Abstract
Although stability and pervasive inflexibility are general criteria for DSM-IV personality disorders (PD), borderline PD (BPD) is characterized by instability in several domains including interpersonal behavior, affect, and identity. We hypothesized that such inconsistencies notable in BPD may relate to instability at the level of the basic personality traits that are associated with this disorder. Five types of personality trait stability across 4 assessments over 6 years were compared for BPD patients (N = 130 at first interval) and patients with other PDs (N = 302). Structural stability did not differ across groups. Differential stability tended to be lower for Five Factor Model traits in the BPD group, with the strongest and most consistent effects observed for neuroticism and conscientiousness. Growth curve models suggested that these two traits also showed greater mean-level change, with neuroticism declining faster and conscientiousness increasing faster, in the BPD group. The BPD group was further characterized by greater individual-level instability for neuroticism and conscientiousness in these models. Finally, the BPD group was less stable in terms of the ipsative configuration of FFM facet-level profiles than was the other PD group over time. Results point to the importance of personality trait instability in characterizing BPD.
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) conceptualizes personality disorder (PD) as an enduring pattern of behavior that manifests in at least two domains of functioning (criterion A, p. 689), is pervasive and inflexible across a broad range of situations (criterion B), and is stable and longstanding (criterion C). Despite the emphasis on stability in the general criteria for PD, three of the nine criteria for borderline personality disorder (BPD) involve instability in relationships (criterion 2, p. 710), identity (criterion 3), and affect (criterion 6). Indeed, BPD has been characterized as a disorder marked by “stable instability” (Schmideberg, 1995).
Empirically, individuals with BPD generally evidence more variable affect (Cowdry et al., 1991; Koenisberg et al., 2001; Stein, 1996; Trull et al., 2008), self-esteem (Zeigler-Hill & Abraham, 2006), and interpersonal behavior (Hopwood & Morey, 2007; Russell et al., 2006) than individuals without the disorder. Furthermore, BPD symptoms appear to be much less stable than previously believed (Grilo et al., 2005; Shea et al., 2002; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Reich, et al., 2007), even though the structure of the disorder remains stable over time (Sanislow et al., 2002). Despite the general consensus that instability characterizes this disorder, relatively little is known about the factors that may drive the psychological instability that appears to be such a cardinal feature of BPD. Personality traits may influence BPD instability (Widiger, Trull, Clarkin, Sanderson, & Costa, 2002), given that basic traits systematically relate to the psychological domains that are unstable in BPD such as affect (e.g., Eid & Diener, 1999; Vaidya et al., 2008), interpersonal behavior (Ansell & Pincus, 2004), and self (Robins, Tracy, Trzesniewski, Potter, & Gosling, 2001).
Personality Traits and BPD
Lynam and Widiger (2001) asked experts to use the traits associated with the Five-Factor Model of personality (FFM: neuroticism [N], extraversion [E], openness [O], agreeableness [A], and conscientiousness [C]) to characterize the DSM PDs. Consistent with previous research (e.g., Blais, 1997), BPD was viewed as involving high levels of N and low levels of A and C, with some variability across FFM facets (e.g., experts considered BPD positively associated with openness to feelings and actions but unrelated to openness to values). Trull, Widiger, Lynam, and Costa (2003) found that this prototype was related to other measures of BPD and concluded that BPD represents a maladaptive variant of FFM traits, a position recently reaffirmed by Samuel and Widiger (2008). In other words, according to this perspective, BPD is largely a reflection of a particular constellation of basic personality attributes.
However, these authors also acknowledge that personality traits and PD symptoms represent overlapping but separable domains. For example, Trull et al. (2003) advised that targeted measures of BPD such as the Revised Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines (Zanarini et al., 1987) “will provide a much more specific and thorough assessment of borderline personality disorder than will be provided by the NEO-PI-R” (p. 200), and Samuel and Widiger (2008) noted that “measures of normal personality would not (be expected to) fully account for all the variance in a measure of abnormal personality” (p. 1336). This view is supported by studies by Morey and Zanarini (2000) in which FFM traits and BPD symptoms incremented one another in predicting functioning. Trull et al. (2003) observed similar effects. Morey et al. (2007) extended these findings by showing that FFM traits were more stable than either categorical or dimensional PD diagnoses and that the predictive value of FFM traits was similar for baseline, 2-, and 4-year outcomes, whereas the validity of the DSM PD model was highest for concurrent predictions and lowest for more distal predictions. Furthermore, the FFM traits were limited in their increment of the PDs at baseline, whereas the PDs were limited in their increment of the FFM traits for predictions of prospective functioning. Morey et al. concluded that these results supported a hybrid model of personality functioning in which traits and symptoms of disorders are separable, albeit overlapping constructs that conjunctively predict functioning (see McGlashan et al., 2005 for an exploration of this issue within PD symptoms generally and Zanarini et al., 2007 regarding BPD). Notably, FFM traits predict functioning equally well for individuals with and without PDs (Hopwood et al., 2007), further supporting the separation of pathological and normative personality characteristics.
Consistent with this formulation, Warner et al. (2004) used a cross-lagged structural equation modeling approach to show that change in FFM personality constellations representative of BPD predicted future change in BPD symptoms, whereas change in BPD symptoms did not lead to systematic change in the FFM traits. Warner et al. interpreted this finding as suggesting that FFM representations embody the personological core of PD, whereas symptomatic expressions signify more transient manifestations. Lenzenweger and Willett (2007) reported a similar effect for basic traits using growth curve analyses in a non-clinical sample. These studies demonstrate that changes in personality trait levels lead to changes in PD levels.
To summarize, previous evidence associates BPD with a) greater variability in domains of affect, interpersonal behavior, and self (e.g., Russell et al., 2007; Trull et al., 2008; Zeigler-Hill & Abraham, 2006) and b) relatively high levels of N and relatively low levels of A and C (Samuel & Widiger, 2008). However, even though research also shows that changes in trait levels lead to changes in PD levels (Warner et al., 2004; Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007), other research suggests that trait levels may not be fully informative regarding affective and behavioral instability (Miller & Pilkonis, 2006). One reasonable yet unexplored hypothesis is that greater personality trait instability may characterize individuals with BPD.
Importantly, the link between BPD and variables that have been found to moderate trait stability support this hypothesis. Some moderators of trait stability, such as qualities of the instruments used to measure traits (Terracciano et al., 2006) and stability interval (Fraley & Roberts, 2005; Roberts & Pomerantz, 2004; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006) have no meaningful relations to BPD, whereas others may. Research suggests that clinical severity is inversely related to trait stability (Scheurger, Zarrella, & Hotz, 1989). Age is positively associated with personality stability (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000) and negatively associated with some features of BPD (Stevenson, Meares, & Comerford, 2003). Person-environment fit is positively correlated with stability (Roberts & Robins, 2004), and presumably poor among individuals with BPD. Mood disorder may also be linked to both BPD (Zanarini et al., 1998) and trait stability (Hirschfeld et al., 1983). Finally, some of the traits associated with BPD, such as high N and low C (Lynam & Widiger, 2001; Morey et al., 2001), are themselves associated with instability on other traits (Donnellan et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2001; Scheurger et al., 1989). The relation of these moderators to BPD, in concert with evidence that BPD is consistently associated with instability in various domains that are related to or influenced by personality, lends credence to the hypothesis that FFM traits may be relatively less stable in individuals with BPD.
Characterizing Personality Trait Stability
One of the major lessons that has emerged from recent research on adult personality development (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988, 1994; Donnellan, Conger, & Burzette, 2007; Fraley & Roberts, 2005; Hampson & Goldberg, 2006; Lockenhoff et al., 2008; Roberts & Del Vecchio, 2000; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006; Terracciano, Costa & McCrae, 2006; Watson & Humrichouse, 2006; Vaidya, Gray, Haig, Mroczek, & Watson, 2008) is that there are several conceptually and statistically distinct forms of personality stability and change. Indeed, researchers have identified at least five such types (De Fruyt, Bartels et al., 2006; De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen et al., 2006; Donnellan & Robins, 2009; Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2001; Vaidya et al., 2008).
The first is structural stability, which often refers to consistency in the patterns of covariation among traits. This is type of stability is typically evaluated by testing the invariance of correlation matrices across measurement occasions and is commonly regarded as a prerequisite for analyses of other kinds of stability. Unlike others types, however, structural stability does not appear to be conceptually related to BPD.
Differential stability refers to consistency in the rank ordering of individuals on a given trait over time. This type of stability is typically assessed with test-retest correlations over substantial intervals. Higher retest coefficients suggest that a construct is more trait-like because individuals who are relatively high (or low) in that dimension at one point are also relatively high in that dimension at a second time point. Given that instability appears to be characteristic of BPD (e.g., Schmideberg, 1959), estimates of differential stability may be lower in those with this disorder in comparison to other groups.
Mean-level stability refers to consistency in the average level of traits over time. Average trait levels change across the life span (Donnellan et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2001, 2006; Vaidya et al., 2008). Previous work with clinical samples suggests that personality pathology tends to improve over time such that average levels of symptoms decline (Grilo et al., 2004). Given the general relation of N (+), E, A, and C (-) to personality pathology (Morey et al., 2002; Saulsman & Page, 2004), it might be expected that N would decrease whereas E, A, and C would show average increases for clinical samples. If this were the case, relatively greater mean-level instability in this direction for the BPD group would suggest that the disorder is less chronic than other PDs at the level of personality traits. Such a finding would challenge the DSM-IV and other conceptions of BPD as a condition of chronic personality problems. Rather, it would imply that BPD is best characterized by extreme traits that are also more dynamic, or perhaps more environmentally malleable, than is the case in other PDs.
Individual-level stability refers to absolute consistency at the level of the individual person. One method for detecting the presence of this kind of stability is to test for intraindividual differences in change in a growth modeling context (i.e., the presence of a random effect around slopes). Variance in slopes confirms the presence of exceptions to the normative trend for the sample (i.e., the fixed effect) with more variability corresponding to a greater amount of individual change. Intraindividual difference in change implies that whereas some individuals demonstrate dramatic increases or decreases, others remain fairly consistent over time, as might be expected in BPD.
Finally, ipsative stability refers to consistency in the patterning of personality traits within the individual. This type of stability captures configural changes over time in terms of personality trait profiles. In other words, investigations of ipsative stability address questions about how well the constellation of personality attributes within the person is preserved over time. Ipsative stability tends to be higher in non-clinical samples than in clinical samples (De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen, et al., 2006), suggesting that instability in an individual’s unique personality profile is associated with pathology and personality immaturity (Roberts et al., 2001). Given the greater relative severity often observed for BPD (Skodol et al., 2002), it can be proposed that individuals with BPD will show less ipsative stability than individuals without other PDs.
The Present Study
The purpose of this study was to compare the stability of FFM traits in individuals diagnosed with BPD to trait stability of individuals with other PDs across 4 assessment intervals over six years in a PD sample. The identification of trait instability as a marker for BPD would have important implications for clinical research and practice. Specifically, this finding would imply further consideration of personality trait variability, in addition to trait levels, as potentially informative regarding BPD and perhaps other forms of psychopathology. We hypothesized that BPD individuals would generally show significantly less stable FFM traits than individuals with other PDs. Given the pervasive instability observed in other domains of functioning such as interpersonal behavior, affect, and identity, we further hypothesized that findings would be consistent across differential, mean-level, individual-level, and ipsative representations of stability and across all five traits.
Method
Participants
This study used FFM data from 432 individuals in the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality disorders Study (CLPS; Gunderson et al., 2000) who were assigned to either BPD or other PD (OPD) groups based on structured interviews (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Sickel, & Yong, 1996). Age at study entry ranged from 18 to 45 by study design. Average age (32.64, S.D. = 8.35) did not significantly differ between individuals with BPD and OPDs. At baseline, there were 291 women and 141 men; 306 were white, 63 African-American, 54 Hispanic, and 9 were of other ethnicities. Traits were assessed once every 2 years over the first 6 years of study participation.
Follow-up analyses revealed a higher percentage of women in the BPD (76.5%) than OPD (62.8%) group (χ2 (1) = 7.79, p < .01), as expected based on previously observed gender ratios of PDs (APA, 1994). Ethnicity rates did not significantly differ across samples (χ2(3) = 6.39, p > .05). Individuals who persisted through 6 years of the study were younger (F = 6.37, p < .01), more conscientious (F = 9.08, p < .01), more likely to be Caucasian (χ2 = 32.57, p < .01), and more likely to be women (χ2 = 3.93, p < .01) than those who attrited. These groups did not significantly differ on any other study variable.
Measures
The NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is a self-report questionnaire designed to comprehensively assess the five FFM traits and 30 FFM facets. Participants answer its 240 items on a five point scale. The NEO-PI-R was administered at baseline and 2-year (N = 432; BPD N = 130; OPD N = 302), 4-year (Total N = 417; BPD N = 120; OPD N = 297), and 6-year follow up (Total N = 379; BPD N = 108; OPD N = 271). The internal consistency coefficients for the NEO-PI-R are well established in this (Morey et al., 2002) and other samples, and tests of independent samples correlations showed that BPD and OPD samples did not significantly differ in internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) at any assessment period (p > .05).
The Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV; Zanarini et al., 1996), a structured interview, assesses each of the 10 DSM-IV PDs. The inter-rater (median k = .92) and test-retest reliability (median k = .68) of the original DIPD are acceptable (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Chauncey, & Gunderson, 1987), and reliability testing in the present study (Zanarini et al., 2000) suggested similar results. All study participants in the BPD group met diagnostic criteria for BPD at baseline, and no participants in the OPD group met criteria for BPD at baseline. As reported elsewhere (McGlashan et al., 2000), many participants in both groups had several co-occurring Axis I and II disorders. For the current report, study groups were defined by baseline diagnoses.
The Structured Interview for DSM-IV Axis I (SCID-I; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, & Williams, 1996) was administered to assess mood disorder. The diagnosis of a major depressive episode during the study was used as a covariate in the current report. Overall, 252 (58.3%) participants experienced a major depressive episode during the study. The BPD group (66.7%) had significantly more episodes than the OPD group (55.9%; χ2(1) = 4.40, p < .05).
The Longitudinal Interval Follow-up Evaluation (LIFE; Keller et al., 1987) is a commonly used interview that assesses, among other variables, ongoing treatment utilization. Participants were asked at each assessment interval whether they had participated in several modalities of treatment since the most recent assessment. Modalities included individual psychotherapy, hospitalizations, medications, group therapy, family therapy, and self-help groups. The sum of treatments participants had utilized represented a covariate in the current study.
Analyses
Structural stability was examined by fitting covariance structure models that constrained the covariance matrix among FFM domains to be equal across time in the full sample, the BPD sample, and the OPD sample. For the covariance matrix among five factors, this included 15 unique parameters constrained at each time period. Due to attrition in the longitudinal design, these models were estimated using the LISREL 8.54 Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) missing data routine (Joreskog & Sorbom, 2003). Although this missing data treatment is superior to alternative missing data strategies for analyzing longitudinal designs (Newman, 2003), LISREL’s FIML routine does not support the full complement of fit indexes: only χ2 and RMSEA indexes are reported.
Differential stability was examined by computing within-trait correlations across each time lag. Independent correlations tests were then used to compare the BPD and OPD groups. Partial correlations were also tested across groups when controlling for several factors that have been shown to affect personality trait stability, including mood disorder (Hirschfeld et al., 1983), age (Roberts & Del Vecchio, 2000), African-American ethnicity (Lockenhoff et al., 2008), treatment utilization (De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen et al. 2006), and trait levels themselves, particularly neuroticism and conscientiousness given their associations with psychological maturity, (Donnellan et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2001).
Mean-level stability was tested with growth curve modeling (Lenzenweger, Johnson, & Willett, 2004; Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007; Vaidya et al., 2008) in SAS Proc Mixed. A multilevel model (time points nested within individuals) was estimated for each FFM trait. The models’ fixed effects are group-level (pooled individual-level) intercepts and slopes. The time variable was coded on a 4-point scale (each scale point corresponds to a 2-year interval), and then grand mean-centered (so the temporal midpoint of the study equaled zero). Each trait score was modeled as a function of time (Level 1 predictor), diagnostic status (Level 2 predictor), and their cross-level interaction (diagnosis × time). This allowed tests of group differences for each trait, the effect of time on trait scores, and the interaction effect of diagnostic status on the stability of means. Mean-level analyses were conducted both with and without group-level covariates.
We followed Vaidya et al. (2008) in reporting individual-level variability in growth trajectories in lieu of reporting differences in rates of reliable change (Christensen & Mendoza, 1986; Jacobson & Truax, 1991). Growth trajectory analysis estimates the amount of between-person variance in within-person slope parameters. In extension of these previous authors’ work, we further compare slope variance parameters across diagnostic groups, to test whether BPD individuals exhibit greater variability in time slopes.
Finally, differences in ipsative stability were investigated by comparing q correlations for each sample within each time lag. This coefficient captures the degree of stability in the shape of the personality profiles and is calculated by correlating each sample member’s vector of scores on the 30 FFM facets at one assessment with the corresponding vector at another assessment.
Results
Results of the test of structural stability over time appear in Table 1. We base our interpretation on the RMSEA practical index of fit (Steiger, 1990), rather than upon χ2, because the χ2 fit index is directly proportional to sample size. As shown in Table 1, fit indices reached acceptable levels for all samples (RMSEA < .07; Hu & Bentler, 1999), supporting structural stability over time and suggesting that longitudinal comparisons are meaningful within each sample.
Table 1.
Model | χ2 | df | RMSEA |
---|---|---|---|
Full PD Sample, Covariances Equal over Time | 100.54 | 45 | .045 |
Borderline Sample, Covariances Equal over Time | 77.38 | 45 | .063 |
Other PD Sample, Covariances Equal over Time | 73.41 | 45 | .038 |
Table 2 presents data on differential stability. For the overall sample, N (6-year stability = .57) was the least stable trait and O (6-year stability = .76) the most stable. As might be expected based on research showing an age effect on personality stability (Roberts & Del Vecchio, 2000), coefficients tended to increase with each 2-year lag for all five traits. Consistent with our hypothesis, the BPD group exhibited lower differential stability coefficients than the OPD group in 25/30 comparisons (83%, sign test p < .001). These differences were statistically significant for 4/6 comparisons involving N, 3/6 for C, 2/6 for O, 1/6 involving A, and none for E. With some exceptions (i.e., N at baseline-24 months and C at baseline-72 months), statistically significant comparisons remained so after controlling for age, African-American ethnicity, treatment utilization, mood disorder, N, and C. Further, even those comparisons that were no longer significant after controlling for other factors remained lower for the BPD than OPD group by a difference ≥ .08.
Table 2.
Time Interval | Full Sample | Borderline | Other PD |
---|---|---|---|
Baseline to 24 months | N = 432 | N = 130 | N = 302 |
Neuroticism* | .65 | .55 | .68 |
Extraversion | .71 | .68 | .72 |
Openness* | .79 | .74 | .81 |
Agreeableness | .75 | .75 | .74 |
Conscientiousness* | .73 | .62 | .77 |
24 to 48 months | N = 340 | N = 97 | N = 243 |
Neuroticism* | .77 | .70 | .79 |
Extraversion | .79 | .77 | .80 |
Openness | .84 | .81 | .85 |
Agreeableness | .81 | .80 | .82 |
Conscientiousness* | .79 | .67 | .84 |
48 to 72 months | N = 348 | N = 98 | N = 250 |
Neuroticism | .77 | .74 | .78 |
Extraversion | .81 | .82 | .81 |
Openness | .86 | .85 | .87 |
Agreeableness | .84 | .81 | .85 |
Conscientiousness | .80 | .81 | .80 |
Baseline to 48 months | N = 417 | N = 120 | N = 297 |
Neuroticism | .62 | .53 | .64 |
Extraversion | .70 | .70 | .69 |
Openness | .78 | .76 | .79 |
Agreeableness | .74 | .72 | .75 |
Conscientiousness | .72 | .67 | .73 |
24 to 72 months | N = 311 | N = 92 | N = 219 |
Neuroticism* | .68 | .54 | .72 |
Extraversion | .76 | .73 | .77 |
Openness* | .79 | .72 | .83 |
Agreeableness | .77 | .79 | .77 |
Conscientiousness | .71 | .64 | .73 |
Baseline to 72 months | N = 379 | N = 108 | N = 271 |
Neuroticism* | .57 | .41 | .61 |
Extraversion | .67 | .63 | .68 |
Openness | .76 | .73 | .77 |
Agreeableness* | .71 | .64 | .74 |
Conscientiousness* | .65 | .52 | .69 |
Note. Statistical difference test (one-tailed) between independent correlations:
= p < .05.
All significant differences remained so (p < .05) with age, African-American ethnicity, presence of mood disorder, treatment utilization, and neuroticism/conscientiousness covaried except neuroticism at baseline-24 months and conscientiousness at baseline to 72 months.
Table 3 depicts the results from mean-level stability analyses. These analyses tested three effects for each trait: time, diagnostic status, and the interaction between time and diagnostic status. Time was a significant factor for four of the five personality traits: mean levels of N and O decreased, whereas mean levels of E and A increased over the course of the study. Cohen’s d effect sizes for the full sample on baseline to 72 month mean change were as follows: N = -.67, E = .13, O = -.14, A = .24, C = .22. The BPD group had significantly higher scores on N and lower scores on A and C than the OPD group (Morey et al., 2002). The critical test for our hypothesis that BPD is less stable than other PDs was the time × diagnosis interaction, which was significant for N and C. On N, the BPD group mean tended to decrease over time at a faster rate than the OPD group mean (d = -.80 versus d = -.58). In contrast, the BPD group mean on C increased faster than the OPD group mean (d = .32 versus d = .15). Both cases suggest steeper slopes (faster mean change) for BPD.
Table 3.
Trait | Estimate | t |
---|---|---|
Neuroticism | ||
Time | -4.87 | -11.12* |
Borderline | 8.35 | 4.23* |
Time X Borderline | -2.20 | -2.67* |
Extraversion | ||
Time | 1.02 | 3.00* |
Borderline | 3.05 | 1.73 |
Time X Borderline | -0.56 | -0.89 |
Openness | ||
Time | -0.68 | -2.47* |
Borderline | -1.79 | -1.01 |
Time X Borderline | -0.86 | -1.67 |
Agreeableness | ||
Time | 1.60 | 5.76* |
Borderline | -4.07 | -2.55* |
Time X Borderline | 0.18 | 0.35 |
Conscientiousness | ||
Time | 0.67 | 1.78 |
Borderline | -7.37 | -3.87* |
Time X Borderline | 1.81 | 2.57* |
Note.
p < .05;
Time effects are per 24-month interval; for Borderline variable, Borderline = 1 and Other PD = 0. All significant differences remained so (p < .05) with age, African-American ethnicity, presence of mood disorder, treatment utilization, and neuroticism/conscientiousness covaried, except the borderline main effect on neuroticism (p-value increased to .06).
As De Fruyt, Barels et al. (2006) and Vaidya et al. (2008) discuss in detail, individual-level stability differs from mean-level stability in capturing the extent to which individuals exhibit trait changes around the group mean. For example, a small mean change can mask important individual-level changes that occur in opposite directions across individuals. Table 4 reveals statistically significant levels of variability in time slopes across individuals. Consistent with our prediction, BPD individuals showed consistently more slope variance than OPD individuals across all five traits. However, this difference was only statistically significant for N, with a trend (p < .10) approaching significance for C.
Table 4.
Trait | Full sample | Borderline | Other PD | -2LL Difference BPD vs. OPD |
---|---|---|---|---|
Neuroticism | ||||
Variance of Intercept | 422.01* | 334.10* | 428.65* | 1.2 |
Variance of Slope [Time] | 23.14* | 33.91* | 17.26* | 6.8* |
Extraversion | ||||
Variance of Intercept | 343.67* | 323.63* | 345.73* | 0.0 |
Variance of Slope [Time] | 15.00* | 15.77* | 14.76* | 0.9 |
Openness | ||||
Variance of Intercept | 363.26* | 366.57* | 361.45* | 0.0 |
Variance of Slope [Time] | 8.61* | 8.44* | 7.93* | 2.7 |
Agreeableness | ||||
Variance of Intercept | 292.98* | 297.63* | 279.29* | 0.2 |
Variance of Slope [Time] | 9.58* | 13.17* | 6.98* | 1.8 |
Conscientiousness | ||||
Variance of Intercept | 450.64* | 365.55* | 407.28* | 0.0 |
Variance of Slope [Time] | 19.91* | 20.74* | 18.28* | 3.4† |
Note.
p < .05;
p < .10;
all estimates from unconditional linear growth models; -2LL is -2 log likelihood (fit index), which is compared against a χ2 distribution with df = 1.
A final test of stability involved the within-person correlation across facets between two time points. The average q correlation across 30 NEO-PI-R facets was reasonably large in the current sample, ranging from .57 to .77 (see Table 5). As with previous tests of configural stability (e.g., De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen et al., 2006), substantial ranges were observed for the full sample (e.g., ranging from -.50 to .99). Consistent with our overall hypothesis, the q correlation calculated from the BPD group was smaller at every assessment period than the average q correlation calculated from the OPD group, with this difference reaching statistical significance in 5 of 6 comparisons.
Table 5.
Time Interval | BPD | OPD | Full Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Baseline to 24 months (lag = 2 yrs) * | .61 | .70 | .67 |
24 months to 48 months (lag = 2 yrs) * | .68 | .74 | .73 |
48 months to 72 months (lag = 2 yrs) * | .72 | .77 | .77 |
Baseline to 48 months (lag = 4 yrs) | .62 | .67 | .66 |
24 months to 72 months (lag = 4 yrs) * | .63 | .71 | .69 |
Baseline to 72 months (lag = 6 yrs) * | .57 | .65 | .63 |
Note.
p < .05,
independent t-test for within-person q correlations of NEO-PI-R facet profiles across BPD and OPD groups. Very similar results are obtained when analyses are limited to NEO-PI-R domain profiles.
Discussion
Most previous research on the associations among personality traits and personality disorders has tested hypotheses involving relationships between static trait levels or configurations of static trait levels and personality disorders (e.g., Samuel & Widiger, 2008), with somewhat less research on the comparative validities of traits and disorders (e.g., Morey et al., 2007; Trull et al., 2003) and longitudinal relationships between trait levels and PD levels (e.g., Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007; Warner et al., 2004). We extended this previous work by investigating the role of personality trait stability for understanding BPD. We hypothesized that, beyond its well-characterized associations with certain traits, BPD would also have marked longitudinal variability in FFM traits. Overall, results supported this general prediction.
Although we had hypothesized that all FFM traits would tend to be less stable in BPD participants, our results indicate some specificity with respect to the stability of the particular personality traits. Individuals with BPD tended to exhibit greater differential change, mean level change, and individual variability in change slopes for N and C. In other words, the rank ordering of BPD patients on N and C tends to be less predictable over time than it does in a group of patients with other PDs; individuals with BPD tend to exhibit greater decreases on N and increases on C than those with other PDs; and, despite this greater rate of change, there was more variability within individuals who have BPD with regard to changes in N and C compared to individuals with other PDs. It is important to note that N and C also strongly differentiate BPD from other PDs in terms of trait levels (as shown by the disorder main effects in Table 3 and previously reported by Morey et al., 2002; see also Samuel & Widiger, 2008). These findings expand what can be understood as the importance of these traits for BPD by showing that variability in N and C may represent a marker of BPD that is independent of the extremity of these traits. Alternatively, the observed instability may be associated with greater measurement error at the extremes of these traits.
Although these findings highlight the specific importance of instability of N and C in regards to BPD, ipsative analyses also point to the importance of the stability of overall FFM trait configurations. Members of the BPD group had lower ipsative correlations of FFM configurations over time than individuals with other PDs, meaning that the relative patterning or prominence of traits within a given individual was more variable over time in the BPD group. Allport (1955) argued that the unique organization of traits within an individual is the “most outstanding” feature of personality (p. 23). From this perspective, personality is hierarchically organized, not just in a normative sense such as the NEO-PI-R domains and facets, but also hierarchically in an idiographic sense, with broad individualized dispositional traits tending to “attract, guide, (or) inhibit the more elementary units” (p. 92). Such elementary units might include affective experiences, interpersonal behaviors and self processes of the type that tend to be dysregulated and unstable in individuals with BPD.
Personality Stability and Personality Disorders
Overall, our findings indicate that individuals with BPD are less stable with regard to N and C as well as in the organization of FFM traits relative to those with other PDs. This result may imply that some of the symptomatic, behavioral, and emotional instability among individuals with BPD may represent consequences of various kinds of instability in these personality traits. In addition, results from this study may also suggest a more general association between clinical improvement and trait stability. Average levels of the FFM traits generally changed in a healthier direction over time in the full sample, with N decreasing and E, A, and C increasing. Traits concurrently appeared to gain differential and ipsative stability with each assessment lag. Attrition-robust missing data analyses (i.e., FIML; Enders, 2001, Newman, 2003) showed the same strong and consistent trend of increasing retest correlations over time, mitigating an attrition-based explanation for increasing stability. The normative influences of maturity (Donnellan et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2001) are also an unlikely explanation for these effects, as the pattern of differences in rank-order and mean-level stabilities across assessment intervals remained similar controlling for age. Thus, these results point to a general increase in average levels of personality traits that was not simply a consequence of normative age-related increases or to the biasing effects of selective attrition. That said, increasing health over time in the CLPS sample may have alternatively reflected treatment, regression to the mean, or other factors. Although the effect tended to persist with treatment utilization controlled, this quantitative variable does not speak directly to the quality of treatment, leaving the possibility that this general trend reflects the consequence of therapeutic change largely open.
As it stands, we offer the substantive interpretation that these mean-level changes and increases in stability are evidence for the clinical postulate that stability and integration of personality is a developmental process related to greater health and functioning (Kernberg, 1976; 1984; Clarkin et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2001) which may be particularly salient for BPD (Bender & Skodol, 2007). Such as interpretation in conjunction with observed empirical changes casts doubt on the historical conceptualization of BPD as largely unchangeable. Moreover, this position is inconsistent with the DSM-IV description of PDs as indicating stable, rigid, and enduring personality characteristics. To the contrary, we contend that personality trait stability is generally a hallmark of psychological maturity and health.
All in all, we propose that personality trait stability is a potentially important consideration when attempting to understand personality pathology as well as when trying to conceptualize the connections between normal personality dimensions and personality pathology. Accordingly, we believe that personality stability, along with normative personality traits themselves, should be duly considered in the revision of the DSM. Trait stability may be a robust marker of psychological well-being whereas instability may be a risk factor for BPD as well as, perhaps, a wider range of pathologies. Building on the current results, further research is needed to optimally characterize links between personality stability and psychiatric classification and prediction.
Limitations and Directions for Further Research
Several study limitations suggest the need for further research and replication. As with most studies in PD populations, participants usually had multiple co-occurring Axis I and II disorders during the course of the study (McGlashan et al., 2000). Additionally, despite this substantial comorbidity, individuals in the OPD group were selected to have one of three other PDs (avoidant, obsessive-compulsive, or schizotypal). Although comorbidity is representative of typical patients and likely supports the generalizability of these findings, these factors may have muddled comparisons across groups and limited power to find group differences. Because diagnostic overlap is characteristic of PDs in research and clinical practice including the sample used here, future research with cleaner diagnostic groups may yield stronger, more nuanced findings.
A second complicating issue involves the potential role of unstable affect states on ratings of personality traits (Santor, Bagby, & Joffe, 1997). In the current study, controlling for depression during personality trait assessment had a limited effect on results. However, research that disentangles the effect of affective states and the stability of traits should be a priority among future investigators, particularly given the construct overlap of mood and FFM traits (Vaidya et al., 2008). More generally, research linking various kinds of stability, such as stability of affective experience, interpersonal behavior, identity, and personality traits, is needed to better understand whether instability has a broad and pervasive quality or is unrelated across these domains.
A related topic involves the role of treatment. Previous research with depressed patients has been mixed with regard to the effect of therapy on personality change (Bagby, Joffe, Parker, Kalemba, & Harkness, 1995; Cyranowski et al., 2004; De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen et al., 2006; Jain, Blais, Otto, Hirschfeld, & Sachs, 1999). As discussed above, the index for treatment utilization used in this study was not optimal, as it represented the number of modalities used, rather than the nature (e.g., specific types of psychotherapy or pharmacotherapy) and extent of use within and across modalities. Further research is needed to disentangle the effects of treatment on personality stability, and the extent to which treatment effects explain the relation of BPD to instability (Bender et al., 2006).
A final and important area for future research concerns the mechanisms of the instability observed in the current results. These findings represent a useful starting point for considering the neurobiological, social, and developmental correlates of personality change that might be relevant to or affected by BPD. Greater understanding of such correlates may assist in depicting the causal direction of the BPD-trait instability association. Fraley and Roberts (2005) provided a framework for research questions involving the mechanisms of personality change consisting of constant, transactional, and stochastic elements of personality development that could be useful for this purpose.
Conclusion
Recent advances in conceptualizations of personality structure, personality stability, the structure and stability of personality pathology, and relationships between personality traits and disorders have been substantial. The current study contributes to these literatures by linking variability in personality traits with a specific disorder that is thought to be defined by pervasive instability in thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Results enhance the current understanding of BPD and more broadly suggest the need for further investigations concerning associations between personality trait instability and psychopathology.
Acknowledgments
Supported by NIMH grants MH 50837, 50838, 50839, 50840, 50850; MH01654 (McGlashan); MH073708 (Sanislow); MH75543 (Hopwood). This publication has been reviewed and approved by the Publications Committee of the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/pubs/journals/abn.
Contributor Information
Christopher J. Hopwood, Michigan State University
Daniel A. Newman, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
M. Brent Donnellan, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University.
John C. Markowitz, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Weill Medical College of Cornell University
Carlos M. Grilo, Yale University School of Medicine
Charles A. Sanislow, Yale University School of Medicine
Emily B. Ansell, Yale University School of Medicine
Thomas H. McGlashan, Yale University School of Medicine
Andrew E. Skodol, The Sunbelt Collaborative and University of Arizona College of Medicine
M. Tracie Shea, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University.
John G. Gunderson, McLean Hospital and Harvard Medical School
Mary C. Zanarini, McLean Hospital and Harvard Medical School
Leslie C. Morey, Texas A&M University
References
- Allport GW. Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1955. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition, text-revised (DSM-IV-TR) Washington, D.C: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ansell EB, Pincus AL. Interpersonal perceptions of the five-factor model of personality: An examination using the structural summary method for circumplex data. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2004;39:167–201. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagby RM, Joffe RT, Parker JDA, Kalemba V, Harkness KL. Major depression and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders. 1995;9:223–234. [Google Scholar]
- Bender DS, Skodol AE. Borderline personality disorder as self-other representational disturbance. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2007;21:200–217. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.5.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bender DS, Skodol AE, Pagano ME, Dyck IR, Grilo CM, Shea MT, Sanislow CA, Zanarini MC, Yen S, McGlashan TH, Gunderson JG. Prospective assessment of treatment use by patients with personality disorders. Psychiatric Services. 2006;57:254–257. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.57.2.254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blais MA. Clinician ratings of the five-factor model of personality and the DSM-IV personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1997;185(6):388–393. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199706000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen AL, Mendoza L. A method of assessing change in a single subject: An alteration of the RC index. Behavior Therapy. 1986;17:305–308. [Google Scholar]
- Clarkin JF, Lenzenweger MF, Yeomans F, Levy KN, Kernberg OF. An object relations model of borderline pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2007;21:474–499. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.5.474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:853–863. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.5.853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, McRae RR. Professional manual: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In: Heatherton TF, Weinberger JL, editors. Can personality change? Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1994. pp. 21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Cowdry RW, Gardner DL, O’Leary KM, Leibenluft E, Rubinow DR. Mood variability: A study of four groups. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1991;148:1505–1511. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.11.1505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyranowski JM, Frank E, Winter E, Rucci P, Novick D, Pilkonis P, Houck P, Maccarelli L, Kupfer DJ. Personality pathology and outcome in recurrently depressed women over 2 years of maintenance interpersonal psychotherapy. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34:659–669. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Fruyt F, Bartels M, Van Leeuwen KG, De Clercq B, Decuyper M, Mervielde I. Five types of personality continuity in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:538–552. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Fruyt F, Van Leeuwen K, Bagby RM, Rolland JP, Rouillon F. Assessing and interpreting personality change and continuity in patients treated for major depression. Psychological Assessment. 2006;18:71–80. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Burzette RG. Personality development from late adolescence to young adulthood: Differential stability, normative maturity, and evidence for the maturity-stability hypothesis. Journal of Personality. 2007;75(2):237–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Robins RW. The development of personality across the life span. In: Matthews G, Corr P, editors. Cambridge Handbook of Personality. Cambridge University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Eid M, Diener E. Intraindividual variability in affect: Reliability, validity, and personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;76:662–676. [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK. A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms for use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:128–141. [Google Scholar]
- Fraley RC, Roberts BW. Patterns of continuity: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review. 2005;112:60–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Gunderson JG, Pagano ME, Yen S, Zanarini MC, Shea MT, Skodol AE, Stout RL, Morey LC, McGlashan TH. Two-year stability and change in schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;72:797–775. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Skodol AE, McGlashan TH, Morey LC, Stout RL, Zanarini MC, Grilos CM, Oldham JM, Keller MB. The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality disorders Study: Development, aims, design, and sample characteristics. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2000;14:300–315. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampson SE, Goldberg LR. A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:763–779. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL, Clayton P, Keller MB, McDonald-Scott P, Larkin B. Assessing personality: Effects of the depressive state on trait measurement. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1983;140:695–699. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.6.695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC. Psychological conflict in borderline personality as represented by inconsistent self-report item responding. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2007;26:1097–1107. [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Markowitz JC, Ansell EB, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Yen S, Shea MT, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE. The convergent and discriminant validity of five-factor traits: Current and prospective social, work, and leisure functioning. Journal of Personality Disorders. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.5.466. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, Skodol AE. Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2007;195:266–269. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253747.44836.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59:12–19. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jain U, Blais MA, Otto MW, Hirshfeld DR, Sachs GS. Five-factor personality traits in patients with seasonal depression: Treatment effects and comparisons with bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1999;55:51–54. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(98)00206-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joreskog KG, Sorbom D. LISREL 8.5. Lincolwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Keller MB, Lavori PW, Friedman B, Nielsen E, Endicott J, McDonald-Scott P, Andreason NC. The Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation: A comprehensive method for assessing outcome in prospective longitudinal studies. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1987;44:540–548. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180050009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kernberg OF. Object relations theory and clinical psychoanalysis. New York, NY: Jason Aronson, Inc; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Kernberg OF. Severe personality disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Koenigsberg HW, Harvey PD, Mitropoulou V, New AS, Goodman M, Silverman J, Serby M, Schopick F, Siever LJ. Are the interpersonal and identity disturbances in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked to the traits of affective instability and impulsivity? Journal of Personality Disorders. 2001;15:358–370. doi: 10.1521/pedi.15.4.358.19181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF, Willett JF. Predicting individual change in personality disorder features by simultaneous individual change in personality dimensions linked to neurobehavioral systems: The longitudinal study of personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:684–700. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lockenhoff CE, Terraciano A, Bienvenu OJ, Patriciu NS, Nestadt G, McCrae RR, Eaton WW, Costa PT., Jr Ethnicity, education, and the temporal stability of personality traits in the East Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42:577–598. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markon KE, Krueger RF, Watson D. Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:139–157. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlashan TH, Grilo CM, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Morey LC, Zanarini MC, Stout RL. The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Baseline axis I/II diagnostic co-occurence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102:256–264. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004256.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlashan TH, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Ralevski E, Morey LC, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE, Shea MT, Zanarini MC, Bender DS, Stout RL, Yen S, Pagano ME. Two-year prevalence and stability of individual criteria for schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: Toward a hybrid model. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:883–889. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JD, Pilkonis PA. Neuroticism and affective instability: The same or different? American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:839–845. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey LC, Gunderson JG, Quigley BD, Shea MT, Skodol AE, McGlashan TH, Stout RL, Zanarini MC. The representation of borderline, avoidant, obsessive-compulsive, and schizotypal personality disorders by the five-factor model. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2002;16:215–234. doi: 10.1521/pedi.16.3.215.22541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey LC, Hopwood CJ, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE, Shea MT, Yen S, Stout RL, Zanarini MC, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, McGlashan TH. A comparison of personality disorder models. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:983–994. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey LC, Zanarini MC. Borderline personality: Traits and disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:733–737. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman DA. Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing data: A simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organizational Research Methods. 2003;6:328–362. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. The kids are alright: Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:670–683. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Del Vecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Pomerantz EM. On traits, situations, and their integration: A developmental perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2004;8:402–416. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Robins RW. Person-environment fit and its implications for personality development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:89–110. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00257.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robins RW, Fraley RC, Roberts BW, Trzesniewski KH. A longitudinal study of personality change in young adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2001;69:617–664. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.694157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robins RW, Tracy JL, Trzesniewski KH, Potter J, Gosling SD. Personality correlates of self-esteem. Journal of Research on Personality. 2001;35:463–482. [Google Scholar]
- Russell JJ, Moskowitz DS, Zuroff DC, Sookman D, Paris J. Stability and variability of affective experience and interpersonal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:578–588. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samuel DB, Widiger TA. A meta-analytic review of the relationships between the five-factor model and DSM-IV-TR personality disorders: a facet level analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:1326–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanislow CA, Morey LC, Grilo CM, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Skodol AE, Stout RL, Zanarini MC, McGlashan TH. Confirmatory factor analysis of DSM-IV borderline, schizotypal, avoidant and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: findings from the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2002;105:28–36. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.0_479.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santor DA, Bagby RM, Joffe RT. Evaluating stability and change in personality and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;73:1354–1362. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saulsman LM, Page AC. The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;23:1055–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2002.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmideberg M. The borderline patient. In: Arieti S, editor. American handbook of psychiatry. Vol. 1. New York: Basic Books; 1959. pp. 398–416. [Google Scholar]
- Schuerger JM, Zarrella KL, Hotz AS. Factors that influence the temporal stability of personality by questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989;56:777–783. [Google Scholar]
- Shea MT, Stout R, Gunderson JG, Morey LC, Grilo CM, McGlashan T, Skodol AE, Dolan-Sewell R, Dyck I, Zanarini MC, Keller MB. Short-term diagnostic stability of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:2036–2041. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, McGlashan TH, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Bender DS, Grilo CM, Shea MT, Zanarini MC, Morey LC, Sanislow CA, Oldham JM. Functional impairment in schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:276–283. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol AE, Oldham JM, Bender DS, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Morey LC, Shea MT, Zanarini MC, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, Gunderson JG. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV personality disorders: Relations to functional impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1919–1925. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1990;25:173–180. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein KF. Affect instability in adults with a borderline personality disorder. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 1996;10:32–40. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9417(96)80084-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson J, Meares R, Comerford A. Diminished impulsivity in older patients with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(1):165–166. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terracciano A, Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. Personality plasticity after age 30. Personailty and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:999–1009. doi: 10.1177/0146167206288599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ, Solhan MB, Tragesser SL, Jahng S, Wood PK, Piasecki TM, Watson D. Affective instability: Measuring a core feature of borderline personality disorder with ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:647–661. doi: 10.1037/a0012532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ, Widiger TA, Lynam DR, Costa PT. Borderline personality disorder from the perspective of general personality functioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:193–202. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya JG, Gray EK, Haig JR, Mroczek DK, Watson D. Differential stability and individual growth trajectories of Big Five and affective traits during young adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2008;76:267–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00486.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warner MB, Morey LC, Finch JF, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE, Sanislow CA, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Grilo CM. The longitudinal relationship of personality traits and disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:217–227. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Humrichouse J. Personality development in emerging adulthood: Integrating evidence from self- and spouse-ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:959–974. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widiger TA, Trull TJ, Clarkin JF, Sanderson C, Costa PT., Jr . A description of the DSM-IV personality disorders with the five-factor model of personality. In: Widiger TA, Costa PT Jr, editors. Personality Disorders and the Five-Factor Model of Personality. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 89–99. [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Chauncey DL, Gunderson JG. The diagnostic interview for personality disorders: Inter-rater and test-retest reliability. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1987;28:467–480. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(87)90012-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Silk KR, Hudson JI, McSweeney LB. The subsyndromal phenomenology of borderline personality disorder: A 10-year follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:929–935. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Sickel AE, Yong L. The Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV) Belmont, MA: McLean Hospital; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Gunderson JG, Marino MF, Schwartz EO, Frankenburg FR. DSM-III disorders in the families of borderline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders. 1998;2:292–302. [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Skodol AE, Bender D, Dolan R, Sanislow CA, Schaefer E, Morey LC, Grilo CM, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Gunderson JG. The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Reliability of axis I and axis II diagnoses. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2000;14:291–299. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeigler-Hill V, Abraham J. Borderline personality features: Instability of self-esteem and affect. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2006;25:668–687. [Google Scholar]