Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2011 Dec 21;33(1):53–75. doi: 10.1007/s10827-011-0374-4

Improved measures of phase-coupling between spikes and the Local Field Potential

Martin Vinck 1,, Francesco Paolo Battaglia 1, Thilo Womelsdorf 2, Cyriel Pennartz 1
PMCID: PMC3394239  PMID: 22187161

Abstract

An important tool to study rhythmic neuronal synchronization is provided by relating spiking activity to the Local Field Potential (LFP). Two types of interdependent spike-LFP measures exist. The first approach is to directly quantify the consistency of single spike-LFP phases across spikes, referred to here as point-field phase synchronization measures. We show that conventional point-field phase synchronization measures are sensitive not only to the consistency of spike-LFP phases, but are also affected by statistical dependencies between spike-LFP phases, caused by e.g. non-Poissonian history-effects within spike trains such as bursting and refractoriness. To solve this problem, we develop a new pairwise measure that is not biased by the number of spikes and not affected by statistical dependencies between spike-LFP phases. The second approach is to quantify, similar to EEG-EEG coherence, the consistency of the relative phase between spike train and LFP signals across trials instead of across spikes, referred to here as spike train to field phase synchronization measures. We demonstrate an analytical relationship between point-field and spike train to field phase synchronization measures. Based on this relationship, we prove that the spike train to field pairwise phase consistency (PPC), a quantity closely related to the squared spike-field coherence, is a monotonically increasing function of the number of spikes per trial. This derived relationship is exact and analytic, and takes a linear form for weak phase-coupling. To solve this problem, we introduce a corrected version of the spike train to field PPC that is independent of the number of spikes per trial. Finally, we address the problem that dependencies between spike-LFP phase and the number of spikes per trial can cause spike-LFP phase synchronization measures to be biased by the number of trials. We show how to modify the developed point-field and spike train to field phase synchronization measures in order to make them unbiased by the number of trials.

Keywords: Spike-triggered average, Spike-field locking, Spike-LFP, Phase locking, Spike-field coherence, Phase-synchronization

Introduction

Neuronal spiking outputs are often temporally coordinated across neuronal groups. Temporal coordination often manifests itself as rhythmic or phase synchronization, i.e. phase-coupling of oscillatory activity, which has been proposed as a candidate mechanism for the flexible routing of neuronal signals (Abeles 1982; Fries 2005; Varela et al. 2001; Burchell et al. 1998; Womelsdorf et al. 2010b; Gielen et al. 2010; Börgers and Kopell 2008; Salinas and Sejnowski 2001; Buschman and Miller 2007; Benchenane et al. 2011; Gregoriou et al. 2009), assembly formation (Singer 1999; Buzsáki 2010; Kopell et al. 2011; Canolty et al. 2010; Battaglia et al. 2011), and coding (König et al. 1995; Fries et al. 2007; Panzeri et al. 2010; Vinck et al. 2010a; O’Keefe and Recce 1993; Azouz and Gray 2003; Nadasdy 2010; Reimer and Hatsopoulos 2010; Siegel et al. 2009; VanRullen et al. 2005; Havenith et al. 2011; Koepsell et al. 2010).

To quantify rhythmic synchronization, we need to relate signals generated by separate sources. An increasingly popular choice of signals is the combination of spiking activity and the LFP (Local Field Potential) (e.g. see Fries et al. 1997, 2001; Gregoriou et al. 2009; Pesaran et al. 2002, 2008; Ray and Maunsell 2010; Colgin et al. 2009; Csicsvari et al. 2003; Vinck et al. 2010a; van Wingerden et al. 2010b; Chalk et al. 2010; O’Keefe and Recce 1993; Eeckman and Freeman 1990; Womelsdorf et al. 2010a; Brown et al. 2004; Jarvis and Mitra 2001; Zeitler et al. 2006; Lepage et al. 2011; Sirota et al. 2008; Siapas et al. 2005; Jacobs et al. 2007; Donoghue et al. 1998; Gray et al. 1989; Wehr and Laurent 1996; Cardin et al. 2009; Sohal et al. 2009; Okun et al. 2010; Bichot et al. 2005; Buffalo et al. 2011; Canolty et al. 2010; Montemurro et al. 2008; Kayser et al. 2009; Mitchell et al. 2009; Ray et al. 2008; Saalmann et al. 2007; van der Meer and Redish 2009; Denker et al. 2011; Lakatos et al. 2007; Besserve et al. 2010; Zanos et al. 2011; Destexhe et al. 1999).

Spike-LFP measures are thought to be more sensitive to phase synchronization than spike-spike measures, because the LFP measures the compound synaptic activity generated by a large pool of neurons (Mitzdorf 1985; Logothetis 2003). This sensitivity is especially advantageous for the study of long-range interactions: Many studies have revealed functionally relevant changes in cross-areal spike- LFP phase synchronization (e.g. Pesaran et al. 2008; Womelsdorf et al. 2007; Siapas et al. 2005; Gregoriou et al. 2009; Benchenane et al. 2010; Colgin et al. 2009; Sigurdsson et al. 2010). In comparison to LFP-LFP measures, spike-LFP measures have the advantage that they provide an indirect measure of the relationship between spiking outputs and synaptic inputs (Okun et al. 2010; Harvey et al. 2009), and that they can be related to neuronal firing rate selectivity (e.g. Gregoriou et al. 2009; van Wingerden et al. 2010b) and cell type (Benchenane et al. 2010; Tukker et al. 2007; Klausberger et al. 2003; Csicsvari et al. 2003; Hasenstaub et al. 2005; van Wingerden et al. 2010a; Wulff et al. 2009; Sohal et al. 2009; Cardin et al. 2009; Bragin et al. 1995; Gulyas et al. 2010), which can provide useful insights into the mechanisms of rhythmogenesis (for an overview, see Wang 2010). At the same time, spike-LFP measures avoid the problem that volume-conduction and common referencing can create spurious correlations between LFP signals (e.g. Vinck et al. 2011; Nolte et al. 2004; Sirota et al. 2008; Stam et al. 2007; Nunez and Srinivasan 2006).

A wide variety of spike-LFP measures exist, and they can be categorized into two classes. Firstly, some measures are based on a quantification of the consistency of individual spike-LFP phases at a certain frequency (e.g. Vinck et al. 2010b; Csicsvari et al. 2003; Fries et al. 1997, 2001; Grasse and Moxon 2010; Canolty et al. 2010; Tukker et al. 2007; Hansen and Dragoi 2011; Siapas et al. 2005; Sirota et al. 2008; Colgin et al. 2009; Denker et al. 2011). In this paper, we will refer to these measures as point-field phase synchronization measures (Fig. 1), because they relate single points, i.e. spikes, to the ongoing LFP oscillations, i.e. fields. A major problem bound to these point-field phase synchronization measures is that they can be strongly biased by the number of spikes. Previously, we addressed this problem by introducing the pairwise phase consistency (PPC) (Vinck et al. 2010a).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Illustration of point-field phase synchronization measures. Spikes (vertical, solid lines) are related to LFPs by determining the instantaneous LFP phases at the times of spiking by centering a window (horizontal, solid lines) around every spike. Thus, the phase for each spike is determined from the LFP snippet that is centered around it. The instantaneous LFP phase at the time of spiking can be directly gauged by using the dotted lines that connect the spikes to the LFP. The frequency resolution, which is inversely proportional to time resolution, is proportional to the window length, which needs to be sufficiently large. However, if the windows are too long, then the loss of time-resolution may lead to a decrease in spike-LFP phase consistency. The instantaneous spike-LFP phases are depicted by the unit arrows that originate from the spikes. The LFP was modeled as band-stop filtered Gaussian white noise, as in Zeitler et al. (2006). Here, we show only two (m = 1 and m = 2) out of M available trials. The measures of point-field phase consistency are computed over the n relative spike-LFP phases. By vector addition of the n spike-LFP phase vectors, a resultant vector is obtained, whose length is conventionally taken as a measure of point-field phase consistency

However, there are two further problems related to point-field phase synchronization measures that were not solved by the PPC measure. Firstly, the problem that by the direct quantification of the consistency of individual spike-LFP phases through circular statistics, it is implicitly assumed that individual spike-LFP phases can be modeled as outcomes of statistically independent random variables. This assumption is typically violated because neurons display non-Poissonian history effects, such as refractoriness and bursting (Sections 2 and 3). For example, given the observation of a certain spike-LFP phase for a bursting neuron, we may be more likely to observe a similar spike-LFP phase again, independent of the modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase. To solve the latter problem, we develop a new point-field phase synchronization measure that is not affected by a dependence between spike-LFP phases that were obtained from the same trial (Section 3). We achieve this by modifying the PPC such that its computation is now based only on pairs of spike-LFP phases that were obtained from separate trials (see Eq. (3.1) below). A second problem that we address here is that dependencies between spike count and spike-LFP phase, e.g. as consequence of theta or gamma phase precession/shifting (O’Keefe and Recce 1993; Vinck et al. 2010a; Tiesinga and Sejnowski 2010; Harris et al. 2002; Mehta et al. 2002; Schmidt et al. 2009; Siegel et al. 2009; Huxter et al. 2008), can cause point-field phase synchronization measures to become biased by the number of trials. We show how the point-field PPC measure can be modified in order to render it unbiased as a function of the number of trials (see Eq. (4.3) below).

The second class of spike-field measures is inspired by the classical coherence approach for EEG and MEG data (e.g., see Nunez and Srinivasan 2006; Mitra and Pesaran 1999; Walter 1963; Adey et al. 1961; Lachaux et al. 1999) and is characterized by quantifying, for a certain frequency, the consistency of the relative phases between spike train and concurrent LFP signals across trials, as opposed to quantifying it across spikes (Fig. 2) (e.g. Fries et al. 2008; Gregoriou et al. 2009; Jarvis and Mitra 2001; Pesaran et al. 2008; Chalk et al. 2010; Pesaran et al. 2002; Lepage et al. 2011; Mitra and Bokil 2008). In this paper, we will refer to this second type of measures as spike train to field phase synchronization measures. With this approach, the spike trains are not broken down into single spikes that are then related to the LFPs around them, as opposed to the point-field phase synchronization measures. Instead, the spike train to field phase synchronization measures are based on the computed cross-spectra between the spike train signals of a certain duration and the concurrent LFPs.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Illustration of spike train to field phase synchronization measures. Spikes (vertical, solid lines) are related to LFPs by computing, using the Fast Fourier Transform, the respective phase offsets of the spike train and the LFP at a particular frequency (grey arrows) and subsequently determining the relative phase between these phase offsets (black arrows). Thus, the relative phases are computed for a time-period, not for an individual spike. The LFP was generated using the same model as in Fig. 1. Shown here are two trials (m = 1 and m = 2). The measures of spike train to field phase consistency are computed over the M relative phases. By vector addition of the M relative phase vectors, a resultant vector is obtained, whose length is conventionally taken as a measure of the consistency of the relative phases between LFPs and spike trains

Although both point-field and spike train to field phase synchronization measures are widely applied, it is unknown how they compare. In this paper, we derive an exact, analytical relationship (see Eq. (5.9) below) between the point-field PPC measures and the spike train to field PPC, which is a quantity closely related to the squared spike-field coherence (e.g. see Jarvis and Mitra 2001), with the only difference that the latter spike train to field index is a concurrent measure of amplitude co-variations as well, while the former spike train to field PPC measure only considers the consistency of relative phases. From this relationship, it follows that the spike train to field PPC is strictly positively dependent on the number of spikes per trial if the consistency of individual spike-LFP phases is independent of the number of spikes per trial. These findings are consistent with the previous result that the spike train to field coherence is positively dependent on the number of spikes (Zeitler et al. 2006; Lepage et al. 2011). Our analytic derivation is exact and directly leads to two corrected versions of the spike train to field PPC measures that are fully independent of the number of spikes per trial (Section 5).

The paper is organized as follows. We first introduce conventional point-field phase synchronization measures (Section 2). In Section 3, we study the effect of violations of the assumption of independency between spike-LFP phases (for different spikes) on these conventional point-field phase synchronization measures, and introduce a point-field measure that does not require this assumption. In Section 4, we study the consequences of a dependence between spike-LFP phase and spike count, and introduce a measure that is not affected by this dependence. In Section 5, we demonstrate an exact, analytical relationship between point-field and spike train to field phase synchronization measures and study the dependence of spike train to field phase synchronization measures on the number of spikes per trial. We end with a comparison of the advantages and disadvantages of the developed measures.

Definitions of conventional indices

A mathematical framework for point-field phase synchronization measures

We first develop a mathematical framework for point-field phase synchronization measures. Our starting point is the observed data, i.e. spike counts and spike-LFP phases, and we then proceed with modeling the observed data as outcomes of random variables. Suppose we perform simultaneous recordings of an LFP and the spiking activity of a neuronal source. We model the trial-wise spike counts by defining the vector of identically and independently distributed random variables Inline graphic, where M is the number of trials, whose observed value is defined as Inline graphic, i.e. the experimentally observed counts of spikes emitted in each trial. Define Inline graphic, and its observed value Inline graphic, the observed total number of spikes. The set of trials that contained at least one spike is defined as Inline graphic, such that Inline graphic is the number of trials with at least one spike. For every spike, we take the LFP segment that is centered around it, as illustrated in Fig. 1, and spectrally decompose it, to obtain the spike-LFP phase at each frequency f. The spectral decomposition of the LFP may, without loss of generality, be based on e.g. the Discrete Fourier Transform or the Hilbert Transform on the filtered signal. Thus for all trials Inline graphic we observe, at a particular frequency f, a data vector Inline graphic containing n m spike-LFP phase values, which are depicted in Fig. 1. The dependence of Inline graphic and related statistics on frequency is omitted and implicitly assumed in what follows.

In order to make statements about the statistics of the population from which these sample spike-LFP phase data were drawn, we model the observed spike-LFP phases as outcomes of random variables. A straightforward approach may seem to define n independent and identically distributed random variables (Θ1, ..., Θn) (as in Vinck et al. 2010b). However, the number of spikes itself is a random variable, such that we cannot restrict ourselves to defining n random variables. Furthermore, for a given trial, the spike-LFP phase may be statistically dependent on the number of spikes, as it is for example the case when theta phase precession (O’Keefe and Recce 1993) is present. To account for these two intricacies, we proceed as follows. For all m ∈ { 1, ..., M }, let Inline graphic be a vector of random variables. For all Inline graphic, we let the observed value of Θk,m equal a randomly chosen—without replacement—element of Inline graphic, and obtain a vector representation of the spike-LFP phases by defining the spike-LFP phase vector U k,m ≡ (cos( Θk,m ), sin( Θk,m )).

Thus, for all Inline graphic, we model all Θk,m elements of Inline graphic for which 1 ≤ k ≤ N m as identically distributed (i.e., across time). In practice, it can occur that spike-LFP phases from the same trial are not identically distributed, because the mean spike-LFP phase can be a function of a time-varying variable, which occurs for example during theta phase precession (O’Keefe and Recce 1993) and gamma phase-shifting (Vinck et al. 2010a). Mixing distributions with different mean spike-LFP phases causes an average decrease in point-field phase synchronization measures. If the mean spike-LFP phase is indeed non-stationary, then this reduction in point-field phase synchronization measures may be avoided by using one of the following two strategies. Firstly, by computing our point-field phase synchronization measures over smaller trial segments for which the mean spike-LFP phase is approximately stationary, at the cost of an increase in estimator variance. Secondly, by explicitly modeling the mean spike-LFP phase as a function of a time-varying random variable, and subsequently computing our point-field phase synchronization measures over the residual model error terms, as in Vinck et al. (2010a).

Further, for all k ∈ { 1, 2, ...}, we model all elements of the vector (Θk,1, ..., Θk,M) as identically distributed (i.e., across trials). In practice, it can occur that spike-LFP phases are not identically distributed across trials, e.g. as a consequence of pooling trials across different experimental conditions or as a consequence of learning processes. Again, the consistency of spike-LFP phases is, on average, decreased by mixing distributions with varying mean spike-LFP phases.

Thus far, we did not make statements about the statistical dependence or independence between the different Θk,m’s (the spike-LFP phases). While the assumption of independence between the spike-LFP phases from the same trial is convenient from a statistical perspective (Section 2.2), it may be often violated in practice (Section 3). Henceforth, we assume that a dependence between spike-LFP phases can only occur for spike-LFP phases from the same trial, i.e. we assume that random variables are statistically independent if they represent spike-LFP phases from separate trials (Assumption 1).

Assumption 1

Spike-LFP phase vectors from separate trials are conditionally independent given observation of the spike count.

This assumption implies that for all (k,m,j,l) for which l ≠ m, the equation

graphic file with name M15.gif 2.1

holds, where · is the dot product operation. From the independence of N m and N l, it follows that the equation E { U k,m ·U j,l } = E { U k,m } ·E { U j,l } holds as well. The justification of Assumption 1 is that dependencies will mostly act on short timescales, and that spike train statistics will typically be reset by an inter-trial interval. Assumption 1 does not require that the LFPs are not phase-synchronized across trials. If the LFP attains the same phase offset relative to a trial event in every trial, then the spike-LFP phase vectors from different trials can still be independent of each other, e.g. if the spike trains are generated by a homogeneous Poisson process, or if the inter-trial interval duration is sufficiently long. Nevertheless, phase-synchronization of the LFPs across trials may well cause Assumption 1 to be violated if the inter-trial interval is short relative to the oscillatory cycle duration. In that case, non-Poissonian history effects such as autorhythmicity can cause dependencies between spike-LFP phases across trials. The violation of Assumption 1 has the same consequences as a violation of Assumption 2 (which will be introduced in Section 2.2), namely a systematic increase or decrease in the PPC (Section 3).

Direct application of circular statistics to spike-LFP phase data

It is common practice in neurophysiology to directly compute circular statistics over the concatenated data vector of spike-LFP phases. Whether this leads to valid conclusions about the relationship between the LFP and spiking activity remains to be demonstrated. Suppose that the following two additional assumptions hold however:

Assumption 2

Spike-LFP phase vectors from the same trial are conditionally independent given observation of the spike count.

This assumption implies that for all (k,j,m) for which k ≠ j the equality

graphic file with name M16.gif 2.2

holds. In other words, this assumption holds that, given observation of the spike count N m, the spike-LFP phase vectors from the mth trial are independent, i.e. that the observation of one spike-LFP phase vector does not change the expected value of another spike-LFP phase vector from the same trial. The conditional independence (given N m) is important here: If spike-LFP phase and spike count are statistically dependent, then the unconditional independence between the spike-LFP phase vectors from the same trial does not necessarily hold given Assumption 2. The reason for this is that the observation of a certain spike-LFP phase may change the expected value of N m, which could in turn change the expected value of the other spike-LFP phase vectors. Unconditional independence will only be attained by making the additional assumption of independence between spike count and spike-LFP phase (Assumption 3, see below). Example cases where Assumption 2 is violated, e.g. as a consequence of non-Poissonian history effects, will be presented in Section 3.

Assumption 3

Independence between spike count and spike-LFP phase.

This assumption holds that the expected value of U k,m is independent of N m, for k ≤ N m, i.e. that the expected value of the spike-LFP phase vector U k,m is independent of the number of spikes that were observed in the mth trial. Example cases where this assumption is violated will be presented in Section 4.

Based on the Θk,m’s (spike-LFP phases) and N m’s (spike counts), we can now define several statistics that estimate the consistency of spike-LFP phases, e.g. the resultant vector length (Eq. (2.5)). In order to define the population parameters corresponding to these estimators, we will now define random variables that are identically distributed to the U k,m’s. Let N (1) and N (2) be two random variables independently and identically distributed, such that for any n m ∈ ℕ, Inline graphic Inline graphic. Let Θ be a random variable and U (1) and U (2) two statistically independent random vectors identically distributed to the vector (cos(Θ), sin(Θ)), such that the equation Inline graphic holds for 1 ≤ k ≤ n m. Given Assumptions 1–3, it follows that for any (N (1), N (2)) ∈ ℕ2, the equation

graphic file with name M20.gif 2.3

holds.

We now proceed with defining conventional statistics quantifying the consistency of spike-LFP phases, relying on Assumptions 1–3. A natural statistic for the consistency of the spike-LFP phases is provided by the circular resultant length, which is defined, on the population level, as

graphic file with name M21.gif 2.4

Its direct estimator is defined as

graphic file with name M22.gif 2.5

This statistic is sometimes also referred to as the (point-field) PLV (‘phase locking value’). Given our Assumptions 1–3, it follows that the estimator Inline graphic is strongly biased by sample size, i.e. approaches R from above. This is a problem when comparing neurons or experimental conditions with a small, varying and uncontrollable number of spikes (Vinck et al. 2010b). Therefore, the PPC estimator (Vinck et al. 2010b), which is graphically illustrated in Fig. 3(a), is our preferred statistic at the descriptive level, and for performing simple parametric inferential statistics at the group level, and is defined as

graphic file with name M24.gif 2.6

The corresponding population parameter of the PPC is defined as

graphic file with name M25.gif 2.7
Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Illustration of the point-field PPC Inline graphic and the point-field PPC Inline graphic. (a) Nine spike-LFP phases from four different trials are recorded. These spike-LFP phases are depicted by the arrows, which are duplicated on the x- and y-axis. This gives 9 ·9 = 81 potential 2-combinations of spike-LFP phases across trials. The extent to which two spike-LFP phase vectors coincide is measured by the dot product. The point-field PPC Inline graphic equals the average dot product of spike-LFP phase vectors across all 2-combinations of different spikes. The nine 2-combinations of spikes with themselves are not taken into account, which is indicated by the diagonal patterns. (b) Same as in (a), but the point-field PPC Inline graphic equals the average dot product of spike-LFP phase vectors across all 2-combinations of spikes from different trials.The 2-combinations of spikes from the same trial are not taken into account, which is indicated by the diagonal patterns

The quadruple sum in Eq. (2.6) computes the dot product for all combinations of two spike-LFP phase vectors (U k,m ·U j,l) for a given pair of trials (m and l), and repeats this for all pairs of trials (Inline graphic and Inline graphic), such that the dot product is computed for all N(N − 1) combinations of two spike-LFP phase vectors. The numerator term − N accounts for the fact that the quadruple sum in Eq. (2.6) contains N dot products of phase vectors for which (k,m) = (j,l). The dot product of a spike-LFP phase vector with itself (the diagonal entries in Fig. 3(a)) is always one, and does not contribute to the Inline graphic statistic.

The PPC can be interpreted as quantifying the average coincidence between any two spikes in the LFP phase domain. Two spikes are maximally coincident in the LFP phase domain if their relative spike-LFP phase equals zero, and they are maximally non-coincident if their relative spike-LFP phase equals ±π. The dot product measures to what extent two spikes (with observed phases θ k,m and θ j,l) are coincident in the LFP phase domain, by outputting cos(θ k,m − θ j,l). For example, the dot product takes on the value − 1 if two spikes are maximally non-coincident, and +1 if two spikes are maximally coincident. The choice of the dot product function instead of the absolute angular distance (i.e., relative phase) as a measure of coincidence is motivated by the relationship P = R 2 between the PPC and the resultant vector length (Vinck et al. 2010b).

The expected value of Inline graphic is obtained by taking its expected value conditional on Inline graphic, which is required because the N m’s are random variables themselves, and subsequently taking the expected value over all possible realizations of Inline graphic. Assume that Inline graphic, because the PPC is not defined for N < 2, because we need at least one pair of spike-LFP phase vectors to compute the PPC. Using Assumptions 1 and 2, it follows that

graphic file with name M37.gif 2.8

Assumptions 1 and 2 allow us to replace the U k,m and U j,l terms by the independent and identically distributed random variables U (1) and U (2), such that the term n m n l appears in the numerator. By using Assumption 3 and Eq. (2.3), Eq. (2.8) simplifies to

graphic file with name M38.gif 2.9

Equation (2.9) shows that Inline graphic does not depend on Inline graphic. Hence, it follows that Inline graphic, showing that, given Assumptions 1–3, Inline graphic is an unbiased estimator of P. Note that while 0 ≤ P ≤ 1, we have Inline graphic.

If we make Assumptions 1–3, then P > 0 indicates that a neuron has a preferred LFP phase of firing, i.e. that spiking probability is modulated with LFP phase. To test whether the inequality P > 0 holds, one could for example use the Rayleigh test, or test whether the distribution of Inline graphic’s across neurons differs from zero. As will be shown in Sections 3 and 4, the validity of these statistical inferences is based on Assumptions 1–3 as laid out above.

The problem of statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases

The assumption that observed spike-LFP phases can be modeled as outcomes of independent random variables (Assumption 2) is typically violated. One cause of this violation is that neurons typically do not behave as homogenous or inhomogeneous Poisson sources (Shinomoto et al. 2009), because neurons have several physiological properties, e.g. refractoriness, autorhythmicity and burstiness, that cause non-Poissonian history effects (Section 3.2.1). Another reason for dependencies between spike-LFP phases to occur is that the LFP may not cover all phases uniformly in a given trial (Section 3.2.2). If we ignore that Assumption 2 is violated and directly apply circular statistics to the observed vector of spike-LFP phases as in Eqs. (2.5) and (2.6), then we may draw invalid conclusions about the relationship between spiking activity and the LFP, especially if the number of trials is small.

The problem posed by dependencies between spike-LFP phases is that the dot product term U k,m ·U j,l in Eq. (2.6) may be biased towards producing higher or lower values if l = m, i.e. if spike-LFP phases fall in the same trial, in comparison to the case l ≠ m, i.e. if spike-LFP phases fall in separate trials.

If we make Assumption 1, but not Assumption 2, then we can conclude that the statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases may especially have a large impact on point-field phase synchronization measures if the number of trials is relatively small, because the number of independent spike-LFP phases scales with N(N − 1)/2, whereas the number of possibly dependent spike-LFP phases scales only with approximately Inline graphic. For example, suppose that all spike-LFP phases within the same trial are statistically dependent, and that the number of spikes per trial equals N/M = 40. For one, two, and ten trials, the fraction of dependent spike-LFP phases equals 1, 0.5 and 0.1 respectively, and it tends to zero as M → ∞. The regime in which M is small can be of great importance to neurophysiology, e.g. when studying the involvement of rhythmic neuronal synchronization in memory and learning processes (e.g., van Wingerden et al. 2010b; Hansen and Dragoi 2011; Tort et al. 2009; Popescu et al. 2009).

Solution to the dependence problem

The goal of this section is to provide a solution to the problem that spike-LFP phases from the same trial can be statistically dependent. We solve the problem of statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases as follows. Instead of computing the dot product of spike-LFP phase vectors across all combinations of two spikes (Fig. 3(a)), we compute it only for combinations of spikes that were recorded from separate trials (Fig. 3(b)). Let Inline graphic. We define the point-field PPC Inline graphic estimator of P as

graphic file with name M48.gif 3.1

Note the similarity between Eqs. (2.6) and (3.1), whereas they differ in that the quadruple sum now runs only over all pairs of separate trials (Inline graphic and Inline graphic). If we make Assumption 3 and the assumption that Inline graphic, then the conditional expected value of this estimator equals

graphic file with name M52.gif 3.2

Assumption 1 allows to replace the U k,m and U j,l terms by the independent and identically distributed random variables U (1) and U (2), such that the term n m n l appears in the numerator. The product n m n l acts as a weighting factor, assigning heavier weight to expected values of spike-LFP phase vectors for trials with many spikes. However, if Assumption 3 holds, then this weighing factor falls away by division, and equation

graphic file with name M53.gif 3.3

holds. Thus, the Inline graphic statistic is not affected by a dependence between the spike-LFP phases (note that the derivation of Eqs. (3.1) and (3.2) does not make use of Assumption 2). This is our first main result, improving the point-field PPC Inline graphic measure. We will show in Section 5 that the PPC Inline graphic measure allows to derive an analytical relationship between point-field (Fig. 1) and spike train to field phase-synchronization measures (Fig. 2).

It should be noted, however, that the Inline graphic estimate cannot be obtained in case only a single trial is available. This problem may to some extent be circumvented by removing pairs of spike-LFP phase vectors on the basis of a time-difference threshold, i.e. by rejecting combinations of spikes if they lie too closely together in time. The same approach, i.e. rejection on the basis of a time-threshold, is possible for data-sets that are not divided into trial and inter-trial interval periods. Finally, it should be noted that the Inline graphic estimator has a higher variance than the Inline graphic estimator if Assumption 2 does hold, especially if the number of trials is small, because the computation of the Inline graphic statistic is based on more combinations of two spike-LFP phase vectors than the computation of the Inline graphic statistic.

Simulations of the effect of a statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases on the Inline graphic and Inline graphic estimators

Effect of refractoriness and burstiness

To study the effect of a statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases on point-field phase synchronization statistics, we performed simulations with the following model. Initially we assumed that neuronal firing was described as a Poisson process, with the instantaneous rate parameter defined by λ(ϕ(t)) = β/ 2 π, where the LFP-phase was defined as a linear function of time, i.e. ϕ(t) = 2πf t, such that there was no modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase. The probability of observing a spike in the interval [t,t + δ] was equal to the product δ λ(ϕ(t)) for small δ. Thus, spiking probability was a linear function of the parameter β, which equaled the expected number of spikes per oscillatory cycle. We assumed that the trial duration was of length k/f for integer k. In this model, spike-LFP phases can be considered as outcomes of independent random variables: The observation of a spike at a particular time-point does not change the probability density of firing at other time-points. Correspondingly, the observation of a spike at a particular LFP phase does not alter the probability density of firing at other LFP phases, and the point-field PPC Inline graphic would be an unbiased estimator.

To demonstrate the effect of a refractory period with simulations, we set λ(ϕ(t)) = 0 for the duration of the refractory period after a spike was fired. We chose f = 20 Hz, a time-window that comprised 500 time-steps of 0.0001 s (such that k = 1) and β = 100. Without a refractory period, the number of spikes equaled on average β·500 ·0.0001 = 5 per trial. We then examined two lengths of the refractory period: 8 (Fig. 4(a)) and 40 ms (Fig. 4(b)). Figure 4(a) and (b) show the expected Inline graphic value (solid line) as a function of the number of trials. Figure 4(a) shows that in the presence of a refractory period of 8 ms, the expected Inline graphic value attained negative values and approached its asymptotic value from below. This result can be explained as follows: If the refractory period is short in comparison to the oscillatory cycle period, then the observation of a particular spike-LFP phase makes the observation of similar spike-LFP phases less likely, causing the expected value of the dot product of spike-LFP phase vectors from the same trial to decrease. However, when the refractory period was relatively long in comparison to the oscillatory cycle length, then Inline graphic approached its asymptotic value from above (Fig. 4(b)). The latter result can be explained as follows: If the duration of the refractory period approaches the duration of the oscillatory cycle period, then the observation of a spike-LFP phase makes the observation of similar spike-LFP phases on average more likely, causing the expected value of the dot product of spike-LFP phase vectors from the same trial to increase. In contrast to the point-field Inline graphic measure, we did not observe any dependence of the Inline graphic measure on the number of trials. Thus, the Inline graphic measure correctly indicated, on expectation, that there was no modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Influence of dependence between spike-LFP phases on point-field phase synchronization measures. (a) Estimated, expected point-field PPC Inline graphic (solid), and point-field PPC Inline graphic values (dotted) (y-axis) as a function of the number of trials (x-axis), in the presence of an absolute refractory period of 8 ms. The grey portion of the solid Inline graphic curve highlights that no Inline graphic estimate exists when only one trial is available. (b) Similar to (a), but now in the presence of an absolute refractory period of 40 ms. (c) Influence of burstiness on point-field phase synchronization measures. Estimated expected point-field PPC Inline graphic (solid), and point-field PPC Inline graphic value (dotted) (y-axis) as a function of the number of trials (x-axis). For every spike fired, we added one extra spike with the same phase. (d) Influence of incomplete coverage of LFP phases on estimated, expected point-field PPC Inline graphic (solid), and point-field PPC Inline graphic values (dotted) (y-axis) as a function of number of trials (x-axis), with the neuron firing only during 20% of the cycle duration

To demonstrate the effect of burstiness we performed the following simulation. Instead of adding a refractory period to the Poisson model, we now added for every spike fired an additional spike with the same phase (i.e., duplicating all spike-LFP phases). Figure 4(c) shows that a strong negative dependence of the expected Inline graphic value on the number of trials emerged, such that Inline graphic approached its asymptotic population value from above. Even though the probability of firing was equal for all LFP phases, the expected Inline graphic value still attained positive values for a finite number of trials. Again, in contrast to the point-field Inline graphic measure, we did not observe any dependence of the Inline graphic measure on the number of trials. Thus, the Inline graphic measure correctly indicated, on expectation, that there was no modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase. The effect of bursting on the PPC may be explained as follows. The PPC quantifies coincidences of spikes in the LFP phase domain. Spikes from the same neuron are expected to be coincident in the LFP phase domain if the probability of spiking is modulated by LFP phase. Bursting can cause spikes from the same trial to have a certain amount of coincidence in the time domain. Because the LFP phase is an approximately linear function of time (e.g., see SI Fig. 1 in Canolty et al. 2010), this time-domain coincidence can cause spikes from the same trial to become coincident in the spike-LFP phase domain as well, even though spiking probability is not modulated with LFP phase.

Effect of non-uniform sampling of the LFP phases

Dependencies between spike-LFP phases can also occur because of statistical dependencies between instantaneous LFP phases within the same trial, i.e. can be a consequence of properties of the LFP signal instead of the spike train signal. For example, a dependence between instantaneous LFP phases occurs if the window length that is used to compute a point-field phase synchronization measure does not equal an integer multiple of 1/f, causing the instantaneous LFP phases to be non-uniformly covered across time. Consequently, given the observation of a particular instantaneous LFP phase at a certain time-point, regardless of whether a spike was fired there, we will be more likely to observe similar instantaneous LFP phases at other time-points within the same trial. This is exemplified by the simulation that was performed for Fig. 4(d). We set the trial duration to k/f = 0.01 s (in 100 steps of 0.0001 s) with k = 0.2, f = 20, β = 100, as above, but without further non-Poissonian history effects. The Inline graphic statistic was negatively biased by the number of trials (Fig. 4(d)), despite the fact that spike trains were generated according to a homogeneous Poisson process. In contrast to the point-field Inline graphic measure, we did not observe any dependence of the Inline graphic measure on the number of trials, and the Inline graphic measure correctly indicated that there was no modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase.

Taken together, the results from Section 3.2 show that dependencies between spike-LFP phases within the same trial can arise because of many factors. There are various consequences: (i) The equation Inline graphic does not hold in general. (ii) It is not recommendable to use the Rayleigh test to assess whether spiking probability is modulated with LFP phase because the test assumes that phases are outcomes of independent random variables. (iii) The equation Inline graphic can hold even in the absence of a modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase. (iv) The estimator Inline graphic is unbiased by the number of spikes only if the spike-LFP phases can be treated as outcomes of independent random variables. However, in practice, they usually cannot, and as a result Inline graphic can be both positively and negatively biased by the number of trials. Because the number of spikes per trial determines the ratio of the number of possibly dependent over the number of independent spike-LFP phases, it affects the Inline graphic estimator as well. In opposition to our previous claim (Vinck et al. 2010b), we conclude that the Inline graphic statistic is not an unbiased estimator if Assumption 2 is violated.

Effect of dependence between spike count and phase

We dealt with the violation of Assumption 2 by developing the point-field PPC Inline graphic measure. In order for this measure to be unbiased as a function of the number of trials, we need to make Assumption 3. However, in practice this assumption may be violated, which can introduce a positive or negative bias as a function of the number of trials.

There are two possible relationships between the spike count and the distribution of spike-LFP phases. Firstly, the consistency of spike-LFP phases and the spike count can be statistically dependent. For example, the strength of V1 gamma-band synchronization has been found to be positively dependent on stimulus size, while the V1 firing rate has been found to be negatively dependent on stimulus size (Gieselmann and Thiele 2008). To give an example of the effect of a negative dependence between spike-LFP phase consistency and firing rate on the point-field PPC Inline graphic, we studied the model

graphic file with name M97.gif 4.1

where g is the von Mises density, κ = 0.9, μ = 0, and β m was a uniformly distributed random variable on the interval [200, 600]. Spiking probability was a linear function of the parameter β m, which equaled the expected number of spikes per oscillatory cycle. The LFP phase was defined as

graphic file with name M98.gif 4.2

where ϵ(t) was a random variable uniformly distributed on the interval [0,1]. For large β m, a large error term was added to the LFP phase. Thus, small amplitude noise was added to spike-LFP phases when a trial contained a small number of spikes (i.e., β m small), and large amplitude noise was added when a trial contained a large number of spikes (i.e., β m large), thereby implementing a negative correlation between the firing rate and phase locking as in the V1 data (Gieselmann and Thiele 2008) mentioned above. Figure 5 shows that in this model, the Inline graphic measure was positively biased by the number of trials.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Influence of dependence of spike-LFP phase consistency and spike count on point-field phase synchronization measures. Estimated, expected point-field PPC Inline graphic (solid), and point-field PPC Inline graphic values (black, dotted) (y-axis) as a function of the number of trials (x-axis), where trials with a large number of spikes have a noisier phase distribution

Secondly, it can occur that the mean spike-LFP phase co-varies with the spike count. For example, in case of pooling trials across experimental conditions (e.g. orientations), it can occur that trials with higher firing rates correspond to an advanced spike gamma phase (Vinck et al. 2010a), which can bias the Inline graphic measure as a function of the number of trials.

A point-field measure unaffected by rate-phase dependencies

We will now define a statistic that is not affected by phase-spikecount dependencies, and hence not biased by the number of trials. We call the defined statistic the point-field PPC Inline graphic measure, and define it as

graphic file with name M104.gif 4.3

The key difference with the Inline graphic measure is that in Eq. (4.3), the sum of dot products across combinations of two spike-LFP phase vectors for a pair of separate trials (m,l) is first averaged, i.e. normalized by the number of combinations N m N l. The average dot product for a pair of separate trials is then averaged across all pairs of separate trials (Inline graphic and Inline graphic). Thus, in the computation of Inline graphic, a trial with few spikes will be given the same weight as a trial with many spikes. Consequently, spike-LFP phases from trials with few spikes are given a stronger weight than spike-LFP phases from trials with many spikes. The rationale behind the Inline graphic measure is the following: Because the number of combinations of two spike-LFP phase vectors equals N m N l for a pair of separate trials, both trials are given equal weight in the computation of the average dot product for that pair of trials. As a trivial example, suppose that the first trial contains 20 spikes and the second trial contains 100 spikes, then there are 100 ·20 = 2,000 combinations of spike-LFP phases containing spikes from the first trial, but also 2,000 combinations of spike-LFP phases containing spikes from the second trial. For a larger number of trials, the trials with a large number of spikes will contribute to more combinations of spike-LFP phase vectors for the Inline graphic statistic. For example if we add a third trial that contains 50 spikes, then the first trial will contribute to 20 ·100 + 20 ·50 = 3,000 combinations, but the second trial will contribute to 100 ·20 + 100 ·50 = 7,000 combinations of two spike-LFP phases. This occurs because the normalization term in Eq. (3.1) (Inline graphic) is a function of the n m’s itself, such that it is less influenced by a single n m if the number of trials is large. This explains why in Fig. 5, the Inline graphic measure is negatively dependent on the number of trials.

From Assumption 1, it follows that the expected value of Inline graphic conditional on Inline graphic, assuming that Inline graphic, equals

graphic file with name M116.gif 4.4

Note that, in comparison to Eq. (3.1), we removed the weighing factor n m n l.

The expected value over all possible outcomes of the spikecounts for which Inline graphic is then given as

graphic file with name M118.gif 4.5

The results of the simulation that are presented in Fig. 5 show that, indeed, phase-spikecount dependence does not lead to a bias of Inline graphic on the number of trials.

Note that

graphic file with name M120.gif 4.6

i.e. E { U (1) } corresponds to the expected spike-LFP phase vector, and is not affected by a potential dependence between spike count and spike-LFP phase. If Assumption 2 holds, then Inline graphic. Also, for Inline graphic, the equality Inline graphic holds. However, as Inline graphic, Inline graphic where the resultant vector E { Z } equals

graphic file with name M126.gif 4.7

Relationship between the point-field PPC Inline graphic and the spike train to field PPC

Definition of the spike train to field locking indices

We will now demonstrate a relationship between on the one hand the point-field Inline graphic and Inline graphic measures, and spike train-LFP phase synchronization measures on the other hand. Suppose we observe M trials of LFP and spike train signals. Again, let Θk,m be the relative phase between a concurrent LFP segment and an individual spike at a particular frequency, with the spike-LFP phase now defined relative to the concurrent LFP segment (Fig. 2), i.e., not in a centered fashion as in Section 2.

For all Inline graphic, the cross-spectrum between spike train and LFP in a given trial is then defined as

graphic file with name M131.gif 5.1

where A m is defined as the magnitude of the Fourier spectrum of the mth LFP signal at frequency f and the spikes are assigned unit energy at frequency f. The single-trial resultant length over the individual spike-LFP phases is defined as

graphic file with name M132.gif 5.2

where Inline graphic. Note that 0 ≤ R m ≤ 1. The power of the spike-train (at a given frequency) equals Inline graphic. If at a given frequency all the spikes are in phase, then the power of the spike train equals Inline graphic. The single-trial circular mean spike-LFP phase is defined as

graphic file with name M136.gif 5.3

and the variables N m, R m and Inline graphic are related by the equation

graphic file with name M138.gif 5.4

where Inline graphic. Finally, we define the phase vector Inline graphic, which is depicted in Fig. 2. Note that Inline graphic is the relative phase between spike train and LFP for the mth trial. We define the spike train to field PLV as the resultant length across spike train to field phases, i.e. Inline graphic. The spike train to field PLV is a quantity closely related to the ‘spike-field coherence’ (e.g., see Jarvis and Mitra 2001), except for the fact that the former ignores amplitude co-variations and assigns an equal weight to every trial that contains at least one spike, while the latter weighs the trials according to the product of LFP and spike train spectral magnitude, i.e. A m N m R m (at a given frequency), and is a concurrent measure of amplitude co-variations. We may quantify the consistency of relative phases across trials by directly using the PPC, which removes the bias of the spike train to field PLV, by defining

graphic file with name M143.gif 5.5

and we refer to this measure as the uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic. By normalizing by Inline graphic, the measure takes into account that no predictions about the LFP phase can be made if a trial does not contain spikes. Let Inline graphic be a random variable identically distributed to Inline graphic for all Inline graphic. The statistic Inline graphic is an unbiased estimator of its corresponding population value, which is defined as Inline graphic (Vinck et al. 2010b).

It may occur that neuronal output is highly predictive of LFP phase, but that a neuron fires only in a small number of trials, such that the predictive value of the spike train across all trials is much lower than indicated by the Inline graphic measure. To accommodate this, and to assess to what extent spike trains predict the LFP phase across all trials, we define the measure

graphic file with name M152.gif 5.6

Note that because Inline graphic, the inequality Inline graphic holds.

In Eq. (5.5), all Inline graphic relative phases are assigned the same weight. However, the pairwise statistic may be turned into a weighted pairwise statistic, similar to the weighted phase-lag index in Vinck et al. (2011), defining

graphic file with name M156.gif 5.7

If we define W m = R m N m A m, then the expected value of the numerator equals the numerator of the squared spike-field coherence (e.g. see Jarvis and Mitra 2001). A special case of weighing is obtained when we let W m = R m N m, i.e., the spectral magnitude of the spike train at a particular frequency, defining

graphic file with name M157.gif 5.8

We refer to this measure as the uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic . The rationale behind this weighted measure is that every individual spike-LFP phase is given an equal weight by weighing by N m N l, as will be shown below. Further, we will show that the weighting factor R m R l can be used to correct for the number of spikes per trial.

Relationship between point-field PPC and spike train to field PPC

We now derive a relationship between the point-field Inline graphic measure and the Inline graphic measure. We observe that the equations

graphic file with name M161.gif 5.9

hold.

The final expression in Eq. (5.9) is nearly identical to the right-hand expression in Eq. (5.8). We define the corrected (using the abbreviation ‘corr.’) spike train to field PPC Inline graphic as

graphic file with name M163.gif 5.10

Note that this measure is, similar to the Inline graphic statistic, not unbiased as a function of the number of trials, unless we make the assumption that the single spike-LFP phases are independent of the spike count (Assumption 3). Similarly, we define the corrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic as

graphic file with name M166.gif 5.11

Similar to the Inline graphic measure, this measure is not biased by the number of trials, even if Assumptions 2 and 3 are violated. The main difference between the Inline graphic and the Inline graphic measure is that the former assigns an equal weight to every spike-LFP phase (i.e., Θk,m), by weighing the influence of a trial by N m N l, whereas the latter assigns an equal weight to every phase obtained for a trial (i.e., Inline graphic), thereby weighing those spikes heavier that occurred in trials with few spikes.

If we define the individual spike-LFP phases in an identical way for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, then the equality Inline graphic holds and the measures have mathematically equivalent definitions. Thus, we conclude that there exists a simple mathematical relationship between point-field PPC Inline graphic and the uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic. Because the point-field Inline graphic and Inline graphic measures are unaffected by the number of spikes per trial, we conclude that the same property holds for the measures Inline graphic and Inline graphic respectively, solving the problem that their respective uncorrected counterparts can strongly depend on the number of spikes. We may thus regard Inline graphic and Inline graphic as measures of both the consistency of individual spike-LFP phases and of the relative phases between spike trains and LFP signals across trials, with a correction factor for the number of spikes per trial.

The definitions of Inline graphic and Inline graphic are equivalent to the definitions of their respective spike train to field counterparts Inline graphic and Inline graphic, expect for the fact that the individual spike-LFP phases are not computed in the same way. We predict that, as a consequence of this difference in phase estimation, the inequalities Inline graphic and Inline graphic typically hold if Inline graphic is small. This prediction has the following rationale: For spike train to field phase-synchronization measures, spikes can fall at arbitrary positions relative to the concurrent LFP segment (Fig. 2), such that they sometimes fall close to the borders of the concurrent LFP segment. On the other hand, for point-field phase-synchronization measures, they fall per definition at the center of the LFP segment that is used to compute the spike-LFP phase (Fig. 1). The instantaneous LFP phase is not a strict linear function of time, but decorrelates over time, because neuronal (e.g. gamma or theta) oscillations contain energy over a relatively large range of frequencies. We assume that spiking probability at a given time-point is especially modulated by the local fluctuations in the LFP around that time-point, but not by distant past or future fluctuations in the LFP. The decorrelation of LFP phase across time will then cause a decrease in the magnitude of the side-lobes of the spike-triggered LFP average as a function of time (e.g. see Fig. 1H-I in Fries et al. 2001). If a spike falls at the border of an LFP segment, then its spike-LFP phase is determined predominantly from either past or future LFP fluctuations, and will be computed based on a larger portion of the LFP signal to which the spikes are not locked. For the point-field measure, the spike-LFP phase will be computed based on a larger portion of the LFP signal to which the spikes are locked. To counter the decorrelation in LFP phases across time, the LFP segment could be decreased in duration. However, this would decrease the frequency resolution, and thereby increase the amount of spectral leakage.

Dependence of spike train to field PPC on the mean and variance of the spike count

From the properties of the resultant length R m it follows that Inline graphic and Inline graphic are statistically dependent on Inline graphic. The equation 0 ≤ R m ≤ 1 holds, and R m is positively biased by N m, i.e. approaches its asymptotic value from above (e.g. see Vinck et al. 2010b). Thus, given a fixed distribution of individual spike-LFP phases, it follows that the expected values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic are positively dependent on the expected value of N m, consistent with previous results on the spike-field coherence (Zeitler et al. 2006; Lepage et al. 2011). There are two cases in which Inline graphic and Inline graphic are approximately equal. Firstly, if for all m ∈ { 1, ..., M }, Inline graphic, then Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Secondly, if the consistency of the individual spike-LFP phases is very high, then R m is close to 1, and Inline graphic. However, if the consistency of the phases is very low and the number of spikes per trial is large, then Inline graphic will be small, yet much larger than Inline graphic (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Comparison of uncorrected and corrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic. (a) Y-axis: corrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic (dotted) and uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic (solid). X-axis: number of spikes per trial. Different lines represent different coupling strength, with the von Mises dispersion parameter κ ∈ { 0.1, 0.5, 1, 20 }. (b) Empirically observed and analytically predicted correction factor for spike train to field PPC Inline graphic (y-axis) as a function of the number of spikes per trial (x-axis), again for κ ∈ { 0.1, 0.5, 1, 20 }

These relationships can be intuitively explained as follows. The statistics Inline graphic and Inline graphic measure the consistency of relative phases across trials. For a given trial, the computation of a relative phase is based on averaging across multiple single spike-LFP phases. The average of multiple single spike-LFP phases is more consistent than the single spike-LFP phases themselves, because averaging will remove variance across the single spike-LFP phases. If the single spike-LFP phases are only weakly consistent, then the average over many single spike-LFP phases will be much more consistent than the single spike-LFP phases themselves.

However, the expression R m R l is a function not only of N m and N l, but also of the statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases. If spike-LFP phases become, ceteris paribus, more positively (negatively) dependent, then the expected value of R m will increase (decrease), and consequently the expected values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic will decrease (increase). Thus, the expected values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic depend on three main factors, namely the number of spike-LFP phases per trial, the statistical dependence of the individual spike-LFP phases, and the consistency of the individual spike-LFP phases. The Inline graphic and Inline graphic measures remove the dependence on the first two factors and offer a purer measure of the last factor.

To demonstrate the relationship between the expected value of Inline graphic and the number of spikes by simulations, we drew samples of spike-LFP phases from a von Mises distribution with dispersion parameter k ∈ { 0.1, 0.5, 1, 20 }. The number of spikes per trial N m was fixed, such that for all m, N m ∈ { 1, ..., 100 }. The number of trials M equaled 100. Figure 6(a) shows the estimated expected values of Inline graphic (dotted) and Inline graphic (solid) as a function of the number of spikes per trial. The Inline graphic measure attained values independently of N m. For N m = 1, the equality Inline graphic held. As N m increased, Inline graphic strongly increased. The ratio Inline graphic indeed had the form of the correction factor in Eqs. (5.10) (Fig. 6(b)). Note that for large expected values of Inline graphic, Inline graphic rapidly approached one, while it rose linearly with the number of spikes per trial for a small expected value of Inline graphic. This linear relationship implies that an x% increase in firing rate, without a concurrent change in the distribution of single spike-LFP phases, entails an x% increase in uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic for low expected values of Inline graphic and a relatively small number of spikes per trial.

If our aim is to compare Inline graphic or Inline graphic between two experimental conditions that have a different distribution of spike counts, then it is insufficient that the means of the spike counts are equal. As we will now show, other parameters of the distribution of spike counts, especially the variance, should be equal as well, and these parameters may very well differ between conditions. For example, the variance of spike counts can be a function of attention (e.g. Mitchell et al. 2007). To demonstrate the influence of the spike count variance (across trials), we compared two conditions with an equal average spike count, but with a different variance of the spike counts. Phase samples were drawn from a von Mises distribution with dispersion parameter κ = 1, and we set the number of trials M = 100. In the first condition, the variance of the counts equaled zero (zero-variance condition). In the second condition, the spike count was distributed according to a Poisson distribution, i.e. the spike count variance equaled the mean spike count (Poisson condition). Figure 7 shows the difference in Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic between the Poisson and the zero-variance condition, as a function of the expected spike count per trial, E { N m }. The Inline graphic measure attained higher values for the Poisson distribution than for the zero-variance condition. The cause of this difference is that the weights N m in Eq. (5.8) give a stronger weight to trials with more spikes, and that the average spike-LFP phase obtained from these trials is more reliable. The Inline graphic measure on the other hand showed two opposite trends. Because trials with zero spikes (i.e., N m = 0) are left out in Eq. (5.5), the effective firing rate was higher for the Poisson condition, which increased the value of Inline graphic for the Poisson condition for small E { N m }. However, the results of Fig. 6(a) imply that the consistency of Inline graphic does not depend linearly on N m, but is a concave function of N m. This concavity led to a relative decrease in the value of Inline graphic for the Poisson condition, an effect which became dominant as E { N m } ≥ 5. For the Inline graphic measure, only the latter effect was present, causing the Inline graphic measure to be higher for the zero-variance condition than for the Poisson condition.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7

Influence of firing rate variance on spike train to field phase synchronization measures. Dashed: spike train to field PPC Inline graphic for Poisson distribution minus spike train to field PPC Inline graphic for zero-variance distribution, as a function of the number of spikes per trial. Positive values indicate a larger Inline graphic value for Poisson distribution. Solid: as for dashed, but now for spike train to field PPC Inline graphic. Dashed-dotted: as solid, but now for spike train to field PPC Inline graphic

Discussion

Relationship between firing rate and uncorrected spike train to field PPC measures

We demonstrated that the spike train to field PPC measures can be corrected for the positive dependence on the number of spikes per trial. However, correcting for this positive dependence does not preclude the possibility that the consistency of the single spike-LFP phases (i.e., point-field phase-synchronization) is correlated with the firing rate. Still, the latter correlation would be the result of a physiological relationship, and a change in the firing rate could be the consequence of a change in spike-LFP phase-coupling as well. On the other hand, the positive effect of the firing rate on the uncorrected spike train to field PPC measures is a result of a statistical relationship, i.e. higher (lower) spike train to field phase-synchronization appears because a neuron has more (less) spikes per trial.

No a priori statements can be made about the correlation between the consistency of single spike-LFP phases and the firing rate. Independent variation can be caused by subsampling spike trains, or by a failure of one’s measurement or spike-sorting system to detect a spike. A negative correlation between the firing rate and the consistency of spike-LFP phases may be a result of an increase in the amplitude of inputs that are not coherent with the LFP (Zeitler et al. 2006), or it may be a consequence of the inhibitory nature of the rhythm generators (Gieselmann and Thiele 2008; van Wingerden et al. 2010a; Donoghue et al. 1998; Jensen and Mazaheri 2010). In case of such a negative correlation, it can occur that the point-field PPC measures decrease while the increase in firing rate causes a simultaneous increase in the uncorrected spike train to field PPC measures. A positive correlation between point-field phase synchronization and firing rate can be found in visual cortex, where visual stimulation and high contrast stimuli can increase both firing rates and the precision of gamma-band synchronization (Gray et al. 1989; Henrie and Shapley 2005). Selective visual attention can increase both gamma-band synchronization and firing rate (Fries et al. 2001, 2008; Gregoriou et al. 2009) in V4 and FEF (but not in V1 (Chalk et al. 2010)). If the firing rate is positively correlated with the consistency of individual spike-LFP phases, then both the point-field PPC measures and the uncorrected spike train to field PPC measures increase, although the proportional increase in the value of the latter will typically be larger.

Comparison between single-unit field and multi-unit field phase synchronization measures

Spike-field phase synchronization measures can be based either on single unit activity (SUA), or multi unit activity (MUA). Because MUA contains more spikes per second than SUA, and because spiking activity of different neurons is partially driven by uncorrelated inputs which renders their respective spike-LFP phases more independent, MUA-field measures will in general attain higher uncorrected spike train to field PPC values, as shown by Zeitler et al. (2006).

This relative gain of the MUA is not present for the point-field PPC and corrected spike train to field PPC measures however. In fact, we predict that the point-field PPC measures typically attain lower values for MUA-field pairs than SUA-field pairs: Different neuronal cell types, e.g. fast spiking basket cells and pyramidal cells, fire preferentially at different LFP phases (Klausberger et al. 2003; Tukker et al. 2007; Csicsvari et al. 2003; Hasenstaub et al. 2005; Okun and Lampl 2008). Furthermore, the preferred LFP-phase of spiking of neurons in the hippocampus and visual cortex is input-dependent (Vinck et al. 2010a; O’Keefe and Recce 1993). This divergence of preferred LFP-phases, a factor that was ignored in Zeitler et al. (2006), tends to broaden the distribution of individual spike-LFP phases for the MUA. The effect of linearly mixing spiking activity from different cells is similar to linearly mixing trials across experimental conditions with different mean spike-LFP phases (Section 3). Thus, MUA-field point-field synchronization will typically be weaker than SUA-field point-field synchronization.

Comparison of point-field PPC Inline graphic and PPC Inline graphic

In Sections 6.3, 6.4 and 6.5, we will discuss the advantages and disadvantages of the developed measures, whose properties are summarized in Table 1. The Inline graphic estimator (Eq. (3.1)) solves the problem that the Inline graphic measure can be affected by dependencies between spike-LFP phases from the same trial. We believe that the effect of dependencies between spike-LFP phases on the Inline graphic measure should be considered undesirable, and that the Inline graphic measure is therefore to be preferred. It should be emphasized however that e.g. refractoriness or burstiness can cause a true change in the consistency of spike-LFP phases, which should be detected by a point-field phase synchronization measure. However, besides changing the consistency of spike-LFP phases, refractoriness and burstiness also introduce a statistical dependence between spike-LFP phases from the same trial, an effect which should be distinguished from the change in the distribution of spike-LFP phases. For a large number of trials, Inline graphic will be exclusively determined by the modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase. For a small number of trials, Inline graphic can also determined by the dependencies between spike-LFP phases. For example, spiking probability may not be modulated with LFP phase, however burstiness can cause Inline graphic to exceed zero. The question that we are interested in is whether neurons tend to fire consistently at particular phases of the LFP oscillation, i.e., whether the ongoing phase of the LFP can be predicted from the observation of a spike. However, a dependence between spike-LFP phases will affect the Inline graphic measure even if we replace the LFP by a white noise signal that bears no relationship to neuronal firing. The availability of a phase consistency measure that alleviates the dependency from firing rate and history-effects goes in the same direction as the derivation of statistically independent measure of single cell spiking intensity and cell-cell interaction in the information geometry paradigm (Nakahara et al. 2006).

Table 1.

Summary of performance different phase synchronization indices

  Dependent on number of spikes? Effect of dependence between spike-LFP phases? Effect of dependence between spikecount and phase? Biased by number trials? Relationship with other statistics
Point-field PLV (Inline graphic). +/− +/− +/− As M → ∞, Inline graphic
Point-field PPC Inline graphic 0 +/− +/− +/− As M → ∞, Inline graphic
Point-field PPC Inline graphic 0 0 +/− +/− If spike count and spike-LFP phase are independent, then Inline graphic and Inline graphic
Point-field PPC Inline graphic 0 0 0 0
Spike train to field PLV + +/− 0 As M → ∞, goes to Inline graphic
Uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic + +/− +/− +/− Approximately a factor of Inline graphic larger than Inline graphic where Inline graphic is the average single-trial resultant length.
Uncorrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic + +/− 0 0 About a factor of Inline graphic larger than Inline graphic where Inline graphic is the average single-trial resultant length.
Corrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic 0 0 +/− +/− Only difference to Inline graphic lies in phase-estimation
Corrected spike train to field PPC Inline graphic 0 0 0 0 Only difference to Inline graphic lies in phase-estimation

‘+/−’: means that effect can be positive or negative. ‘0’: means that statistic is not affected

Comparison of point-field PPC Inline graphic and PPC Inline graphic

The Inline graphic (Eq. (4.3)) estimator differs from the Inline graphic measure in that the former cannot be affected by dependencies between the spike-LFP phase and the spike count. Whether or not Inline graphic is a better measure than Inline graphic depends on the properties of the data, and our measurement goals. If there is no relationship between the spike count and the spike-LFP phase distribution, then the Inline graphic estimator is to be preferred above the Inline graphic estimator, because the former has a smaller estimator variance. Whether spike count and spike-LFP phase are indeed dependent may be tested by using the data itself. On the other hand, if there is a relationship between the spike count and the spike-LFP phase distribution, then the Inline graphic and Inline graphic measures estimate a different population parameter. The question is then, what do Inline graphic and Inline graphic measure?

The Inline graphic measure gives an answer to the following question: To what extent is the probability of observing a spike modulated with LFP phase? This follows from the fact that it simply counts how many spikes fell at a particular phase, and characterizes that distribution. The probability of observing a spike in a small interval [ϕ(t), ϕ(t) + δ] is approximately equal to

graphic file with name M287.gif 6.1

where s is a binary variable indicating the occurrence of a spike, f(ϕ) is a probability density function modeling the modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase ϕ(t) ∈ [ − π, π]. The modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase can then be quantified by the resultant vector length

graphic file with name M288.gif 6.2

If for all n m ≥ 1 we let Inline graphic, then this equation equals Eq. (4.7), i.e. the asymptotic value that the Inline graphic measure is estimating.

The question that the Inline graphic measure answers is the following: If we know that a spike occurred at a certain time-point, then to what extent can the LFP-phase at the time of spiking be predicted, without further knowledge of the spike count? In this case, the spike has already occurred, and the estimated phase vector is given by Eq. (4.6). If we would have used information about the spike count then the expected spike-LFP phase would have changed if spike count and spike-LFP phase were statistically dependent on each other. Thus, the Inline graphic measure tells us to what extent we can predict the instantaneous LFP-phase from the occurrence of a spike, if we ignore the information in the spike count, i.e. if we assume that all spike counts are equally likely.

There are two cautionary remarks about the use of the Inline graphic measure however. Firstly, when we mix experimental conditions it should be realized that the average will then be dominated by the experimental conditions with the most spikes. Secondly, while the Inline graphic measure is not biased by the number of spikes or dependencies between spike-LFP phases, it can still be biased as a function of the number of trials. The fact that the Inline graphic measure may in that case have some statistical bias can be dealt with by using e.g. randomization statistics (e.g. Maris et al. 2007). On the other hand, a statistical dependence between spike count and spike-LFP phase cannot ‘create’ an expected Inline graphic value that exceeds zero: If for all trials, firing probability is not modulated with LFP phase, then Inline graphic.

The Inline graphic measure may sometimes detect a relationship between spiking probability and the LFP phase, while the Inline graphic measure does not, because the LFP phase preference of a neuron can sometimes become visible only over trials. For example, if a neuron has a firing rate non-stationarity then the modulation of spiking probability with LFP phase may only become apparent over trials.

Comparison of point-field and corrected spike train to field phase synchronization measures

The corrected spike train to field phase synchronization measures solved the problem that the uncorrected spike train to field phase synchronization measures are strongly dependent on the spike count distribution. The only difference between the point-field and corrected spike train to field phase synchronization measures is that the latter define the phase in a non-centered way, whereas the former define spike-LFP phases in a centered way. In fact, both measures are mathematically equivalent if the individual spike-LFP phases are defined in the same manner. In practice, this means that both measures are roughly equivalent if the windows that are used to determine spike-LFP phases are equally long, provided that the LFP phase does not decorrelate on a too rapid time-scale. An advantage of the spike train to field approach is that it is computationally more efficient, because in case of point-field PPC measures, the instantaneous LFP phase needs to be determined for every spike separately.

If the spike-LFP phases are defined in a centered way, then the point-field PPC measures to what extent the occurrence of a spike can be predicted from the instantaneous, ongoing LFP phase, or, vice versa, to what extent the instantaneous LFP phase can be predicted from the observation of a spike. Spike train to field phase-synchronization measures, with the phase defined in a non-centered way, indicate to what extent the phase of an LFP signal at arbitrary moments in time, including those moments in time where no spike was observed, can be predicted from the observation of the concurrent spike train. Because spike train to field phase-synchronization measures are inspired on the classical EEG-EEG coherence approach (by taking the complete spike train signal) we expect them to be more closely related to EEG-EEG measures or the coherence of the membrane potential of a neuron to the LFP.

The measures and estimation procedures described in this paper are made available in the open source FieldTrip toolbox (http://www.ru.nl/neuroimaging/fieldtrip).

References

  1. Abeles M. Role of the cortical neuron: Integrator or coincidence detector? Israel Journal of Medical Sciences. 1982;18:83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adey WR, Walter DO, Hendrix CE. Computer techniques in correlation and spectral analyses of cerebral slow waves during discriminative behavior. Experimental Neurology. 1961;3:501–524. doi: 10.1016/s0014-4886(61)80002-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Azouz R, Gray CM. Adaptive coincidence detection and dynamic gain control in visual cortical neurons in vivo. Neuron. 2003;37:513–523. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01186-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Battaglia FP, Benchenane K, Sirota A, Pennartz CM, Wiener SI. The hippocampus: Hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2011;15:310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benchenane K, Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Tierney PL, Gioanni Y, Battaglia FP, et al. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal- prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron. 2010;66:921–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: A gateway to memory and attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;3:475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Besserve M, Scholkopf B, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S. Causal relationships between frequency bands of extracellular signals in visual cortex revealed by an information theoretic analysis. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2010;29:547–566. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0236-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bichot NP, Rossi AF, Desimone R. Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area v4. Science. 2005;308:529–534. doi: 10.1126/science.1109676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Börgers C, Kopell NJ. Gamma oscillations and stimulus selection. Neural Computation. 2008;20:383–414. doi: 10.1162/neco.2007.07-06-289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bragin A, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, Hetke J, Wise K, Buzsáki G. Gamma (40–100 hz) oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:47–60. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00047.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown EN, Kass RE, Mitra PP. Multiple neural spike train data analysis: State-of-the-art and future challenges. Nature. Neuroscience. 2004;7:456–461. doi: 10.1038/nn1228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, Desimone R. Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:11262–11267. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011284108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burchell TR, Faulkner HJ, Whittington MA. Gamma frequency oscillations gate temporally coded afferent inputs in the rat hippocampal slice. Neuroscience Letters. 1998;255:151–154. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00676-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315:1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buzsáki G. Neural syntax: Cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron. 2010;68:362–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Canolty RT, Ganguly K, Kennerley SW, Cadieu CF, Koepsell K, Wallis JD, et al. Oscillatory phase coupling coordinates anatomically dispersed functional cell assemblies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:17356–17361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008306107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, et al. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chalk M, Herrero JL, Gieselmann MA, Delicato LS, Gotthardt S, Thiele A. Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in v1. Neuron. 2010;66:114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Colgin L, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, et al. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462:353–357. doi: 10.1038/nature08573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Csicsvari J, Jamieson B, Wise KD, Buzsaki G. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Neuron. 2003;37:311–322. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01169-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Denker M, Roux S, Linden H, Diesmann M, Riehle A, Grun S. The local field potential reflects surplus spike synchrony. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;1111:1111. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Destexhe A, Contreras D, Steriade M. Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:4595–4608. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04595.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Donoghue JP, Sanes JN, Hatsopoulos NG, Gaal G. Neural discharge and local field potential oscillations in primate motor cortex during voluntary movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;79:159–173. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eeckman FH, Freeman WJ. Correlations between unit firing and eeg in the rat olfactory system. Brain Research. 1990;528:238–244. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91663-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fries P, Nikoli D, Singer W. The gamma cycle. Trends in Neuroscience. 2007;30:309–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fries P, Roelfsema PR, Engel AK, König P, Singer W. Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1997;94:12699–12704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fries P, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Desimone R. The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque area V4. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:4823–4835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4499-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gielen S, Krupa M, Zeitler M. Gamma oscillations as a mechanism for selective information transmission. Biological Cybernetics. 2010;103:151–165. doi: 10.1007/s00422-010-0390-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gieselmann MA, Thiele A. Comparison of spatial integration and surround suppression characteristics in spiking activity and the local field potential in macaque V1. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:447–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06358.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Grasse D, Moxon K. Correcting the bias of spike field coherence estimators due to a finite ignoreNumber of spikes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:548–558. doi: 10.1152/jn.00610.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. 1989;338:334–337. doi: 10.1038/338334a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gulyas AI, Szabo GG, Ulbert I, Holderith N, Monyer H, Erdelyi F, et al. Parvalbumin-containing fast-spiking basket cells generate the field potential oscillations induced by cholinergic receptor activation in the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:15134–15145. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4104-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hansen BJ, Dragoi V. Adaptation-induced synchronization in laminar cortical circuits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:10720–10725. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102017108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Harris K, Henze D, Hirase H, Leinekugel X, Dragoi G, Czurkó A, et al. Spike train dynamics predicts theta-related phase precession in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature. 2002;417:738–741. doi: 10.1038/nature00808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW. Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature. 2009;461:941–946. doi: 10.1038/nature08499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hasenstaub A, Shu Y, Haider B, Kraushaar U, Duque A, McCormick DA. Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials carry synchronized frequency information in active cortical networks. Neuron. 2005;47:423–435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Havenith MN, Yu S, Biederlack J, Chen NH, Singer W, Nikolic D. Synchrony makes neurons fire in sequence, and stimulus properties determine who is ahead. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8570–8584. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2817-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Henrie J, Shapley R. LFP power spectra in V1 cortex: The graded effect of stimulus contrast. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:479–490. doi: 10.1152/jn.00919.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Huxter JR, Senior TJ, Allen K, Csicsvari J. Theta phase-specific codes for two-dimensional position, trajectory and heading in the hippocampus. Nature. Neuroscience. 2008;11:587–594. doi: 10.1038/nn.2106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jacobs J, Kahana MJ, Ekstrom AD, Fried I. Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:3839–3844. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4636-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jarvis MR, Mitra PP. Sampling properties of the spectrum and coherency of sequences of action potentials. Neural Computation. 2001;13:717–749. doi: 10.1162/089976601300014312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kayser C, Montemurro MA, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S. Spike-phase coding boosts and stabilizes information carried by spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron. 2009;61:597–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Marton LF, Roberts JD, Cobden PM, Buzsaki G, et al. Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature. 2003;421:844–848. doi: 10.1038/nature01374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Koepsell K, Wang X, Hirsch JA, Sommer FT. Exploring the function of neural oscillations in early sensory systems. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2010;4:53. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.010.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. König P, Engel AK, Roelfsema PR, Singer W. How precise is neuronal synchronization? Neural Computation. 1995;7:469–485. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.3.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kopell N, Whittington MA, Kramer MA. Neuronal assembly dynamics in the beta1 frequency range permits short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:3779–3784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019676108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Human Brain Mapping. 1999;8:194–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4&#x0003c;194::AID-HBM4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lakatos P, Chen CM, O’Connell MN, Mills A, Schroeder CE. Neuronal oscillations and multisensory interaction in primary auditory cortex. Neuron. 2007;53:279–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lepage KQ, Kramer MA, Eden UT. The dependence of spike field coherence on expected intensity. Neural Computation. 2011;23:2209–2241. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Logothetis NK. The underpinnings of the bold functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3371–3963. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-03963.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Maris E, Schoffelen JM, Fries P. Nonparametric statistical testing of coherence differences. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007;163:161–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mehta MR, Lee AK, Wilson MA. Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code. Nature. 2002;417:741–746. doi: 10.1038/nature00807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mitchell J, Sundberg K, Reynolds J. Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron. 2007;55:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mitchell J, Sundberg K, Reynolds J. Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4. Neuron. 2009;63:879–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mitra P, Bokil H. Observed brain dynamics. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  60. Mitra PP, Pesaran B. Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophysical Journal. 1999;76:691–708. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77236-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mitzdorf U. Current source-density method and application in cat cerebral cortex: Investigation of evoked potentials and EEG phenomena. Physiological Review. 1985;65:37–100. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1985.65.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Montemurro MA, Rasch MJ, Murayama Y, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S. Phase-of-firing coding of natural visual stimuli in primary visual cortex. Current Biology. 2008;18:375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Nadasdy Z. Binding by asynchrony: The neuronal phase code. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2010;4:51. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nakahara H, Amari S, Richmond BJ. A comparison of descriptive models of a single spike train by information-geometric measure. Neural Computation. 2006;18:545–568. doi: 10.1162/089976606775623289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nolte G, Bai O, Wheaton L, Mari Z, Vorbach S, Hallett M. Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115:2292–2307. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nunez PL, Srinivasan R. Electric fields of the brain: The neurophysics of EEG. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  67. O’Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–330. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Okun M, Lampl I. Instantaneous correlation of excitation and inhibition during ongoing and sensory-evoked activities. Nature. Neurosciece. 2008;11:535–537. doi: 10.1038/nn.2105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Okun M, Naim A, Lampl I. The subthreshold relation between cortical local field potential and neuronal firing unveiled by intracellular recordings in awake rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4440–4448. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Panzeri S, Brunel N, Logothetis NK, Kayser C. Sensory neural codes using multiplexed temporal scales. Trends in Neuroscience. 2010;33:111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Pesaran B, Pezaris JS, Sahani M, Mitra PP, Andersen RA. Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nature. Neuroscience. 2002;5:805–811. doi: 10.1038/nn890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen. RA. Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature. 2008;453:406–409. doi: 10.1038/nature06849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Popescu AT, Popa D, Pare D. Coherent gamma oscillations couple the amygdala and striatum during learning. Nature. Neuroscience. 2009;12:801–807. doi: 10.1038/nn.2305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ray S, Hsiao SS, Crone NE, Franaszczuk PJ, Niebur E. Effect of stimulus intensity on the spike-local field potential relationship in the secondary somatosensory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7334–7343. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1588-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ray S, Maunsell JHR. Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation. Neuron. 2010;67:885–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Reimer J, Hatsopoulos NG. Periodicity and evoked responses in motor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:11506–11515. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5947-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: How top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. 2007;316:1612–1615. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Salinas E, Sejnowski TJ. Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nature Review. Neuroscience. 2001;2:539–550. doi: 10.1038/35086012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Schmidt R, Diba K, Leibold C, Schmitz D, Buzsaki G, Kempter R. Single-trial phase precession in the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:13232–13241. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2270-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shinomoto S, Kim H, Shimokawa T, Matsuno N, Funahashi S, Shima K, et al. Relating neuronal firing patterns to functional differentiation of cerebral cortex. PLoS Computational Biology. 2009;5:1000433. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA. Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal theta oscillations. Neuron. 2005;46:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Siegel M, Warden MR, Miller EK. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;464:763–767. doi: 10.1038/nature08855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Singer W. Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron. 1999;24:49–65. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80821-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsáki G. Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron. 2008;60:683–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. 2009;459:698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. Phase lag index: Assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28:1178–1193. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Tiesinga P, Sejnowski T. Mechanisms for phase shifting in cortical networks and their role in communication through coherence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:196. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tort AB, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, Eichenbaum H. Theta-gamma coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:20942–20947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911331106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tukker JJ, Fuentealba P, Hartwich K, Somogyi P, Klausberger T. Cell type-specific tuning of hippocampal interneuron firing during gamma oscillations in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:8184–8189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1685-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. van der Meer MAA, Redish AD. Low and high gamma oscillations in rat ventral striatum have distinct relationships to behavior, reward, and spiking activity on a learned spatial decision task. FrontIntegrNeurosci. 2009;3:9. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.009.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. van Wingerden M, Vinck M, Lankelma J, Pennartz C. Learning-associated gamma-band phase-locking of action-outcome selective neurons in orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10025–10038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0222-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. van Wingerden M, Vinck M, Lankelma J, Pennartz C. Theta-band phase-locking of orbitofrontal neurons during reward expectancy. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:7078–7087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3860-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. VanRullen R, Guyonneau R, Thorpe SJ. Spike times make sense. Trends in Neuroscience. 2005;28:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Review. Neuroscience. 2001;2:229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vinck M, Lima B, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Neuenschwander S, et al. Gamma-phase shifting in awake monkey visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:1250–1257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1623-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Vinck M, Oostenveld R, van Wingerden M, Battaglia F, Pennartz CMA. A new improved index of phase-synchronization in the presence of noise, volume-conduction and sample-size bias. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1548–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Vinck M, van Wingerden M, Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Pennartz CMA. The pairwise phase consistency: A bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization. NeuroImage. 2010;51:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Walter D. Spectral analysis for electroencephalograms: Mathematical determination of neurophysiological relationships from records of limited duration. Experimental Neurology. 1963;8:155–181. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(63)90042-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiological Review. 2010;90:1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Wehr M, Laurent G. Odour encoding by temporal sequences of firing in oscillating neural assemblies. Nature. 1996;384:162–166. doi: 10.1038/384162a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Womelsdorf T, Johnston K, Vinck M, Everling S. Theta-activity in anterior cingulate cortex predicts task rules and their adjustments following errors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:5248–5253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906194107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Womelsdorf T, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Desimone R, Engel AK, et al. Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science. 2007;316:1609–1612. doi: 10.1126/science.1139597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Womelsdorf T, Vinck M, Leung LS, Everling S. Selective theta-synchronization of choice-relevant information subserves goal-directed behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:210. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wulff P, Ponomarenko AA, Bartos M, Korotkova TM, Fuchs EC, Bahner F, et al. Hippocampal theta rhythm and its coupling with gamma oscillations require fast inhibition onto parvalbumin-positive interneurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:3561–3566. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0813176106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Zanos TP, Mineault PJ, Pack CC. Removal of spurious correlations between spikes and local field potentials. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105:474–486. doi: 10.1152/jn.00642.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Zeitler M, Fries P, Gielen S. Assessing neuronal coherence with single-unit, multi-unit, and local field potentials. Neural Computation. 2006;18:2256–2281. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.9.2256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Computational Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES