Commentary
The Integrated Project DRUID (Driving under the Influence of Drugs, Alcohol and Medicines) involved researchers from more than 20 European countries. It aimed to gain new insights to the degree of impairment caused by psychoactive drugs and their actual impact on road safety. Part of this large research program that was conducted between 2006 and 2011 has been devoted to the assessment of stimulant drug effects on driving performance in experimental, placebo-controlled studies. These studies are presented in the current issue of psychopharmacology and focus on single-dose effects of MDMA (Bosker et al. 2012) and dexamphetamine (Hjalmdahl et al. 2012) on driving performance before and after a night of sleep deprivation and on the effects of MDMA (Veldstra et al. 2012) and dexamphetamine (Simons et al. 2012) with and without alcohol. The major objective of these studies was to provide scientific basis for harmonized pan-European regulations of driving under the influence (DUI) of stimulants. Research partners agreed on a number of standardized driving scenarios to increase comparability between studies. These included a road tracking test to measure standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP) or “weaving motion” during prolonged highway driving (O'Hanlon et al. 1982), a car-following scenario to measure a driver's ability to adapt to maneuvers of other motorists (Brookhuis and de Waard 1993; Ramaekers and O'Hanlon 1994), and in case of driving simulator studies, risk-taking scenarios. In addition, all partners included a number of laboratory tests measuring skills related to driving. These tests included tracking tasks, attention tasks, reaction tasks, and cognitive tasks. Significant drug effects were statistically evaluated for clinical relevance by equivalence testing. Equivalence testing of drug effects was based on difference in scores from placebo relative to an alcohol criterion (i.e., equivalence to a blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.5 mg/mL). Basically, equivalence testing assessed whether the alcohol criterion value falls within the 95 % confidence interval (CI) for the drug effect. If yes, then the drug effect was considered equivalent to a BAC of 0.05 mg/mL (and thus clinically relevant for traffic safety). If the 95 % CI was below the alcohol criterion value, then a drug effect was considered not relevant. An integrative overview of results from the four experimental studies is presented in Tables 1 and 2.
Table 1.
MDMA–sleep deprivation study | Dexamphetamine–sleep deprivation study | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Bosker et al. 2012) | (Hjalmdahl et al. 2012) | |||||
MDMA | Sleep deprivation | MDMA + sleep deprivation | Dexamphetamine | Sleep deprivation | Dexamphetamine + sleep deprivation | |
Road tracking | No effecta | Increased SDLPb | Increased SDLPb | No effecta | Increased SDLPb | Increased SDLPc |
Impairment > BAC 0.8 mg/mLb | Impairment > BAC 0.8 mg/mLb | Impairment > BAC 0.5 mg/mL SDLPb | Relevance of impairment undecided (95 % CI drug effect includes BAC 0.5 mg/mL as well as 0)c | |||
Car following | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | Dose-related improvement of phase delaya | Impairment of phase delayb | Impairment of phase delayb |
Risk taking | Not assessed | Not assessed | Not assessed | Improvement RT to crossing carsa | Improvement RT to crossing carsa | Improvement RT to crossing carsa |
Laboratory measures of skills related to driving | Neutral on most measuresa | Impairment of attention and impulse controlb | Impairment of attention and impulse controlb | Not assessed | Not assessed | Not assessed |
Improvement on rapid information processinga | ||||||
Subjective measures | Increased arousala | Decreased arousalb | Decreased arousalb | Decreased sleepinessa | Increased sleepinessb | Increased sleepinessb |
RT reaction time
aNeutral effects or “stimulating effects”
b“Impairing” effects
cImpairments associated with a wide 95 % CI, which indicate a large variety in response; some subjects are as impaired as under alcohol, and others perform as under placebo
Table 2.
MDMA–alcohol study | Dexamphetamine–alcohol study | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Veldstra et al. 2012) | (Simons et al. 2012) | |||||
MDMA | Alcohol | MDMA + alcohol | Dexamphetamine | Alcohol | Dexamphetamine + alcohol | |
Road tracking | Decrease SDLPa | Increase SDLPb | Increase SDLPc | No effectsa | Increased SDLPb | Increased SDLPc |
Relevance of impairment undecided (95 % CI drug effect includes BAC 0.5 mg/mL as well as 0)c | Relevance of impairment undecided (95 % CI drug effect includes BAC 0.8 mg/mL as well as 0)c | |||||
Car following | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta |
Risk taking | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | No effecta | Shorter gap acceptance; increased red light crossings and number of crashesb | Shorter gap acceptance; increased red light crossings and number of crashesc |
Relevance of impairment undecided (95 % CI drug effect includes BAC 0.8 mg/mL as well as 0)c | ||||||
Laboratory measures of skills related to driving | Not assessed | Not assessed | Not assessed | No effecta | Impairment of attention, tracking, and RTb | Impairment of attention, tracking, and RTb |
Subjective measures | Decreased sleepinessa | Increased sleepinessb | Increased sleepinessb | Decreased sleepinessa | No effecta | Decreased sleepinessa |
RT reaction time
aNeutral effects or “stimulating effects”
b“Impairing” effects
cImpairments associated with a wide 95 % CI, which indicate a large variety in response; some subjects are as impaired as under alcohol, and others perform as under placebo
Effects of MDMA and dexamphetamine
The effects of MDMA and dexamphetamine on measures of simulated and actual driving were neutral for most of the driving measures or even positive for specific measures (i.e., road tracking). Stimulatory effects were also supported by subjective data that indicated that MDMA and dexamphetamine increased arousal and decreased sleepiness. The stimulatory effects of stimulants on human performance have been widely acknowledged (Ramaekers et al. 2006; Kuypers et al. 2006), and as such are no real surprise. It should be noted, however, that previous research had also demonstrated that stimulant drugs can improve certain aspects of performance while impairing other performance domains at the same time. For example, stimulants have repeatedly been shown to improve neuropsychological skills, such as tracking, impulse control, and reaction time, while impairing cognitive functions such as working memory and movement perception (Kuypers and Ramaekers 2005; Lamers et al. 2003; Silber et al. 2006, 2005; Ramaekers et al. 2009). Thus, the finding that MDMA and dexamphetamine can improve performance in particular driving domains does not automatically mean that these drugs do never have detrimental effects in other domains relevant to driving as well.
Effects of alcohol alone and combination with MDMA and dexamphetamine
Alcohol was administered in two simulated driving studies in order to assess the potential interaction between alcohol–MDMA and alcohol–dexamphetamine. Alcohol significantly impaired road tracking performance in the study by Simons et al. (2012). Coadministration of dexamphetamine did not significantly change the impairing effect of alcohol as evinced by the lack of statistical interaction between dexamphetamine and alcohol. Equivalence testing demonstrated that the 95 % CI of the change in road tracking performance (i.e., SDLP) after combined use of dexamphetamine and alcohol included both the alcohol criterion as well as the placebo reference (zero). The latter basically means that the evaluation of the clinical relevance of the combined effects of dexamphetamine and alcohol are undecided or ambiguous, i.e., it is predicted that some individuals will show impairment, whereas others may not.
Risk scenarios and secondary driving measures employed by Simons et al. (2012) were very sensitive to the effects of alcohol alone and to alcohol–dexamphetamine combined. These measures demonstrated that single doses of alcohol (0.8 g/kg body weight) increased risk-taking behaviors (i.e., shorter gap acceptance, increase of red light crossings, and number of crashes) and impaired tracking, attention, and reaction time during a 3-h period after drinking when BACs declined from 0.9 to 0.2 mg/mL. Moreover, these alcohol impairments were not affected by the coadministration of dexamphetamine 20 mg, indicating that the stimulatory effects of dexamphetamine were not sufficient to overcome the impairing effects of alcohol on skills related to driving.
Alcohol effects were most prominent in the road tracking scenario in the MDMA–alcohol interaction study conducted by Veldstra et al. (2012). As expected, alcohol significantly increased SDLP or the amount of “weaving” during highway driving, suggesting a decrement of road tracking control. This finding nicely replicates earlier demonstration of alcohol-induced impairment of road tracking performance in actual, on-the-road driving test scenarios (Kuypers et al. 2006; Louwerens et al. 1987). Average BACs during the simulated driving tests were around 0.45–0.50 mg/mL during treatments with alcohol. The stimulatory effects of MDMA (100 mg) were sufficient to counteract some of the impairing effects of this low dose of alcohol on SDLP as indicated by a significant MDMA × alcohol interaction. However, equivalence tests again demonstrated that change in SDLP after the combination of MDMA and alcohol included both the alcohol criterion as well as the placebo reference. In other words, due to large variation in subject sensitivity to combination of MDMA and alcohol, some subjects showed impairment, whereas others did not. These findings are in line with previous research that also indicated that stimulatory effects of MDMA are not sufficient to fully overcome alcohol-induced impairments of driving performance, psychomotor function, and cognition (Kuypers et al. 2006; Brookhuis et al. 2004; Dumont et al. 2008; Kuypers et al. 2006; Ramaekers and Kuypers 2006; Hernandez-Lopez et al. 2002).
Effects of sleep deprivation with or without MDMA or dexamphetamine
The sleep deprivation studies demonstrated that sleep loss produced severe impairment in actual and simulated driving performance as expressed by a significant rise in SDLP in the road tracking scenario. In the on-the-road driving study (Bosker et al. 2012), a large number of driving tests were prematurely terminated due to excessive fatigue.
On average, SDLP increased with 4.2 cm in the morning after sleep deprivation, relative to SDLP before sleep deprivation. This increment is about 1.5–2 times greater than that found in two recent driving under the influence of alcohol studies with blood alcohol concentrations between 0.29 and 0.5 mg/mL (Kuypers et al. 2006). From a previous alcohol study that was conducted in order to calibrate SDLP for the dose-related effects of alcohol (Louwerens et al. 1987), it can be concluded that a mean increase in SDLP of 4.2 cm is equivalent to a blood alcohol concentration of approximately 0.8 mg/mL. Equivalence testing even demonstrated that the upper limit of the 95 % CI associated with the mean change in SDLP after sleep deprivation widely exceeded the criterion level of 1.0 mg/mL BAC. These findings were corroborated by results from secondary driving measures as measured in laboratory tests. Critical tracking performance significantly decreased over the night, as a function of hours of sleep loss. Together, this indicates that sleep deprivation caused severe driving impairment comparable to driving under the influence of high to very high BAC.
It is also apparent from the present studies that the stimulant effects of MDMA and dexamphetamine, if any, could not compensate for the impairing effect of sleep loss on simulated and actual driving performance. None of the primary driving measures demonstrated any significant MDMA × sleep loss interaction. The effects of sleep deprivation on driving were highly prominent during MDMA and dexamphetamine treatments and did not change as a function of dose and concentration.
Concentration effect relations of MDMA and dexamphetamine
Two studies were specifically designed to assess driving performance across a wide range of doses and concentrations. The MDMA–sleep deprivation study (Bosker et al. 2012) included three doses of MDMA (25, 50, and 100 mg), whereas the dexamphetamine–sleep deprivation study included a low dose (10 mg) and a high dose (40 mg) of dexamphetamine (Hjalmdahl et al. 2012). It was apparent in both studies that neither MDMA nor dexamphetamine produced any dose- or concentration-related effects on driving. Also, the inability of both stimulants to compensate for the impairing effects of alcohol and sleep deprivation was not affected by dose or concentration.
It should, however, be noted that doses administered in the present studies may have been relatively low for (some) recreational drug users. Dexamphetamine doses (10–40 mg) were well with the accepted therapeutic window when used for medical purposes. Likewise, MDMA doses were close to the amount of MDMA that is generally present in a single ecstasy tablet (i.e., 50–100 mg). However, it is very likely that a significant proportion of recreational drug users will take much higher doses of dexamphetamine and MDMA in real-life situations. High-dose effects of stimulant on driving performance cannot be readily assessed in experimental, placebo-controlled studies due to obvious medical and ethical constraints. It has become evident, however, that MDMA and amphetamine concentrations that are observed in actual DUI cases can be 10-fold higher than during controlled administration in experimental studies. A recent study analyzing drug concentrations in postmortem cases and DUI cases in the Netherlands in 1999–2004 may serve to illustrate this point (Verschraagen et al. 2007). Amphetamine-based drugs were present in 70 postmortem cases and 467 DUI cases. The most detected amphetamine-based drug was MDMA, followed by amphetamine. Median blood concentrations of MDMA in postmortem and DUI cases were 1,600 and 330 ng/mL, respectively. MDMA blood concentrations in the MDMA-related deaths (n = 20) and in the DUI cases (n = 360) varied up to 3,700 and 4,000 ng/mL, respectively. The median concentrations of amphetamine in the amphetamine-related deaths (n = 13) and the DUI cases (n = 208) were 280 and 220 ng/mL, respectively. Amphetamine blood concentrations up to 6,000 and 2,300 ng/mL were seen in the drug-related deaths and DUI cases, respectively. The most frequently encountered amphetamine-based drugs in the investigated deaths were MDMA and amphetamine. The majority of MDMA- and amphetamine-caused deaths, i.e., 90 % of these deaths, occurred with blood concentrations above 1,500 and 800 ng/mL, respectively. Clearly, these data show that amphetamine concentrations in DUI cases can be much higher than amphetamine concentrations that are achieved in controlled studies.
MDMA, dexamphetamine, and driving safety
The pharmacological effects of stimulants and drug use settings seem very much intertwined and are likely to play a crucial role when evaluating driving under the influence offenders. Some will take the present data as an argument to show that the primary reason for impairment observed in DUI cases with stimulants will be sleep deprivation or concomitant use of alcohol and drugs. Others can argue that the use of stimulants may affect a person's ability to subjectively evaluate or recognize their state of impairment. Stimulants increase subjective feelings of arousal, energy, and mood. Such feelings affect the subjective judgment of stimulant users on whether or not it is safe to drive home after spending a night at a rave party. During intoxication with a stimulant, they may not be able to subjectively experience the debilitating effects of sleep loss or concomitant alcohol use to the same degree as stimulant-free drivers, because they feel more energetic. As a consequence, they may decide to drive because they subjectively feel alert while neglecting the objectively impairing effects of other cofactors, such as sleep deprivation or alcohol use.
In the context of a pan-European initiative to combat driving under the influence of drugs, it is advised to distinguish between (1) potential medicinal use of amphetamines (therapeutic doses) and (2) drug abuse of stimulants (polypharmacy and combat of sleep). Stimulants are generally safe for driving when taken alone at regular doses (e.g., as in medicinal use), but stimulant effects are less safe when taken in combination with sleep loss or alcohol intoxication as is often the case in drug abusers. In such cases, it will be very difficult to separate stimulant effects from those of drug use setting. Consequently, drivers should receive specific warnings on driving impairment arising from the use of stimulants during sleep loss or alcohol intoxication.
Acknowledgments
This work was conducted as part of the driving under the influence of drugs, alcohol and medicines (DRUID) research consortium funded by European Union grant TREN-05-FP6TR-S07.61320-518404-DRUID. This report reflects only authors' view. The European community is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein.
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
References
- Bosker WM, Kuypers KP, Conen S, Kauert GF, Toennes SW, Skopp G, Ramaekers JG (2012) MDMA (ecstasy) effects on actual driving performance before and after sleep deprivation, as function of dose and concentration in blood and oral fluid. Psychopharmacology [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brookhuis KA, de Waard D. The use of psychophysiology to assess driver status. Ergonomics. 1993;36:1099–1110. doi: 10.1080/00140139308967981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookhuis KA, de Waard D, Samyn N. Effects of MDMA (ecstasy), and multiple drugs use on (simulated) driving performance and traffic safety. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2004;173:440–445. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1714-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumont GJ, Wezenberg E, Valkenberg MM, de Jong CA, Buitelaar JK, van Gerven JM, Verkes RJ. Acute neuropsychological effects of MDMA and ethanol (co-)administration in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2008;197:465–474. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1056-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez-Lopez C, Farre M, Roset PN, Menoyo E, Pizarro N, Ortuno J, et al. 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) and alcohol interactions in humans: psychomotor performance, subjective effects, and pharmacokinetics. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2002;300:236–244. doi: 10.1124/jpet.300.1.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hjalmdahl M, Vadeby A, Forsman A, Gunnel C, Per W, Kronstrand R (2012) Effects of dextroamphetamine on simulated driving performance before and after sleep deprivation. Psychopharmacology [Epub ehead of print] [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kuypers KP, Ramaekers JG. Transient memory impairment after acute dose of 75 mg 3.4-methylene-dioxymethamphetamine. J Psychopharmacol. 2005;19:633–639. doi: 10.1177/0269881105056670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuypers KP, Samyn N, Ramaekers JG. MDMA and alcohol effects, combined and alone, on objective and subjective measures of actual driving performance and psychomotor function. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2006;187:467–475. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0434-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamers CT, Ramaekers JG, Muntjewerff ND, Sikkema KL, Samyn N, Read NL, et al. Dissociable effects of a single dose of ecstasy (MDMA) on psychomotor skills and attentional performance. J Psychopharmacol. 2003;17:379–387. doi: 10.1177/0269881103174015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louwerens JW, Gloerich ABM, de Vries G, Brookhuis KA, O'Hanlon JF. The relationship between drivers' blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during high speed travel. In: Noordzij PC, Roszbach R, editors. Alcohol, drugs and traffic safety—T86. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1987. pp. 183–186. [Google Scholar]
- O'Hanlon JF, Haak TW, Blaauw GJ, Riemersma JB. Diazepam impairs lateral position control in highway driving. Science. 1982;217:79–81. doi: 10.1126/science.7089544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KP. Acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) on behavioral measures of impulsivity: alone and in combination with alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1048–1055. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers JG, O'Hanlon JF. Acrivastine, terfenadine and diphenhydramine effects on driving performance as a function of dose and time after dosing. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1994;47:261–266. doi: 10.1007/BF02570506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KP, Samyn N. Stimulant effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) 75 mg and methylphenidate 20 mg on actual driving during intoxication and withdrawal. Addiction. 2006;101:1614–1621. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01566.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KP, Wingen M, Heinecke A, Formisano E. Involvement of inferior parietal lobules in prospective memory impairment during acute MDMA (ecstasy) intoxication: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1641–1648. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silber BY, Papafotiou K, Croft RJ, Ogden E, Swann P, Stough C. The effects of dexamphetamine on simulated driving performance. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2005;179:536–543. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2061-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silber BY, Croft RJ, Papafotiou K, Stough C. The acute effects of d-amphetamine and methamphetamine on attention and psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2006;187:154–169. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0410-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons R, Martens M, Ramaekers J, Krul A, Klöpping-Ketelaars I, Skopp G (2012) Effects of dexamphetamine with and without alcohol on simulated driving. Psychopharmacology [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Veldstra JL, Brookhuis KA, de Waard D, Molmans BH, Verstraete AG, Skopp G, Jantos R (2012) Effects of alcohol (BAC 0.5 ‰) and ecstasy (MDMA 100 mg) on simulated driving performance and traffic safety. Psychopharmacology [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Verschraagen M, Maes A, Ruiter B, Bosman IJ, Smink BE, Lusthof KJ. Post-mortem cases involving amphetamine-based drugs in The Netherlands. Comparison with driving under the influence cases. Forensic Sci Int. 2007;170:163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2007.03.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]