Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jul 22.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010 Feb;67(2):168–177. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.184

Neurocognitive Endophenotypes for Bipolar Disorder Identified in Multiplex Multigenerational Families

David C Glahn 1,2,3, Laura Almasy 5, Marcela Barguil 3,4, Elizabeth Hare 3,5, Juan Manuel Peralta 5, Jack W Kent Jr 5, Alabana Dassori 3, Javier Contreras 3,4, Adriana Pacheco 4, Nuria Lanzagorta 6, Humberto Nicolini 6, Henriette Raventós 4, Michael A Escamilla 3
PMCID: PMC3401550  NIHMSID: NIHMS239236  PMID: 20124116

Abstract

Context

Although genetic influences on bipolar disorder are well established, localization of genes that predispose to the illness has proven difficult. Given that genes predisposing to bipolar disorder may be transmitted without expression of the categorical clinical phenotype, one strategy for identifying risk genes is the use of quantitative endophenotypes.

Objective

The goal of the current study is to adjudicate neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder.

Design, Setting, and Participants

709 Latino individuals from the central valley of Costa Rica, Mexico City, Mexico, or San Antonio, Texas participated in the study. 660 of these persons were members of extended pedigrees with at least two siblings diagnosed with bipolar disorder (n=230). The remaining subjects were community controls drawn from each site and without personal or family history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. All subjects received psychodiagnostic interviews and comprehensive neurocognitive evaluations. Neurocognitive measures found to be heritable were entered into analyses designed to determine which tests are impaired in affected individuals, sensitive to genetic liability for the illness and genetically correlated with affection status.

Main Outcome Measures

The main outcome measure was neurocognitive test performance.

Results

Two of the 21 neurocognitive variables were not significantly heritable and were excluded from subsequent analyses. Patients with bipolar disorder were impaired on 6 of these cognitive measures compared to non-related healthy subjects. Non-bipolar first-degree relatives were impaired on five of these and three tests were genetically correlated with affection status: digit symbol coding, object delayed response, and immediate facial memory.

Conclusions

This large-scale extended pedigree study of cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder identified measures of processing speed, working memory and declarative (facial) memory as candidate endophenotypes for bipolar disorder.

Keywords: bipolar disorder, endophenotype, genetics, family studies, neurocognitive, neuropsychological

Introduction

Despite considerable evidence that risk for bipolar disorder is inherited, the molecular genetic basis for this illness remains elusive. A recent genome wide association analysis with over 5,000 cases and 6,000 controls implicated two risk genes for the illness, ANK3 and CACNA1C1. As these genes regulate voltage-gated sodium or calcium channels, respectively, the findings suggest that ion channel dysfunction may play an important role in the pathophysiology of bipolar disorder. However, as noted by the authors, theses genes (SNPs) have relatively small risk-ratios, explaining little of the genetic contribution of the illness. Given the high heritability and familial relative risk of bipolar disorder, there is little doubt that additional genes are involved in the etiology of bipolar disorder. The identification of these genes is of paramount importance as they hold the potential for spurring novel treatments for this common and debilitating illness.

Given that genes predisposing bipolar disorder may be transmitted without expression of the clinical phenotype, one strategy for identifying risk genes is the use of quantitative intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes for the illness2. Indeed, a National Institute of Mental Health workgroup charged with improving research on genetics of affective disorders called for the widespread implementation of endophenotypic markers in the search for genes predisposing to bipolar disorder3. However, relatively few endophenotypes have been proposed for the illness4, and fewer still have been validated in large-scale studies. Findings that asymptomatic individuals with bipolar disorder have neuropsychological impairments5, 6, that these deficits appear to be stable over time7 and that unaffected first-degree relatives of bipolar probands have similar, though less pronounced, impairments8, suggest that neurocognitive measures may be candidate endophenotypes for the illness9. The goal of the current study is to test the hypothesis that neurocognitive tests are candidate endophenotypes for bipolar disorder.

Quantitative neurocognitive endophenotypes can be assessed in both affected and unaffected individuals, providing much greater statistical power to localize and identify disease-related genes than affection status alone10, 11. However, establishing a particular measure as an endophenotype requires that the trait be heritable, sensitive to affection status, and genetically correlated or associated with the illness (e.g. 2, 12). While neurocognitive endophenotypes are appropriate for various linkage and association experimental designs, the establishment of a particular trait as a intermediate phenotype is most efficient with large extended pedigrees (e.g. 13), as these families provide a single sample where all of the criteria for effective endophenotypes can be assessed simultaneously (e.g. heritability, sensitivity to the illness, genetic correlation).

In the current study, comprehensive neurocognitive assessments were conducted on large multigenerational pedigrees selected for sibling pairs concordant for bipolar I disorder or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar subtype, and community controls. The focus on multiplex families reduces the potential of spurious or non-genetic forms of the illness and increases the potential that identified intermediate phenotypes reflect genetic factors that influence risk for bipolar disorder. To determine if a neurocognitive measure is a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder, we document its heritability, demonstrate that patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives are impaired on the measure, and establish genetic correlation between the measure and affection status.

Methods

Participants

709 Latino individuals from the central valley of Costa Rica (n=328, 46% of the sample), Mexico City, Mexico (n=139, 20%), or San Antonio, Texas (n=242, 34%) participated in the study. 660 of these persons were members of 45 families (average family size 14.67±10.56 members, range: 3-37) with at least two siblings diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Probands were recruited through systematic screening of outpatient and inpatient facilities. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were the same across sites and required a previous hospital diagnosis of bipolar I disorder in probands and at least one sibling with bipolar I disorder or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type. In the current study, affected individuals were excluded if they did not provide written consent to contact family members, had history of mental retardation, a neurological disorder, or severe head trauma. Once an affected sibling pair provided informed consent, attempts were made to recruit all 1st, 2nd and 3rd degree relatives. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were identical for family members, with the exception of requiring a personal history of bipolar disorder. The remaining 49 subjects were community controls drawn in equal numbers from each site and without personal or family history (1st degree relatives) of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Community controls were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and via fliers placed in medical clinics and had the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as family members.

All subjects proved written informed consent on forms in the language of their choice (Spanish or English) approved by the review boards of all participating study sites.

Diagnostic Assessment

All participants, regardless of diagnostic or family status, received the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies14 and the Family Interview for Genetic Studies15. Interviews were conducted by psychiatrists with established reliability (kappa = 0.85). Final diagnoses were determined through a best estimation processes16 where two independent psychiatrists review all available records, arrive at independent diagnoses and reach a consensus diagnoses. If consensus could not be reached, a third best estimator reviewed the case independently (this occurred once in this sample).

Neurocognitive Assessment

Each participant received the South Texas Assessment of Neurocognition (STAN), a 90-min neuropsychological evaluation consisting of standard and computerized measures13. Tests were selected on evidence of heritability, sensitivity to bipolar disorder, and minimizing the effects of language (culture) or the availability of parallel English and Spanish forms. Instructions for computerized neuropsychological tests were translated to Spanish by bilingual psychologists and translated back into English by professional translators. Other tests (e.g. the California Verbal Learning Test) have published English17 and Spanish18 editions. All subjects received the same battery of tests, in the language of their choice, in a fixed order and were allowed breaks as needed.

The STAN battery included 16 tests with 21 separate measures (see Table 1). The computerized digit-symbol coding task required subjects to indicate, via button press, if a centrally presented digit-symbol pair was identical to one of the nine digit-symbol pairs in the reference list at the top of the screen13. During the identical pairs continuous performance test, subjects viewed a series of numbers presented briefly on the screen, pressing the space bar whenever the same number appeared twice in a row (e.g.19). During the manual Stroop test, subjects pressed arrow keys that faced in the same direction (congruent) or in the opposite direction (incongruent) as an arrow on the screen (e.g. 20). The spatial delayed response task involved subjects remembering where a set of yellow circles appeared and indicating, via button press, if a green circle was presented in the same location21. During the object delayed response task, subjects remembered three abstract shapes (presented serially) and, after a delay, selected one of four shapes not part previously presented. The Penn facial memory test involved the presentation of 20 target faces and then immediate and delayed recognition (indicated by button press) of these faces mixed with an equal number of foils22. During the Penn conditional exclusion test (PCET), subjects choose stimuli based upon inferred rules and are provided feedback23. On each trial of the PCET, subjects choose which one of four objects (that vary in shape, height or width), do not belong with the other three stimuli23.

Table 1.

Neurocognitive Dependant Measures

Neurocognitive Task Dependant Measure
Letter Fluency56 Number of words, beginning with a specific letter, generated in 60-seconds
Semantic Fluency56 Number of animal names generated in 60-seconds
Digit-Symbol Coding13 Number of correctly identified digit-symbol pairs in 90-seconds
Trail Making Test, Part A56 Time needed to connect letters in ascending order
CPT Hits13 Number of hits during a 6-minute identical pairs continues performance test (CPT)
CPT Catch Trials13 Number of false alarms during a 6-minute identical pairs continues performance test (CPT)
Digit Span Forward57 Number of correctly recalled digit strings in their original order of presentation
Stroop Reaction Time20 Reaction time difference between incongruent and congruent trials of a manual Stroop test (e.g.20)
Digit Span Backward57 Number of correctly recalled digit strings in reverse order of presentation
Letter-Number Span58 Number of correctly recalled number-letter strings in numeric and alphabetical order
Spatial Delayed Response21 Number of correct responses on a simple spatial delayed-response test
Object Delayed Response35 Number of correct trials an object delayed non-match to sample task
CVLT Learning17 Number of items recalled over 5 exposures of the 16-word list of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)
CVLT Semantic Cluster17 Proximal recall of semantically related list items over 5 repeated exposures of the 16-word list
CVLT Delayed Recall17 Number of list item recalled after a 20 min delay
Digit-Symbol Recall13 Number of digits recalled when presented with the corresponding symbols from the Digit-Symbol Coding task
Facial Memory Immediate22 Number of faces recognized during the initial condition of the Penn Facial Memory test
Facial Memory Delay22 Number of faces recognized during the 20-minute delay condition on the Penn Facial Memory test22
Matrix Reasoning59 Number of correctly completed progressive matrices
PCET23 Number correctly matched shapes on the Penn Conditional Exclusion Test (PCET)
Trail Making Test, Part B56 Time needed to connect alternating letters and numbers

Quantitative Genetic Analyses

Analyses were performed to determine which neurocognitive measures were (1) heritable, (2) sensitive to liability for bipolar disorder, and (3) genetically correlated with bipolar disorder. To ensure that the neuropsychological traits conform to the assumptions of normality, an inverse normal transformation was applied. All analyses were conducted with Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines (SOLAR24). The algorithms in SOLAR employ maximum likelihood variance decomposition methods to determine the relative importance of familial and environmental influences on a measure by modeling the covariance among family members as a function of genetic proximity (kinship).

Heritability Analyses

Heritability (h2) represents the portion of the phenotypic variance accounted for by the total additive genetic variance (h2 = σ2g2p). Indices with stronger covariance between genetically more similar individuals than between genetically less similar individuals have higher heritability. Within SOLAR, this is assessed by contrasting the observed covariance matrices for a neuropsychological measure with the covariance matrix predicted by kinship and a likelihood ratio test is performed to test the hypothesis that the heritability is > 0. Heritability analyses were conducted twice: without demographic covariates and again with age, sex, age × sex interaction, age2, age2 x sex interaction, education level (years), and location of assessment included as covariates in the model. While the second h2 estimates are critical for the current investigation, the raw estimates were included for comparability to other published data. Only neuropsychological variables with significant heritability, corrected for multiple comparison at a 5% false discovery rate (FDR25) were included in subsequent analyses.

Sensitivity to Bipolar Disorder

Heritable neuropsychological variables were examined to determine which measures differentiate individuals with bipolar disorder from unrelated, unaffected individuals. Within SOLAR, these analyses were conducted by including a variable that identified individuals in specific liability groups (e.g. bipolar vs. unrelated) and testing whether the mean of these groups differed within the context of the known pedigree structure and covariates (χ2 test). Evidence that a particular neurocognitive measure was sensitive to bipolar disorder was provided if the grouping variable explained a significant proportion of trait variance at 5% FDR. Effect size estimates (standardized mean differences) were derived using the following equation: effect size = ((4*χ)/(n-χ))−1/2, where n is the number of individuals included in the analyses. Given the non-independence of participants, this effect size estimate is conservative.

Neurocognitive measures sensitive to bipolar disorder were examined in analogous analyses in unaffected (non-bipolar) 1st degree relatives to confirm that the candidate endophenotype reflected underlying genetic vulnerability. An individual’s liability for bipolar disorder was defined as the shortest genetic distance to an affected individual.

Bivariate Analyses

To determine if neurocognitive measures sensitive to bipolar disorder are influenced by the same genetic factors that influence risk for the illness, genetic correlation analyses were conducted. More formally, bivariate polygenic analyses were performed to estimate genetic (ρg) and environmental (ρe) correlations between affection status (bipolar or not bipolar) and cognitive measures sensitive to the illness. The significance of these correlations were tested by comparing the In likelihood for two restricted models (with either ρg or ρe constrained to equal 0.0) against the ln likelihood for the model in which these parameters were estimated. The In likelihood values of the general and restricted models were compared using the likelihood ratio test and significance was set at 5% FDR. A significant genetic correlation is evidence for pleiotropy, that a gene or set of genes influences both phenotypes26. In the context of this study, a significant genetic correlation suggests that the same genetic factor or factors may influence risk for bipolar disorder and neurocognitive performance.

Finally, multivariate analyses were performed on those neurocognitive traits genetically correlated with bipolar disorder to determine if these traits represent independent risk factors. Significant genetic correlation between neurocognitive traits correlated with bipolar disorder raises the possibility that a single genetic factor (gene or set of genes) influences multiple endophenotypes, implying that these endophenotypes may not represent independent genetic paths to the illness.

Results

Sample Characteristics

Of the 660 subjects from extended pedigrees, 230 had a best estimate consensus DSM-IV diagnosis of bipolar disorder (161 Type I; 51 Type II; 6 Not Otherwise Specified) or schizoaffective disorder bipolar subtype (n=12) and are considered part of a “broad bipolar phenotype” (Table 2). Six individuals were diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective depressive subtype and were excluded from all analyses. Among family members without major psychosis, 243 were unaffected (non-bipolar spectrum) 1st degree, 86 were unaffected 2nd degree and 42 were unaffected 3rd degree relatives of affected individuals. 108 subjects were not biologically related to affected individuals and were used to form a genetic “unrelated” control sample: 59 were subjects who had married into selected families and 49 were unaffected subjects with no biologic or familial ties to the other subjects. Liability groups differed in average age (F[3,704]=34.57, p<0.0001) and in gender distributions (χ=7.15, p=0.05), but not in education level (F[3,704]=0.89, p=0.44). Individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype had significantly higher rates of anxiety disorders (χ=32.34, p>0.0001) and past alcohol abuse/dependence (χ=63.52, p>0.0001) than their non-bipolar family members and unrelated participants. 56% of the individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype were prescribed psychotropic medications at the time of assessment: 66 were taking antidepressants, 16 were on lithium, 41 were on mood stabilizers, 30 were on anticonvulsants, 57 were on sedatives, 37 were on atypical antipsychotics, 23 were on typical antipsychotics, and 11 were on stimulants.

Table 2.

Demographic Characteristics of Extended Pedigree Sample

n Age1 Education1 %Female % Anxiety % Alcohol
Broad Bipolar Phenotype2 230 40.41 (14.1) [15-77] 10.48 (4.0) [0-24] 61% 35% 40%
Bipolar I Disorder 161 40.67 (13.7) [15-77] 10.32 (3.9) [0-20] 59% 41% 39%
Unaffected 1st Degree 243 44.14 (16.7) [15-85] 10.24 (4.3) [0-22] 63% 20% 21%
Unaffected 2nd & 3rd Degree 128 28.14 (13.5) [15-81] 10.95 (3.2) [3-19] 49% 16% 9%
Unrelated Subjects 108 42.76 (14.0) [16-79] 10.50 (4.2) [0-18] 63% 12% 10%
1

Age and education expressed in years (standard deviations) [range]

2

Bipolar I disorder and bipolar spectrum disorders (schizoaffective disorder, bipolar NOS, bipolar II disorder)

Heritability

Estimated heritability for 21 neurocognitive variables are presented in Table 3. Although demographic covariates were fixed for all analyses, those covariates that reached significance for a specific neurocognitive measure are included in the far right column. Two measures failed to reach significant levels of heritability: CVLT Semantic Clustering and CVLT Delay Recall. Performance on these measures was significantly correlated with education and differed by location. When these covariates were omitted from analyses, these indices were significantly heritable.

Table 3.

Heritability of Neurocognitive Measures

Trait n Heritability1 Heritability2 p-values3 Significant Covariates4
Letter Fluency 685 0.644 (0.08) 0.464 (0.09) 6.9×10−11 ed, loc
Semantic Fluency 685 0.363 (0.08) 0.211 (0.08) 4.7×10−04 age, sex, ed, loc
Digit Symbol Coding 692 0.510 (0.07) 0.335 (0.09) 2.0×10−05 age, age*sex, age2, age2*sex, ed, loc
Trails A 613 0.529 (0.08) 0.287 (0.08) 5.3×10−06 age, sex, age*sex, age2, age2*sex, ed, loc
CPT Hits 548 0.261 (0.09) 0.265 (0.09) 4.5×10−04 age, ed
CPT Catch Trials 548 0.237 (0.11) 0.335 (0.11) 1.4×10−04 age, ed, loc
Digit Span Forward 670 0.487 (0.08) 0.511 (0.09) 7.0×10−11 age, ed
Stroop Reaction Time 567 0.142 (0.08) 0.223 (0.09) 0.0012 age, sex, ed
Digit Span Backward 670 0.402 (0.07) 0.329 (0.08) 3.2×10−08 age, ed, loc
Letter-Number Span 670 0.503 (0.07) 0.477 (0.09) 7.3×10−10 age, ed
Spatial Delayed Response 624 0.346 (0.09) 0.202 (0.09) 0.0043 age, ed
Object Delayed Response 622 0.566 (0.07) 0.473 (0.10) 1.0×10−09 age, ed, loc
CVLT Learning 692 0.297 (0.08) 0.141 (0.08) 0.0194 age, sex, age*sex, ed, loc
CVLT Semantic Clustering 692 0.316 (0.08) 0.090 (0.12) 0.2188 loc
CVLT Delay Recall 687 0.199 (0.07) 0.033 (0.07) 0.3128 age, sex, age*sex, ed, loc
Digit Symbol Recall 692 0.435 (0.08) 0.254 (0.10) 9.0×10−04 age, age*sex, age2, age2*sex, ed, loc
Facial Memory Immediate 699 0.544 (0.08) 0.448 (0.09) 7.1×10−09 age, sex, age2*sex, ed
Facial Memory Delay 699 0.518 (0.08) 0.416 (0.09) 2.6×10−08 age, sex, ed
Matrix Reasoning 694 0.448 (0.06) 0.331 (0.07) 9.7×10−09 age, age*sex, ed, loc
PCET Correct 700 0.192 (0.07) 0.163 (0.07) 0.0028 age, ed, loc
Trails B 578 0.318 (0.10) 0.258 (0.09) 6.5×10−04 age, sex, age*sex, age2*sex, ed, loc
1

Heritability estimate, h2, and (standard error) without demographic covariates

2

Heritability estimate, h2, and (standard error) with demographic covariates

3

p-values for heritability estimates including covariates

4

ed = education, loc = location

Location of Assessment

Location of assessment significantly influenced performance on many neurocognitive measures (see Table 3). Examining differences between individual sites indicated that the effect sizes for these differences were generally small. Indeed the average effect size for neurocognitive performance differences between San Antonio and Costa Rica was 0.32, between San Antonio and Mexico City was 0.20, and between Mexico City and Costa Rica was 0.17. In contrast, the effect size for formal education was 0.61. This pattern of results is consistent with our prior work in these locations13.

Sensitivity to Liability for Bipolar Disorder

Results from the analyses determining the sensitivity of individual neurocognitive measures to liability for bipolar disorder are given in Table 4. Individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype were statically impaired on 6 of the 19 heritable cognitive measures compared to unrelated, unaffected subjects, after controlling FDR. To examine diagnostic specificity, analyses were repeated after constraining the affected group to bipolar I disorder patients alone. Bipolar I patients were impaired on all of the measures identified in the broad phenotype group, and were additional deficit on semantic fluency.

Table 4.

Group Differences for Heritable Neuropsychological Measures

Broad Bipolar Phenotype Bipolar I Disorder 1st Degree Relatives
Trait1 χ 2 p-value Effect Size χ 2 p-value Effect Size χ 2 p-value Effect Size
Letter Fluency 0.99 0.319 0.109 0.68 0.408 0.101
Semantic Fluency 4.34 0.037 0.228 5.83 0.016 0.298 0.34 0.561 0.062
Digit Symbol Coding 27.67 1.4×10−07 0.597 30.74 3.0×10−08 0.718 16.68 4.4×10−05 0.447
Trails A 5.05 0.025 0.246 3.01 0.083 0.213
CPT Hits 0.76 0.384 0.095 0.22 0.637 0.058
CPT Catch Trials 4.04 0.044 0.220 3.11 0.078 0.216
Digit Span Forward 3.63 0.057 0.208 2.10 0.147 0.178
Stroop 0.39 0.533 0.068 0.15 0.696 0.048
Digit Span Backward 2.04 0.153 0.156 1.25 0.264 0.136
Letter-Number Span 17.87 2.4×10−05 0.473 16.14 5.9×10−05 0.505 13.05 3.0×10−04 0.393
Spatial Delayed Response 2.60 0.107 0.176 4.67 0.031 0.266
Object Delayed Response 46.29 1.0×10−11 0.797 42.36 7.6×10−11 0.865 27.15 1.9×10−07 0.579
CVLT Learning 22.14 2.5×10−06 0.529 26.13 3.2×10−07 0.656 2.46 0.117 0.168
Digit Symbol Recall 0.64 0.424 0.087 0.10 0.757 0.038
Facial Memory Immediate 44.36 2.7×10−11 0.777 42.40 7.4×10−11 0.865 44.42 2.7×10−11 0.761
Facial Memory Delay 37.53 9.0×10−10 0.707 37.89 7.5×10−10 0.810 33.57 6.9×10−09 0.650
Matrix Reasoning 1.71 0.190 0.143 2.43 0.119 0.191
PCET Correct 0.75 0.385 0.095 0.94 0.333 0.118
Trails B 3.63 0.057 0.208 3.54 0.060 0.231
1

Statically significant group differences, after controlling the false discovery rate, are bolded

Given that individuals with bipolar disorder have increased rates of comorbid anxiety and alcohol use disorders (e.g. Table 2), it is unclear if neurocognitive impairments are due to these co-occurring illnesses or bipolar disorder per se. Hence, analyses were repeated with lifetime history of anxiety disorders and alcoholism included as covariates. Although anxiety disorders were associated with fewer hits on the CPT (p=9.8×10−3) and alcoholism was linked to deficits on the CPT (hits, p=1.3×10−4) and the trail making test (Trails A p=0.043; Trails B p=3.2×10−3), the addition of these covariates did not substantially alter the pattern of results in Table 4.

As psychotropic medications may influence neurocognitive performance, use of psychotropic medications at the time of assessment was entered as a covariate into the model. However, individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype remained significantly impaired on measures sensitive to bipolar disorder when controlling for medication usage (data not shown). Furthermore, unaffected 1st degree relatives, who do not typically use psychotropic medications, were impaired on five of these measures relative to unrelated control subjects: digit symbol coding, letter-number span, object delayed response, and immediate and delayed facial memory.

Bivariate Analyses

Estimates of genetic and environmental correlations performed on the neurocognitive measures impaired in bipolar disorder and the broad bipolar phenotype are given in Table 5. Of these measures, three were significantly negatively correlated with affection status: digit symbol coding, object delayed response and immediate facial memory, indicating that worse cognitive performance on these measures is related to increasing genetic risk for bipolar disorder. None of the environmental correlations were significant, given the false discovery rate. Similar correlations were observed when affection status was defined as bipolar I disorder (digit symbol coding: ρg ± SE = −0.511±0.21, p=0.037; object delayed response: −0.531±0.17, p=0.009; immediate facial memory = −0.442±0.19, p=0.034).

Table 5.

Bivaraite Analysis between Affection Status and Neurocognitive Measures

Broad Bipolar Phenotype

Genetic Correlation Environmental Correlation
Trait1 ρ g SE P ρ e SE P
Semantic Fluency 0.224 0.27 0.401 −0.169 0.07 0.023
Digit Symbol Coding −0.698 0.18 0.002 −0.017 0.09 0.851
Letter-Number Span −0.165 0.20 0.436 −0.110 0.09 0.250
Object Delay Response −0.629 0.16 0.002 −0.042 0.10 0.688
CVLT Learning −0.466 0.30 0.160 −0.180 0.07 0.016
Facial Memory Immediate −0.573 0.18 0.004 0.032 0.10 0.731
Facial Memory Delay −0.395 0.19 0.067 −0.070 0.09 0.457
1

Statically significant correlations, after controlling the false discovery rate, are bolded

In the full sample, digit symbol coding performance was significantly genetically correlated with object delayed response performance (ρg ± SE = 0.784±.09, p=3.3×10−03) and immediate facial memory (0.593±0.16, p=0.001). Similarly, object delayed response performance was significantly correlated with immediate facial memory (0.619±0.12, p=9.9×10−5). This suggests that these traits were influenced by the same genetic factors.

Discussion

Measures of processing speed, working memory and declarative (facial) memory are candidate endophenotypes for bipolar disorder. Each of these measures was heritable, impaired in individuals with the illness and their non-bipolar relatives, and genetically correlated with affection status. Although a number of investigators have demonstrated that healthy first-degree relatives of bipolar probands have memory or executive functioning impairments, this is the first large-scale family-based study to provide evidence that the neurocognitive deficits found in bipolar disorder are related to genetic liability for the illness and, thus, can be considered intermediate phenotypes for bipolar disorder. While there are various definitions for intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes2, 12, there is universal agreement that these measures must be associated with genetic liability for the illness under investigation. An implicit assumption about endophenotypes is that the same gene or genes that convey risk for disease also influence the intermediate phenotype. More formally, there is an assumption of pleiotropy for illness status and the intermediate phenotype. In quantitative genetic methodology, pleiotropy can be demonstrated through estimation of genetic correlation26. Unfortunately, in non-twin designs, genetic correlation requires relatively large samples (n>300-500) of related individuals and, hence, is rarely applied in psychiatric genetics investigations. However, without formally demonstrating pleiotropy, one of the primary criteria of an intermediate phenotype is left untested. Each of the candidate endophenotypes identified in this study meet the necessary and sufficient criteria for a viable intermediate phenotype, providing testable hypotheses about brain systems implicated in the pathophysiology of bipolar disorder. Indeed, the delineation of three neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder should spur a series of neuropsychological, neuroimaging and molecular genetic studies designed to refine the traits in question and determine their neural and genetic correlates.

Digit-symbol coding performance is an index of speed of processing, or the time needed to execute simple cognitive operations. Although speed of processing has not been localized to a single brain region, cognitive neuroscience emphasizes the integration of information across spatially distinct brain regions27, 28, suggesting that cognitive slowing as indexed by processing rate is related to neuronal efficiency29. Several investigators have reported that euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder have moderate to severe digit-symbol impairments6, 30, 31. Although, speed of processing measures like the digit-symbol may be negatively influenced by psychotropic medications, a recent meta analysis in schizophrenia suggested that medication status had little effect on patient performance32. Furthermore, at least two previous studies have reported poor digit-symbol performance in unaffected family members of bipolar disorder probands33, 34. Our results further these findings by showing a significant negative genetic correlation between bipolar disorder and digit-symbol coding performance.

The object delayed response task35 includes the maintenance and manipulation of memory for complex visual objects and was modeled after measures used in electrophysiological studies of non-human primates36. Functional MRI experiments indicate that task performance engages a complex network of brain areas associated with working memory (e.g. prefrontal, temporal and parietal regions)37. Furthermore, patients with bipolar disorder showed relative hyperactivation in the right prefrontal and anterior cingulate regions and relative hypoactivation in medial temporal and visual processing regions compared to healthy subjects37. While relatively few investigators have used delayed response tasks in bipolar disorder, working memory deficits, particularly when manipulation of information is critical, have been consistently reported in remitted patients5. In contrast, there is little prior evidence for working memory dysfunction in unaffected bipolar relatives (e.g. 8). The current findings suggest that the application of working memory or executive measures specifically designed for use in affective disorders could improve sensitivity of these measures to liability for bipolar disorder38. Nonetheless, the current findings should be confirmed in an independent sample.

Declarative memory impairments are among the most commonly reported cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder. Although most investigators report verbal memory impairments, visuospatial and facial memory deficits have also been observed in euthymic bipolar patients5, 8. Indeed, in an early investigation of monozygotic twin-pairs discordant for bipolar disorder, Gourovitch and colleagues39 reported facial memory impairment in unaffected co-twins of bipolar probands. In the current study, verbal declarative memory was assessed with the California Verbal Learning Test17, 18. While CVLT performance was heritable and impaired in individuals with bipolar disorder, non-bipolar first-degree relatives were not statically different from healthy subjects. Our findings are similar to those reported in a recent meta analysis of cognitive performance in unaffected 1st degree relatives of bipolar probands8. Hence, while the current study nominates a facial declarative memory task as a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder, verbal declarative memory measures can not be excluded and may represent important risk factors for the illness. More generally, while findings from the current study are encouraging, they are limited by the families studied and the neurocognitive tests employed. Additional studies in different samples and with overlapping test batteries are warranted.

Significant heritability estimates indicate that a phenotype is more strongly influenced by genetic than environmental factors. However, heritability estimates do not provide information concerning the underling genetic architecture of a phenotype and are subject to a number of assumptions40. Indeed, phenotypes with high heritable estimates are not necessarily influenced by fewer genes or by genes with larger effects. For example, normal variation in adult height is highly heritable (h2=0.89-0.9341), but current estimates suggest that up to 44 independent loci are associated with normal stature42, 43. In contrast, phenotypes with somewhat lower heritability estimates may have less complex genetic architectures. For example, the estimated heritability of the neuregulin 1 transcript was 0.50, but linkage analysis indicated a single locus (lod=15.8) on chromosome 844, 45. Thus, while an intermediate phenotype should be heritable, the strength of the heritability estimate may be less critical. Heritability estimates for bipolar disorder are typically higher than those reported here for neurocognitive traits. Yet, it remains to be seen if the genetic architectures of these neurocognitive endophenotypes are less complex than for the illness itself. Furthermore, simulation studies indicate that quantitative intermediate phenotypes should be significantly more powerful for identifying genes than qualitative diagnoses46.

Intermediate phenotypes can inform psychiatric noseology. Although bipolar disorder is considered a discreet psychiatric illness with a unique etiology, there is growing evidence that the illness may have some common genetic roots with schizophrenia47. Each of the neurocognitive endophenotypes nominated for bipolar disorder have also been found in schizophrenia. Indeed, the digit-symbol task applied here was the most sensitive cognitive measure of genetic liability for schizophrenia in our extended pedigree study13. Similarly, the facial declarative memory measure has previously been found sensitive risk for schizophrenia48. Although the object delayed response task35 employed here has not been used in schizophrenia family studies, similar measures have been proposed as intermediate phenotypes for the illness48. Although the spatial delayed response task applied in the current study was previously shown to be sensitive to genetic liability for schizophrenia49, 50, individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype were not impaired on this measure, implying some level of diagnostic specificity. However, we have shown in two separate samples that bipolar patients with lifetime history of psychotic symptoms are impaired on this test, while patients without formal psychosis are not38, 51, suggesting that the manifestation of psychosis may be linked to spatial working memory deficits rather than a specific diagnostic category. It is possible that by using a broad bipolar phenotype, where few individuals manifest psychosis, we obscured deficits on this test. Indeed, when affection status was limited to bipolar I disorder alone, impairments on this test were more pronounced, though still below the FDR requirement. These findings raise questions about the specificity of these intermediate phenotypes for bipolar disorder alone, rather than psychotic illness more generally. However, it is unlikely that these questions will be fully addressed until the genetic factors that influence both these neurocognitive endophenotypes and bipolar disorder are identified and found to also confer risk for schizophrenia.

A number of the neurocognitive measures impaired in individuals with bipolar disorder were not impaired in non-bipolar 1st degree relatives. Performance on these measures may be sensitive to the environmental factors necessary for the diathesis of the illness and thus impairment in affected individuals is not associated with genetic risk for bipolar disorder. Alternately, these tests may be influenced by affective or psychotic symptoms, psychotropic medication, sleep disturbances or other sequella of the illness. Indeed, current symptomatology and psychotropic medications used to treat bipolar disorder have been shown to impair performance on specific neurocognitive tests6, 52. Although 56% of the patients included in the study were medicated at the time of assessment, inclusion of a single covariate coding psychotropic medication usage did not significantly alter the observed of results. While this analysis was coarse and did not account for potentially important factors (e.g. lifetime history of medication usage, specific class of medication), each of the proposed intermediate phenotypes was also impaired in non-symptomatic and unmedicated unaffected 1st degree relatives. Hence, it is unlikely that the candidate endophenotypes identified here are explained by patient mood symptoms or medication usage.

Although the pattern of results did not significantly differ when affection status was constrained to bipolar I disorder, it is difficult to make conclusions about diagnostic specificity from this sample. Seventy percent of affected individuals were bipolar type I and those individuals with type II, NOS or schizoaffective disorder were genetically related to bipolar I patients. In order to draw inferences about the utility of these cognitive endophenotypes for genetic investigation of non-bipolar type I illnesses, family with these illnesses without bipolar I should be studied. Similar conclusions can be drawn about psychiatric co-morbidities like alcohol abuse and anxiety disorders.

One potential advantage of quantitative neurocognitive endophenotypes is that they may be less genetically complex than psychiatric diagnoses12, 53. While this potential has yet to be empirically demonstrated54, it is possible to examine multiple endophenotypic markers simultaneously, focusing on the gene or genes common to all traits. Such a strategy could reduce the numbers of candidate genes nominated for functional genomic studies, but is only possible in situations where there is a reasonable potential that the various traits share at least part of their genetic makeup. Given that each of the neurocognitive measures elucidated in the current experiment were genetically correlated with each other, it is unknown if these measures represent independent risk factors for bipolar disorder or are sensitive to a single “cognitive” liability factor. Although this potential genetic overlap facilitates the strategy for prioritizing genes for additional study as described above, additional studies examining the co-segregation of cognitive deficits over multiple generations should be examined.

As demonstrated here and elsewhere13, 48, computerized neurocognitive measures can be efficiently and reliably administered to large numbers of individuals, a requirement for effective endophenotypes55. Indeed, each of the tests sensitive to risk for bipolar disorder was developed specifically for large-scale studies of psychopathology. These tests are currently being applied in a multi-site family-based linkage study of bipolar disorder and in a molecular genetics study of randomly ascertained individuals in large extended pedigrees. Together, these studies should provide clues into the genetic architecture of the neurocognitive endophenotypes identified in the current study and should facilitate localization and identification of specific genes that contribute to these endophenotypes. These genes, in turn, should be examined for association to bipolar disorder. This iterative process should provide a window into the causal neurobiological pathways involved in the illness. The ultimate promise of these measures is the discovery of causal pathway for mental illness and the potential to develop biomarkers for psychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgments

Financial support for this study was provided by NIMH grants MH69856 (PI: MA Escamilla) and MH080912 (PI: DC Glahn) and a NARSAD young investigator award (DC Glahn). NIMH (MH071753) and Fogarty (TW006152) training grants supported Drs. Hare and Barguil, respectively. SOLAR is supported by NIMH grant MH59490 (J Blangero). The authors wish to thank Brandi Weaver and Joseph Peters for assistance in data collection and Mercedes Ramirez, MD, Salvador Contreras, MD, Juan Zavala MD, and Lorena Jimenez, MD for assistance in best estimation.

Funding/Support: Financial support for this study was provided by NIMH grants MH69856 (PI: MA Escamilla) and MH080912 (PI: DC Glahn) and a NARSAD young investigator award (DC Glahn). SOLAR is supported by NIMH grant MH59490 (J Blangero).

Footnotes

Author contributions: Drs. Glahn, Escamilla and Almasy take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the statistical analyses. All authors had full access to all of the data in the study.

Financial disclosure: None of the authors have anything to disclose.

Presentation of Data: Portions of these data were presented at CINP’s 2008 annual conference in Munich, Germany.

References

  • 1.Ferreira M, O’Donovan M, Meng Y, et al. Collaborative genome-wide association analysis supports a role for ANK3 and CACNA1C in bipolar disorder. Nat Genet. 2008 Aug; doi: 10.1038/ng.209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gottesman II, Gould TD. The Endophenotype Concept in Psychiatry: Etymology and Strategic Intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Merikangas K, Chakravarti A, Moldin S, et al. Future of genetics of mood disorders research. Biol Psychiatry. 2002 Sep;52(6):457–477. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01471-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hasler G, Drevets W, Gould T, Gottesman I, Manji H. Toward constructing an endophenotype strategy for bipolar disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2006 Jul;60(2):93–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Robinson L, Thompson J, Gallagher P, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006 Jul;93(1-3):105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Martínez-Arán A, Vieta E, Reinares M, et al. Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004 Feb;161(2):262–270. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Burdick K, Goldberg J, Harrow M, Faull R, Malhotra A. Neurocognition as a stable endophenotype in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006 Apr;194(4):255–260. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000207360.70337.7e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med. 2008 Jun;38(6):771–785. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glahn D, Bearden C, Niendam T, Escamilla M. The feasibility of neuropsychological endophenotypes in the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2004 Jun;6(3):171–182. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00113.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Almasy L, Blangero J. Endophenotypes as quantitative risk factors for psychiatric disease: rationale and study design. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001;105(1):42–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Blangero J, Williams JT, Almasy L. Novel family-based approaches to genetic risk in thrombosis. J Thromb Haemost. 2003 Jul;1(7):1391–1397. doi: 10.1046/j.1538-7836.2003.00310.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bearden C, Freimer N. Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: ready for primetime? Trends Genet. 2006 Jun;22(6):306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2006.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Glahn DC, Almasy L, Blangero J, et al. Adjudicating neurocognitive endophenotypes for schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007 Mar 5;144B(2):242–249. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nurnberger JI, Jr., Blehar MC, Kaufmann CA, et al. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994 Nov;51(11):849–859. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950110009002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Maxwell ME. Manual for the FIGS. National Institute of Mental Health; Bethesda, MD: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leckman JF, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM. Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: a methodological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982 Aug;39(8):879–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) Manual. Harcourt Assessment Company; San Antonio: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fortuny L, Romo D, Heaton R, Pardee R. Manual De Normas y Procedimientos Para La Bateria Neuropsicologica en Espanol. Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers; The Netherlands: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cornblatt BA, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Global attentional deviance as a marker of risk for schizophrenia: specificity and predictive validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1985;94(4):470–486. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.94.4.470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Barch D, Braver T, Akbudak E, Conturo T, Ollinger J, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. Cereb Cortex. 2001 Sep;11(9):837–848. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Glahn DC, Therman S, Manninen M, et al. Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003 Apr 1;53(7):624–626. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01641-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gur R, Ragland J, Moberg P, et al. Computerized neurocognitive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001 Nov;25(5):766–776. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00278-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kurtz M, Ragland J, Moberg P, Gur R. The Penn Conditional Exclusion Test: a new measure of executive-function with alternate forms of repeat administration. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004 Mar;19(2):191–201. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Almasy L, Blangero J. Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees. American Journal of Human Genetics. 1998 May;62(5):1198–1211. doi: 10.1086/301844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Almasy L, Dyer T, Blangero J. Bivariate quantitative trait linkage analysis: pleiotropy versus co-incident linkages. Genet Epidemiol. 1997;14(6):953–958. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1997)14:6<953::AID-GEPI65>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fry AF, Hale S. Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biol Psychol. 2000 Oct;54(1-3):1–34. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00051-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hansell NK, Wright MJ, Luciano M, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Martin NG. Genetic covariation between event-related potential (ERP) and behavioral non-ERP measures of working-memory, processing speed, and IQ. Behav Genet. 2005 Nov;35(6):695–706. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-6188-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rypma B, Berger JS, Genova HM, Rebbechi D, D’Esposito M. Dissociating age-related changes in cognitive strategy and neural efficiency using event-related fMRI. Cortex. 2005 Aug;41(4):582–594. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70198-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ferrier I, Stanton B, Kelly T, Scott J. Neuropsychological function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1999 Sep;175:246–251. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.3.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Thompson J, Gallagher P, Hughes J, et al. Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic patients with bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2005 Jan;186:32–40. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dickinson D, Ramsey M, Gold J. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 May;64(5):532–542. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ferrier I, Chowdhury R, Thompson J, Watson S, Young A. Neurocognitive function in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: a preliminary report. Bipolar Disord. 2004 Aug;6(4):319–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McIntosh A, Harrison L, Forrester K, Lawrie S, Johnstone E. Neuropsychological impairments in people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. Br J Psychiatry. 2005 May;186:378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.5.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Glahn DC, Barrett J, Bearden CE, et al. Dissociable mechanisms for memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2006 Aug;36(8):1085–1095. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Petrides M. Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey. J Neurosci. 1995 Jan;15(1 Pt 1):359–375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00359.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Robinson J, Bearden C, Monkul E, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Velligan D, Frangou S, Glahn D. Fronto-temporal Dysregulation in Remitted Bipolar Patients: An fMRI Delayed-Non-Match-to-Sample (DNMS) Study. Bipolar Disord. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00703.x. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Barguil M, et al. The neurocognitive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 Oct 15;62(8):910–916. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gourovitch M, Torrey E, Gold J, Randolph C, Weinberger D, Goldberg T. Neuropsychological performance of monozygotic twins discordant for bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1999 Mar;45(5):639–646. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00148-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Devlin B, Daniels M, Roeder K. The heritability of IQ. Nature. 1997;388:468–471. doi: 10.1038/41319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Silventoinen K, Sammalisto S, Perola M, et al. Heritability of adult body height: a comparative study of twin cohorts in eight countries. Twin Res. 2003 Oct;6(5):399–408. doi: 10.1375/136905203770326402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Weedon MN, Frayling TM. Reaching new heights: insights into the genetics of human stature. Trends Genet. 2008 Dec;24(12):595–603. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Weedon MN, Lango H, Lindgren CM, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 20 loci that influence adult height. Nat Genet. 2008 May;40(5):575–583. doi: 10.1038/ng.121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Goring HH, Curran JE, Johnson MP, et al. Discovery of expression QTLs using large-scale transcriptional profiling in human lymphocytes. Nat Genet. 2007 Oct;39(10):1208–1216. doi: 10.1038/ng2119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Carless M, Curran J, Charlesworth J, et al. Transcriptional Profiling for Identification of Cis-Regulated Genes Involved in Complex Brain-Related Disorders. Paper presented at: International Society of Psychiatric Genetics; New York, NY, USA. 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wijsman E, Amos C. Genetic analysis of simulated oligogenic traits in nuclear families and extended pedigrees: summary of GAW10 contributions. Genetic Epidemiology. 1997;14:719–735. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1997)14:6<719::AID-GEPI28>3.0.CO;2-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Craddock N, O’Donovan MC, Owen MJ. Genes for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder? Implications for psychiatric nosology. Schizophr Bull. 2006 Jan;32(1):9–16. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Gur R, Nimgaonkar V, Almasy L, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007 May;164(5):813–819. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.5.813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Glahn D, Therman S, Manninen M, et al. Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2003 Apr 1;53(7):624–626. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01641-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Glahn DC, Almasy L, Blangero J, et al. Adjudicating Neurocognitive Endophenotypes for Schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2007;144(2):242–249. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Cakir S, et al. Differential working memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: effects of lifetime history of psychosis. Bipolar Disord. 2006 Apr;8(2):117–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00296.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Holmes M, Erickson K, Luckenbaugh D, et al. A comparison of cognitive functioning in medicated and unmedicated subjects with bipolar depression. Bipolar Disord. 2008 Nov;10(7):806–815. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00628.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Greenwood T, Braff D, Light G, et al. Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 Nov;64(11):1242–1250. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Flint J, Munafò M. The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychol Med. 2007 Feb;37(2):163–180. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Schulze T, McMahon F. Defining the phenotype in human genetic studies: forward genetics and reverse phenotyping. Hum Hered. 2004;58(3-4):131–138. doi: 10.1159/000083539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Holdnack J. Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: an update. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004 Mar;10(2):301–303. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Administration and scoring manual : Wechsler adult intelligence scale. 3rd ed Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997 Feb;54(2):159–165. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140071013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI) Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES