Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2012 Jul 20;7(7):e41306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041306

Use of Motor Abundance in Young and Older Adults during Dual-Task Treadmill Walking

Leslie M Decker 1,*, Fabien Cignetti 1,2, Jane F Potter 3, Stephanie A Studenski 4, Nicholas Stergiou 1,5
Editor: Anthony E Kline6
PMCID: PMC3401175  PMID: 22911777

Abstract

Motor abundance allows individuals to perform any task reliably while being variable in movement's particulars. The study investigated age-related differences in this feature when young adults (YA) and older adults (OA) performed challenging tasks, namely treadmill walking alone and while performing a cognitive task. A goal function for treadmill walking was first defined, i.e., maintain constant speed at each step, which led to a goal equivalent manifold (GEM) containing all combinations of step time and step length that equally satisfied the function. Given the GEM, amounts of goal-equivalent and non-goal-equivalent variability were afterwards determined and used to define an index providing information about the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM. The set was limited in OA compared to YA in treadmill walking alone, indicating that OA made less flexible use of motor abundance than YA. However, this differentiation between YA and OA disappeared when concurrently performing the cognitive task. It is proposed that OA might have benefited from cognitive compensation.

Introduction

Walking is a complex task whose performance relies not only on the sensorimotor system but also critically depends on cognitive resources, specifically executive function that orchestrates goal-directed activities [1], [2]. A usual way to assess the extent to which gait places demands on them is to examine the individuals' ability for dual tasking, which consists in walking while simultaneously performing a secondary cognitive task. Any dual-task-related change in gait reflects that the high-order cognitive resources needed to perform the two tasks concurrently exceed the total capacity of the system so that a reduction of the resources allocated to gait performance occurs [3], [4]. Overall, studies reported an age-related dual-task deficit while walking overground, which includes larger decreased speed and increased stride-to-stride variability in older adults (OA) than in young adults (YA) [5][8]. This deterioration of the dual-task ability with normal aging was also observed in treadmill walking, with larger changes in the variability of the gait patterns when increasing cognitive task difficulty in OA as compared to YA [9], [10]. Moreover, the dual-task ability was found to be further deteriorated with pathological aging, for instance in case of elderly idiopathic fallers or patients suffering either from Alzheimer's disease or Parkinson's disease [11][13]. A likely explanation for these results is that gait control increasingly relies on cognitive processes with aging and age-related neurodegenerative diseases while at the same time attentional capacity and other relevant cognitive resources (“cognitive supply”) are reduced [14].

However, the previous studies are limited by the fact that the observed dual-task-related changes in gait parameters do not provide complete information on the extent to which taxing cognitive processes affects the individuals' walking execution. Indeed, given the number of joints and muscles in the human body, there are typically an infinite number of ways, or solution strategies, for any individual to achieve walking reliably and repetitively while allowing a relatively high variability in the movement's particulars, a feature known as equifinality [15], [16]. In the presence of equifinality, a difficulty is, therefore, to reach a conclusion as to whether dual-task-dependent gait changes reflect, or not, suboptimal solutions for completing the walking task. Consider, for example, studies that have examined changes in stride-to-stride variability during treadmill walking when increasing the difficulty of the concurrent cognitive task [9], [10]. The decreased variability observed at a low difficulty level of cognitive activity was treated as evidence of an improved gait control, while the increased variability at higher difficulty levels was treated as evidence of an altered gait control. Although such interpretations have led to elegant hypotheses with respect to the mechanisms linking gait control to cognitive demand in YA and OA, inferring beneficial and detrimental effects of cognitive activities on gait control from changes in stride-to-stride variability is tricky. Increased or decreased variability is commonly reported in populations with gait abnormalities, such as elderly fallers [17], [18] and individuals with neurodegenerative diseases (e.g. Parkinson's disease) [19], [20], so that too much or too little stride-to-stride variability may be both not optimal in terms of gait control [21]. Therefore, being able to examine dual-tasking effects on gait by answering unambiguously the question of whether or not the walking task is completed appropriately is imperative. In particular, grasping such a functional meaning would be important to make further progress in understanding the complex relationship between falls and gait changes under dual-task conditions [22]. The purpose of the present study was thus to examine age-related dual-task changes in the individuals' walking strategies approached within an equifinality-based framework.

The issue of equifinality in motor tasks has been addressed using geometry-based approaches, including those of the uncontrolled manifold [23], [24], the minimum intervention principle [25], [26], and the body-goal mapping variability [16]. These approaches share the idea that the excess of body-level degrees of freedom over those at the task-goal level gives rise to an entire set of motor solutions, assumed to have the structure of a manifold in the body's variable space. Practically, one can imagine the task of positioning its finger at a particular location in space [27]. In this example, the position of the finger is unique while many joint angle combinations (e.g., combinations of the shoulder, elbow and wrist joint angles) can be adopted to achieve that particular end-effector position. These combinations lie on a multi-dimensional surface in joint space referred to as a manifold, with any point in the manifold representing a goal-equivalent solution to the positioning task. Therefore, achieving a task with a well-prescribed goal comes then down to selecting primarily solutions within the manifold rather than outside of it. This conception can be operationalized by partitioning the variability in the body's variable space (e.g., joint space) into a component that is tangential to the manifold and a component that is orthogonal to it. The component tangential to the manifold is goal-equivalent (i.e., goal-equivalent variability) and is consistent with no variability at the level of the task variable (e.g., the finger position). Alternatively, the component orthogonal to the manifold is non-goal-equivalent (i.e., non-goal-equivalent variability) and is consistent with a variable task variable. As a result, the central nervous system preferentially constrains non-goal-equivalent variability that matter for the stabilization of the task variable and the successful achievement of the task than goal-equivalent variability that do not. With aging, previous studies on finger coordination and manual pointing reported age-related differences in the relative proportion of goal-equivalent variability, with lower ratios of goal-equivalent to non-goal-equivalent variability in OA as compared to YA [28][31]. The sets of effective motor solutions relative to the manifold (or the motor synergies [32]) used by OA to achieve the tasks were thus more limited than those of YA, reflecting a more conservative strategy that consists of constraining the body-level degrees of freedom (or the motor abundance [33]). Such a strategy was proposed to cope with age-related decline in sensorimotor processing [34].

Recently, a step forward in understanding how the central nervous system regulates walking on treadmill was made in the context of equifinality [35]. Since treadmill walking only requires that individuals do not walk off either the front or back end of the treadmill, a goal function was defined: maintain a constant speed at each stride, which mathematically expresses one possible control strategy. This goal function typically leads to a goal equivalent manifold (GEM) containing all the possible stride time and stride length combinations that equally satisfy the function (see section ‘data processing and analysis’ for details). To determine whether humans adopt a strategy that recognizes the GEM, the authors quantified tangential and perpendicular deviations from the GEM [35]. The former deviations are goal-equivalent because they do not affect walking speed while the latter deviations that are non-goal-equivalent do. Goal-equivalent deviations were found to be more widely dispersed than non-goal-equivalent deviations, meaning that humans minimize errors relative to the GEM. In addition, humans immediately corrected non-goal-equivalent deviations at each successive stride, while allowing goal-equivalent deviations to persist across multiple strides. Taken together, these results clearly indicated that humans exploit motor abundance, and so equifinality, to control walking on treadmill.

The present study is built on the analytical framework of a GEM-based control of gait, with YA and OA participating in single- and dual-task treadmill walking. In dual-task, the concurrent cognitive task to treadmill walking was the Boston Naming Test [36]. This test consists in a series of pictures of objects (ranging from high frequency to rare object names) that the participant is required to name. Confrontation naming activates regions involved in executive and word retrieval processes [37]. Thus, naming pictured objects (as found in the Boston Naming Test, employed in the current design) consumes cognitive resources and is likely to cause a mismatch between the resources available and those required for treadmill walking under a dual-task condition. In light of age-related changes in motor synergies, a first hypothesis was that OA would show a lower relative proportion of variability along the GEM (i.e., a more limited set of effective motor solutions relative to GEM) than YA in single-task treadmill walking. Based on age-related dual-task deficit, a second hypothesis was that dual tasking would magnify the previous differentiation between OA and YA (i.e., a group×task interaction effect), involving an even more pronounced reduction of the relative proportion of variability along the GEM (i.e., a much more limited set of effective motor solutions relative to GEM) in OA as compared to YA.

Methods

Participants

YA (n = 23) aged between 20 and 35 years and OA (n = 19) aged at least 65 years were recruited for the experiment. The two samples matched regarding their demographic characteristics (Table 1). All participants were: (i) right-handed; (ii) living independently in the community; (iii) able to ambulate without the use of, or assistance from, a prosthetic device, a fixed or mobile walking frame, or other assistive devices (e.g., brace, cane, crutch), or without the assistance of another person; and (iv) not diagnosed with neurologic conditions (e.g., Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, stroke, and multiple sclerosis) or other conditions (e.g., dementia, moderate or severe chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, weight-bearing pain, chest pain at rest or during activity, previous history of myocardial infarction, dyspnea at rest or use of supplemental oxygen) that may impair the participant's safety during the procedures outlined in the protocol. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to data collection according to the guidelines of the University's Institutional Review Board.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the participants.

Younger Older Statistics
Gender (M/F) 11/12 12/7 x 2(1, N = 42) = 0.99; p = .32
Age (years) 23.56±0.69 70.95±1.47 t (40) = −30.83; p <.0001
Height (m) 1.75±0.02 1.71±0.02 t(40) = 1.19; p = .26
Weight (kg) 71.12±2.88 77.69±3.09 t(40) = −1.54; p = .12
Education (years) 16.35±0.4 17.26±0.81 t(40) = −1.06; p = .29

Entries are mean ± standard error.

Experimental procedure and data collection

The participants first underwent tests of general cognitive functioning, involving the Mini Mental State Exam [38][40] and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd Edition (WAIS-III) forward and backward digit span tests [41], [42]. Vocabulary ability was also determined using the vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-III, which was required for interpreting correctly scores on the Boston Naming Test as explained afterwards. Further, baseline data with respect to the number of falls in the year prior to the experiment, fear of falling (Modified Falls Efficacy Scale [43]) and depression (15-item Geriatric Depression Scale [44]) were obtained in OA.

The experiment then involved a sitting session and a walking session. In the former session, participants performed the Boston Naming Test while being seated to establish the baseline cognitive performance. In the latter session, participants walked at their preferred walking speed (PWS) on a treadmill with and without simultaneous confrontation naming performance. The two experimental sessions were counterbalanced across participants to account for possible order effects.

The sitting session consisted in establishing a baseline for confrontation naming performance. Participants were asked to name aloud a series of pictures of objects displayed on a screen at a fixed rate of four seconds per item. The confrontation naming test lasted three minutes; hence, 45 items were presented successively to participants. The items ranged from common ones like ‘pencil’ or ‘tree’ to less familiar ones like ‘sphinx’ and were randomly taken from the 60-item Boston Naming Test [36]. Despite its apparent simplicity, the confrontation naming task recruit a complex set of mental representations and cognitive processes, notably: (i) recognition of the visual stimulus, (ii) access of the meaning of the pictured object in the semantic system, (iii) retrieval of the lexical representation and access of its phonological word form in the lexical system, and (iv) planning of the motor programs that drive articulation [45]. Thus, this task activates regions involved in executive processes [37], and is commonly employed by neuropsychologists to assess executive functioning [46], [47].

The walking session consisted first in attaching reflective markers to a tight fitting suit at specific anatomical landmarks of each participant's lower limbs [48][49]. The anatomical landmarks were the anterior and posterior superior iliac spine, lumbosacral joint, greater trochanter of the femur, lateral mid-thigh, front lower thigh, lateral and medial epicondyles of the femur, front mid-shank, lateral lower shank, lateral and medial malleoli, lateral border of the fifth metatarsal head, medial border of the first metatarsal head, lateral and medial processes of the calcaneal tuberosity, heel, and between the second and third metatarsophalangeal joints. Participants were then given ample time to familiarize themselves with treadmill walking. The PWS was established using a well-established protocol [50]. Initially, the participants walked at a relatively slow speed, and then the investigator increased the speed in 0.1 km.h−1 increments until the participants reported their PWS. The speed was then increased by approximately 1.5 km.h−1 and decreased by 0.1 km.h−1 until the PWS was re-established. This procedure was repeated until a close match was achieved (less than 0.4 km.h−1 difference). Afterwards, participants walked under a single-task (control) condition (i.e., walking without an explicit cognitive requirement) and a dual-task condition (i.e., walking while simultaneously performing the confrontation naming task as implemented in the sitting session). Each condition was conducted at PWS and lasted for three minutes. This duration was chosen because it was difficult for the participants, especially OA, to sustain attention for a longer period of time. Ample rest time was provided between the two conditions. The three-dimensional positions of the markers were collected at 60 Hz with an eight high-speed cameras Motion Analysis Eagle Digital system using EVaRT software (version 5.0, Motion Analysis Corporation, Santa Rosa, CA).

Data processing and analysis

Confrontation naming was scored based on the percentage of correct responses [37], [51], with a correct response occurring when a picture was identified properly. With respect to gait data, the marker trajectories were first low-pass filtered at 10 Hz with a zero-lag Butterworth filter. Data were then analyzed in the framework of the GEM for treadmill walking [35]. The primary requirement for treadmill walking with speed v is to not walk off the treadmill. Since the net change in displacement for step n is a function of step length, Ln, and step time, Tn, as LnvTn, treadmill walking was formulated as:

graphic file with name pone.0041306.e001.jpg (1)

where the summation is the net displacement walked over N steps and LTM is the treadmill length. The simplest strategy to satisfy Eq. 1, and then to perform the walking task, is to keep v constant at each step, which was formulated using the goal function:

graphic file with name pone.0041306.e002.jpg (2)

All [Ln, Tn] pairs that satisfied Eq. 2 defined the GEM, which was typically a solid line in the [Ln, Tn] plane (Fig. 1A). Tn and Ln were obtained from the time interval and horizontal distance between consecutive toe-off events, respectively. The toe-off was defined as the maximum backward displacement of the toe marker (i.e., the marker located between the second and third metatarsophalangeal joints) during each step. For consistency across subjects, both Ln and Tn time series were shortened to n = 256 steps, which was the number of steps of the slowest subject. Each series was also normalized to unit variance by dividing it by its own standard deviation, which provided an intuitive reference for comparison between participants and conditions [29]. The velocity for step n was subsequently defined as Sn = Ln/Tn and the average walking speed was obtained as Inline graphic, where Inline graphicdenotes the average over the 256 steps.

Figure 1. Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) analysis for treadmill walking.

Figure 1

(A) Step length (Ln) and step time (Tn) were first normalized to unit variance to facilitate the analysis. A triangle represents a particular [Ln, Tn] combination for one individual step. The GEM was then defined from all [Ln, Tn] combinations that achieved the exact same speed v as defined by the goal function (cf. Eq. 2), and was a diagonal solid line. The triangles thus represent the set of effective motor solution relative to the GEM used to achieve treadmill walking. Next, an operating point [T *, L *] and orthonormal basis vectors Inline graphic centered on this point and aligned tangent to and perpendicular to the GEM were defined. (B) Finally, deviations along the GEM, Inline graphic, and perpendicular to the GEM, Inline graphic, were obtained using a linear coordinate transformation (cf. Eq. 3). The relative proportion of variability along the GEM was evaluated from the ratio Inline graphic. The lower the ratio, the lower the relative amount of variability along the GEM, and the more limited the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM.

Next, an operating point of coordinates Inline graphic and Inline graphic and orthonormal basis vectors Inline graphic centered on this point and aligned tangent to and perpendicular to the GEM were defined (Fig. 1A). The coordinates were then re-expressed as Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Finally, (goal-equivalent) deviations along the GEM, Inline graphic, and (non-goal-equivalent) deviations perpendicular to the GEM, Inline graphic, were calculated as follows (Fig. 1B):

graphic file with name pone.0041306.e016.jpg (3)

Standard deviations (σ) for Inline graphic and Inline graphic time series were computed and the relative proportion of variability along the GEM was calculated as the ratio Inline graphic. Considering that Inline graphic and Inline graphic, higher variability is present along the GEM than perpendicular to it [35]. It reflects that humans adopt a strategy that recognizes the GEM. The lower the ratio, the lower the relative proportion of variability along the GEM and the more limited the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM.

Scaling exponents α were also computed from Inline graphic and Inline graphic time series using the Detrended Fluctuation Analysis [52][54]. This provided information about the rapidity of the participants to correct deviations along and perpendicular to the GEM. Briefly, the mean square roots of (linearly) detrended residuals, F(n), of the integrated Inline graphic and Inline graphic time series were calculated over a range of box lengths n, with n the number of steps. The log10[F(n)] vs. log10(n) plots were then fitted with a linear function and α was obtained from the slope of this line over the range of box lengths n = 17 to n = 45. This range provided the most stable estimates of α, as determined using a DFBETA procedure [55]. When α<0.5, the time series contained anti-persistent correlations, consistent with an immediate correction to the GEM. When α>0.5, the time series contained persistent correlations, reflecting a correction to the GEM that is not immediate [35], [56]. All gait data were processed using MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).

Statistical analysis

The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test confirmed the normality of distribution for all dependent variables (i.e., % correct responses for confrontation naming, and σ and α for gait). General Linear Models for analyses of variances (ANOVAs) with between- and within-subjects factors were subsequently used. The factors were group (YA vs. OA) and session (sitting vs. walking) for performance in confrontation naming, and group and task (single-task walking vs. dual-task walking) for gait. Importantly, ANOVA results for confrontation naming and gait were adjusted for the covariates vocabulary and velocity, respectively. The former adjustment accounted for false-positive rates for naming deficit due to poorer vocabulary knowledge [57], [58], and the latter adjustment for gait differences resulting from differences in gait velocity [50], [59][61]. Adjusted (least squares) mean (M) and standard error of the mean (SE) are then reported for all dependent variables. When ANOVAs yielded significant results, post-hoc multiple comparisons were conducted using the Tukey's HSD test to examine for differences between the factors' levels. Effect sizes are reported as η 2 = SSexplained/SStotal. Statistical significance was set at 0.05. Statistica v10 (Statsoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA) was used to perform all analyses.

Results

Baseline characteristics of the YA and OA

Baseline characteristics of the analytic samples are shown in Table 2. All participants scored more than 25 on the Mini Mental State Exam, and more than 6 and 5 on the WAIS-III forward and backward digit span tasks, respectively, which reflected a preserved general cognitive function and working memory. Moreover, OA reported no falls in the year prior to the experiment, had no fear of falling (i.e., scores close to 10 on the Modified Falls Efficacy Scale), and had an average score of 1 on the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale that indicated no symptoms of depression. Finally, vocabulary performance and preferred walking speed were significantly higher and lower in OA as compared to YA. As previously mentioned, such differences were statistically controlled in the ANOVAs by co-varying vocabulary and speed to avoid confounding.

Table 2. Baseline characteristics of the participants.

Younger Older Statistics
Cognition Digit Span Forw. 11.6±0.54 10.68±0.46 t(40) = 1.26; p = .21
Back. 7.74±0.45 6.47±0.55 t(40) = 1.77; p = .08
Tot. 19.35±0.88 17.21±0.88 t(40) = 1.69; p = .09
MMSE 29±0.21 28.89±0.31 t(40) = 0.28; p = .77
Depression GDS 1±0.28
Fear of falling MFES 9.91±0.05
Vocabulary WAIS III 1.11±0.04 1.55±0.06 t (40) = −5.98; p <.0001
Gait PWS (m/s) 1.08±0.04 0.77±0.04 t (40) = 4.86; p <.0001

Entries are mean ± standard error. MMSE: Mini Mental State Exam. GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale. MFES: Modified Falls Efficacy Scale. PWS: Preferred Walking Speed. WAIS III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd Edition.

Percentage of correct responses in confrontation naming

A significant main effect of group (F [1], [39] = 5.93, p = 0.019, η 2 = 0.12) and a significant group×session interaction effect (F [1], [39] = 5.64, p = 0.022, η 2 = 0.14) were observed. There was no significant main effect of session (i.e., sitting vs. walking). Post-hoc examination of the interaction effect indicated that the percentage of correct responses in OA (M = 90.03, SE = 1.58) was significantly lower (p = 0.005) than that in YA (M = 97.16, SE = 1.48) when sitting. This percentage significantly increased (p = 0.02) in OA when walking (M = 92.84, SE = 1.31), but remained the same in YA (M = 96.03, SE = 1.22). Thus, OA performed worse on confrontation naming than YA when sitting, but this difference disappeared when walking as a result of an improved performance in OA.

Variability along and perpendicular to the GEM

A significant main effect of task for Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 12.06, p = 0.001, η 2 = 0.04, and Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 8.31, p = 0.006, η 2 = 0.03, was observed. In addition, a group×task interaction effect was also significant for these two variables, with F [1], [38] = 19.47, p<10−5, η 2 = 0.07, for Inline graphic, and F [1], [38] = 7.82, p = 0.008, η 2 = 0.03, for Inline graphic. There was no significant main effect of group. Post-hoc tests revealed the origins of the interaction effect. First, Inline graphic and Inline graphic values in OA (M = 1.11, SE = 0.02 and M = 0.85, SE = 0.03; respectively) were significantly lower (p = 0.001) and larger (p = 0.024), respectively, than those in YA (M = 1.23, SE = 0.02 and M = 0.76, SE = 0.02; respectively) in the single-task condition (walking alone) (Fig. 2A, 2B). Second, significantly decreased (p = 0.0001) and increased (p = 0.01) values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic, respectively, occurred for YA in the dual-task condition (M = 1.11, SE = 0.02 and M = 0.85, SE = 0.03; respectively), while no changes occurred for OA (M = 1.12, SE = 0.02 and M = 0.83, SE = 0.03; respectively) (Fig. 2A, 2B). Therefore, the GEM-based control of treadmill walking was more extensive in YA than in OA in the single-task condition, involving more variability (elongation) along the GEM (i.e., in the goal-equivalent direction) and less variability (compression) perpendicular to the GEM (i.e., in the non-goal-equivalent direction). In addition, only YA exhibited dual-task-related changes in their gait control strategy, with variability values in the goal-equivalent and non-goal-equivalent directions of the GEM decreasing and increasing, respectively, and becoming similar to those of OA.

Figure 2. Results of the Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) analysis for treadmill walking in single-task (ST) and dual-task (DT).

Figure 2

(A) Variability along the GEM, as evaluated from Inline graphic. (B) Variability perpendicular to the GEM, as evaluated from Inline graphic. (C) Relative proportion of variability along the GEM, as evaluated from the ratio Inline graphic. YA: Young Adults. OA: Older Adults. Significant differences are indicated by stars (* p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001). Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

Relative proportion of variability along the GEM

A significant main effect of task, F [1], [38] = 5.52, p = 0.024, η 2 = 0.02, and a group×task interaction effect with F [1], [38] = 9.33, p = 0.004, η 2 = 0.04, were observed for the ratio Inline graphic. There was no significant main effect of group. The interaction effect comes from a statistically lower (p = 0.015) ratio in OA compared to YA in the single-task condition (M = 1.38, SE = 0.09 vs. M = 1.63, SE = 0.08; respectively), and a significantly reduced (p = 0.001) ratio in YA (M = 1.35, SE = 0.11), but not in OA (M = 1.44, SE = 0.11), in the dual-task condition (Fig. 2C). Thus, although the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM used by YA during single-task treadmill walking was larger than that of OA, dual tasking annihilated such a difference by making the YA's set smaller.

Corrected deviations along and perpendicular to the GEM

A significant main effect of task for Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 3.93, p = 0.047, η 2 = 0.04, and Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 5.21, p = 0.028, η 2 = 0.08, and a significant group×task interaction effect for Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 6.86, p = 0.012, η 2 = 0.07, and Inline graphic, F [1], [38] = 6.69, p = 0.013, η 2 = 0.03, were observed. The interaction effect for Inline graphic resulted from a significant decrease (p = 0.019) of the OA's exponent from single- to dual-task condition (M = 0.76, SE = 0.04 vs. M = 0.59, SE = 0.04; respectively) while the YA's exponent remained equivalent (M = 0.71, SE = 0.04 vs. M = 0.77, SE = 0.04; respectively); the OA's exponent being significantly lower (p = 0.027) than the YA's exponent in the dual-task condition (Fig. 3A). Inversely, the interaction effect for Inline graphic resulted from a significant increase (p = 0.003) of the YA's exponent from single- to dual-task condition (M = 0.35, SE = 0.03 vs. M = 0.49, SE = 0.03; respectively) while the OA's exponent remained unchanged (M = 0.34, SE = 0.03 vs. M = 0.35, SE = 0.03; respectively); the YA's exponent being significantly higher (p = 0.001) than the OA's exponent in the dual-task condition (Fig. 3B). Thus, these results indicated that deviations along and perpendicular to the GEM tended to become no longer corrected in OA and in YA, respectively, in the dual-task condition.

Figure 3. Results of the Detrended Fluctuation Analysis applied to deviations from Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) in single-task (ST) and dual-task (DT).

Figure 3

(A) Scaling exponent of the deviations along the GEM, Inline graphic. (B) Scaling exponent of the deviations perpendicular to the GEM, Inline graphic. YA: Young Adults. OA: Older Adults. Significant differences are indicated by stars (* p<0.05, ** p<0.01). Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

Discussion

Age-related differences in single-task treadmill walking

During single-task treadmill walking, OA exhibited a lower relative proportion of variability along the GEM (i.e., a lower Inline graphic ratio) than YA, due to lower Inline graphic and larger Inline graphic. This result confirmed our hypothesis: the body-level degrees of freedom were more constrained in OA than in YA, giving rise to a more limited set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM for successful treadmill walking. This finding echoes previous evidence that OA make less flexible use of motor abundance than YA, as revealed by examining finger coordination and manual pointing using the uncontrolled manifold method [28][31]. Two explanations may account for the observed age-related differences in the way body-level degrees of freedom were functionally constrained to accomplish treadmill walking. A first explanation [31], [34] stipulates that constraining motor abundance is an adaptive “choice” strategy used by OA to successfully cope with the task goal despite a system faced with sensorimotor decline, involving especially altered sensorimotor processing in the brain [62], increased neuromuscular noise [63], [64], and loss of strength and flexibility [65]. Considering that gait relies on many elements of the neuromuscular system [66][70], it is likely that OA were more vulnerable to unsecured gait on treadmill than YA and operated closer to their performance limits. Consequently, they might have opted for a more conservative strategy of constraining motor abundance. Moreover, it is plausible that the important affective dimension of treadmill walking (risk-taking) might have reinforced the above strategy, further restraining motor abundance in OA to ensure a safe gait. On the other hand, a second explanation [31] is that the generalized spread of (non-selective recruitment of) brain activity that occurs with normal aging when performing motor tasks, so-called dedifferentiation [14], [71], [72], would underlie the restriction of motor abundance in OA. The dedifferentiation hypothesis in fact stipulates that there is a reduced specificity of the functional cortical networks with aging so that sensorimotor representations in the central nervous system are more easily confused, which might have led at the behavioral level to less numerous effective motor solutions to cope with the treadmill walking task.

Although the explanations of an adaptive “choice” strategy or a dedifferentiation for the restricted use of motor abundance in OA are not mutually exclusive, further results from the present study rather support the former explanation. As dedifferentiation commonly leads to impairments in motor performance due to a loss of neural specialization [73], the control law for treadmill walking would likely have been altered in OA. However, no difference was found between OA and YA regarding the regulation over time of the deviations relative to the GEM, immediately correcting non-goal-equivalent deviations at each successive step (Inline graphic) and slowly correcting goal-equivalent deviations across multiple steps (Inline graphic). Therefore, OA may have chosen to use a more limited set of motor solutions to ensure success and safety of their gait while regulating treadmill walking in the same way YA do, by over-correcting small deviations in walking speed at each step [35]. However, future studies are warranted to investigate whether the less flexible use of motor abundance in OA reflects an adaptive strategy to account for sensorimotor decline or rather results, in a mechanistic sense, from an inability to master body-level degrees of freedom due to sensorimotor decline.

Age-related similarities in dual-task treadmill walking

Contrary to our hypothesis, the difference between YA and OA in terms of Inline graphic ratio was vanished, and not magnified, under the dual-task condition. This effect resulted from a decreased YA's ratio that became equivalent to that of OA when dual tasking, through a drop in Inline graphic and an increase in Inline graphic. Hence, the concurrent cognitive task, namely confrontation naming, affected the arrangement of the body-level degrees of freedom only in YA in such a way that the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM used for treadmill walking was reduced. Considering that the most common finding in dual-task treadmill walking studies is an impaired walking performance when the two tasks are performed concurrently due to resource sharing [5][10], the absence of dual-task effect in OA is counterintuitive. Indeed, resources allocated to treadmill walking is reduced when performing confrontation naming, and so a more conservative strategy, that is constraining motor abundance, should have been adopted to secure gait, as observed in YA. An explanation, inspired from previous discussions about changes in effective motor solutions relative to the manifold [32], [74], may be that the lower ratio in OA in the single-task walking condition left less room for change in the dual-task condition, with OA operating already very close to their walking performance limits. Besides, changes in the way deviations in both directions of the GEM were regulated in the dual-task condition further support this explanation. Deviations from the GEM became less regulated (α-value closer to 0.5) in OA only in the goal-equivalent direction, i.e. in the direction in which changes do not affect walking speed and then do not challenge the goal function of treadmill walking. Inversely, deviations from the GEM became less regulated in YA only in the non-goal-equivalent direction, challenging the task goal through less controlled changes in walking speed across steps. Contrary to YA, OA thus maintained a very robust GEM-based strategy to stabilize treadmill walking performance when dual tasking, likely reflecting a lower performance limit in the sense of poorer gait flexibility and adaptability.

However, two elements call for an additional explanation. First, a control of treadmill walking that recognizes GEM occurs from the moment that Inline graphic [35]. There was then theoretically room for dual-task-related gait changes as the average OA ratio in the single-task condition was 1.38±0.09. Second, OA slightly increased their performance in confrontation naming in the dual-task condition, reaching a performance level similar to that of YA. Taken together, these results suggest that OA benefited, to some extent at least, from dual tasking while YA did not. The compensation hypothesis for neurocognitive aging assumes that, under certain circumstances, networks in the aging brain can be overactivated (“work harder”) but also additional networks can be recruited to make up for neural and behavioral deficits [73], [75], allowing OA to perform equivalently to YA. Specifically, such a compensatory brain activity was evidenced, when performing complex, either cognitive [76], [77] or motor [73], tasks and occurred mainly in frontal brain regions. In the present study, older participants may have perceived the dual task sufficiently challenging to strongly rely on compensation to meet and maintain successful performance of each task at hand, gait and confrontation naming. Although such interpretation has merit, caution is required due to some limitations of the study. First, it would have been important to manipulate the dual-task demands (e.g., by increasing either the confrontation naming demand or gait demand or both) to provide strong support to the previous interpretation. If OA actually engage more neural resources for a given dual-task demand level than do YA, they would have likely reached the limit of available resources with increased dual-task difficulty, resulting in altered gait and cognitive performances. Second, the participants performed near ceiling level in naming accuracy (means ranging from 90 to 97% of correct answers), especially YA. Accordingly, the age-differential effect of dual-tasking on naming accuracy, with an improved accuracy occurring only in OA, is difficult to interpret as it might simply result from a lack of sensitivity of the measure to detect the improvement conferred by the dual task in YA. In particular, there is a body of literature that demonstrated improvement in cognitive function in both YA and OA when performing acute exercise [78], [79], as a bout of moderate treadmill walking. Accordingly, more informative measures of cognitive function would have been worth to be considered (e.g., reaction time during confrontation naming) to test further the hypothesis of cognitive compensation.

In conclusion, findings from the present study are twofold. First, the relative proportion of variability along the GEM was smaller in OA compared to YA under single-task treadmill walking. This result revealed that the former makes a less flexible use of motor abundance than the latter and extended previous findings obtained with simpler motor tasks. Second, the differentiation between OA and YA in the use of motor abundance disappeared under the dual-task condition, with a drop in the relative proportion of variability along the GEM occurring only in YA. Furthermore, performance of OA in the concurrent cognitive (confrontation naming) task to treadmill walking appeared to slightly increased, reaching a level similar to that of YA. Thus, gait and cognitive performance of OA benefited from dual tasking to some extent. An explanation might be that when OA attempt to perform two tasks at once, such as walking and confrontation naming, they may strongly rely on neural compensation to stabilize both task performance. Future studies are needed to test such compensation hypothesis and to evaluate whether previous results extend to other samples of less vigorous OA, as frail OA and OA with cognitive impairments or high risk of falling. This latter point would contribute better characterizing these phenotypes.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Nate Hunt for his assistance with data collection and initial processing.

Footnotes

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Funding: Funding was provided by United States National Institutes of Health grant 1K99AG033684. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Snijders AH, van de Warrenburg BP, Giladi N, Bloem BR. Neurological gait disorders in elderly people: clinical approach and classification. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:63–74. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70678-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23:329–342. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kahneman D. Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002;16:1–14. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00156-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Smith L, Dennis A, Howells K, et al. Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Beauchet O, Kressig RW, Najafi B, Aminian K, Dubost V, et al. Age-related decline of gait control under a dual-task condition. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1187–1188. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hollman JH, Kovash FM, Kubik JJ, Linbo RA. Age-related differences in spatiotemporal markers of gait stability during dual task walking. Gait Posture. 2007;26:113–119. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lindenberger U, Marsiske M, Baltes PB. Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychol Aging. 2000;15:417–436. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.3.417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lövdén M, Schaefer S, Pohlmeyer AE, Lindenberger U. Walking variability and working-memory load in aging: A dual-process account relating cognitive control to motor control performance. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2008;63:121–128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.3.p121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Verrel J, Lövdén M, Schellenbach M, Schaefer S, Lindenberger U. Interacting effects of cognitive load and adult age on the regularity of whole-body motion during treadmill walking. Psychol Aging. 2009;24:75–81. doi: 10.1037/a0014272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Camicioli RM, Howieson DB, Lehman S, Kaye JA. Talking while walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1997;48:955–958. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.4.955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Yogev G, Simon ES, et al. Dual tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov Disord. 2006;21:950–957. doi: 10.1002/mds.20848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, et al. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson's disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:1248–1256. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, et al. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bernstein N. The coordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cusumano JP, Cesari P. Body-Goal Variability Mapping in an Aiming Task. Biol Cybern. 2006;94:367–379. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0052-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hausdorff JM, Edelberg HK, Mitchell SL, Goldberger AL, Wei JY. Increased gait unsteadiness in community-dwelling elderly fallers. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1997;78:278–283. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(97)90034-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brach JS, Berlin JE, VanSwearingen JM, Newman AB, Studenski SA. Too much or too little step width variability is associated with a fall history in older persons who walk at or near normal gait speed. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2005;2:21. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-2-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hausdorff JM, Cudkowicz ME, Firtion R, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson's disease and Huntington's disease. Mov Disord. 1998;13:428–437. doi: 10.1002/mds.870130310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Schaafsma JD, Giladi N, Balash Y, Bartels AL, Gurevich T, et al. Gait dynamics in Parkinson's disease: relationship to Parkinsonian features, falls and response to levodopa. J Neurol Sci. 2003;212:47–53. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(03)00104-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stergiou N, Decker LM. Human movement variability, nonlinear dynamics, and pathology: is there a connection? Hum Mov Sci. 2011;30:869–888. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Allali G, Berrut G, Herrmann FR, et al. Recurrent falls and dual task-related decrease in walking speed: is there a relationship? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1265–1269. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01766.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Scholz J, Schöner G, Latash M. Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting. Exp Brain Res. 2000;135:382–404. doi: 10.1007/s002210000540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Scholz JP, Schöner G. The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126:289–306. doi: 10.1007/s002210050738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Liu D, Todorov E. Evidence for the flexible sensorimotor strategies predicted by optimal feedback control. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9354–9368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1110-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1226–1235. doi: 10.1038/nn963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Reisman DS, Scholz JP. Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain. 2003;126:2510–2527. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Olafsdottir H, Zhang W, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Age related changes in multifinger synergies in accurate moment of force production tasks. J Appl Physiol. 2007;102:1490–1501. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00966.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Shim JK, Lay BS, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Age-related changes in finger coordination in static prehension tasks. J Appl Physiol. 2004;97:213–224. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00045.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shinohara M, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Finger interaction during accurate multi-finger force production tasks in young and elderly persons. Exp Brain Res. 2004;156:282–292. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1786-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Verrel J, Lövdén M, Lindenberger U. Normal aging reduces motor synergies in manual pointing. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:e1–e10. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schöner G. Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Mot Control. 2007;11:276–308. doi: 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Latash M. There is no motor redundancy in human movements. There is motor abundance. Mot Control. 2000;4:259–260. doi: 10.1123/mcj.4.3.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Latash ML. Synergy. 2008. Oxford University Press, New York.
  • 35.Dingwell JB, John J, Cusumano JP. Do humans optimally exploit redundancy to control step variability in walking? PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6:e1000856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Abrahams S, Goldstein LH, Simmons A, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of verbal fluency and confrontation naming using compressed image acquisition to permit overt responses. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;20:29–40. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF. Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. J Am Med Ass. 1993;269:2386–2391. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Escobar JI, Burnam A, Karno M, Forsythe A, Landsverk J, et al. Use of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in a community population of mixed ethnicity: cultural and linguistic artifacts. J Nervous Ment Dis. 1986;174:607–614. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198610000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Weschler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale- Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hodges JR. Cognitive assessment for clinicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hill KD, Schwarz JA, Kalogeropoulos AJ, Gibson SJ. Fear of falling revisited. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1996;77:1025–1029. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(96)90063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Recent evidence and development of a shorter version, In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to Assessment and Intervention. Binghamton, NY: The Haworth Press; 1986. pp. 165–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Trebuchon-Da Fonseca A, Guedj E, Alario FX, Laguitton V, Mundler O, et al. Brain regions underlying word finding difficulties in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain. 2009;132:2772–2784. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.) New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nigg BM, Cole GK, Nachbauer W. Effects of arch height of the foot on angular motion of the lower extremities in running. J Biomech. 1993;26:909–916. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(93)90053-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Vaughan C, Davis B, O'Connor J. Dynamics of Human Gait. Cape Town, South Africa: Kiboho Publishers; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Jordan K, Challis JH, Newell KM. Walking speed influences on gait cycle variability. Gait Posture. 2007;26:128–134. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Randolph C, Lansing AE, Ivnik RJ, Cullum CM, Hermann BP. Determinants of confrontation naming performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14:489–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hausdorff JM, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Is walking a random walk? Evidence for long-range correlations in stride interval of human gait. J Appl Physiol. 1995;78:349–358. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1995.78.1.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Peng CK, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Simons M, Stanley HE, et al. Mosaic organization of DNA nucleotides. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 1994;49:1685–1689. doi: 10.1103/physreve.49.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Peng CK, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL. Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. Chaos. 1995;5:82–87. doi: 10.1063/1.166141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Damouras S, Chang MD, Sejdić E, Chau T. An empirical examination of detrended fluctuation analysis for gait data. Gait Posture. 2010;31:336–340. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Re-interpreting detrended fluctuation analyses of stride-to-stride variability in human walking. Gait Posture. 2010;32:348–353. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hawkins KA, Sledge WH, Orleans JF, Quinlan DM, Rakfeldt J, et al. Normative implications of the relationship between reading vocabulary and Boston Naming Test performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1993;8:525–837. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Killgore WD, Adams RL. Prediction of Boston Naming Test performance from vocabulary scores: preliminary guidelines for interpretation. Percept Mot Skills. 1999;89:327–337. doi: 10.2466/pms.1999.89.1.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Lecordroch Y, Allali G, Dubost V, et al. Walking speed-related changes in stride time variability: effects of decreased speed. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2009;6:32. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Beauchet O, Dubost V, Herrmann FR, Kressig RW. Stride-to-stride variability while backward counting among healthy young adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2005;11:26. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-2-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dingwell JB, Marin LC. Kinematic variability and local dynamic stability of upper body motions when walking at different speeds. J Biomech. 2006;39:444–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Yordanova J, Kolev V, Hohnsbein J, Falkenstein M. Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. Brain. 2004;127:351–362. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Tracy BL, Maluf KS, Stephenson JL, Hunter SK, Enoka RM. Variability of motor unit discharge and force fluctuations across a range of muscle forces in older adults. Muscle Nerve. 2005;32:533–540. doi: 10.1002/mus.20392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Vaillancourt DE, Larsson L, Newell KM. Effects of aging on force variability, single motor unit discharge patterns, and the structure of 10, 20, and 40 Hz EMG activity. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:25–35. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00014-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kang HG, Dingwell JB. Separating the effects of age and walking speed on gait variability. Gait Posture. 2008;27:572–577. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Dietz V. Spinal cord pattern generators for locomotion. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1379–1389. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00120-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Morris ME, Huxham FE, McGinley J, Iansek R. Gait disorders and gait rehabilitation in Parkinson's disease. Adv Neurol. 2001;87:347–361. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Morton SM, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar control of balance and locomotion. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:247–259. doi: 10.1177/1073858404263517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Nielsen JB. How we walk: central control of muscle activity during human walking. Neuroscientist. 2003;9:195–204. doi: 10.1177/1073858403009003012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pahapill PA, Lozano AM. The pedunculopontine nucleus and Parkinson's disease. Brain. 2000;123:1767–1783. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.9.1767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bernard JA, Seidler RD. Evidence for motor cortex dedifferentiation in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.021. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Li SC, Lindenberger U, Sikström S. Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:479–486. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01769-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. J Neurosci. 2008;28:91–99. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Domkin D, Laczko J, Jaric S, Johansson H, Latash ML. Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2002;143:11–23. doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0944-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA. Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17:177–182. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Carp J, Gmeindl L, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Age differences in the neural representation of working memory revealed by multi-voxel pattern analysis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;22:4–217. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mattay VS, Fera F, Tessitore A, Hariri AR, Berman KF, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity. Neurosci Lett. 2006;392:32–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kamijo K, Hayashi Y, Sakai T, Yahiro T, Tanaka K, et al. Acute effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:356–363. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES