Abstract
Objective:
Despite prevention efforts, driving after drinking (DAD) is a prevalent high-risk behavior among college students and is a leading cause of death and injury. Examination of factors predicting future DAD behavior is necessary to develop efficacious targeted interventions to reduce this behavior among college students. The current study evaluated demographic, social cognitive, and behavioral predictors of DAD using longitudinal data.
Method:
Participants were 655 nonabstaining college students (67.2% female; 60.3% White; Mage = 19.3 years) who completed online surveys at two time points 12 months apart.
Results:
Results revealed that participants consistently overestimated their peers’ approval (injunctive norms) of DAD. In a three-step hierarchical logistic regression model, injunctive norms, age, and past DAD behavior uniquely contributed to the prediction of this behavior 12 months later. Neither sex nor membership in a sorority or fraternity emerged as significant predictors.
Conclusions:
The findings provide important new insights into the longitudinal predictors of DAD among college students and highlight the need for DAD interventions, particularly among older students.
Approximately 16%–30% of u.s. college students report driving after drinking (DAD) alcohol (Beck et al., 2010; Fromme et al., 2008; Hingson et al., 2009; Quinn and Fromme, 2011), exposing themselves and others to serious potential consequences. For example, an estimated 49% of traffic fatalities among students are alcohol related (Hingson et al., 2009). Previous cross-sectional research has identified a number of risk factors for DAD among college students, including male sex, fraternity/sorority affiliation, and being 21 years of age or older (Kenney et al., in press; Wechsler et al., 2003), as well as owning a fake identification card (Nguyen et al., 2011), having a family history of alcohol problems (LaBrie et al., 2011), and having sensation-seeking personality characteristics (Zakletskaia et al., 2009). Further, heavy drinkers are more likely to drive after drinking alcohol (Kenney et al., in press; LaBrie et al., 2011; Quinn and Fromme, 2011), perhaps because of a decreased perception of subjective intoxication and perceived driving impairment (Marczinski et al., 2008).
The social norms approach suggests that perceptions of both how others behave (descriptive norms) and how accepting or approving they are of certain behaviors (injunctive norms) can exert considerable influence on individuals’ behavior (Berkowitz, 2004; Borsari and Carey, 2001; Cialdini, 1991). Congruent with this approach, a significant body of evidence has demonstrated that injunctive norms for drinking are strongly predictive of college students’ alcohol use (Borsari and Carey, 2001; LaBrie et al., 2010a; Neighbors et al., 2007, 2008). A limited number of studies specifically investigating injunctive norms for DAD have indicated that those who perceive their friends to be more approving of DAD are more likely to engage in it (Gastil, 2000; McCarthy et al., 2007). Further, recent research has demonstrated that students tend to overestimate how approving a typical student is toward DAD and that personal attitudes toward DAD mediate the relationship between misperception of typical student approval and this behavior (Kenney et al., in press). These findings diverge from the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), which posits that attitudes and perceived norms independently predict motivation to engage in DAD (Armitage et al., 2002). Instead, Kenney and colleagues’ (in press) findings align with models of social norms (Perkins, 1985) that suggest perceived peer norms can shape personal attitudes toward DAD, in addition to directly influencing such behavior.
A major limitation of previous collegiate DAD findings is the scarcity of longitudinal assessment of DAD risk factors. Past longitudinal studies have identified demographic risk factors for DAD including male sex and turning 21 years of age (Beck et al., 2010). Longitudinal data from high school students indicates that frequency of alcohol use and prior DAD are associated with increased likelihood and frequency of subsequent DAD (McCarthy and Pedersen, 2009). Further, prevalence of DAD has been shown to decrease as students transition from high school to college (Fromme et al., 2008). In the current study, we aimed to extend previous collegiate research by using longitudinal data to examine the role of social cognitive variables, (e.g., attitudes toward DAD and injunctive norms) in predicting future DAD behaviors. We hypothesized that less disapproving attitudes toward DAD, perceptions of typical students as less disapproving of DAD, greater alcohol use, and having engaged in DAD in the past would be associated with future DAD behavior after controlling for established demographic factors.
Method
Procedures and participants
Participants were students from a medium-sized private university who took part in two larger studies in two consecutive fall semesters. During both years, students were randomly selected from the student population and mailed and emailed invitations to complete online surveys (more detailed descriptions of Time 1 and Time 2 data collection may be found in LaBrie et al., 2010a and 2010b, respectively). A total of 2,219 students were invited to participate at both time points. Of these students, 27.2% completed one survey and 34.2% completed both surveys. The sample for the current study consisted of 655 nonabstainers who participated at both time points. More of the participants (67.2%) were female, and the mean age was 19.3 years (SD = 0.86; range: 18–22 years). Of students participating, 60.3% were White, 12.6% multiracial, 7.1% Asian, 3.8% African American/Black, 1.8% Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander and 14.4% other.
Measures
Participants reported age, sex, race, ethnicity, and whether they were affiliated with a fraternity or sorority at Time 1. In addition, the following were assessed:
Driving after drinking.
At both time points, participants completed an item assessing whether, within the past 3 months, they had driven shortly after drinking three or more drinks. Participants responded using a scale from 0 (never) to 4 (more than 10 times). This item, modeled after the 23-item Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (White and Labouvie, 1989), has previously been used as a measure of DAD (La-Brie et al., 2010a).
Driving after drinking attitude and injunctive norms.
The Injunctive Norms Questionnaire (Baer, 1994) was completed at both time points and measured DAD attitudes and injunctive norms for the typical student at their school. Participants indicated the extent to which they and the typical student approved of “driving a car after drinking” on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disapprove to 7 = strongly approve).
Weekly alcohol use.
At Time 1, the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (Collins et al., 1985; Dimeff et al., 1999) assessed the number of standard drinks consumed in a typical week during the past month. Participants were provided with guidelines of what constitutes a standard drink (e.g., 12 oz. of beer, 4 oz. of wine, 1 cocktail with 1 1/4 oz. of 80 proof distilled spirits).
Family history.
Participants’ family history of alcohol problems was assessed at Time 1 by asking if any biological relatives “had a significant drinking problem—one that should or did lead to treatment” (Miller and Marlatt, 1984). Participants responded yes or no to this question.
Analysis plan
Because of the limited number of participants who had engaged in DAD more than twice in the past 3 months (Time 1: 2.6%; Time 2: 4.5%), the DAD variables were coded into binary variables that indicated whether participants had driven after drinking three or more drinks (1 = yes, 0 = no). A three-step hierarchical logistic regression was performed with baseline alcohol consumption, baseline self-report of DAD, and demographic variables entered as the first hierarchical block. Given that attitudes mediate the relationship between injunctive norms and behavior (Kenney et al., in press), injunctive norms were entered in the second block, and students’ attitudes toward DAD were entered in the third block.
Results
Overall, 27.9% of students reported engaging in DAD at either time point, with the proportion of students reporting DAD increasing significantly from Time 1 (15.7%) to Time 2 (21.1%), Z = 2.29, p = .011. At Time 1, the vast majority of students (86.1%) reported strongly disapproving of DAD, whereas just over half (50.2%) believed that the typical student strongly disapproved of DAD. There were no significant differences in participants’ attitudes toward DAD at Time 1 (M = 1.2, SD = 0.69) and Time 2 (M = 1.3, SD = 0.71), t(651) = 1.64, p = .10, or differences in their perceptions of the typical student's approval of DAD across time (Time 1: M = 1.8, SD = 1.06; Time 2: M = 1.8, SD = 1.05), t(651) = 0.55, p = .59. At both points, the participants’ perceptions of the typical student were more approving than the actual student approval level (i.e., students overestimated the actual level of approval/injunctive norm), Time 1: t(654) = 14.96, p < .001; Time 2: t(652) = 13.65, p < .001.
Logistic regression analyses
Before the logistic regression, multiway cross-tabulations of all categorical independent variables were examined (Field, 2009). The inclusion of family history of alcohol problems and race/ethnicity resulted in empty cells and low expected frequencies; therefore, these variables were not included in the regression model. In bivariate chi-square analyses, neither of these variables was associated with self-report of DAD at Time 2. Correlations between Time 2 DAD and continuous independent variables are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variable | Time 2 DAD | Age | Baseline drinking | Injunctive norms |
Age | .17** | |||
Baseline drinking | .32** | .05 | ||
Injunctive norms for DAD | .12* | .07 | −.02 | |
Attitude toward DAD | .24** | .03 | .23** | .40** |
p < .01
p <.001.
Results from the hierarchical logistic regression are presented in Table 2. The final model was statistically significant, χ2(7) = 127.04, p < .001, Nagelkerke R2 = .28, Cox and Snell R2 = .18. Collinearity diagnostics were performed, and variance inflation factor for each variable did not exceed 1.38, suggesting that multicollinearity was not encountered. At Step 1, age (odds ratio [OR] = 1.59, 95% CI [1.23, 2.04]), baseline alcohol consumption (OR = 1.07, 95% CI [1.04, 1.10]), and baseline DAD (OR = 4.76, 95% CI [2.89, 7.82]) emerged as significant predictors of DAD at Time 2, with older students, those who drank more alcohol at baseline, and those who reported engaging in DAD at baseline more likely to report DAD at Time 2. Although men (64/215; 29.7%) were more likely than women (74/440; 16.8%) to report DAD, χ2(1) = 14.56, p < .001, in the multivariate model, participant sex did not significantly contribute to the prediction of DAD. Membership in a sorority or fraternity was not associated with self-reports of DAD at Time 2.
Table 2.
Predictor | B | SE | Wald | Odds ratio [95% CI] | ΔNagelkerke R2 |
Step 1 | .25** | ||||
Sexa | 0.17 | 0.25 | 0.43 | 1.18 [0.72, 1.95] | |
Age | 0.44 | 0.13 | 11.59 | 1.56 [1.21, 2.01] | |
Fraternity/sorority affiliationb | −0.37 | 0.26 | 2.06 | 0.69 [0.42, 1.14] | |
Baseline drinking | 0.06 | 0.01 | 20.75 | 1.07 [1.04, 1.09] | |
Baseline DADc | 1.47 | 0.27 | 30.86 | 4.35 [2.59, 7.31] | |
Step 2 | .02* | ||||
Injunctive norms | 0.25 | 0.11 | 5.13 | 1.28 [1.03, 1.58] | |
Step 3 | .00 | ||||
Attitude | 0.21 | 0.16 | 1.71 | 1.23 [0.90, 1.68] |
Notes: Odds ratios are reported for the final step of the logistic regression. CI = confidence interval.
For sex the reference level was female;
for fraternity/sorority affiliation the reference group was nonmembership in a fraternity/sorority;
for baseline driving after drinking the reference group was no driving after drinking.
p < .01;
p < .001.
In Step 2, the OR for injunctive norms for DAD was 1.36 (95% CI [1.12, 1.65]), indicating that those who perceived the typical student to be more approving of DAD were more likely to engage in DAD at Time 2. In Step 3, attitudes toward DAD did not significantly contribute to prediction of DAD (OR = 1.23, 95% CI [0.90, 1.68]). In the final model, age, baseline drinking, baseline DAD, and injunctive norms for DAD significantly contributed to the prediction of DAD at Time 2. We examined the sensitivity of the decision to collapse the DAD variable into two categories by re-running the analysis using a Poisson regression. The results were largely similar to the logistic regression. After controlling for demographic variables, baseline DAD, baseline alcohol use, and injunctive norms for the typical student predicted DAD at Time 2.
Discussion
The current study examined factors associated with DAD longitudinally in a sample of college students. More than one quarter of the participants reported DAD at one or both time points, indicating that, despite public health efforts, this behavior continues to be a significant problem. Older students—and students who at Time 1 reported DAD, drank more alcohol, and had less disapproving injunctive norms for a typical student—were more likely to report DAD at Time 2. The findings add further support to cross-sectional data demonstrating that normative beliefs are associated with DAD intentions and behavior (Armitage et al., 2002; LaBrie et al., 2011).
Consistent with past research (Armitage et al., 2002; Fairlie et al., 2010; Zakletskaia et al., 2009), older students were more likely than younger students to report DAD. This may reflect that older students are more likely to drink at venues that require transportation (Fromme et al., 2011), increasing their likelihood to engage in DAD. Harm-reduction efforts among college students often target freshmen, who tend to have the highest rates of alcohol consumption (Turrisi et al., 2000) and display alcohol-dependence symptoms at higher rates than the general adult population (Grekin and Sher, 2006). In contrast, because older students often “mature” out of risky alcohol use and report gradual decreases in their alcohol consumption (Larimer et al., 1998), fewer interventions target problematic drinking among this population. However, our results combined with past research showing the tremendous harm associated with DAD (Hingson, 2010; Hingson et al., 2009) suggest that older students are in fact a high-risk group that would benefit from specific interventions targeting this behavior.
DAD at Time 1 was the strongest predictor of DAD at Time 2: The odds of reporting DAD at Time 2 were more than four times higher than for participants who did not report DAD at Time 1. These results are consistent with previous research suggesting that past behavior is often the best predictor of future behavior (Conner and Armitage, 1998). Although the current study did not test for mediation effects, the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991) posits that past behavior influences future behavior through shaping people's beliefs and attitudes. Past behavior's unique contribution to predicting Time 2 DAD may indicate that social cognitive variables other than norms and attitudes may be important mediators of the relationship between past and future DAD (Norman and Conner, 2006). For example, past DAD may shape individuals’ perceptions of their ability to drive under the influence of alcohol or the perceived likelihood of risks associated with DAD, which in turn influences future DAD. Identifying the mechanism by which past DAD behavior affects future behavior should be the subject of future research.
The emergence of perceived injunctive norms for the typical student as a significant predictor when controlling for the other variables reveals that what students think about the attitudes of other students on their campus plays an important role in determining DAD. Extending the cross-sectional findings of Kenney et al. (in press), participants significantly overestimated the level of approval of the typical student across both time points. Although students perceived the typical student to be generally disapproving of DAD, the consistent overestimation of peers’ approval, along with the fact that greater perceived approval was associated with increased risk for DAD, provides further support for the potential benefits of normative interventions targeting college students’ DAD behaviors and using injunctive norms for DAD.
Although injunctive norms for DAD emerged as a significant predictor in the final model, students’ own attitudes toward the behavior did not. This finding is consistent with models of social norms that suggest students sometimes conform their behavior to social norms, even when doing so contradicts their personal attitude (Perkins, 1985). For example, for students who drive with friends to attend a social event, both peer pressure to take the group home and perceptions of others’ acceptance of DAD may have a greater impact on a student's decision to drive after drinking alcohol than the driver's own attitudes toward DAD. The results of the current study, however, differ from earlier cross-sectional research that suggests attitudes are associated with DAD intentions and behavior (Armitage et al., 2002; Kenney et al., in press). There are several reasons why the current study may have found that attitudes did not significantly predict DAD in the logistic model. For example, attitudes were correlated with both Time 1 DAD, r(653) = .31, p < .001, and baseline drinking, r(653) = .23, p < .001, and inclusion of these as covariates in the model may have made the unique contribution of attitudes become nonsignificant. Also, students’ self-reports of their attitudes may be more susceptible to social desirability bias than reports of injunctive norms for DAD. For instance, students may be more willing to report that students in general approve of DAD than to acknowledge that they personally approve of DAD. Indeed, 86.1% of students reported that they strongly disapproved of DAD, which may indicate that findings are a result of a floor effect.
Contrary to previous work (LaBrie et al., 2011), membership in a fraternity or sorority was not associated with DAD. Data for the present study came from a college at which DAD is discouraged among fraternity/sorority students by providing group transport to off-campus fraternity/sorority-sponsored social events, the locations of which are kept undisclosed. It is possible that campus differences in characteristics of fraternities/sororities may affect whether fraternity/sorority affiliation emerges as a risk factor for DAD.
Changing DAD behavior is likely to be challenging given the persistent nature of this behavior. Because both injunctive norms for DAD and past behavior uniquely contribute to predicting DAD, it may be beneficial to develop multipronged interventions that focus on correcting normative misperceptions as well as targeting various environmental factors (e.g., safe ride programs or placement of inhibiting cues in drinking environments).
This study has several limitations, including the use of a single-item measure of DAD, which did not capture other variables associated with level of intoxication (e.g., time spent drinking before driving). Further, the reliance on self-report of data may be subject to self-presentation biases and inaccurate recall. The online survey emphasized the confidentiality of participant responses, but because of the potentially illegal nature and perceived disapproval of this behavior, students may have underreported DAD. The current study relied on data from a single college campus, and further studies should use longitudinal data to examine factors associated with DAD across diverse campuses. Nonetheless, the longitudinal nature of the data in this study and their findings yield important insight into the phenomenon of college student DAD and point to significant sources of new preventative interventions, especially those targeting injunctive norms for DAD.
Footnotes
This research was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants R01 AA012547-06A2 and R21 AA020104. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism or the National Institutes of Health.
References
- Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991;50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
- Armitage CJ, Norman P, Conner M. Can the Theory of Planned Behaviour mediate the effects of age, gender and multidimensional health locus of control? British Journal of Health Psychology. 2002;7:299–316. doi: 10.1348/135910702760213698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer JS. Effects of college residence on perceived norms for alcohol consumption: An examination of the first year in college. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1994;8:43–50. [Google Scholar]
- Beck KH, Kasperski SJ, Caldeira KM, Vincent KB, O'Grady KE, Arria AM. Trends in alcohol-related traffic risk behaviors among college students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:1472–1478. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01232.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz AD. The social norms approach: Theory, research, and annotated bibliography. 2004. Retrieved from http: http://www.alanberkowitz.com/articles/social_norms.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB. Peer influences on college drinking: A review of the research. Journal of Substance Abuse. 2001;13:391–424. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(01)00098-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA, Reno RR. A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1991;24:201–234. [Google Scholar]
- Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.53.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner M, Armitage CJ. Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1998;28:1429–1464. [Google Scholar]
- Dimeff LA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA. Brief alcohol screening and intervention for college students (BASICS): A harm reduction approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Fairlie AM, Quinlan KJ, DeJong W, Wood MD, Lawson D, Witt CF. Sociodemographic, behavioral, and cognitive predictors of alcohol-impaired driving in a sample of U.S. college students. Journal of Health Communication. 2010;15:218–232. doi: 10.1080/10810730903528074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. London, England: SAGE; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Fromme K, Corbin WR, Kruse MI. Behavioral risks during the transition from high school to college. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:1497–1504. doi: 10.1037/a0012614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fromme K, Wetherill RR, Neal DJ. Turning 21 and the associated changes in drinking and driving after drinking among college students. Journal of American College Health. 2011;59:21–27. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2010.483706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gastil J. Thinking, drinking, and driving: Application of the theory of reasoned action to DWI prevention. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30:2217–2232. [Google Scholar]
- Grekin ER, Sher KJ. Alcohol dependence symptoms among college freshmen: Prevalence, stability, and person-environment interactions. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;14:329–338. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW. Magnitude and prevention of college drinking and related problems. Alcohol Research & Health. 2010;33:45–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, Supplement. 2009;16:12–20. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenney SR, LaBrie JW, Lac A. Injunctive normative peer misperceptions and the mediation of self-approval on risk for driving after drinking among college students. Journal of Health Communication. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.727963. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Hummer JF, Neighbors C, Larimer ME. Whose opinion matters? The relationship between injunctive norms and alcohol consequences in college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2010a;35:343–349. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Kenney SR, Lac A. The use of protective behavioral strategies is related to reduced risk in heavy drinking college students with poorer mental and physical health. Journal of Drug Education. 2010b;40:361–378. doi: 10.2190/DE.40.4.c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Kenney SR, Mirza T, Lac A. Identifying factors that increase the likelihood of driving after drinking among college students. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2011;43:1371–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer ME, Marlatt A, Baer JS, Quigley LA, Blume AW, Hawkins EH. Harm reduction for alcohol problems: Expanding access to and acceptability of prevention and treatment services. In: Marlatt A, editor. Harm reduction: Pragmatic strategies for managing high-risk behaviors. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Marczinski CA, Harrison ELR, Fillmore MT. Effects of alcohol on simulated driving and perceived driving impairment in binge drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2008;32:1329–1337. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy DM, Lynch AM, Pederson SL. Driving after use of alcohol and marijuana in college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21:425–430. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy DM, Pedersen SL. Reciprocal associations between drinking-and-driving behavior and cognitions in adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2009;70:536–542. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Marlatt A. Brief drinking profile. Odessa, FL: Psychology Assessment Resources; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Larimer ME. Are social norms the best predictor of outcomes among heavy-drinking college students? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:556–565. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neighbors C, O'Connor RM, Lewis MA, Chawla N, Lee CM, Fossos N. The relative impact of injunctive norms on college student drinking: The role of reference group. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008;22:576–581. doi: 10.1037/a0013043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen N, Walters ST, Rinker DV, Wyatt TM, DeJong W. Fake ID ownership in a US sample of incoming first-year college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:759–761. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.01.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman P, Conner M. The theory of planned behaviour and binge drinking: Assessing the moderating role of past behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2006;11:55–70. doi: 10.1348/135910705X43741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins HW. Religious traditions, parents, and peers as determinants of alcohol and drug use among college students. Review of Religious Research. 1985;27:15–31. [Google Scholar]
- Quinn PD, Fromme K. Event-level associations between objective and subjective alcohol intoxication and driving after drinking across the college years. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0024275. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0024275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrisi R, Padilla KK, Wiersma KA. College student drinking: An examination of theoretical models of drinking tendencies in freshmen and upperclassmen. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2000;61:598–602. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2000.61.598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Nelson TF, Lee JE, Seibring M, Lewis C, Keeling RP. Perception and reality: A national evaluation of social norms marketing interventions to reduce college students’ heavy alcohol use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64:484–494. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Labouvie EW. Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;50:30–37. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zakletskaia LI, Mundt MP, Balousek SL, Wilson EL, Fleming MF. Alcohol-impaired driving behavior and sensation-seeking disposition in a college population receiving routine care at campus health services centers. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2009;41:380–386. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]