Abstract
A general factor of personality (GFP) has been proposed as the apex of a personality trait hierarchy that explains covariance among the lower-order factors measured by various personality inventories. In this study we evaluated the GFP hypothesis across several personality inventories, unlike most previous research in which the GFP has been derived from individual instruments in isolation. Exploratory analyses did not produce substantial evidence for the existence of a single cross-instrument higher-order factor of factors and efforts to specify a range of GFP-inspired models in a confirmatory framework led to significant estimation difficulties and poor fit to the data. Overall these results fail to support a common GFP that is positioned at the top of a personality trait hierarchy.
Keywords: Personality Hierarchy, General Factor of Personality, Factor Analysis, Personality Traits, Self-Report Assessment
Multi-trait models such as the Big Five (Goldberg, 1990; John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008) or Big Three (e.g., Clark & Watson, 2008; Tellegen, 1985) provide organizational frameworks for classifying broad personality dispositions into conceptually meaningful units with empirical utility. An important assumption in these frameworks is the idea that each dimension represents a fundamental unit of personality. For example, the Big Five domains (i.e., neuroticism [N], extraversion [E], openness [O], agreeableness [A], and conscientiousness [C]) are thought to reflect basic tendencies that are linked with reasonably distinct neurobiological systems (McCrae & Costa, 2008). One complicating empirical fact is the repeated demonstration that observed scores on measures of the Big Five and similar models are correlated. The existence of such correlations points to the possibility of higher-order personality factors.
Digman (1997) identified two higher-order “metatraits” that he argued explained the associations among Big Five dimensions such that a superordinate “Alpha” factor explained associations among neuroticism, conscientiousness, and agreeableness whereas a super-ordinate “Beta” factor explained the correlation between extraversion and openness. Alpha can be thought of as a constellation of socially advantageous attributes that reflect the ability to get along well in society. Beta can be thought of a constellation of attributes that facilitate personal growth and exploration. More recently, DeYoung (2006) respectively referred to these meta-traits as Stability and Plasticity because he believed that those terms were more descriptive than Digman’s Alpha and Beta terminology (see e.g., p. 1149). Theoretical precedent exists for conceptualizing two similar higher-order organizing concepts termed Agency (Beta/Plasticity) and Communion (Alpha/Stability) (Bakan, 1966; Wiggins, 1991).
The identification of meta-traits or higher-order dimensions of personality that explain associations among lower order dimensions has led to more recent interest in a General Factor of Personality (GFP; see Rushton & Irwing, 2011). The GFP is posited to exist at the apex of the personality trait hierarchy, thus explaining the correlation that is sometimes found between Alpha and Beta (e.g., Musek, 2007; Rushton, 1985, 1990, Rushton, Bons, & Hur, 2008; Rushton & Irwing, 2008, 2009a, 2009b; van der Linden, te Nijenhuis, & Bakker, 2010). The GFP is thought to capture a broad array of attributes that facilitate or inhibit personality-related success (Rushton, 1985; 1990; Rushton & Irwing 2011), analogous to the ‘g’ factor of cognitive abilities (Spearman, 1904).
A series of studies that have identified a single higher-order “factor of factors” from omnibus personality inventories have been offered as evidentiary support for the GFP. For example, using data from 16 Big Five studies Rushton and Irwing (2009a) reported that a Confirmatory Factor Analytic (CFA) model with orthogonal higher-order Alpha and Beta factors fit worse than a model that specified that Alpha and Beta were explained by a single higher-order factor – the GFP1. Several similar studies based on factor analytic techniques using a variety of measures have been presented as further evidence in favor of the GFP (e.g., Rushton and Irwing, 2009b; van der Linden et al. 2010). Other studies suggest that the correspondence of the GFP is stronger among monozygotic than dizygotic twins (Veselka et al., 2009), and that the GFP is related to criterion variables such as self-esteem (Erdle, Irwing, Rushton, & Park, 2009) and peer reports of popularity and likeability (van der Linden, Scholte, et al., in press).
Some of these findings are predictable given that it is already well-known that personality dimensions are partly heritable (e.g., Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001) and able to predict consequential life outcomes (Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007; Ozer & Benet-Martínez, 2006). Moreover, previous analyses of Big Five measures have suggested the existence of two oblique higher order factors (Digman, 1997; DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2002; DeYoung, 2006). The fact that the Big Two factors are sometimes modestly correlated (DeYoung et al., 2002) means that this association can be re-expressed as a higher-order factor.
However, there are grounds to suspect that some of the correlations found in CFA studies of personality measures are inflated. Although it is possible to create reasonably orthogonal measures of the Big Five (Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006; Saucier, 2002), even in the most discriminant valid instruments modest domain-level inter-correlations often persist in practice. Yet Marsh et al. (2010) suggested that a considerable amount of this covariation emerges in CFA contexts is attributable to unspecified cross-loadings that usually accompany these kinds of analyses. These cross-loadings are often small by exploratory factor analytic standards (e.g. .30 or less) but omitting these parameters from the CFA models will often lead to inflated inter-factor correlations. Indeed, inter-factor correlations are reduced when these seemingly trivial cross-loadings are estimated as occurs when using exploratory-based factor analytic techniques (see Marsh et al., 2010). Given these hints in the psychometric literature, we suspect that some of the evidence for the GFP that is based on CFA methods may be artifactual.
Consistent with this suspicion, the substantive interpretation of GFP offered by Rushton and Irwing has been questioned by researchers who have interpreted the GFP as a psychometric artifact. For instance, Ashton et al. (2009) suggested that the correlations among broad traits reflect the fact that many lower-order factors (and even personality items, see Goldberg & Velicer, 2006) represent blends of different broad traits. When this fact is not accounted for in the analyses, non-substantive higher-order factors seem to emerge. However, in a direct comparison of higher-order and ‘blended variable’ models, the latter fit personality trait data better than the former (Ashton et al., 2009). As another example, Backstrom et al. (2009) showed using non-evaluative, neutral items limits the potential to identify higher-order factors, suggesting that such factors may be a function of social desirability effects in responding rather than substantive effects (see also Anusic et al., 2009).
The potential existence of a GFP is important for personality science, personality assessment, and evolutionary personality psychology. For example, to the extent that personality is related to basic neurological systems, the number of basic dimensions that emerge from personality inventories will influence the number of candidate neural systems sought in studies of the biological underpinnings of personality. Likewise, decisions by evolutionary theorists to regarding how many and which personality dimensions to focus on are currently guided, in part, by psychometric considerations. As a practical matter, if the GFP exists as a substantive dimension, it might be efficient to simply construct reliable and valid measures of that construct for use in studies designed to predict important life outcomes. It might be reasonable to develop a single GFP measure that could provide a “personality quotient”. The implications of such a quotient may be broad, given the widespread influence of IQ on how individuals think about their own and others’ cognitive abilities.
The goal of this study was to evaluate evidence for the GFP across several personality inventories that were administered to the same sample. Proponents of the GFP argue that it is a “robust” dimension that emerges across a diverse range of personality inventories (see e.g., Rushton & Irwing, 2011, p.155 – 156). Thus, the same higher-order dimension should emerge across different omnibus inventories that are designed to capture a wide range of important individual differences in personality. However the majority of prior studies that have explored the GFP have evaluated one inventory at a time. Evidence of strong convergence across a set of comprehensive personality measures would be consistent with the GFP hypothesis, whereas failure to find strong convergence would cast doubt on the hypothesis of a robust and unitary GFP.
Method
This study used data from the Eugene-Springfield (Oregon) Community Sample (ESCS; Goldberg, 2008), in which individuals in the community completed a number of commonly-used personality measures. Data were collected via the mail from 1993-2003. We analyzed data from the 16pf (Conn & Rieke, 1994; N = 680), 6 Factor Personality Questionnaire (6fpq; Jackson, Paunonen, & Tremblay, 2000; N = 714), California Psychological Inventory (CPI; Gough & Bradley, 1996; N = 792), HEXACO Personality Inventory (HEXACO-PI; Lee & Ashton, 2004; N = 734), Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI; Hogan & Hogan, 1995; N = 742), Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Tellegen & Waller, 1994; N = 733), NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992; N = 857), and Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised (TCI-R; Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994; N = 727). The features of these well-known measures are described in primary sources and their psychometric characteristics in the ESCS are described in previous reports (e.g., Hopwood & Donnellan, 2010; Grucza & Goldberg, 2007).
We conducted three sets of analyses. First, we subjected the lower order scales from each of these instruments to a set of separate principal axis exploratory factor analyses (EFA). We estimated and saved factor scores for factors with eigenvalues >1. We then factored the estimates derived in the first analysis for each instrument separately, using the same analytic methods. We continued this process until we achieved a single factor, which we regarded as an estimate of that instrument’s GFP. We then conducted a cross-instrument principal axis EFA on the GFP estimates derived from each instrument. We expected that the different GFPs embedded in each inventory would cohere into a common higher order factor based on the idea that each inventory ultimately assesses the same GFP.
Second, we conducted a joint EFA on the lower-order scales of all the inventories and evaluated the matrix of factor correlations emerging from oblique rotations. This approach is similar to the strategy used by Markon, Kreuger, and Watson (2005) to evaluate the hierarchy of personality constructs. However, our explicit focus was on the magnitude of the factor inter-correlations that emerge from a joint analysis of multiple personality inventories whereas Markon et al. (2005) used orthogonal rotations so that the factor inter-correlations were fixed to zero. We used EFA rather than CFA for these analyses to limit the likelihood that correlations among higher-order factors would be artificially high because of unspecified cross-loadings.
Third, we specified a series of seven CFA models testing the validity of a cross-instrument GFP, as described in detail below. These CFA models were intended to be analogous to those that have been tested in the literature with single instruments. Thus, we only used the scales of measures from the ESCS for which the structure has been tested in previous GFP research, which were the CPI (Rushton & Irwing, 2009a), HPI (Rushton & Irwing, 2008) MPQ (Rushton & Irwing, 2009b), NEO-PI-R (Rushton & Irwing, 2008), and TCI (Rushton & Irwing, 2009a).
Results
Testing Convergence of Single Higher Order Factors Derived from Multiple Inventories
We first conducted a principal axis EFA in PASW 18 on the lower order scales of all eight instruments we evaluated and rotated the resulting factors with Oblimin. We then factored these factor score estimates, factored the resulting estimates, and so on until we were able to achieve a single ‘factor of factors’ for each instrument2. The inter-correlations of factor scores in this analysis are reported in Table 1. Notably, there were several modest correlations in this matrix (e.g., 15 of 28 correlations were < ȣ.30ȣ), providing an initial indication that these GFP variables were not tapping the same construct. To evaluate the coherence of general factors across different inventories, we conducted a principal axis EFA on the factor score estimates from each independent inventory. The first three factors had eigenvalues of 3.43, 1.77, and 1.04 which together explained 61.73% of the covariance in factor scores. Although parallel analysis suggested extracting only two factors, doing so rendered the communality for the 6fpq GFP estimate .04, and it did not load strongly onto either factor (pattern coefficients = −.03, .20). This suggested that the GFP estimate from this measure was splitting off as its own, third factor. In the interest of meaningful evaluating GFP estimates from all inventories, we extracted three factors. The pattern coefficients, after Oblimin rotation, are given in Table 2. The factor scores from four measures tended to load more strongly on factor 1 (CPI, HPI, MPQ, TCI) whereas three others loaded more strongly on factor 2 (16pf, HEXACO-PI, NEO-PI-R). As expected, the 6fpq alone loaded most strongly on factor 3. Thus our first effort to identify a unitary cross-instrument GFP failed.
Table 1.
16pf | 6fpq | CPI | HEXACO-PI | HPI | MPQ | NEO-PI-R | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
6fpq | .10 | ||||||
CPI | −.28 | .06 | |||||
HEXACO-PI | .27 | .03 | .49 | ||||
HPI | −.28 | −.15 | .66 | .31 | |||
MPQ | .39 | .29 | .29 | .58 | .07 | ||
NEO-PI-R | −.38 | −.04 | .68 | .32 | .62 | .12 | |
TCI | −.21 | .00 | .56 | .35 | .61 | .27 | .74 |
Note. N = 433. 6fpq = Six Factor Personality Questionnaire; CPI = California Personality Inventory; HPI = Hogan Personality Inventory; MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; TCI = Temperament Character Inventory.
Table 2.
Communality | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | Factor 3 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
16pf | .49 | −.42 | .60 | −.05 |
6fpq | .31 | −.03 | .00 | .56 |
CPI | .68 | .81 | .07 | .06 |
HEXACO-PI | .65 | .42 | .66 | −.08 |
HPI | .66 | .78 | .02 | −.23 |
NEO-PI-R | .75 | .17 | .69 | .38 |
MPQ | .82 | .91 | −.11 | .06 |
TCI | .59 | .76 | .07 | .05 |
6fpq = Six Factor Personality Questionnaire; CPI = California Personality Inventory; HPI = Hogan Personality Inventory; MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; TCI = Temperament Character Inventory. Factor 1 correlated .18 with factor 2 and .00 with factor 3; factors 2 and 3 correlated .23.
To further evaluate the interpretive meaning of these cross-instrument factors, we correlated them with the Big 5 dimensions from the NEO-PI-R (Table 3). Factor 1 seems to involve emotional resilience and surgency, as its strongest correlates were extraversion and (low) neuroticism, and it correlated positively with openness and conscientiousness. Factor 2 seems to involve antagonism and sensation-seeking, given its strong negative correlation with agreeableness and positive correlations with extraversion and openness. Factor 3 primarily reflects openness. We also correlated the GFP estimate from each instrument with the NEO-PI-R scales (Table 3), and observed notable variability in these patterns of correlations. For instance, whereas GFP estimates from measures that loaded strongly on factor 1 tended to have sizeable correlations with neuroticism (Mdn ȣrȣ = .51), the 6fpq estimate, which did not load strongly on factor 1, was uncorrelated with this trait.
Table 3.
Neuroticism | Extraversion | Openness | Agreeableness | Conscientiousness | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cross-Instrument Higher Order Factors | |||||
Factor 1 | −.73 | .69 | .46 | .05 | .46 |
Factor 2 | .30 | .24 | .28 | −.57 | −.19 |
Factor 3 | .15 | −.01 | .56 | .04 | −.35 |
| |||||
Within Instrument GFPs | |||||
16pf | .45 | −.18 | −.09 | −.51 | −.16 |
6fpq | .00 | −.25 | .39 | −.08 | −.33 |
CPI | −.52 | .57 | .47 | −.12 | .17 |
HEXACO-PI | −.05 | .45 | .39 | −.47 | .02 |
HPI | −.63 | .52 | .08 | −.05 | .38 |
MPQ | .22 | .34 | .45 | −.37 | −.22 |
NEO | −.72 | .67 | .50 | .23 | .58 |
TCI | −.50 | .59 | .34 | .13 | .44 |
6fpq = Six Factor Personality Questionnaire; CPI = California Personality Inventory; HPI = Hogan Personality Inventory; MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; TCI = Temperament Character Inventory.
Evaluating the Existence of the GFP Using a Joint EFA of Several Personality Inventories
Our second set of analyses was based on the approach used by Markon et al. (2005) to evaluate the hierarchical structure of personality. First, we created a file that had the lower-order scales from the three large omnibus inventories that were also included in the Markon et al. (2005) investigations – the NEO-PI-R, the MPQ, and the TCI. A total of 72 personality scales were used for this analysis (30 from the NEO-PI-R, 11 from the MPQ, and 31 from the TCI; N = 946; this covariance matrix is available at www..edu). We then subjected these scales to a series of EFA analyses using the oblique GEOMIN rotation procedure with maximum likelihood estimation as implemented in Mplus 6 (this estimation procedure also addresses missing data issues). Our focus here was on both the nature of the factors that emerged from the analyses and especially the degree of correlation between the factors, as a considerable degree of factor inter-correlations seems like a natural starting point for inferring the existence of higher-order factors.
Thirteen eigenvalues based on the sample correlation matrix (the procedure used in Mplus) were above 1 but an inspection of the scree plot and various solutions suggested an interpretable Big 5 structure (see also Markon et al., 2005). Of note, the inter-factor correlations for the five factor model were not substantial using most rules of thumb (N and E: −.23; N and A: .03; N and C: −.23; N and O: −.03; E and A: .05; E and C: .06; E and O: .39; A and C: −.02; A and O: −.16; C and O: .02). We then submitted this matrix to a second maximum likelihood EFA and attempted to extract higher-order factors. However, this model would not converge within 1000 iterations and the estimates were not sensible when the number of iterations was increased to 5,000 (e.g., the loading for E was estimated at 33.497 and no standard error could be computed).
To evaluate whether our results were a function of the GEOMIN rotation, we also used an Oblimin rotation to extract five factors from the 72 scales. The matrix of inter-factor correlations was generally similar to the GEOMIN results (N and E: −.21; N and A: .−01; N and C: −.26; N and O: .00; E and A: .10; E and C: .12; E and O: .26; A and C: .01; A and O: −.06; C and O: .07). When this matrix was submitted to a second EFA, it was possible to obtain a solution; however, only the 1 factor solution would converge. Loadings for this dimension were −.27 for N, .79 for E, .10 for A, .18 for C, and .31 for O. Thus, this common factor seems to approximate Extraversion or perhaps Beta/ Plasticity/Agency more so than the GFP.
We also conducted similar analyses using the scales from the 6fpq, the 16pf, the NEO-PI-R, the HEXACO-PI, and the MPQ given that previous research suggests that these inventories have a reasonably well defined higher-order structure ranging from 3 to 6 factors each. A total of 99 personality scales were used for this analysis (18 from the 6fpq, 16 from the 16pf, 24 from the HEXACO-PI, 11 from the MPQ, and 30 from the NEO-PI-R; N = 956; covariance matrix available at www..edu). Seventeen eigenvalues for the sample correlation matrix were above 1. Once again, an inspection of the scree plot and the interpretability of several solutions suggested a Big Five structure. As before, the inter-factor correlations for the five factor model were modest (E and O: .22; E and N: −.14; E and A: .03; E and C: .07; O and N: −.17; O and A: −.20; O and C: −.05; N and A: .07; N and C: −.17; A and C: −.01). We then submitted this matrix to a second maximum likelihood EFA and attempted to extract higher-order factors. We could not estimate a two-factor solution but a 1 factor model would estimate. Loadings for this dimension were .18 for E, 1.24 for O, −.13 for N, −.16 for A, and −.05 for C. This solution was not clearly interpretable as a GFP. In fact, it was mostly related to the Big Five domain with the most modest relation to adaptation, Openness to Experience.
Evaluating the Existence of the GFP Using CFA Techniques
In a final set of analyses, we used a confirmatory factor analytic (CFA) framework to investigate the evidence for the convergence of GFPs across inventories. Following prior published work on the GFP (e.g., Rushton & Irwing, 2008, 2009a, 2009b) we used the TCI, NEO-PI-R, CPI, HPI, and MPQ in these analyses. In order to derive manifest variables across measures that were comparable to each other, we first conducted EFAs on the MPQ and CPI, which each have a large number of lower order scales that have been shown to load on a smaller number of higher-order factors. This was accomplished using principal axis factoring with Oblimin rotation in PASW 18. In an effort to remain consistent with prior GFP work, we followed Rushton and Irwing (2009a, 2009b) by requesting five factors for the MPQ and six for the CPI, whereas we used the scale structure defined by the authors of the measure for the NEO-PI-R, TCI, and HPI. There were a total of 29 scales used as manifest variables in the CFA models (7 for TCI; 5 for NEO-PI-R; 6 for CPI; 6 for HPI, and 5 for MPQ; N = 556; covariance matrix available at www..edu). Given the wide differences in variance across scales from different measures, the variables were rescaled to standard scores in order to avoid problems with CFA estimation associated with differing variances across indicators. All CFA models were estimated in Mplus 6 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010) from the observed data file using maximum likelihood.
Three modeling approaches were adopted in an effort to estimate an overall GFP in a CFA framework. The first general approach (Models 1 and 2) attempted to evaluate the GFP directly by modeling the scales from all instruments in hierarchical models. The second general approach (Models 3-5) conceptualized an intermediary factor structure involving ‘Alpha’, ‘Beta’, and in some cases ‘Gamma’ constructs for each instrument. The final general approach (Models 6 and 7) conceptualized these intermediary factors across instruments.
Models 1 and 2 – Convergence Across Measure Specific GFPs
Model 1 allowed each scale from a given measure to load on to one of five factors (i.e., measure specific general factors) which were allowed to correlate freely. That is, we estimated a TCI GFP, a NEO-PI-R GFP, a CPI GFP, a HPI GFP, and a MPQ GFP. These five GFPs were then freed to correlate with each other. This model had a poor fit across multiple indices (Table 4). In addition, the matrix for the factor correlations had a large number of Heywood cases (i.e., correlations greater than 1.00). In spite of these limitations, we estimated Model 2 with a second-order structure that allowed all of the measure specific GFPs to load freely on one higher-order factor (i.e., the across measure GFP). Overall fit for this model was also poor and demonstrated a decrease in fit (χ2 df = 5; χ2 = 87.57, p < .001). Moreover, there were indications of serious misspecification with three (NEO-PI-R, MPQ, HPI) of the five first-order factors having negative variances. Thus, this approach failed to provide clear support for a GFP.
Table 4.
df | χ 2 | χ2 p | RMSEA | RMSEA 90% CI |
CFI | TLI | SRMR | BIC | AIC | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | 367 | 8042.24 | 0.00 | 0.194 | 0.190-0.198 | 0.34 | 0.27 | 0.19 | 42385.86 | 41966.74 |
Model 2 | 372 | 8129.81 | 0.00 | 0.194 | 0.190-0.197 | 0.33 | 0.27 | 0.19 | 42441.83 | 42044.32 |
Model 3 | 318 | 5066.78 | 0.00 | 0.164 | 0.160-0.168 | 0.59 | 0.48 | 0.14 | 39720.12 | 39089.28 |
Model 4 | 363 | 7365.54 | 0.00 | 0.186 | 0.183-0.190 | 0.40 | 0.32 | 0.19 | 41298.04 | 41734.44 |
Model 5 | 364 | 6984.86 | 0.00 | 0.181 | 0.177-0.185 | 0.43 | 0.36 | 0.21 | 41347.44 | 40915.36 |
Model 6 | 356 | 4583.88 | 0.00 | 0.146 | 0.142-0.150 | 0.64 | 0.58 | 0.15 | 38997.03 | 38530.38 |
Model 7 | 361 | 4626.82 | 0.00 | 0.146 | 0.142-0.149 | 0.63 | 0.59 | 0.15 | 39008.36 | 38563.32 |
Note. N = 556 . df = Degrees of Freedom; χ2 = Chi-Square; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; RMSEA 90% CI = 90% Confidence interval for RMSEA; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis Fit Index; SRMR = Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; AIC = Akaike Information Criterion.
Models 3, 4, and 5 – Intermediary Alpha, Beta, and Gamma Factors
The majority of prior research has used observed variables at the same level of analysis as we have employed here (e.g., NEO-PI-R Extraversion Domain Scores as opposed to facets scores). However, past research has also overwhelmingly relied on an intermediary factor structure between the observed personality scales and the GFP’s single factor by having the basic input measures serve as indicators for higher-order factors identified in the literature (e.g., Alpha and Beta) which in turn serve as indicators for GFP. This practice is consistent with the idea that the GFP is a factor of factors. Therefore, a series of models were estimated based on prior GFP research specifying two intermediary factors.
First, Model 3 was estimated in which Alpha and Beta, or Alpha, Beta, and Gamma in the case of the TCI (see Rushton & Irwing, 2009a), were specified for each omnibus inventory. This model had eleven higher order factors (Alphas and Betas for all 5 inventories and 1 Gamma for the TCI) which were freed to correlate. This model seemed to fit the underlying data better than Models 1 and 2 (see Table 4). However, there were a number of inadmissible values (i.e., greater than 1) in the factor correlations. Estimating a second-order general factor in Model 4 resulted in a notably worse fit across all indices (Table 4). Additionally, this model demonstrated clear misspecification in the form of two negative first-order factor variances and two negative error variances (TCI Cooperativeness and NEO-PI-R Neuroticism). The fit of Model 4 presented in Table 4 is from a modified model with these error variances fixed to 0. Figure 13 provides a conceptual diagram along with the standardized factor loadings and error variances.
Model 5 also estimated a two-factor higher-order structure. This model, however, specified higher-order Alphas and Betas across inventories. This was accomplished by allowing each first-order “Alpha” factor and the TCI “Gamma” factor to load freely on a higher order “Alpha” factor, and each of the first-order “Beta” factors to load on a higher-order “Beta” factor. Relative to the one-factor higher-order model this model had significantly improved fit (Table 4). The conceptual model and standardized coefficients are presented in Figure 2. Although this model converged, four error variances were negative and subsequently fixed to zero whereas three first-order factor variances were negative. Attempts to specify factors with non-negative variance resulted in models that would not converge. The overall model is probably untrustworthy but we note that the correlation between the alpha and beta factors was .28 (i.e., they share less than 8% of their variance). No single-factor third-order model was estimated as this would merely be a quantitative rearticulation of this correlation.
Models 6 and 7 – Modeling the cross-instrument trait hierarchy
We anticipated that a model with 5 to 7 first-order factors might provide a better fit to the data, given that the indicators from different inventories often measure similar dimensions of personality. To determine the structure most likely to achieve the best fit, an EFA was conducted on the 29 scales, again with Oblimin rotation. Six factors were retained with eigenvalues > 1. Model 6 was estimated as a freely covarying six-factor first-order solution suggested by the results of the EFA (Figure 3). Factor loadings of .30 and above from the EFA were used as the basis for freeing factor loadings in this model. This 6 factor structure appears to have two factors associated with the broader Extraversion domain, which we have tentatively labeled “Social Dominance” and “Social Vitality” following prior empirical work that has emphasized a similar distinction (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006).
Table 5 summarizes the factor correlations and standardized factor loadings. Model 6 proved to be a considerably better fit to the data than prior models (Table 4). Finally, Model 7 was estimated with a three-factor higher-order structure which resulted in a decrease in fit from Model 6 based on the χ2 difference test (Δdf = 5; Δχ2 = 42.94, p < .001), but the remaining fit indices did not evidence substantial change. A conceptual diagram and higher-order factor loadings is provided in Figure 3. As with the majority of previous models, this model exhibited some negative first-order factor variance. Nonetheless, as compared with previous models, the models based on a six-factor solutions show much better specification in the form of fewer negative factor and error variances4. Again, a third-order model was not estimated5.
Table 5.
Emotional | Social | Social | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Stability | Constraint | Vitality | Conventionality | Agreeableness | Dominance | |
Factor Correlation Matrix | ||||||
Emotional Stability | 1.00 | |||||
Constraint | −0.24 | 1.00 | ||||
Social Vitality | 0.33 | −0.13 | 1.00 | |||
Conventionality | −0.43 | −0.45 | −0.21 | 1.00 | ||
Agreeableness | 0.43 | −0.24 | 0.70 | −0.11 | 1.00 | |
Social Dominance | 0.20 | 0.43 | 0.26 | −0.76 | 0.02 | 1.00 |
Standardized Factor Loadings | ||||||
CPI Communion | 0.52 | |||||
CPI Independence | 0.24 | 0.55 | ||||
CPI Dependability | 0.53 | −0.51 | ||||
TCI Harm Avoidance | −0.56 | −0.57 | ||||
TCI Directedness | 0.71 | |||||
NEO Neuroticism | −0.88 | |||||
MPQ Neuroticism | −0.85 | |||||
HPI Adjustment | 0.84 | |||||
CPI Originality | 0.47 | |||||
TCI Novelty Seeking | 0.69 | |||||
NEO Conscientiousness | −0.55 | |||||
MPQ Conscientiousness | −0.77 | |||||
HPI Prudence | −0.78 | |||||
TCI Reward Dependence | 0.80 | |||||
CPI Ascendance | 0.30 | −0.80 | ||||
NEO Extraversion | 0.54 | 0.58 | ||||
MPQ Agreeableness | 0.72 | |||||
HPI Likeability | 0.76 | |||||
CPI Conventionality | 0.57 | |||||
NEO Openness | −0.59 | |||||
HPI Intelectance | −0.62 | |||||
TCI Cooperativeness | 0.82 | |||||
TCI Self-Transcendence | 0.50 | |||||
NEO Agreeableness | 0.72 | −0.40 | ||||
MPQ Openness | 0.31 | |||||
TCI Persistence | 0.43 | |||||
MPQ Extraversion | 0.77 | |||||
HPI Ambition | 0.69 | |||||
HPI Sociability | 0.77 |
Note. N = 556. TCI = Temperament Character Inventory; HPI = Hogan Personality Inventory; NEO = NEO Personality Inventory Revised; MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; CPI = California Personality Inventory.
Discussion
Researchers have become increasingly interested in the existence and interpretation of higher-order dimensions of personality, including the possibility of a General Factor of Personality (GFP). Rushton and Irwing (2011, p. 132) suggest that high scores on the GFP reflect a “good” personality whereas low scores reflect a “difficult” personality. Proponents of the GFP suggest that this dimension is the outcome of evolutionary processes that selected certain attributes like emotional stability, openness, cooperativeness, agency, and conscientiousness to promote survival, growth, and reproduction (Musek, 2007; Rushton, 1985; Rushton & Irwing, 2011). Proponents also regard the GFP as considerably robust based on their identification of this higher-order dimension in personality inventories of varying content (e.g., normal vs. disordered personality) and development approach (e.g., theoretical vs. eclectic, empirical vs. rational). Indeed, they note that: “Its location at the apex of the hierarchy should be almost completely fixed in any large data set” (Rusthon & Irwing, 2011, p. 155).
In general, our findings do not support the existence of a GFP as a single factor of factors at the apex of a personality trait hierarchy that is common across personality inventories. In an EFA context we found that interpretively different GFPs seemed to emerge from different instruments. The discrepancies across measures may be explained by their different content foci (e.g., some measures, such as the HPI, may have been constructed to emphasize adaptation to a work environment more so than others) or development strategies (e.g., the role of factor analysis was greater for some measures, such as the NEO-PI-R, than others, such as the CPI). In general, to the extent that the covariation among personality scales cannot be represented with a single construct, a factor summarizing the covariation of all the scales of any broadband measure should be somewhat arbitrary. It would be questionable in this case to expect a summary score from one measure to relate to a similar summary score on another measure. We suspect the fact that there was any convergence at all likely reflects the validity of some higher-order level of personality below the supposed GFP. For instance, the results from this study suggested that the apex factor of some measures appeared to reflect emotional resilience and social surgency (Table 3, factor 1) whereas the apex factor of others involved agreeableness vs. antagonism (factor 2). It would be interesting for future research to investigate the relative saturation of various measures from personality variance associated with the Big 2 alpha/stability/communion and beta/plasticity/agency conceptualizations.
We experienced a similar failure to find strong evidence for the GFP when we examined the matrices of inter-factor correlations that emerge from a joint analysis of the scales from multiple inventories. This approach was based on the strategy used by Markon and colleagues (2005) to evaluate the hierarchical nature of pathological personality traits. However, here we used oblique rotations so that we could uncover correlated dimensions of personality rather than assuming that factors were uncorrelated as would be the case with orthogonal rotations. Using this approach, we replicated previous evidence for the robustness of Big Five domains but we noted that these five dimensions were reasonably uncorrelated. This finding undermines the case for the GFP, as second-order factors can only be obtained from correlated first-order factors.
Last, we tested a series of CFA models, based on the common use of this method in previous GFP studies. We constructed a series of models that varied from estimating a single-factor solution for each measure, a two-factor solution for each measure, and a shared 6-factor solution. However, we encountered several estimation challenges and a number of modeling approaches based on prior GFP research yielded inadmissible solutions. Despite our attempts to address Heywood cases and negative factor variances, the various CFA models yielded a complicated pattern of results that failed to support the GFP hypothesis.
In summary, our general strategy was to use a variety of factor analytic approaches in the hope of finding a robust GFP “signal” that was strong enough to exceed the noise of different ways of modeling personality data. This strategy was guided by the view that if the GFP is as robust as Rushton and Irwing (2011) suggest, it should emerge across a variety of analytic techniques and instruments. The fact that we failed to find convincing evidence for the GFP that converged across instruments casts doubt on its importance as a substantive personality construct.
We acknowledge that there are many potential strategies for testing the degree to which a single trait can explain the covariation among all other traits, and recognize the possibility that different GFP estimation methods could lead to different conclusions about the consistency of the GFP across measures. Indeed many approaches have been employed to identify the GFP in previous research (Ferguson et al., 2011). Because standards for factor analytic practice are fuzzy (e.g., Hopwood & Donnellan, 2010; Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004) there exists a good deal of subjectivity in the interpretation of these kinds of results. In this context proponents of the GFP with considerable factor analytic expertise might find a way to indentify a very specific model or small number of models that would appear to support the GFP proposal. However, we suspect that such models would likely fail to generalize to new samples and may even contradict less controversial features of the personality trait hierarchy (e.g., which traits from the Big Five level load onto the Big Two).
We would also note that the failure of a GFP to explain the covariance among all personality factors in a manner that is consistent across several measures does not necessarily contradict the idea of a broad higher-order factor related to some aspects of personality functioning. For example, some clinical theories posit such a trait (e.g., Menninger, Waymer, & Pruyser, 1963; Kernberg, 1984), and recent research has found that the simple sum of all of the DSM-IV personality disorder symptoms reflects a strong predictor of clinical dysfunction (Hopwood et al., in press; Jahng et al., in press). One could select certain items or scales among the measures examined here that are consistently related to psychological health, scale them on the same metric and in the same direction for each inventory, and derive meaningful “personality severity” scores. These scores may furthermore relate systematically to one another across measures. Such an index could serve the applied purpose of predicting broad dysfunction without necessarily having theoretical coherence or construct validity in the traditional sense. Alternatively, such theories may have identified a factor similar to Alpha/Stability/Communion, which appears to be more strongly related to psychopathology than Beta/Plasticity/Agency. In either case this broad trait from the clinical literature would not be expected to explain the covariation of all normative and pathological personality dimensions, as has been proposed for the GFP.
The present results are situated within an emerging body of substantive and methodological critiques of the GFP (see e.g., Ashton et al., 2009; Ferguson et al., 2011). For instance, Rushton and Irwing’s (2011) analogy between the GFP for personality and ‘g’ in cognitive abilities appears to break down in several respects. Foremost, unlike ‘g’, for which higher scores are unambiguously better, the relation between personality traits and adaptation may be non-linear or moderated to a greater extent by environmental factors. For example, some personality theorists assert that extreme scores on otherwise beneficial personality traits are maladaptive, such as when high agreeableness connotes pathologically dependent behavior (Lowe, Edmundson, & Widiger, 2009). To the extent that extreme traits are generally maladaptive, the suggestion that the GFP evolved via directional selection (such that over time population means shifted in a certain direction because individuals at one end of a trait are more likely to survive) may be called into question (Ferguson et al., 2011).
Evolutionary personality theorists have also argued that unlike cognitive abilities, traits tend to be differentially selective in different environments – which would subject traits to different evolutionary mechanisms than ‘g’ (Denissen & Penke, 2009). For instance, personality researchers drawing on evolutionary theory have argued that even relatively high levels of certain dimensions of personality that seem so problematic in contemporary society (such as neuroticism; see Lahey, 2009) can confer benefits for survival and reproduction in some specific environments (see e.g., Nettle, 2006). Likewise, whereas ‘g’ is a robust correlate of attributes linked to fitness (e.g., longevity, mate preferences, reproductive success, and adaptation to environmental disruption), different personality traits seem to have more specific connections to behaviors linked with fitness (Ferguson et al., 2011). For example, Conscientiousness predicts longevity (Roberts et al., 2007), Neuroticism predicts adaption to stress (Lahey, 2009), and Extraversion predicts exploratory tendencies (see e.g., Nettle, 2011). Finally, whereas in cognitive abilities the higher levels of the hierarchy are more heritable than the lower levels (Friedman et al., 2008), this is not the case in personality, where the heritability of the GFP (Rushton, Bons, & Hur, 2008) is in the range of heritability estimates for lower order traits.
Beyond these substantive concerns, methodological critiques have also been leveled at the substantive interpretation of the GFP, as discussed in the Introduction. These include that the GFP is an artifact of the preponderance of scales that reflect blends of core traits (e.g., Ashton et al., 2009) and that response tendencies such as socially desirable responding and halo effects contribute to general factors that explain covariation among personality scales (e.g., Anusic et al., 2009; Bäckström et al., 2009). It has also been suggested that the overall statistical importance of a GFP is of a much lower magnitude than that of ‘g’ in the cognitive domain (Revelle & Wilt, 2010), implying that less attention should accordingly be paid to the substantive interpretation of GFP than is given to the interpretation of ‘g’.
In conclusion, this study was designed to contribute to ongoing debates in the personality literature about the existence and meaning of the GFP. We noted that previous studies supporting the GFP are generally based on analyses of single, omnibus personality inventories. However, the GFP proposal would appear to predict that a higher-order factor from different broad-band personality inventories should correlate strongly across measures. Our objective was to determine how strongly GFPs from different inventories converged with each other using a variety of analytic techniques. We reasoned that strong convergence of apex dimensions across inventories would provide important evidence for the substantive case for the GFP6. Overall, the failure to find convergence across these measures in this study casts significant doubt on the importance of the GFP.
A General Factor of Personality (GFP) has been proposed as the apex of the personality trait hierarchy.
We evaluated the cross-measure consistency of the GFP.
We used various factor analyses of multiple personality inventories in a large sample.
Results fail to support a common GFP that is positioned at the top of a personality trait hierarchy.
Acknowledgement
We thank Lewis R. Goldberg for making the ESCS data available to us.
Footnotes
We note that in a CFA context it is impossible to model a higher-order factor with only two or three first order factors without imposing constraints on the model (see Brown 2006, p. 326). Thus, interpretively, a correlated stability-plasticity model and a GFP based on alpha and beta are interchangeable given that the existence of the correlation between stability and plasticity implies a higher-order factor.
We note that this often required us to factor scores with rather modest correlations (the median across all instruments was .14) and thus to tolerate estimates based on analyses with very low communalities, suggesting some potential problems with the notion that meaningful factors of factors can be derived from these measures.
Readers may note that the indicators for MPQ Alpha and Beta are inconsistent with the standard loading patterns (i.e., MPQ Conscientiousness loads on Beta). This was necessary as attempts to model the traditional structure resulted in non-convergence. Additionally, past work on Alpha and Beta in the MPQ has resulted in models that deviated from the traditional structure as well, with Agreeableness loading on Beta (i.e., Plasticity) and Extraversion loading on Alpha (i.e., Stability) (see Rushton & Irwing, 2009b Figure 1).
Note that in each case where a model had difficulties in estimation (e.g., negative variances, Heywood cases) considerable efforts were expended to resolve these problems via fixing of parameters, freeing of parameters manually varying start value combinations, and manually inputting start values from previously generated solutions. In no model did these adequately resolve estimation issues.
CFA model specifics (e.g., path coefficients) are not reported in detail here because of the poor fit observed in the models we examined.
It is possible that factors such as a common response style tendency could account for convergence across the first factors of various personality inventories, thus this approach could be regarded as a weak test of the GFP hypothesis.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Christopher J. Hopwood, Michigan State University
Aidan G.C. Wright, Pennsylvania State University
M. Brent Donnellan, Michigan State University.
References
- Anusic I, Schimmack U, Pinkus RT, Lockwood P. The nature and structure of correlations among Big Five ratings: The halo-alpha-beta model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;97:1142–1156. doi: 10.1037/a0017159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashton MC, Lee K. Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2007;11:150–166. doi: 10.1177/1088868306294907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashton MC, Lee K, Goldberg LR, de Vries RE. Higher order factors of personality: Do they exist? Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2009;13:79–91. doi: 10.1177/1088868309338467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling. 2009;16:397–438. [Google Scholar]
- Bäckström M, Björklund F, Larsson MR. Five-factor inventories have a major general factor related to social desirability which can be reduced by framing items neutrally. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:335–344. [Google Scholar]
- Bakan D. The duality of human existence. Beacon; Boston: 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Bouchard TJ, Loehlin JC. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics. 2001;31:243–273. doi: 10.1023/a:1012294324713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. The Guilford Press; New York: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Buss DM. How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and individual differences? Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:359–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark LA, Watson D. Temperament: An organizing paradigm for trait psychology. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and Research. 3rd ed Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 265–286. [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; St. Louis, MO: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Conn SR, Rieke ML. The 16PF fifth edition technical manual. Institute for Personality and Ability Testing; Champaign, IL: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Denissen JJA, Penke L. Motivational individual reaction norms underlying the Five-Factor model of personality: First steps toward a theory-based conceptual framework. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42:1285–1302. [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG. Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:1138–1151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. Higher-order factors of the big five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;33:533–552. [Google Scholar]
- Digman JM. Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;73:1246–1256. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Oswald FL, Baird BM, Lucas RE. The mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality. Psychological Assessment. 2006;18:192–203. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson E, Chamorro-Premuzic T, Pickering A, Weiss A. Five into one doesn’t go: A critique of the General Factor o Personlity. In: Chamorro-Premuzic T, von Stumm S, Furnham A, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. Wiley-Blackwell; West Sussux, UK: 2011. pp. 162–186. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn JR. The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin. 1984;95:29–51. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman NP, Miyake A, Young SE, DeFries JC, Corley RP, Hewitt JK. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2008;137:201–255. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. The structure of personality traits: Vertical and horizontal aspects. In: Funder DC, Parke RD, Tomlinson-Keasy C, Widaman K, editors. Studying lives through time: Personality and Development. American Psychological Association; Washington DC: 1993. pp. 169–188. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. The Eugene-Springfield community sample: Information available from the research participants. No. 1. Vol. 48. Oregon Research Institute; 2008. Technical Report. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR, Velicer WF. Principles of exploratory factor analysis. In: Strack S, editor. Differentiating normal and abnormal personality. Second edition Springer; New York, NY: 2006. pp. 209–237. [Google Scholar]
- Gough HG, Bradley P. CPI Manual. Third Edition Consulting Psychologists Press; Mountain View, CA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Grucza RA, Goldberg LR. The Comparative Validity of 11 Modern Personality Inventories: Predictions of Behavioral Acts, Informant Reports, and Clinical Indicators. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2007;89:167–187. doi: 10.1080/00223890701468568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Donnellan MB. How should the internal structure of personality inventories be evaluated? Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2010;14:332–346. doi: 10.1177/1088868310361240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Malone JC, Ansell EB, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Pinto A, Markowitz JC, Shea MT, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, Morey LC. Personality Assessment in DSM-V: Empirical support for rating severity, style, and traits. Journal of Personality Disorders. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2011.25.3.305. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogan R, Hogan J. Hogan Personality Inventory Manual. Second Edition Hogan Assessment Systems; Tulsa, OK: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson DN, Paunonen SV, Tremblay PF. Six Factor Personality Questionnaire. Sigma Assesment Systems; Port Huron, MI: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Jahng S, Trull TJ, Wood PK, Tragesser SL, Tomko R, Grant JD, Bucholz KK, Sher KJ. Distinguishing general and specific personality disorder features and implications for substance dependence comorbidity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0023539. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D. LISREL 8.80 for Windows. Scientific Software International, Inc.; Lincolnwood, IL: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and Research. 3rd ed Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 114–158. [Google Scholar]
- Kernberg OF. Severe personality disorders: Psychotherapeutic strategies. Yale University Press; New Haven: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB. The public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist. 2009;64:241–256. doi: 10.1037/a0015309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee K, Ashton MC. Psychometric properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2004;39:329–358. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe JR, Edmundson M, Widiger TA. Assessment of dependency, agreeableness, and their relationship. Psychological Assessment. 2009;21:543–553. doi: 10.1037/a0016899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markon KE, Krueger RF, Watson D. Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:139–157. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh HW, Hau KT, Wen Z. In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indices and dangers of overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling. 2004;11:320–341. [Google Scholar]
- Marsh HW, Lüdtke O, Muthén B, Asparouhov T, Morin AJS, Trautwein U, Nagengast B. A new look at the Big Five factor structure through exploratory structural equation modeling. Psychological Methods. 2010;22:471–491. doi: 10.1037/a0019227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr. The Five-Factor theory of personality. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and Research. 3rd ed Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 159–181. [Google Scholar]
- Menninger K, Wayman M, Pruyser P. The vital balance: The life process in mental health. Viking; New York: 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Musek J. A general factor of personality: Evidence for the big one in the five factor model. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41:1213–1233. [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D. The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist. 2006;61:622–631. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.6.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D. Evolutionary perspective on the Five-Factor Model of personality. In: Buss DM, Hawley PH, editors. The Evolution of personality and individual differences. Oxford University Press; New York: 2011. pp. 5–28. [Google Scholar]
- Ozer DJ, Benet-Martinez V. Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:401–421. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Revelle W, Wilt J. A methodological critique of claims for a general factor of personality. European Conference on Personality; Brno, Czech Republic. 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Kuncel N, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. The power of personality: A comparative analysis of the predictive validity of personality traits, SES, and IQ. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2007;2:313–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP. Differential K theory: The sociobiology of individual and group differences. Personality and Individual Differences. 1985;6:441–452. [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP. Sir Francis Galton, epigenetic rules, genetic similarity theory, and human life history analysis. Journal of Personality. 1990;58:117–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Bons TA, Hur Y-M. The genetics and evolution of a General Factor of Personality. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42:1173–1185. [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Bons TA, Ando J, Hur YM, Irwing P, Vernon PA, Petrides KV, Barbaranelli C. A general factor of personality from multitrait-multimethod data and cross-national twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:356–365. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.4.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruston JP, Irwing P. A general factor of personality (GFP) from two meta-analyses of the big five: Digman (1997) and Mount, Barrick, Scullen, and Rounds (2005) Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:679–683. [Google Scholar]
- Ruston JP, Irwing P. A general factor of personality in 16 sets of the big five, the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey, the California Psychological Inventory, and the Temperament and Character Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009a;47:558–564. [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Irwing P. A general factor of personality (GFP) from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009b;47:571–576. [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Irwing P. The general factor of personality: Normal and abnormal. In: Chamorro-Premuzic T, von Stumm S, Furnham A, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. Wiley-Blackwell; West Sussux, UK: 2011. pp. 132–161. [Google Scholar]
- Saucier G. Orthogonal markers for orthogonal factors: The case of the Big Five. Journal of Research in Personality. 2002;36:1–31. [Google Scholar]
- Spearman C. “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology. 1904;15:201–293. [Google Scholar]
- Tellegen A. Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In: Tuma AH, Maser JD, editors. Anxiety and the anxiety disorders. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1985. pp. 681–706. [Google Scholar]
- Tellegen A, Waller NG. Exploring personality through test construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. In: Briggs SR, Cheek JM, editors. Personality measures: Development and evaluation. Vol 1. JAI Press; Greenwich, CT: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB. Rethinking “Generation Me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976-2006. Perspectives in Psychological Science. 2010;5:58–75. doi: 10.1177/1745691609356789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Linden D, te Nijenhuis J, Bakker AB. The general factor of personality: A meta-analysis of big five intercorrelations and a criterion-validity study. Journal of Research in Personality. 2010;44:315–327. [Google Scholar]
- van der Linden D, Scholte RHJ, Cillessen AHN, Nijenhuis J. t., Segers E. Classroom ratings of likeability and popularity are related to the Big Five and the general factor of personality. Journal of Research in Personality. (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Veselka L, Schermer JA, Petrides KV, Vernon PA. Evidence for a heritable general factor of personality in two studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:254–260. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.3.254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiggins JS. Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the understanding and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In: Grove WM, Cicchetti D, editors. Thinking clearly about psychology. Vol. 2. Personality and psychopathology. University of Minnesota Press; Minneapolis: 1991. pp. 89–113. [Google Scholar]