal and neonatal complications and autism. Confounding by birth order has been suggested because prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal complications are more often observed in first-, fourth-, and later-born offspring, and an increased risk of autism has been reported among those who are born first, fourth, and later.50,71 Although some studies have shown that associations were attenuated and no longer significant after adjusting for parity,39,59 other studies50,71 have shown that the positive relationship persists. A second noncausal hypothesis is that obstetrical and neonatal complications may occur as a result of the autistic condition in the offspring or as a consequence of other factors (eg, genetic factors) that are the true causal determinants of autism.50 In this epiphenomena explanation, perinatal complications simply reflect the abnormalities of autistic fetal development (autism causes suboptimality) or the same familial factors cause both autism and obstetrical and neonatal complications. The study conducted by Bolton et al50 provided strong evidence in support of the shared-risk hypothesis because there was an association between composite prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal suboptimality and measures of autism severity and familiality, and the suboptimality scores in the cases were highly correlated with that of their affected siblings. In addition, probands with increased prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal complications had more family members with the broader autism phenotype, which was characterized as similar yet milder impairments in language, communication, and social skills or repetitive stereotyped interests and behaviors compared with diagnostic criteria for autism. However, this finding was not replicated in a second study by Zwaigenbaum et al.71 The shared-risk hypothesis also was supported by the findings in the Zwaigenbaum et al study that indicated more composite prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal adversity among unaffected siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders that had high familial loading for the broader autism phenotype.71

Methodological limitations that may have impaired the precision and validity of the results of the studies in this review include small sample size, nonnormal control groups (eg, Down's syndrome), broad disease definition, and retrospective parental recall of exposures. Of 64 studies included, only 19 had over 80% power to detect an RR of 2 for an exposure with 10% prevalence. A total of 19 studies used broad diagnostic criteria, resulting in the inclusion of case subjects with other autism spectrum disorders, which may limit the ability to detect associations due to etiologic heterogeneity. A total of 21 studies assessed the exposure variables retrospectively, resulting in the high possibility of recall bias. However, the use of medical records also has the limitation of being incomplete. Finally, the majority of studies included only univariate analyses and did not assess potential confounding. These methodological weaknesses also were likely sources of heterogeneity of effects across studies. A possible additional source of heterogeneity of effects across studies was variability in the definitions of risk factors across studies. In fact, risk factor definitions and criteria often were lacking in the manuscripts reviewed. As mentioned, when possible we analyzed both broad variable definitions (eg, abnormal presentation at birth) and narrow variable definitions (eg, breech presentation).

This meta-analysis has a few limitations. First, only published data were used. Second, of 64 studies reviewed, only 40 reported the data necessary for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Within these 40 studies, the investigators did not report the necessary data for a meta-analysis on all factors examined. And although 40 studies were included in the meta-analysis overall, for each factor there generally were fewer than 6 studies included, limiting the statistical power to detect heterogeneity across studies and potential effect modification by study characteristics. Third, because of the rarity of many of the exposures examined and the small sample sizes in many studies, there were several instances of 0 cell counts within studies. The relatively small addition of 0.5 to the cell counts may have had an impact on the overall results because of the small sample sizes. Fourth, a few studies only reported an effect estimate and an indication of whether the results were statistically significant. In these cases, the CIs were estimated on the basis of assumptions regarding the actual P value (P = .05 if significant, P = .50 if not significant). In the case of statistically significant findings, these assumptions resulted in estimated confidence limits that were wider, less precise, and therefore more conservative than might have been expected. Fifth, the tests of publication bias were underpowered because of the limited number of studies in each meta-analysis. Finally, many studies simply examined all available perinatal and neonatal data using designs with methodological weaknesses and without a priori hypotheses or knowledge about reproductive epidemiology. As a result, significant associations observed due to chance are possible in this meta-analysis.

The current review and meta-analysis was not restricted to studies with particular methodological strengths. In addition, individual study characteristics were examined in meta-regressions rather than assigning studies aggregate quality scores. These strategies are consistent with the recommendations proposed by the Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Group, which advocated the use of broad inclusion criteria for studies along with regression analyses to relate specific study design characteristics to outcome.94 This maximizes the amount of data available for review. In addition, different methodological considerations are relevant for each exposure. However, the increased probability for heterogeneity of results using the broad inclusion criteria is important to note.

CONCLUSIONS

Autism is a devastating condition with no known cure. The rising prevalence, coupled with the severe emotional and financial impact on the families, underscores the need for large, prospective, population-based studies with the goal of elucidating the early-life modifiable risk factors.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Dr Gardener received a National Research Service Award Grant from the Training Program in Psychiatric Epidemiology and Biostatistics (T32 MH17119). Partial funding was provided by the Stanley Medical Research Institute. The funding sources had no role in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

We thank Ruifeng Li, MS, Harvard School of Public Health, Department of Biostatistics, for providing additional statistical and programming support. We thank Drs Tatjana Rundek, Yueh-Hsiu Mathilda Chiu, and Handan Wand for their translations of articles published in languages other than English. No compensation was received by the individuals acknowledged and listed above.

Dr Gardener contributed to the study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, literature search and acquisition of data, drafting of the manuscript statistical analysis, and administrative, technical, or material support. Dr Spiegelman contributed to the study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, statistical analysis, and study supervision. Dr Buka contributed to the study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, and study supervision.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated that they have no personal financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Abbreviations:

RR
relative risk
CI
confidence interval

REFERENCES

  • 1. Klauck SM. Genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14(6):714–720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med. 1995;25(1):63–77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Newschaffer CJ, Fallin D, Lee NL. Heritable and noninheritable risk factors for autism spectrum disorders. Epidemiol Rev. 2002;24(2):137–153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Santangelo SL, Tsatsanis K. What is known about autism: genes, brain, and behavior. Am J Pharmacogenomics. 2005;5(2):71–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. DiCicco-Bloom E, Lord C, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. The developmental neurobiology of autism spectrum disorder. J Neurosci. 2006;26(26):6897–6906 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Kolevson A, Gross R, Reichenberg A. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism: a review and integration of findings. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):326–333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL. Prenatal risk factors for autism: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(1):7–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Akçakin M, Polat S. A comparison of demographic and birth-related characteristics of autistic and intellectually handicapped children. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi. 1993;4(1):39–46 [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Badawi N, Dixon G, Felix JF, et al. Autism following a history of newborn encephalopathy: more than a coincidence? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48(2):85–89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Barak Y, Ring A, Sulkes J, Gabbay U, Elizur A. Season of birth and autistic disorder in Israel. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152(2):798–800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Bolton P, Pickles A, Harrington R, Macdonald H, Rutter M. Season of birth: issues, approaches and findings for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(3):509–530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Brimacombe M, Ming X, Lamendola M. Prenatal and birth complications in autism. Matern Child Health J. 2007;11(1):73–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Burd L, Severud R, Kerbeshian J, Klug MG. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism. J Perinat Med. 1999;27(6):441–450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Croen L, Grether J, Selvin S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: who is at risk? J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(3):217–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Van de Water J. Maternal autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders: a case control study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(2):151–157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Croen LA, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Newman TB. Neonatal hyperbilirubinemia and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2005;115(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/115/2/e135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Cryan E, Byrne M, O'Donovan A, O'Callaghan E. Brief report: a case-control study of obstetric complications and later autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 1996;26(4):453–460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Viral exposure and autism. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109(6):628–638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Pregnancy, delivery, and neonatal complications among autistic children. Am J Dis Child. 1980;134(9):860–864 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Eaton WW, Mortensen PB, Thomsen PH, Frydenberg M. Obstetric complications and risk for severe psychopathology in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(3):279–285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Finegan J, Quarrington B. Pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors and infantile autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1979;20(2):119–128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Gillberg C. Maternal age and infantile autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1980;10(3):293–297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Gillberg C. Do children with autism have March birthdays? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1990;82(2):152–156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Glasson EJ, Bower C, Petterson B, de Klerk N, Chaney G, Hallmayer JC. Perinatal factors and the development of autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(6):618–627 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Guillem P, Cans C, Guinchat V, Ratel M, Jouk P. Trends, perinatal characteristics, and medical conditions in pervasive developmental disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48(11):896–900 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hultman CM, Sparen P, Cnattingius S. Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology. 2002;13(4):417–423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Juul-Dam N, Townsend J, Courchesne E. Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, and the general population. Pediatrics. 2001;107(4):63–68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Klug MG, Burd L, Kerbeshian J, Benz B, Martsolf JT. A comparison of the effects of parental risk markers on pre- and perinatal variables in multiple patient cohorts with fetal alcohol syndrome, autism, Tourette syndrome, and sudden infant death syndrome: an enviromic analysis. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2003;25(6):707–717 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Knobloch H, Pasamanick B. Some etiologic and prognostic factors in early infantile autism and psychosis. Pediatrics. 1975;55(2):182–191 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kocijan-Hercigonja D, Remeta D, Orehovac M, Brkljacic D. Prenatal, perinatal and neonatal factors in infantile autism. Acta Med Croatica. 1991;45(4–5):357–362 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Larsson HJ, Eaton WW, Madsen KM, et al. Risk factors for autism: perinatal factors, parental psychiatric history, and socioeconomic status. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(10):916–925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: a nationwide register-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(9):963–971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Laxer G, Rey M, Ritvo ER. A comparison of potentially pathologic factors in European children with autism, Down Syndrome, and multiple physical handicaps. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18(2):308–314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Levy S, Zoltak B, Saelens T. A comparison of obstetrical records of autistic and nonautistic referrals for psychoeducational evaluations. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18(4):573–581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Lobascher ME, Kingerlee PE, Gubbay SS. Childhood autism: an investigation of aetiological factors in twenty-five cases. Br J Psychiatry. 1970;117(540):525–529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Maimburg RD, Vaeth M. Perinatal risk factors and infantile autism. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;114(4):257–264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, et al. The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics. 1990;86(4):514–519 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Matsuishi T, Yamashita Y, Ohtani Y, et al. Brief report: incidence of and risk factors for autistic disorder in neonatal intensive care unit survivors. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1999;29(2):161–166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Piven J, Simon J, Chase GA, et al. The etiology of autism: pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32(6):1256–1263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Reichenberg A, Bresnahan M, Rabinowitz J, Lubin G, Davidson M. Adancing paternal age and autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(9):1026–1032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Soldin OP, Lai S, Lamm SH, Mosee S. Lack of a relation between human neonatal thyroxine and pediatric neurobehavioral disorders. Thyroid. 2003;13(2):193–198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Stein D, Weizman A, Ring A, Barak Y. Obstetric complications in individuals diagnosed with autism and in healthy controls. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47(1):69–75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Sugie Y, Sugie H, Fukuda T, Ito M. Neonatal factors in infants with autistic disorder and typically developing infants. Autism. 2005;9(5):487–494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Tanoue Y, Oda S, Asano F, Kawashima K. Epidemiology of infantile autism in Southern Ibaraki, Japan: differences in prevalence in birth cohorts. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18(2):155–166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Ward AJ. A comparison and analysis of the presence of family problems during pregnancy of mothers of “autistic” children and mothers of normal children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1990;20(4):279–288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Wier ML, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Grether JK, Croen LA. Congenital anomalies associated with autism spectrum disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48(6):500–507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Williams G, Oliver JM, Allard A, Sears L. Autism and associated medical and familial factors: A case control study. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2003;15(4):335–349 [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Atladóttir HO, Parner ET, Schendel D, et al. Variation in incidence of neurodevelopmental disorders with season of birth. Epidemiology. 2007;18(2):240–245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Beversdorf DQ, Manning SE, Hillier A, et al. Timing of prenatal stressors and autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 2005;35(4):471–478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Bolton PF, Murphy M, Macdonald H, Whitlock B, Pickles A, Rutter M. Obstetric complications in autism: consequences or causes of the condition? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(2):272–281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Bryson SE, Smith IM, Eastwood D. Obstetrical suboptimality in autistic children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1988;27(4):418–422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Deb S, Prasad KBG, Seth H, Eagles JM. A comparison of obstetric and neonatal complications between children with autistic disorder and their siblings. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1997;41(1):81–86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Gillberg C, Gillberg IC. Infantile autism: a total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1983;13(2):153–166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Harper J, Williams S. Early environmental stress and infantile autism. Med J Aus. 1974;1(10):341–346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(12):1366–1376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Konstantareas MM, Hauser P, Lennox C, Homatidis S. Season of birth in infantile autism. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1986;17(1):53–65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Li S, Yang X, Jia M. A study of perinatal risk factors in patients with autism. Chin J Psychiatry. 1998;31(3):178–180 [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Links PS, Stockwell M, Abichandani F, Simeon J. Minor physical anomalies in childhood autism, part I: their relationship to pre-and perinatal complications. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1980;10(3):273–285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Lord C, Mulloy C, Wendelboe M, Schopler E. Pre- and perinatal factors in high-functioning females and males with autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1991;21(2):197–209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Mouridsen SE, Nielsen S, Rich B, Isager T. Season of birth in infantile autism and other types of childhood psychoses. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1994;25(1):31–43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T. Brief report: parental age in infantile autism, autistic-like conditions, and borderline childhood psychosis. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1993;23(2):387–396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Park RJ, Bolton PF. Pervasive developmental disorder and obstetric complications in children and adolescents with tuberous sclerosis. Autism. 2001;5(3):237–248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, et al. A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway, and Sweden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30(3):405–416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Stevens MC, Fein DH, Waterhouse LH. Season of birth effects in autism. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22(3):399–407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Torrey EF, Dhavale D, Lawlor JP, Yolken RH. Autism and head circumference in the first year of life. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56(11):892–894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Torrey EF, Hersh SP, McCabe KD. Early childhood psychosis and bleeding during pregnancy: a prospective study of gravid women and their offspring. J Autism Child Schizophr. 1975;5(4):287–297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Tsai LY, Stewart MA. Etiological implications of maternal age and birth order in infantile autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1983;13(1):57–65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Wilkerson DS, Volpe AG, Dean RS, Titus JB. Perinatal complications as predictors of infantile autism. Int J Neurosci. 2002;112(9):1085–1098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Yeates-Frederikx MH, Nijman H, Logher E, Merckelbach HL. Birth patterns in mentally retarded autistic patients. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):257–262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Zambrino CA, Balottin U, Bettaglio E, Gerardo A, Lanzi G. Obstetrical suboptimality in autistic children: an Italian sample. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1997;27(4):493–494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Zwaigenbaum L, Szatmari P, Jones MB, Bryson SE, MacLean JE, Mahoney WJ. Pregnancy and birth complications in autism and liability to the broader autism phenotype. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(5):572–579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177–188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Takkouche B, Cadarso-Suarez C, Spiegelman D. Evaluation of old and new tests of heterogeneity in epidemiologic meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;150(2):206–215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Sterne JAC, Bradburn MJ, Egger M. Meta-analysis in stata. In: Egger M, Smith GD, Altman DG. eds. Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-analysis in Context. London, UK: BMJ Publishing Group; 2006, pp 347–372 [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br Med J. 2003;327(7414):557–560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Stram DO. Meta-analysis of published data using a linear mixed-effects model. Biometrics. 1996;52(2):536–544 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Khoshdel A, Attia J, Carney SL. Basic concepts in meta-analysis: a primer for clinicians. Int J Clin Pract. 2006;60(10):1287–1294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50(4):1088–1101 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br Med J. 1997;315(7109):629–634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Incerpi MH. Operative delivery. In DeCherney AH, Nathan L. eds. Current Diagnosis and Treatment Obstetrics and Gynecology, 10th Edition. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2007, pp 445–476 [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Park MM. Births: final data for 2000. National Vital Statistics Reports. 2002;50(5):1–104 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Cunningham FG, Leveno KL, Bloom SL, Hauth JC, Gilstrap LC, Wenstrom KD. Williams Obstetrics, 22nd Edition New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Previc FH. Prenatal influenced on brain dopamine and their relevance to the rising incidence of autism. Med Hypotheses. 2007;68(1):46–60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Breslau N. Psyciatric sequelae of low birth weight. Epidemiol Rev. 1995;17(1):96–106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Wichers MC, Purcell S, Danckaerts M, et al. Prenatal life and post-natal psychopathology: evidence for negative gene-birth weight interaction. Psychol Med. 2002;32(7):1165–1174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Hack M, Taylor HG, Klein N, Eiben R, Schatschneider C, Mercuri-Minich N. School-age outcomes in children with birth weights under 750 g. N Engl J Med. 1994;331(12):753–759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Aram DM, Hack M, Hawkins S, Weissman BM, Borawski-Clark E. Very-low-birthweight children and speech and language development. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(5):1169–1179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. McCormick MC, Gortmaker SL, Sobol AM. Very low birth weight children: behavior problems and school difficulty in a national sample. J Pediatr. 1990;117(5):687–693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Johnson EO, Breslau N. Increased risk of learning disabilities in low birth weight boys at age 11 years. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(6):490–500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Veen S, Ens-Dokkum MH, Schreuder AM, Verloove-Vanhorick SP, Brand R, Ruys JH. Impairments, disabilities, and handicaps of very preterm and very-low-birthweight infants and five years of age: the Collaborative Project on Preterm and Small for Gestational Age Infants (POPS) in the Netherlands. Lancet. 1991;338(8758):33–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Aylward GP, Pfeiffer SI, Wright A, Verhulst SJ. Outcome studies of low birth weight infants published in the last decade: a metaanalysis. J Pediatr. 1989;115(4):515–520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Schothorst PF, van Engeland H. Long-term behavioral sequelae of prematurity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(2):175–183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Pharoah PO, Stevenson CJ, Cooke RW, Stevenson RC. Prevalence of behaviour disorders in low birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child. 1994;70(4):271–274 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies for epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. JAMA. 2000;283(15):2008–2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Pediatrics

RESOURCES