Abstract
Previous studies in cat visual cortex reported that area 18 can actively drive neurons in area 17 through cortico-cortical projections. However, the dynamics of such cortico-cortical interaction remains unclear. Here we used multielectrode arrays to examine the spatiotemporal pattern of neuronal activity in cat visual cortex across the 17/18 border. We found that full-field contrast reversal gratings evoked oscillatory wave activity propagating from area 18 to 17. The wave direction was independent of the grating orientation, and could not be accounted for by the spatial distribution of receptive field latencies, suggesting that the waves are largely mediated by intrinsic connections in the cortex. Different from the evoked waves, spontaneous waves propagated along both directions across the 17/18 border. Together, our results suggest that visual stimulation may enhance the flow of information from area 18 to 17.
Introduction
Theoretical studies showed that a traveling wave is an emergent behavior of systems with spatially restricted connectivity [1], [2], [3]. Experimental studies have observed waves in a variety of brain regions, including the olfactory bulb [4], [5], [6] , hippocampus [7], primary sensory cortices [8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], and motor cortex [17], suggesting that waves may contribute to cortical function.
In the visual cortex, various wave-like activities have been observed. For example, a single spike can initiate a radial wave across the surface of cortex [18], and a local visual stimulus can evoke a traveling wave spreading from the retinotoptic representation of the stimulus to neighboring cortical area [8], [10]. The biophysical substrate for the spreading activity may involve horizontal connections in the visual cortex [19]. Traveling waves were also reported in the visual cortices of ferrets and turtles [11], [20], [21]. These waves traveled across the border of different visual areas, probably mediated by long range cortico-cortical connections. The inter-cortical wave-like activity may serve as a means of communication between different areas [11], [22].
Connections between areas 17 and 18 in cat visual cortex are spatially reciprocal [23], [24], [25]. Previous studies showed that neurons in area 17 can be directly driven by neurons in area 18 in addition to the feedforward inputs from layer 4 [26], [27], suggesting that visual information may flow from area 18 to 17. At the population level, however, the spatiotemporal pattern of dynamic interaction between the two areas remains to be investigated. In particular, it is not clear whether the information flow can be manifested as propagating waves. On the other hand, since waves may occur under both sensory stimulation and spontaneous conditions [9], [28], it is also of interest to examine the relationship between the dynamics of evoked and spontaneous activities.
In this study, we have examined the spatiotemporal properties of population activity across areas 17 and 18 by recording local field potentials (LFPs) from the superficial layers with multielectrode arrays. Using full-field contrast reversal gratings that covered the receptive fields (RFs) of all recording sites, we showed that the visually evoked LFPs exhibited systematic phase shifts across the cortical surface, and the activity propagated as waves from area 18 to 17. The direction of the waves was independent of stimulus orientation and the direction of phase gradient could not be accounted for by the spatial gradient of RF latencies. Furthermore, the wave speed increased with the frequency of neuronal oscillation. In spontaneous activity, propagating waves traveled in both directions between areas 17 and 18. Thus, visual stimulation may modulate spontaneous activity to facilitate wave propagation from area 18 to 17.
Materials and Methods
Surgery
All procedures were in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines, and the protocol was approved by the Biological Research Ethics Committee of the Shanghai Institutes for Biological Sciences, Chinese Academy of Sciences (permit number: ER-SIBS-221105C).
The methods for surgery have been previously described in detail [29]. Briefly, adult cats (weighing 1.5–4.5 kg each) were initially anesthetized with ketamine (25–30 mg/kg, intramuscularly) and injected with atropine sulfate (0.05 mg/kg, subcutaneously). After tracheotomy, the animals were artificially ventilated. During recordings, anesthesia was maintained with sodium pentothal (3 mg/kg/h) or urethane (13–20 mg/kg/h) and paralysis with Gallamine (10–20 mg/kg/h). The end-tidal CO2 was maintained at ∼3.5%, and body temperature at 37.5°C–38.5°C. Eyes were fitted with appropriate contact lenses with artificial pupils of 3-mm diameter, and focused on a tangent screen. A craniotomy (∼6 mm in diameter) was performed over the primary visual cortex (centered at P2–4 and L2–3, including both areas 17 and 18).
Recordings
Recordings were made with multielectrode arrays (Blackrock Microsystems), consisting of 8×8 or 10×10 grids of microelectrodes (1-mm electrode length, 400-µm electrode separation). To record from the superficial layers, the arrays were inserted 0.5–0.6 mm into the cortex using a pneumatic insertion device. To prevent pulsation, the array and the exposed cortex were covered in 1.5–2% agar. Signals were amplified using a Cerebus 128-channel system (Blackrock microsystems). All LFP signals were sampled at 2 KHz per channel with a wide-band front-end filter (0.3–500 Hz). LFP data were post-processed by removing channels that may not be functional due to broken electrodes or noise.
Visual Stimulation
Visual stimuli were generated with a PC containing a Leadtek GeForce 6800 video card and displayed on a CRT monitor (Sony CPD-G520, mean luminance of 32 cd/m2, 1024×768 resolution, refresh rate 120 Hz) placed 57 cm away from the animal's eyes. Luminance nonlinearities were corrected through software. Stimuli were presented to the contralateral eye. For each experiment, we first mapped the receptive fields (RFs) of the LFP responses using sparse noise stimuli, in which a white or black square (0.5°–2°) was flashed on a gray background at each of the 16×16 (or 12×12) positions in a pseudorandom sequence. The stimuli were presented with an effective frame rate of 60 Hz so that each sparse noise image appeared for 16.7 ms. Each square was presented 30–60 times. We then presented full-field (20°–30°) contrast reversal gratings (spatial frequency: 0.2–0.5 cycle/°, mostly 0.32 cycle/°; contrast: 75%), covering the RFs of all recording sites. In 10 experiments, the gratings were contrast-reversed at 4 Hz. In 4 experiments, the gratings were contrast-reversed at 2, 3, 4, or 5 Hz. These frequencies were chosen because the optimal temporal frequency for neurons in the primary visual cortex was 3–4 Hz [30], [31]. The gratings were presented at 4 different phases (0°, 90°, 180°, and 270°) and 8 different orientations (spaced at 22.5°). Both the phase and the orientation of the gratings were randomized. Each grating was presented for 1 sec and repeated 80 times. In 9 experiments, we measured spontaneous responses using a blank screen of mean luminance as well as evoked responses using contrast-reversal gratings at 4 Hz. In 4 experiments, we measured spontaneous responses as well as evoked responses using contrast-reversal gratings at 2, 3, 4, and 5 Hz.
Data Analysis
All data analyses were implemented with custom software written in Matlab.
Power Spectrum Estimation
We used a multi-taper method [32] to estimate the power spectrum of the responses. For a given LFP signal , the power spectrum was given by
where is the length of the LFP signal, and is the Slepian function. The Slepian functions are orthogonal basis functions that are characterized by bandwidth in frequency and length in time. There are tapers that are spectrally concentrated in the frequency band . In our analysis, we used Hz and .
Spatial Coherence
To isolate coherent responses within separate frequency bands, we computed spatial coherence using a method of space-frequency singular value decomposition (SVD) [12]. The LFP signal was transformed from the time domain to the frequency domain:
where represents the LFP signal on channel at time , is the Slepian function at time and bandwidth . To decompose the signals, we performed an SVD on the complex matrix :
where = 5. The coherence for a given frequency was measured by:
where is the number of non-zero singular values. If the spatial pattern of activity is completely coherent in the given frequency band, there is only one non-zero singular value. Thus, approaches 1 for highly coherent responses and for random responses that are spatially uniform.
Filtering, Hilbert Transform, and Estimation of Wave Parameters
We applied a Kaiser filter [33] to band-pass-filter the LFP signals evoked by contrast reversal gratings. The passband of the Kaiser filter was 2–6, 4–8, 6–10, and 10–12 Hz for the responses evoked by gratings that contrast-reversed at 2, 3, 4, and 5 Hz, respectively. Other parameters of the Kaiser filter were: transition bandwidth = 1 Hz, passband ripple = 0.01 dB, and stopband attenuation = 60 dB [33]. Forward and backward filtering was used to prevent phase distortion. For the experiments in which we measured both the spontaneous responses and the evoked responses, spontaneous responses were filtered in a similar manner. After filtering, the signals in each channel were independently Z scored [18]. We used the Hilbert transform to extract the analytic phase from the band-limited signals [17]. The analytical signal of is defined as:
in which is the analytic phase, which is given by , and is the analytic amplitude. Although the analytic phase does not have a clear physiological meaning, the phases across space reflect the relative timing of activity across space [17].
For the filtered signal at each time and coordinate of the array, we calculated an instantaneous phase . The phases in the array were unwrapped in both spatial and temporal dimensions, either for the purpose of computing phase gradient or displaying the phase map averaged over the time points within one cycle of oscillation [17]. The velocity of coherent activity was defined as the velocity of the contours of constant phase [34]. We computed the wave velocity, , by taking the total derivative of with respect to time [17]:
where denotes the spatial gradient of the instantaneous phases across the array. To accurately estimate the speed and direction of the waves, for each time point we first defined a measure of phase gradient directionality () to determine the degree of alignment of the phase gradients across the array [17]:
For a time point at which the phase gradients at all positions are perfectly aligned, . For a time point with randomly distributed phase gradients, Since measures how well the phase gradients at all sites are aligned, responses at those time points with had well defined propagation velocity and direction [17], and thus were considered to be wave-like. To estimate the proportion of wave-like time points for the single-trial responses, we defined wave probability as the proportion of time points at which . From the single-trial responses, we estimated the direction and speed of the waves using those time points with . The wave direction was estimated by:
and the wave speed by:
Linear Fit between Wave Speed and Oscillation Frequency
The relationship between wave speed and the frequency of neuronal oscillation was fitted by a linear function, . The goodness of fit was estimated by the statistics:
where is the fitted value, is the observed value, and is the average of all observed values. An close to 1 indicates that the linear function well fits the data, whereas an close to 0 indicates that the fit is not significantly better than approximating the data by its mean.
Wave Snapshots Display
To display the spatiotemporal pattern of the activity, the responses from each channel were normalized to their maximum amplitude [35]. The normalized signals were then color coded according to a linear pseudocolor scale. Data of broken channels were interpolated (Matlab function ‘interp2’). Each individual snapshot was again linearly interpolated for display purposes.
Fourier Analysis
For the evoked responses (i.e., the responses averaged over all trials) in each experiment, we also used Fourier analysis to obtain a phase map and analyze the phase gradient. To compute a single Fourier component for the evoked responses, we multiplied the raw signal by the corresponding complex exponential:
where is the frequency of interest. The phase of this Fourier component is given by
where and denote the imaginary and real parts of , respectively. We then obtained a phase map using the phase of the 2nd harmonic component in each channel. The phase obtained by this method represented the latency of response [10]. The gradient directionality () of the phase map was measured by where denotes the spatial gradient of the phases.
RF Mapping and RF Latency Estimation
Spatiotemporal RF maps were obtained by cross-correlating the LFP with the visual stimulus [36], [37]:
where represents the spatiotemporal visual stimulus and represents the LFP signal from a recording site. Since previous studies reported that the signal-to-noise ratio of responses to black squares was higher than that to white squares [38], [39] (which was confirmed by our own data), we used the RF mapped with black squares for subsequent analysis.
We computed the temporal impulse response as the variances of spatial RFs at different time delays [37], [40]:
Spatial RF at peak variance was obtained for each recording site in the array. The location of the 17/18 border can be estimated from the changes in RF size and the reversal of RF progression [41], [42] (Figure S1). RF latency was defined as the time when the rising phase of the impulse response reached 40% of its peak value [43]. For each experiment, we measured the of RF latency map (a map that contained the latencies of all recording sites) by where denotes the spatial gradient of the RF latencies across the array.
Results
Stimulus-Induced Oscillations Propagated As Waves Across the 17/18 Border
We recorded LFPs in response to full-field contrast reversal gratings from the superficial layers of areas 17 and 18 using multielectrode arrays (Materials and Methods). As shown in Figure 1A and B, 4-Hz contrast reversal gratings induced responses that oscillated at approximately 8 cycles/sec ( i.e., the 2nd harmonic), consistent with previous studies [10], [44]. Power spectra of the LFPs across multiple channels showed prominent peaks at the 2nd harmonic frequency (Figure 1C). To examine whether the broadband responses contained a distinct spatiotemporal structure of the 2nd harmonic responses, we performed a space-frequency SVD analysis to compute the spatial coherence within different frequency bands (Materials and Methods). We found that the coherence peaked at frequencies around 8 Hz (Figure 1D). Because high spatial coherence is a necessary (but not sufficient) condition for wave-like activity [17], this result suggests that visually evoked LFPs may propagate as waves. We next extracted the visually induced oscillatory signals by band-pass filtering the responses at 6–10 Hz (Materials and Methods). During the oscillations in single trials, we observed systematic phase differences of the responses across different channels distributed along the medial-lateral axis (Figure 2A, middle). When we examined the spatial pattern of the normalized responses at a series of time points (Materials and Methods), we found plane waves traveling across the array (Figure 2B). To further examine the spatial organization of the phases, we extracted the instantaneous phase at each time point for each channel in the array (Materials and Methods). A phase map across the array was computed by averaging the phases over one cycle of oscillation for each channel (Figure 2A, right). For the wave-like responses shown in Figure 2B, the phase maps exhibited clear gradients, the directions of which were approximately along the medial-lateral axis (Figure 2B, right).
We further examined the responses averaged over all trials (i.e., evoked responses). Figure 2C shows the evoked responses for 7 recording sites across the array as indicated in Figure 2A. The presence of phase offsets in these sites suggests that, the wave activity is evoked by the stimulus rather than caused by random noise that would be cancelled out by averaging. Moreover, systematic phase shifts occurred in every cycle of the evoked responses, indicating that the propagating activity is in the form of one-cycle-one-wave [45]. Figure 2D shows the images of evoked responses for the time points within two cycles of oscillation recorded from the right hemisphere of one cat. In all 10 experiments, the propagation direction of the evoked waves was from area 18 to 17. To analyze the phase organization of the evoked responses shown in Figure 2D, we extracted the analytic phase at each time point using Hilbert transform (Materials and Methods) and computed a phase map by averaging the phases over all cycles of oscillation. We also performed Fourier analysis on the unfiltered LFPs and computed a phase map from the 2nd harmonic component of the response (Materials and Methods). For the two phase maps estimated by the two methods (Figure 2D, right panel), the degree of phase shift across space was similar , with a systematic increase in phase from the medial to the lateral part of the cortex. For all 10 experiments, when we quantified the gradient of the 2nd harmonic phase map by the measure of (Materials and Methods),we found that the GD was above 0.5 in most experiments (see below), further supporting the idea that the evoked responses propagated as waves.
Direction and Speed of Stimulus-Induced Waves
To quantitatively characterize the waves in single trials, for each time point we used the analytic phases at all sites to obtain an instantaneous phase map, from which we further computed the instantaneous propagation direction and propagation speed (Materials and Methods). Because only a well-defined phase gradient indicates the presence of wave, we first quantified the degree of alignment of the phase gradients using the measure (Materials and Methods). is 1 for a time point at which the phase gradients at all positions are perfectly aligned, i.e., the propagation direction is spatially coherent, and 0 if the phase gradients are random. We then used the time points at which the instantaneous to estimate the wave direction (Materials and Methods). Figure 3A shows the distribution of propagating directions for waves recorded from one experiment. Clearly, the waves propagated in a dominant direction along the axis that was approximately parallel to the medial-lateral axis and almost perpendicular to the 17/18 border. To illustrate the spatial dynamics of this dominant propagation direction, we used the data points within 30° of the peak direction to plot an average phase map (Figure 3B), which also showed phase gradient across the array. For the stimulation condition of different orientations, the distributions of wave directions were similar (an example was shown in Figure 3C), suggesting that the waves may be mediated by intrinsic connectivity. In all 10 experiments, the dominant wave direction for single-trial responses was from area 18 to area 17 (Figure 3D). For those experiments in which we measured responses using gratings contrast-reversed at different temporal frequencies (2, 3, 4, or 5 Hz), the distributions of wave directions were also similar for different frequencies (an example was shown in Figure 3E). We also estimated the propagation speed using those time points with . For the responses evoked by 4-Hz contrast reversal gratings (Figure 4A), the waves propagated with a mean speed of 0.12±0.03 m/sec (mean ± SD). When we examined the wave speed for responses evoked by stimuli at different temporal frequencies (Figure 4B), we found that the mean wave speed was positively correlated with the frequency of neuronal oscillation (y = 0.015·x, = 0.987, Figure 4C). Thus, the stimulus-evoked responses satisfied a dispersion relation [46], suggesting that the responses propagate as waves.
RF Latency Cannot Account for the Phase Shifts of Evoked Responses
Theoretical studies suggest that traveling waves may arise from a network of weakly coupled oscillators [47]. If the waves we observed were explained by such a mechanism, the phase gradient would depend on the intra- and inter-cortical interactions. On the other hand, area 18 is known to be predominantly innervated by Y-type thalamic afferents, whereas area 17 is innervated by both X- and Y-type axons [48]. Since the conduction velocity of Y-type axons is faster than that of X-type axons [49], [50], the phase gradient across the 17/18 border may be explained by the spatial distribution of response latencies due to feedforward inputs. To examine this issue, we measured the LFPs in response to sparse noise stimuli, in which each small square was supposed to mainly activate the feedforward inputs. Response latency for each site was estimated from the temporal impulse response of the RF (Materials and Methods, Figure S1). As shown in Figure 5A, while the 2nd harmonic phase maps measured with large stimuli exhibited a clear gradient along the medial-lateral axis, there was no such gradient in the RF latency maps. The for the 2nd harmonic phase map was significantly higher than that for the RF latency map (P<0.01, n = 10, Wilcoxon signed rank test, Figure 5B), suggesting that the systematic phase differences evoked by the full-field contrast reversal gratings cannot be fully accounted for by the timing differences of the feedforward inputs. Instead, intra- and inter-cortical interactions [47] may be involved in generating the visually evoked waves.
Comparison Between Evoked Waves and Spontaneous Waves
Previous studies showed that spontaneous responses also propagated as waves [9], [16], and the spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous activity resembled the sensory-evoked responses [51]. Since the visually evoked waves were likely mediated by intrinsic cortical connections, we wondered whether a similar wave pattern was present in the spontaneous activity. We compared the spontaneous responses and the responses evoked by 4-Hz contrast reversal gratings measured from the same animals (Materials and Methods). We analyzed the spatial coherence of spontaneous responses across all frequency bands and found that high coherence was limited to low frequencies (Figure S2A). Because our method to characterize wave parameters only applies to signals in a narrow frequency band (Materials and Methods), we band-pass filtered the spontaneous responses at 6–10 Hz, in order to compare them with the 8-Hz oscillatory responses evoked by the 4-Hz contrast reversal gratings. Similar to the stimulus-evoked responses, we obtained the analytic phase by the method of Hilbert transform and analyzed the phase gradient for each time point of the spontaneous responses. We first compared the wave probability (Materials and Methods) between the evoked and spontaneous activities. We found that wave probability was significantly higher in the stimulus-evoked (27.0%±1.6%, mean ± SEM) than the spontaneous activity (17.7%±1.9%, mean ± SEM) (P<0.05, Wilcoxon signed rank test). We then estimated wave direction and wave speed for spontaneous activity using those time points at which . Figure 6A shows the distributions of wave directions for spontaneous (orange) and evoked activities (blue) from two experiments. In contrast with the evoked waves, the spontaneous waves appeared to show two propagating directions. For the spontaneous activity, average phase maps computed using data points within 30° of the two peak directions showed opposite phase gradients (Figure 6B). Snapshots of different spontaneous events measured in the same animal also revealed opposite wave directions (Figure 6C). In all 9 experiments, we found that the spontaneous activity propagated with two major directions, either from area 17 to 18 or from area 18 to 17, whereas the evoked activity mostly propagated from area 18 to 17 (Figure 6D). When we analyzed the spontaneous responses at a broader frequency band (3–10 Hz), we also observed two peaks in the distribution of wave directions (Figure S2B). This suggests that visual stimulation modulate the propagating directions of ongoing wave activity. In addition to the difference in propagating direction, the distribution of wave speed was significantly shifted to higher values under visual stimulation than spontaneous condition (P<0.005, n = 9, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, Figure 6E). Such speed difference between evoked and spontaneous responses increased with the frequency of neuronal oscillation (Figure S2C).
Discussion
We have investigated the spatiotemporal pattern of population activity in cat visual cortex using LFP recordings with multielectrode arrays. We found that the stimulus-entrained oscillatory activity propagated as waves from area 18 to 17. The direction of the evoked waves was not affected by the stimulus orientation and could not be predicted from the spatial distribution of RF latencies measured with local stimuli. Propagating waves were also observed in the spontaneous activity, which were bidirectional between areas 17 and 18. Furthermore, wave probability and wave speed were higher in the stimulus-induced than the spontaneous responses. Together, these results suggest that visual stimulation may modulate the spontaneous activity to facilitate the information flow from area 18 to 17.
Anatomical and physiological studies show that areas 17 and 18 receive direct inputs from LGN and the two areas process different aspects of visual information in a parallel manner [52]. Area 17 is mainly activated by X- and Y-fibers from LGN, whereas area 18 is activated by the Y-signals [48]. In addition to receiving ascending LGN inputs, areas 17 and 18 also receive cortico-cortical inputs from each other [23], [24], [25]. When the Y inputs from LGN were deactivated, neurons in area 18 were tuned to slower stimuli, suggesting that area 17 may activate area 18 through cortico-cortical connections [53]. By inactivating the activity in LGN, Mignard and Malpeli [26] showed that the cells in layers 2/3 of area 17 were well driven if area 18 was intact, but their responses were reduced when area 18 was destroyed, suggesting that neurons in layers 2/3 of area 18 can directly drive neurons in layers 2/3 of area 17. In agreement with this result, Martinez-Conde et al [54] showed that pharmacological blockade of neuronal activity in layers 2/3 of area 18 could change the visual responses of cells in layers 2/3 of area 17. In our study, we found that the spontaneous activity propagated as traveling waves along both directions between areas 17 and 18, which may be mediated by the intrinsic cortico-cortical connections. During oscillatory responses elicited by visual stimulation, the waves predominantly traveled from area 18 to 17, suggesting that cortical processing of visual information involves modulating the spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous activity. To determine whether the direction of evoked waves is due to the latency difference inherit from the feedforward inputs, we measured the latencies for RFs in all recording sites. The spatial distribution of RF latencies did not exhibit clear gradient, suggesting that the evoked waves may be largely mediated by intrinsic cortical circuits and may emerge from the modulation of spontaneous activity.
We used wave probability, defined as the proportion of time points at which , to estimate the proportion of wave-like time points for single-trial responses. The low percentage of wave-like time points found in our study may be due to the following reasons. First, the visually-evoked waves may be suppressed by the spontaneous activity, as demonstrated by previous studies [28], [55]. Second, we detected waves based on the gradients of phase maps, which had low spatial resolution (8×7 resolution in most cases) and thus may be sensitive to noise. For example, if one channel was contaminated by noise, phase gradients at the neighbouring four channels would be inaccurate. This may decrease the degree of alignment among the phase gradients across the array, leading to a decrease in the proportion of time points with . Although the wave probability in single trials was low, waves in the evoked responses were continuous and propagated in a pattern of one-cycle-one wave. We analyzed the probability of time points with for each cycle of the single-trial responses induced by 4-Hz contrast reversal gratings (Figure S3). Although the probability in the first and the last cycle was a bit lower than that in the other cycles (probably because the stimulus onset and offset disrupted the neuronal oscillation induced by the contrast reversal gratings), the probability was more or less close to 27% in each cycle. Since the wave-like activity was present in each cycle, the pattern of one-cycle-one-wave could be preserved by averaging over trials. Generally, we found that evoked responses could propagate as waves as long as the responses were well entrained by the contrast reversal gratings.
Using voltage-sensitive dye (VSD) imaging in rat visual cortex, Xu et al [9] reported that the visually evoked waves were initiated in V1 and were compressed/reflected at the V1/V2 border, whereas the spontaneous waves propagated across the cortex without compression and reflection. Although our method could not analyze compression/reflection of the waves, both our study and this previous work showed that the evoked waves exhibited differences from the spontaneous waves. Using VSD imaging in cat visual cortex, Benucci et al [10] examined waves in the space domain and orientation domain, respectively, with focal or full-field contrast reversal gratings. In their analysis of the cortical responses to full-field stimuli, the authors focused more on the phase differences across different orientation columns than those across different cortical distances. While Benucci et al [10] investigated the difference between circuits that underlie spatial selectivity and orientation selectivity, our study aimed to explore the spatiotemporal pattern of population activity across cortical areas. By imaging cortical responses to drifting gratings, a recent study by Onat et al [56] also observed propagating waves in areas 17 and 18, but the wave direction was dependent on the drifting direction of the grating and the wave speed was related to the speed of the grating.
Wave activity has mostly been studied using VSD imaging, which revealed a variety of wave patterns, including plane waves, spiral waves, and target waves [45]. Quantitative description of the waves is essential for us to understand the dynamics of population activity. Early work used a method of SVD to identify wave patterns [12]. However, it is difficult to use this method to automatically detect waves. The algorithm we adapted was based on computing the gradient of instantaneous phase obtained by Hilbert transform [17]. This method allows us to describe propagating activity in the oscillations in a specified frequency band, estimate wave direction and wave speed from single-trial responses, and detect waves under both evoked and spontaneous conditions. However, the method we used can only be applied to plane waves. Recent work described an algorithm based on comparing the correlation of temporal features across space with established flow templates [22], [57]. This method works well in detecting target waves and spiral waves in addition to plane waves, but the method is computationally intensive.
The functional significance of the waves is still unclear. Since the direction of evoked waves was independent of stimulus orientation, we speculate that the waves may be involved in communication between areas 17 and 18 rather than coding of specific visual features. Previous studies in cat visual cortex suggested that the interaction between areas 17 and 18 plays an important role in the integration of border and surface information [58], [59]. In addition, neurons in areas 17 and 18 can respond to second-order stimuli [60], [61], which may be important in figure-ground segregation. It is of interest for future study to use more complex stimuli to investigate whether the waves across areas 17 and 18 contribute to visual scene segmentation. Furthermore, phase information of neuronal oscillations is important in cortical processing. For example, the spike times of a single neuron relative to the phase of network oscillation can be used to carry stimulus information [33]. Future studies may explore the functions of waves by combining information coding and the spatiotemporal pattern of oscillation phases.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank H-Y. Zhong and W-Q. Xu for technical assistance. We thank Y. Zhu and C. Baker for discussions and comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was supported by 973 program (2011CBA00405). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Kuramoto Y. 1984. Chemical Oscillations, Waves and Turbulence: Springer, New York.
- 2.Kopell N, Ermentrout GB. Symmetry and phaselocking in chains of weakly coupled oscillators. Communications on Pure and Applied Mathematics. 1986;39:623–660. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Breakspear M, Heitmann S, Daffertshofer A. Generative models of cortical oscillations: neurobiological implications of the kuramoto model. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:190. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Orbach HS, Cohen LB. Optical monitoring of activity from many areas of the in vitro and in vivo salamander olfactory bulb: a new method for studying functional organization in the vertebrate central nervous system. J Neurosci. 1983;3:2251–2262. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-11-02251.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lam YW, Cohen LB, Wachowiak M, Zochowski MR. Odors elicit three different oscillations in the turtle olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2000;20:749–762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00749.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Delaney KR, Gelperin A, Fee MS, Flores JA, Gervais R, et al. Waves and stimulus-modulated dynamics in an oscillating olfactory network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91:669–673. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.2.669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lubenov EV, Siapas AG. Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves. Nature. 2009;459:534–539. doi: 10.1038/nature08010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Grinvald A, Lieke EE, Frostig RD, Hildesheim R. Cortical point-spread function and long-range lateral interactions revealed by real-time optical imaging of macaque monkey primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2545–2568. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02545.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Xu W, Huang X, Takagaki K, Wu JY. Compression and reflection of visually evoked cortical waves. Neuron. 2007;55:119–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Benucci A, Frazor RA, Carandini M. Standing waves and traveling waves distinguish two circuits in visual cortex. Neuron. 2007;55:103–117. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Roland PE, Hanazawa A, Undeman C, Eriksson D, Tompa T, et al. Cortical feedback depolarization waves: a mechanism of top-down influence on early visual areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:12586–12591. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604925103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Prechtl JC, Cohen LB, Pesaran B, Mitra PP, Kleinfeld D. Visual stimuli induce waves of electrical activity in turtle cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94:7621–7626. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.14.7621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Song WJ, Kawaguchi H, Totoki S, Inoue Y, Katura T, et al. Cortical intrinsic circuits can support activity propagation through an isofrequency strip of the guinea pig primary auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:718–729. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kleinfeld D, Delaney KR. Distributed representation of vibrissa movement in the upper layers of somatosensory cortex revealed with voltage-sensitive dyes. J Comp Neurol. 1996;375:89–108. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961104)375:1<89::AID-CNE6>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Petersen CC, Grinvald A, Sakmann B. Spatiotemporal dynamics of sensory responses in layer 2/3 of rat barrel cortex measured in vivo by voltage-sensitive dye imaging combined with whole-cell voltage recordings and neuron reconstructions. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1298–1309. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-04-01298.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Han F, Caporale N, Dan Y. Reverberation of recent visual experience in spontaneous cortical waves. Neuron. 2008;60:321–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rubino D, Robbins KA, Hatsopoulos NG. Propagating waves mediate information transfer in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1549–1557. doi: 10.1038/nn1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nauhaus I, Busse L, Carandini M, Ringach DL. Stimulus contrast modulates functional connectivity in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:70–76. doi: 10.1038/nn.2232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1983;3:1116–1133. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-05-01116.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Senseman DM, Robbins KA. Modal behavior of cortical neural networks during visual processing. J Neurosci. 1999;19:RC3. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-j0004.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Senseman DM, Robbins KA. High-speed VSD imaging of visually evoked cortical waves: decomposition into intra- and intercortical wave motions. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1499–1514. doi: 10.1152/jn.00475.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Takagaki K, Zhang C, Wu JY, Lippert MT. Crossmodal propagation of sensory-evoked and spontaneous activity in the rat neocortex. Neuroscience Letters. 2008;431:191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.11.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Symonds LL, Rosenquist AC. Corticocortical connections among visual areas in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1984;229:1–38. doi: 10.1002/cne.902290103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Symonds LL, Rosenquist AC. Laminar origins of visual corticocortical connections in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1984;229:39–47. doi: 10.1002/cne.902290104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Salin PA, Kennedy H, Bullier J. Spatial reciprocity of connections between areas 17 and 18 in the cat. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1995;73:1339–1347. doi: 10.1139/y95-188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mignard M, Malpeli JG. Paths of information flow through visual cortex. Science. 1991;251:1249–1251. doi: 10.1126/science.1848727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bullier J, McCourt ME, Henry GH. Physiological studies on the feedback connection to the striate cortex from cortical areas 18 and 19 of the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1988;70:90–98. doi: 10.1007/BF00271851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Petersen CC, Hahn TT, Mehta M, Grinvald A, Sakmann B. Interaction of sensory responses with spontaneous depolarization in layer 2/3 barrel cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:13638–13643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2235811100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wang C, Yao H. Sensitivity of V1 neurons to direction of spectral motion. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:964–973. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ. Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurones in areas 17 and 18 of the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol. 1978;283:101–120. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.DeAngelis GC, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat's striate cortex. II. Linearity of temporal and spatial summation. J Neurophysiol. 1993;69:1118–1135. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.4.1118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Percival DB, Walden AT. Spectral analysis for physical applications : multitaper and conventional univariate techniques. Cambridge ; New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Montemurro MA, Rasch MJ, Murayama Y, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S. Phase-of-firing coding of natural visual stimuli in primary visual cortex. Curr Biol. 2008;18:375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fleet DJ, Jepson AD. Computation of Component Image Velocity from Local Phase Information. International Journal of Computer Vision. 1990;5:77–104. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jin W, Zhang RJ, Wu JY. Voltage-sensitive dye imaging of population neuronal activity in cortical tissue. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;115:13–27. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(01)00511-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jones JP, Palmer LA. The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:1187–1211. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.6.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Xing D, Yeh CI, Shapley RM. Spatial spread of the local field potential and its laminar variation in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11540–11549. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2573-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Yeh CI, Xing D, Shapley RM. “Black” responses dominate macaque primary visual cortex v1. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11753–11760. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1991-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Jin JZ, Weng C, Yeh CI, Gordon JA, Ruthazer ES, et al. On and off domains of geniculate afferents in cat primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:88–94. doi: 10.1038/nn2029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Malone BJ, Kumar VR, Ringach DL. Dynamics of receptive field size in primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:407–414. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tusa RJ, Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC. The retinotopic organization of area 17 (striate cortex) in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1978;177:213–235. doi: 10.1002/cne.901770204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tusa RJ, Rosenquist AC, Palmer LA. Retinotopic organization of areas 18 and 19 in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1979;185:657–678. doi: 10.1002/cne.901850405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jin J, Wang Y, Lashgari R, Swadlow HA, Alonso JM. Faster thalamocortical processing for dark than light visual targets. J Neurosci. 2011;31:17471–17479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2456-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ. Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat's striate cortex. J Physiol. 1978;283:53–77. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Wu JY, Huang X, Zhang C. Propagating Waves of Activity in the Neocortex: What They Are, What They Do. Neuroscientist. 2008;14:487–502. doi: 10.1177/1073858408317066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Nunez PL, Srinivasan R. A theoretical basis for standing and traveling brain waves measured with human EEG with implications for an integrated consciousness. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2006;117:2424–2435. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.06.754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ermentrout GB, Kleinfeld D. Traveling electrical waves in cortex: insights from phase dynamics and speculation on a computational role. Neuron. 2001;29:33–44. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00178-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Dreher B, Leventhal AG, Hale PT. Geniculate input to cat visual cortex: a comparison of area 19 with areas 17 and 18. J Neurophysiol. 1980;44:804–826. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.44.4.804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cleland BG, Dubin MW, Levick WR. Sustained and transient neurones in the cat's retina and lateral geniculate nucleus. J Physiol. 1971;217:473–496. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1971.sp009581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hoffmann KP, Stone J, Sherman SM. Relay of receptive-field properties in dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1972;35:518–531. doi: 10.1152/jn.1972.35.4.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kenet T, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M, Grinvald A, Arieli A. Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature. 2003;425:954–956. doi: 10.1038/nature02078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Payne BR, Peters A. The concept of cat primary visual cortex. In: Payne BR, Peters A, editors. The cat primary visual cortex. New York: Academic press; 2002. pp. 1–129. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Dreher B, Michalski A, Cleland BG, Burke W. Effects of selective pressure block of Y-type optic nerve fibers on the receptive-field properties of neurons in area 18 of the visual cortex of the cat. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9:65–78. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800006374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Martinez-Conde S, Cudeiro J, Grieve KL, Rodriguez R, Rivadulla C, et al. Effects of feedback projections from area 18 layers 2/3 to area 17 layers 2/3 in the cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2667–2675. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A. Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science. 1996;273:1868–1871. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Onat S, Nortmann N, Rekauzke S, Konig P, Jancke D. Independent encoding of grating motion across stationary feature maps in primary visual cortex visualized with voltage-sensitive dye imaging. Neuroimage. 2011;55:1763–1770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Takagaki K, Zhang C, Wu JY, Ohl FW. Flow detection of propagating waves with temporospatial correlation of activity. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;200:207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hung CP, Ramsden BM, Chen LM, Roe AW. Building surfaces from borders in Areas 17 and 18 of the cat. Vision Res. 2001;41:1389–1407. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00075-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hung CP, Ramsden BM, Roe AW. A functional circuitry for edge-induced brightness perception. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1185–1190. doi: 10.1038/nn1948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Zhou YX, Baker CL., Jr Spatial properties of envelope-responsive cells in area 17 and 18 neurons of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1038–1050. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.3.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Baker CL., Jr Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9:461–466. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80069-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.