Abstract
Formal thought disorder is a feature schizophrenia that manifests as disorganized, incoherent speech, and is associated with a poor clinical outcome. The neurocognitive basis of this symptom is unclear but it is thought to involve an impairment in semantic processing classically described as a loosening of meaningful associations. Using a paradigm derived from the n400 event-related, potential, we examined the extent to which regional activation during semantic processing is altered in schizophrenic patients with formal thought disorder. Ten healthy control and 18 schizophrenic participants (9 with and 9 without formal thought disorder) performed a semantic decision sentence task during an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment. We employed analysis of variance to estimate the main effects of semantic congruency and groups on activation and specific effects of formal thought disorder were addressed using post-hoc comparisons. We found that the frontotemporal network, normally engaged by a semantic decision task, was underactivated in schizophrenia, particularly in patients with FTD. This network is implicated in the inhibition of automatically primed stimuli and impairment of its function interferes with language processing and contributes to the production of incoherent speech.
1. Introduction
Bleuler's work in psychosis continues to be highly influential in furthering understanding of the signs and symptoms of schizophrenia [1]. Nevertheless, one of his primary conceptual contributions in understanding schizophrenia, “disturbance of associations” [2, 3], remains to be explained in terms of underlying neural basis. In turn, Bleuler's ideas were influenced by Jung's word association task [4]. Regardless of the psychological or affective mechanisms that may influence the production of speech, Jungian word association is by nature a semantic association test. Interpretations of the broader meanings of “split mind” and “association”, arising from the psychoanalytical field, are not incompatible with an inbuilt characteristic of this test, which taps into the concept of semantic priming [5], extensively investigated in schizophrenia (see Minzenberg et al. [6] for a review in single-word semantic priming in schizophrenia). However, some studies do not take into account specific symptoms proposed by Bleuler, such as formal thought disorder, as an underlying factor that would interfere with task performance (e.g., [7]), mixing up performance of patients with a range of distinct symptoms. Of note, there is a line of investigation suggesting that formal thought disorder (FTD) is associated with hyperactivation of the semantic network [8–14]. These studies mainly employ semantic tasks based on word-pair semantic priming paradigms. It is important to note that such kind of studies, although provided evidence for altered semantic processing associated with FTD, may fail to tap into the type of communication impairment clinically seen in FTD patients. The latter manifests itself in linguistic units that range from large thoughts, such as whole periods with several sentences embedded, to small utterances, such as words within the same sentence [15], or even a single word (e.g., neologism).
Bleuler also suggested that, within its symptoms, FTD is possibly the closest to the neural substrate of schizophrenia [2]. Biological evidence for a neural basis of FTD has been provided by methods employing functional measures of brain activity. EEG studies employing the n400, an event-related potential (ERP) that is a neural marker of semantic processing [16–18], demonstrated a reduction in n400 amplitudes and increased latency in schizophrenia during a sentence processing task [19–21], consistent with impairment in semantic integration, particularly associated with FTD [22–26].
Functional neuroimaging of schizophrenia suggested that FTD is associated with altered resting activity in the medial temporal cortex [27] and with altered activity (brain metabolism or Blood Oxygen Level Dependent-BOLD-activity) during tasks that engage language processing in the dorsolateral prefrontal, bilateral anterior cingulate, and lateral temporal cortices [28–32]). FTD has also been linked to reductions in grey matter volume in the inferior frontal [33, 34] and the temporo-parietal cortex [35]. Using a model of information processing, MacDonalds III and collaborators demonstrated diminished activity in the middle frontal gyrus in schizophrenic patients with disorganization symptoms (includes FTD) associated with context processing deficits [31]. Collectively, the studies particularly implicate the prefrontal, middle frontal, temporal, and anterior cingulate cortices in the pathophysiology of FTD in schizophrenia.
In summary, FTD is a symptom of schizophrenia that manifests itself as disorganized, incoherent speech. Andreasen [36] has operationalized studying this symptom by creating a scale that decomposes the concept of “loosening of meaningful associations” into measurable items. Thus, this clinical manifestation of schizophrenia, which can be objectively measured, is thought to involve impairment in semantic processing, deficits in executive functioning, and altered brain activity in the left frontal, temporal, and anterior cingulate cortices.
The objective of the present study was to use event-related fMRI to examine the extent to which regional activation during semantic processing is altered in schizophrenic patients with FTD. Methodological issues which were not tackled by some of the functional imaging studies mentioned above (small sample sizes, block design fMRI task and an overt production of speech) were carefully addressed. Particularly, we devised a task likely to engage areas associated with linguistic processes with heavier demands [37] using a paradigm derived from semantic decision procedures used in the ERP studies that originally correlated FTD with n400 abnormalities in schizophrenia. We sought to engage the top down modulation of semantic processing by requiring suppression or inhibitory mechanisms to take place, necessary to process semantic incongruent information. Activation of the left inferior frontal and left middle temporal [38, 39], right anterior cingulate [38], and bilateral precuneus [39] was found in studies that required the generation of semantically incongruent endings to complete a previously primed incomplete sentence stem. Therefore, these areas are activated in tasks requiring suppression of endings (meanings) automatically activated by sentences.
We hypothesized the following:
Incongruent sentences would engage left inferior frontal, left middle temporal, right anterior cingulate, and bilateral precuneus more than congruent sentences.
-
FTD patients with schizophrenia would show diminished activation relative to controls and patients without FTD in:
- areas previously demonstrated to show aberrant activity in this group: left prefrontal (middle and inferior), left temporal, and bilateral anterior cingulate cortex and
- areas associated with manipulation of incongruent material (i.e., the above and also the precuneus).
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects
Ten healthy adult volunteers and 18 patients meeting DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia [40] took part in the study. Acutely psychotic patients with either high or low levels of positive FTD were recruited from the South London and Maudsley NHS Trust. Controls were recruited from the same geographical area through local advertisement.
All subjects were dextral [41], males with National Adult Reading Test Revised [42] IQ ≥ 80, and native speakers of British English. Exclusion criteria for controls were a previous history of a neurological or psychiatric disorder, substance dependence, or a medical disorder that could affect the brain. Exclusion criteria for patient were another DSM-IV axis I diagnosis and age of onset of schizophrenia prior to 18 years of age. Subjects received oral and written information about the procedures and gave written consent to participate, with £30 in return for participating. The project was approved by the Research Ethical Committee of the Institute of Psychiatry.
Psychopathology was assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) [36] and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [43]. Positive FTD was assessed on the basis of the corresponding 8 items in the SAPS (derailment; incoherence; illogicality; circumstantiality; pressure of speech; distractible speech; tangentiality; clanging).
In each patient, the score on each FTD SAPS item (range 0–5) was summed to yield a total FTD score (range 0–40). The FTD score was then used both as a classifier to split the patients sample in two subgroups (as described below) and as a continuous measure (see Figure 2(b)), without distinctions within the patients. Thus, patients who had a FTD score 0–4 were classified as non-FTD and those with scores ranging from 5–40 were classified as FTD. The patients sample was split into two groups on this basis producing a subgroup with (FTD, n = 9 and a subgroup without FTD (Non-FTD, n = 9). There were no significant differences between these subgroup in SAPS total score, SANS total score, scale for the Assessment of Global Functioning (GAF) [40] score, antipsychotic medication dose (in chlorpromazine equivalent), duration of illness, number of psychiatric admissions, or age at first admission (Table 1). There were also no differences in the score for negative FTD, as defined by the SANS items (poverty of speech, poverty of content of speech, thought blocking, and increased latency of response).
Table 1.
Group | Non-FTD | FTD | P value |
---|---|---|---|
Positive FTD | 0 (1) 0–2 | 17 (11) 5–20 | <0.001a |
Negative FTD | 5 (7) 0–9 | 6 (6) 3–14 | 0.546a |
SAPS (sum) | 23 (16) 8–50 | 14 (10) 6–26 | 0.471b |
SANS (sum) | 19 (9) 1–34 | 27 (13) 11–52 | 0.588b |
GAF | 31 (13) 21–51 | 30 (14) 20–40 | 0.546a |
Chlorpromazine equivalent in (mg/day) | 700 (967) 133–1800 | 517 (550) 320–1200 | 0.772a |
Duration of illness (in years) | 6 (12) 0.5–17 | 11 (14) 8–28 | 0.190a |
Hospital admissions | |||
Number | 2 (4) 1–15 | 6 (9) 1–20 | 0.142a |
Age at 1st | 25 (9) 19–60 | 26 (11) 15–31 | 0.222a |
aMann-Whitney [median (interquartile range) min and max], bANOVA [Mean (SD) and range], cFischer Exact.
The two patient subgroups and the healthy controls had similar demographic characteristics (Table 2).
Table 2.
Groups | AGE Median (interquartile range); min and max | Nart IQ Mean (SD); range | Years of full-time education median (interquartile range); min and max |
---|---|---|---|
Controls | 35 (10); 24–54 | 108.3 (13.5); 89–126 | 14.3–(5.3); 9–24 |
NON-TD | 38 (21); 24–63 | 101.4 (12.7); 86–122 | 11.0 (1.5)*; 9–15 |
TD | 33 (16); 23–55 | 99.2 (10.7); 82–115 | 11.0 (3)*; 9–12 |
Analysis results | P = 0.222a | P = 0.267b | P = 0.029a |
aKruskal Wallis, bone-way ANOVA, *no difference between TD and non-TD patients for years of education.
2.2. Task
2.2.1. Sentence Stems
Eighty sentence stems (i.e., last word missing) were used with the degree of constraint previously defined in a large sample drawn from the same population as the control subjects in the present study [44]. Forty stems had a high semantic constraint (HCt) (cloze probability > 0.94) and 40 low semantic constraint (LCt) (cloze probability < 0.34). For example, “He posted the letter without a….”, is a HCt stem (completed by the word “stamp” by 96% of subjects), while “She couldn't imagine anyone less…” is a LCt stem.
2.2.2. Target Words
Two types of words were presented as target stimuli, taken from completions that had originally been produced for the stems [44]. These words were either congruous with the sentence stem (i.e., semantically related) or incongruous (semantically unrelated). To avoid phonological priming [45], words with the same initial phoneme as the most frequent congruent completion (best completion) for that stem were excluded.
2.3. Experimental Paradigm
2.3.1. fMRI Stimuli
Sentence stems were presented onto a screen subtending a visual angle of approximately 1° (text height) by 7°, (stem length). Stems were presented for 2.5 sec. After an interval of 0,7 sec during which the screen was blank, the target word appeared (Figure 1). During the inter stimulus interval, subjects were instructed to fixate on an asterisk for at least 13.7 sec. Eighty trials of 20.4 seconds duration were presented, divided into 5 runs of 16 trials each. Trial type per run was pseudorandomized to control for order effects between subjects. Subjects were instructed to read the sentence stem, then decide if the target completed the stem in a sensible way or not making their choice using one of two buttons on a button box (for accuracy and response time recordings). Prior to scanning, all subjects underwent a training session to make sure they were able to understand and perform the task.
2.3.2. fMRI Data Acquisition
Gradient-echo planar MR images were acquired using a quadrature head coil in a 1.5 Tesla GE Signa System (General Electric, Milwaukee, WS, USA). Head movement was minimised by foam padding and a supporting band across the forehead. A gradient echo EPI axial acquisition (TR 1700 milliseconds-ms, TE 40 ms, FA 90°, matrix 64 × 64, FOV 24 cm, thickness 7 mm, gap 0.7 mm, 192 volumes) was used to collect 12 slices parallel to the intercommissural (AC-PC) plane. The total number of images acquired (in 5 runs) was 960 with just under five minutes between them. Structural images were acquired using gradient echo IR EPI sequence (TR 1600 ms, TE 40 ms, TI 180 ms, matrix 128 × 128, FOV 20 cm, thickness 3 mm, gap 0.3 mm, NEX 8, 43 slices). This latter image was used for normalisation to a standard template.
2.4. Analysis
This experiment is part of a larger study [46] in which a series of experiments were designed to test the semantic network and executive functions in FTD patients. The present experiment manipulated semantic constraints and congruencies. In this paper, we only addressed effects of semantic congruency.
2.5. Online Behavioral Data
Repeated Measures (SPSS, General Linear Model) analyses were performed for accuracy scores and reaction times (RT) using semantic congruency as within-subject factors. A “yes” response for a semantically incongruent word or “no” response to a congruent word was considered inaccurate. The percentage of correct responses across trials of the same condition was used as within-subject factor.
2.6. Neuroimaging Data
Imaging data were analysed with xbam v3.4, Department of Biostatistics and Computing, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London [47]. More details about the software can be obtained at http://www.brainmap.it/.
2.6.1. Movement Estimation and Correction
The mean signal intensity over all time points (960) was averaged to create a target image (average image intensity at every voxel across all time points) and the sum of absolute differences in grey scale values between the voxels of each observed image (at each time point) and its corresponding base image was computed. A rigid body registration search algorithm was used to estimate the extent of translation and rotation (3 rotations + 3 translations), minimising the total difference between match and base images. The match images were realigned relative to the base image by tricubic spline interpolation and the realigned T2*-weighted time series were regressed on the concomitant and lagged time series of estimated movement at each voxel [48]. Residual time series resulting from the last stage of this procedure were thus uncorrelated with estimated rigid motion in 3D.
2.6.2. Data Analysis
A nonparametric procedure [49] was adopted that avoids assumptions about the distribution of statistics under the null hypothesis. First, the experimental design was convolved with two poisson functions chosen to model the haemodynamic delays of 4 and 8 seconds (see Figure 1). The best fit (weighted sum) of these two convolutions to the observed time series at each voxel was then computed by least squares analysis. The use of the two convolutions within the experimental design allows the time delay between stimulus onset and peak bold response to vary between these limits, which encompass the normal range of haemodynamic delays. Using the parameters of the least squares model fit, the sums of squares of deviations from the mean image intensity over the whole time series due to the model and the residuals were calculated. The ratio of these sums of squares of residuals (model/residuals) was then calculated (and called SSQ ratio). The appropriate null distribution of this statistic was obtained by repeating the model fitting procedure after wavelet-based permutation of the time series [47] twenty times at each voxel and combining the data over all voxels. For any desired type I error rate, the appropriate critical threshold value of SSQ ratio could be obtained from this distribution. Any value of SSQ ratio that exceeded this threshold was deemed to indicate the presence of a voxel responding to the experimental paradigm.
In order to construct group images, the observed and “null” SSQ ratio maps for each experimental condition were then transformed into Talairach space [50]. Median activation maps for each condition were computed after smoothing with a Gaussian filter (FWHM 7.2 mm). Three maps were produced: (1) condition “b” versus “a”, (2) condition “c” versus “a”, and (3) condition “b” versus “c”, where “a” means baseline and “b” and “c” the experimental conditions tested.
The main effect of semantic congruency was assessed by contrasting the 40 congruent trials (20 low congruent + 20 high congruent) and 40 incongruent trials (20 low incongruent + 20 high congruent) [conditions “b” and “c” maps]. In order to probe the analysis to the main cognitive process under investigation, we chose the 3rd TR in an event-related design, in which the presentation both of the sentence stem and the final word has already taken place, as shown in Figure 1.
Differences in the responses between conditions were obtained at each voxel by regression of the linear model F = a 0 + a 1 P + e, where the vector of responses in all subjects at a voxel, P is a dummy coding vector expressing the particular contrast between experimental conditions of interest, and e is the vector of random errors. Values of a 1* were tested for statistical significance by randomly reallocating the data of each voxel between conditions, thus realising the null distribution under the hypothesis of no difference between responses to different experimental conditions. The analyses reported in this study were carried out using cluster-level statistics to avoid harsh multiple comparison corrections required if each voxel is tested individually [51]. Initially, maps of a 1* were thresholded retaining only clusters with a probability of type I error = 0.0001, for main effects of the task in healthy controls. However, the patients group showed a diminished overall strength of activation relative to healthy controls. We thus decided to report results from maps thresholded at a voxel/cluster probability of type I error = 0.05/0.01, adjusted to get less than 1 false positive per map, to facilitate interpretation of the results, particularly the differences found between controls and patients and between patients themselves (FTD and Non-FTD). The integral of the SSQ ratio values for each of the resulting three-dimensional clusters was then tested against the distribution of cluster SSQ ratio integrals generated after random reallocation of data between conditions at each voxel (see above). Maps reflecting the contrast between conditions (i.e., congruent versus incongruent) in each group were analysed with ANOVAS, producing 2 maps, one showing regions more activated in the first condition and the other showing the reverse.
To minimize the potentially confounding effect of differences in task performance on brain activation, all incorrect trials were excluded from the image analysis. Thus the neuroimaging data were derived from trials in which subjects correctly decided whether the final word was or not semantically congruent with the previous sentence stem.
2.6.3. Data Analyses to Compare Groups
To investigate between group differences due to effects of semantic congruency, the images resulting from the contrast between semantic congruent versus incongruent trials were directly compared between groups, first contrasting all groups in a 3-Group ANOVA with cluster probability of type I error set at 0.01. This analysis produced a range of maps in which one group showed greater activation than the other groups and vice-versa. These maps are presented in the results section below (Figure 2 and Table 3).
Table 3.
Contrast | Size | Tal(x) | Tal(y) | Tal(z) | Probability | Cerebral region |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Congruent > Incongruent | 2189 | 43.33 | 14.81 | 14.85 | 0.000656 | Left inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal portion |
312 | 46.94 | 29.63 | −7.15 | 0.000656 | Right inferior frontal gyrus, orbital portion | |
181 | 0.00 | 11.11 | 31.35 | 0.000656 | Bilateral anterior cingulate gyrus | |
3 | 14.44 | 11.11 | 25.85 | 0.008530 | Right anterior cingulate gyrus | |
15 | 10.83 | 22.22 | −1.65 | 0.000656 | Right caudate | |
7 | −36.11 | −48.15 | 3.85 | 0.001969 | Left middle temporal gyrus | |
6 | −21.67 | −29.63 | −12.65 | 0.001969 | Left parahippocampal gyrus | |
6 | 21.67 | −66.67 | 36.85 | 0.004593 | Right precuneus | |
| ||||||
Incongruent > Congruent | 3094 | −32.50 | 37.04 | 25.85 | 0.000678 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
15 | 3.61 | 44.44 | 25.85 | 0.004749 | Right medial frontal gyrus | |
14 | −21.67 | 55.56 | 9.35 | 0.000678 | Left superior frontal gyrus |
Results | Size | Tal(x) | Tal(y) | Tal(z) | Probability | Cerebral Region |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls > NTD > TD | 79 | −43.33 | 14.81 | 14.85 | 0.007784 | Left inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal portion |
Results | Size | Tal(x) | Tal(y) | Tal(z) | Probability | Cerebral Region |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls > TD | 172 | −43.33 | 14.81 | 14.85 | 0.000284 | Left inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal portion |
95 | 28.89 | −70.37 | −18.15 | 0.000851 | Right cerebellum, posterior lobe, declive | |
92 | 50.56 | 25.93 | 14.85 | 0.002553 | Right inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal portion | |
67 | −57.78 | −37.04 | 3.85 | 0.011211 | Left middle temporal gyrus | |
42 | −25.28 | −62.96 | 36.85 | 0.012766 | Left Precuneus | |
35 | −39.72 | −62.96 | −18.15 | 0.012482 | Left cerebellum, posterior lobe, declive | |
25 | −10.83 | −92.59 | −7.15 | 0.012482 | Left lingual gyrus | |
| ||||||
NON-FTD > TD | 185 | −43.33 | 14.81 | 20.35 | 0.000256 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
127 | 46.94 | 18.52 | 25.85 | 0.001282 | Right middle frontal gyrus | |
37 | 0.00 | 18.52 | 36.85 | 0.008720 | Bilateral anterior cingulate | |
NON-FTD < TD | 81 | 7.22 | −55.56 | 20.35 | 0.003912 | Right posterior cingulate |
Further analyses were conducted to investigate the nature of the results arising from the semantic congruency analysis: we wanted to determine whether differences in activation maps observed between controls and patients could be due to a specific pattern of activation associated with FTD or not. Therefore, separate post-hoc ANOVAS contrasting FTD with controls and FTD with Non-FTD were performed.
3. Results
3.1. Online Behavioral Responses
3.1.1. Accuracy
We did not observe an effect of semantic congruency in performance accuracy (F = 0.65, P = 0.43, df = 1). Equally, there was no significant interaction between congruency and group (F = 0.46, P = 0.64, df = 1). Post- hoc tests (Tukey) demonstrated that there was a significant contrast between FTD and controls [mean Difference = −0.128; Std error = 0.397; P = 0.009; CI: −0.227 to −0.029], reflecting impairment seen in incongruent trials (Figure 4) with FTD showing the worse performance (more mistakes than both other groups). Non-FTD showed an intermediate level of performance, making more mistakes than FTD and fewer relative to controls, without significant differences in both contrasts.
3.1.2. Reaction Times
In all groups, responses to congruent trials were faster than incongruent ones (see Figure 5), with an observed significant effect of semantic congruency on reaction times (F = 14.17, P = 0.001, df = 1) and no significant interactions between semantic congruency and group (F = 0.84, P = 0.442, df = 2). A robust main effect of group on RT was found (F = 34.10, P < 0.001, df = 2). Non-FTD patients were slower than FTD and controls in both conditions (congruent and incongruent trials). Controls were faster than FTD in both conditions. Post-hoc contrasts demonstrated significant differences in RT between all pairs contrasted, that is, controls versus FTD [mean difference = −0.442; std error = 0.800; P < 0.001; CI: −0.639 to −0.244], controls versus NON-FTD [mean difference = −0.672; std error = 0.085; P < 0.001; CI: −0.880 to −0.463], and FTD versus NON-FTD [mean difference = −0.229; std error = 0.088; P = 0.036; CI: −0.446 to −0.013].
4. Neuroimaging Results
4.1. Control Subjects
4.1.1. Congruent and Incongruent Sentences (Table 3)
Congruent trials were associated with increased activation relative to incongruent trials in the dorsal portion of the left inferior frontal gyrus, the orbital portion of the right inferior frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate bilaterally, the right caudate nucleus, the left posterior middle temporal gyrus, the left parahippocampal gyrus, the right precuneus, and the right inferior frontal gyrus. Conversely, incongruent trials were associated with more activation than congruent trials in the left middle frontal gyrus, the right medial frontal gyrus, and the left superior frontal gyrus.
4.1.2. Between Group Differences: Controls versus Non-FTD versus FTD (Table 3)
The contrast between congruent and incongruent sentences was associated with greater activation in the dorsal portion of the left inferior frontal cortex in controls than Non-FTD patients, which in turn had increased activation relative to FTD patients in the same region (Figure 2(a)). In this analysis, we did not find regions in which the patients groups showed more activation than controls.
4.1.3. Activation Associated with FTD
POST-HOC contrasts of FTD patients versus controls and FTD versus Non-FTD patients confirmed attenuated activation in the left inferior frontal cortex in the FTD group. Relative to controls, FTD patients showed less activation in the dorsal portion of the inferior frontal cortex bilaterally, the left middle temporal gyrus, precuneus, and lingual gyrus and in the cerebellum bilaterally (Figure 3(a)). No areas were relatively more activated in the FTD than the control group.
Relative to Non-FTD patients, FTD patients showed less activation in the left middle frontal gyrus and bilaterally in the anterior cingulate gyrus (Figure 3(b) and Table 3). Conversely, the FTD subgroup showed more activation than the Non-FTD subgroup in the right posterior cingulate gyrus and the left cerebellum (Figure 3(c)).
5. Discussion
5.1. Behavioral Results
Semantic congruency influenced RTs, with minimum values in trials with congruous endings, as expected (see Figure 5). Therefore, we observed an effect of inhibition (or interference) associated with processing of incongruent sentences in all groups. The patients made more errors than controls, particularly those with FTD. This was most evident on incongruent trials where patients with FTD performed much worse than both patients with no FTD and controls, showing deficiency in processes where inhibition is expected.
5.2. Neuroimaging Results
5.2.1. Semantic Processing in Healthy Controls
We found that incongruent sentences activated the left middle frontal cortex more than congruent ones, an area associated with suppression when the same type of semantic material was employed [39] and with inhibition of stereotyped responses [52]. However, in our study, we observed greater activity for congruent than incongruent trials in healthy controls in the right precuneus, contrary to our hypothesis. Therefore, our first hypothesis was partially confirmed. A possible explanation for these differences observed between ours and Allen et al.'s study may be the fact that their task required overt production of a final completing word for visually presented priming sentence stems, whilst ours only required making a decision upon a visually presented final word.
The general pattern of activation we found was expected because the semantic task used placed high demands on executive control, such as maintaining competing items on hold whilst checking for semantic appropriateness [53] and inhibiting unnecessary items. Thus, in healthy controls, we observed more activation for congruent than incongruent trials in the right inferior (orbital) and left inferior (dorsal) cortices, left posterior temporal cortex, bilateral anterior cingulate, and left precuneus. Our findings are in line with data supporting an important role of the left hemisphere in the final integration of semantic aspects present in sentences and texts [54] and recent meta-analytical studies showing activity in these regions associated with semantic processing [55–57].
5.2.2. Semantic Processing in FTD Schizophrenic Patients
We also hypothesized that FTD patients would show decreased activation in the prefrontal (middle and inferior), temporal, and cingulate cortices. Relative to controls, FTD patients showed a diminished activation in dorsal inferior frontal cortex (bilaterally) and in the left middle temporal gyrus when processing incongruent relative to congruent sentences. We predicted that these differences would also be evident when contrasting patients with and without FTD. We found that for the incongruent/congruent contrast, the FTD subgroup showed less activation than Non-FTD patients bilaterally in the left inferior/middle frontal gyri and the anterior cingulate gyri. Thus, our second hypothesis was confirmed. The differential brain activity observed in FTD patients while performing a task, without mistakes, seem to be arising from an early point of the semantic processing, since these attenuated activations were observed in areas in which controls showed increased activation for congruent trials relative to incongruent ones and not the other way round (contrary to our initial prediction). Although this may seem a contradictory finding, the current study confirms altered brain activity in these areas specifically associated with FTD in schizophrenia [28–32].
Manipulation of incongruent sentences requires overcoming a prepotent response, that is, suppression of automatically activated word(s) after a primed sentence stem [36]. The latter process needs to be maintained “active” for a longer period. Activity in the DLPFC has been associated with working memory and contextual information in experiments requiring heavier semantic processing demands [31, 58]. These group differences in inferior frontal and left middle temporal responses reflected less activation of these areas in patients with FTD relative to both controls and patients without FTD. This differential pattern of response is similar to that evident in their behaviour, where patients with FTD performed markedly worse than both other groups on trials involving semantically incongruent sentences, making more mistakes and also having a shorter RT than Non-FTD patients. Relative to Non-FTD, FTD also had less activation in the anterior cingulate cortices, which have been suggested by several authors to participate in demanding processes, such as monitoring conflict of response [59], engagement of cognitive control [60], top-down inhibitory modulation [61], detection of mnemonic competition, and retrieval induced forgetting [62].
A final point should be made about the activity in the precuneus. This region has been demonstrated to be involved in semantic processing [39, 57, 63, 64] episodic memory retrieval [65], retrieval-induced facilitation [66], and dopamine regulation of working memory [67]. FTD patients, but not Non-FTD patients, had less activation in the left precuneus than controls. Despite some subtle inconsistencies in hemispheric localization (not rare in fMRI studies of schizophrenia), this finding suggests a problem in episodic memory retrieval that might have contributed to the FTD group worse task performance. Thus, FTD patients showed robust diminished activation in areas implicated in the suppression of irrelevant material as well as detection and resolution of mnemonic conflict [68], selecting context-appropriate meanings in the presence of competing meanings [69], and differences between impaired and facilitated information [66]. Interestingly, Arcuri [46] found that FTD patients produced significantly more expected words than controls, when overtly producing anomalous endings for sentences of a high degree of constraint, in a modified version of the Hayling Task [70]. That study included the scanned FTD patients (n = 9) from the present study but had a larger sample of FTD patients (n = 21). Thus, FTD patients who showed diminished activation in areas implicated in aspects of executive control, when deciding whether a final target word is semantically appropriate for a previously presented sentence, also had difficulties in inhibiting automatically activated words that followed sentences of high semantic constraint, when they were asked to openly generate semantically incongruent completions for those sentences.
The differences between FTD and the Non-FTD patients are not attributable to sociodemographic or other clinical differences as they are matched in all respects other than the severity of positive FTD. Moreover, although the FTD patients performed the task more poorly than Non-FTD patients and controls, the image analysis was restricted to correct trials, indicating that the differences in activation were not simply secondary to the more impaired task performance in this subgroup. The differences in regional responses may thus be linked to the pathophysiology of incoherent speech. The prominent role of the inferior frontal cortex in language processing and particularly semantic processing is consistent with this suggestion. However, the fact that a qualitatively similar but less severe reduction in activation was evident in patients with no FTD indicates that findings in this area are also associated with the disorder of schizophrenia, independent of the presence of this particular symptom.
Liddle et al. [27] proposed that the disorganisation syndrome (disorders of the form of thought and innappropriate affect) is associated with a differential pattern of brain activity in frontal, temporal, and cingulate cortices relative to the two other syndromes (psychomotor poverty and reality distortion). In our sample, although we focused specifically on the symptom of FTD, it was significantly correlated with inappropriate affect, that is, patients with FTD had higher scores in innappropriate effect as well, pointing to the same direction of a distinct syndrome of disorganisation within schizophrenia.
5.3. Limitations
The sample was small, and larger studies should be done to confirm our results and possibly explain the limitations in the observed data, which might have contributed to a possible lack of power in our analyses.
6. Conclusion
The frontotemporal network normally engaged by a semantic decision task was underactivated in schizophrenia, particularly in patients with FTD. This network is implicated in the inhibition of automatically primed stimuli and impairment of its function interferes with language processing and contributes to the production of incoherent speech.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful for all the participants for their cooperation, particularly the patients. All authors contributed critically to paper writing. S. M. Arcuri designed and performed the experiment, recruited, interviewed, and tested participants (healthy controls and patients), analysed the data, and interpreted the results. M. R. Broome recruited patients and contributed to experiment performance. V.Giampietro and M. Brammer designed the fmri analysis software and contributed to fmri data analysis. E. Amaro Jr. contributed to experimental design and to event-related fmri expertise for data collection and analysis. T. T. J. Kircher contributed to recruitment and experiment performance in healthy control participants. S. C. R. Williams and C. M. Andrew provided input for the experimental design and fmri procedure. R. G. Morris contributed to the experimental design and offline data analysis and interpretation of results. P. K. McGuire provided critical input for testing a psychiatric population, data analysis, and interpretation. The authors thank Dr. Tamara Russell for helpful comments. This work is part of Dr Arcuri's Ph.D. scholarship CAPES (BEX 20–74/95-1) Brasilia, Brazil.
References
- 1.Peralta V, Cuesta MJ. Eugen bleuler and the schizophrenias: 100 Years after. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37(6):1118–1120. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Heckers S. Bleuler and the neurobiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37(6):1131–1135. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hardy-Baylé M-C, Sarfati Y, Passerieux C. The cognitive basis of disorganization symptomatology in Schizophrenia and its clinical correlates: toward a pathogenetic approach to disorganization. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2003;29(3):459–471. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Moskowitz A, Heim G. Eugen Bleuler's Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias (1911): a centenary appreciation and reconsideration. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37(3):471–479. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Spitzer M. Associative networks, formal thought disorder and schizophrenia. On the experimental psychopathology of speech-dependent thought processes. Nevernartz. 1993;64(3):147–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Minzenberg MJ, Ober BA, Vinogradov S. Semantic priming in schizophrenia: a review and synthesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(5):699–720. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702801357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ragland JD, Gur RC, Raz J, et al. Effect of schizophrenia on frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition: a PET cerebral blood flow study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(7):1114–1125. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Spitzer M, Braun U, Hermle L, Maier S. Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: direct evidence from indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry. 1993;34(12):864–877. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90054-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Spitzer M, Weisker I, Winter M, Maier S, Hermle L, Maher BA. Semantic and phonological priming in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103(3):485–494. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.3.485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Spitzer M. A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1997;23(1):29–50. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kiefer M, Spitzer M. Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation. NeuroReport. 2000;11(11):2401–2407. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kiefer M, Martens U, Weisbrod M, Hermle L, Spitzer M. Increased unconscious semantic activation in schizophrenia patients with formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research. 2009;114(1–3):79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Goldberg TE, Aloia MS, Gourovitch ML, Missar D, Pickar D, Weinberger DR. Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, I: the semantic system. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155(12):1671–1676. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.12.1671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kerns JG, Berenbaum H. Cognitive impairments associated with formal thought disorder in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(2):211–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.McGlashan TH. Eugen bleuler: centennial anniversary of his 1911 publication of dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37(6):1101–1103. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McCarthy G, Nobre AC, Bentin S, Spencer DD. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: I. Intracranial distribution and neural generators. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15(2):1080–1089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01080.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nobre AC, McCarthy G. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15(2):1090–1098. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01090.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Holcomb PJ. Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology. 1993;30(1):47–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03204.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mitchell PF, Andrews S, Fox AM, Catts SV, Ward PB, McConaghy N. Active and passive attention in schizophrenia: an ERP study of information processing in a linguistic task. Biological Psychology. 1991;32(2-3):101–124. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(91)90004-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Niznikiewicz MA, O’Donnell BF, Nestor PG, et al. ERP assessment of visual and auditory language processing in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106(1):85–94. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Andrews S, Shelley AM, Ward PB, Fox A, Catts SV, McConaghy N. Event-related potential indices of semantic processing in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 1993;34(7):443–458. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90235-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Adams J, Faux SF, Nestor PG, et al. ERP abnormalities during semantic processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 1993;10(3):247–257. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(93)90059-r. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature. 1984;307(5947):161–163. doi: 10.1038/307161a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Salisbury D. N400 to lexical ambiguity and semantic incongruity in schizophrenia. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2010;75(2):127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kostova M, Passerieux C, Laurent JP, Hardy-Baylé MC. N400 anomalies in schizophrenia are correlated with the severity of formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;78(2-3):285–291. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Laurent JP, Kostova M, Passerieux C. N400 and P300 modulation as functions of processing level in schizophrenia patients exhibiting formal thought disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2010;75(2):177–182. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Hirsch SR, Jones T, Frackowiak RSJ. Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;160:179–186. doi: 10.1192/bjp.160.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.McGuire PK, Quested DJ, Spence SA, Murray RM, Frith CD, Liddle PF. Pathophysiology of “positive” thought disorder in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;173:231–235. doi: 10.1192/bjp.173.3.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kircher TTJ, Bulimore ET, Brammer MJ, et al. Differential activation of temporal cortex during sentence completion in schizophrenic patients with and without formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research. 2001;50(1-2):27–40. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00042-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kircher TTJ, Liddle PF, Brammer MJ, Williams SCR, Murray RM, McGuire PK. Neural correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: preliminary findings from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58(8):769–774. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.MacDonald AW, III, Carter CS, Kerns JG, et al. Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficits to schizophrenia in never-medicated patients with first-episode psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(3):475–484. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Park IH, Park HJ, Chun JW, Kim EY, Kim JJ. Prefrontal functional dissociation in the semantic network of patients with schizophrenia. NeuroReport. 2008;19(14):1391–1395. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32830cebff. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nakamura M, Nestor PG, Levitt JJ, et al. Orbitofrontal volume deficit in schizophrenia and thought disorder. Brain. 2008;131(1):180–195. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sun J, Maller JJ, Guo L, Fitzgerald PB. Superior temporal gyrus volume change in schizophrenia: a review on Region of Interest volumetric studies. Brain Research Reviews. 2009;61(1):14–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Horn H, Federspiel A, Wirth M, et al. Structural and metabolic changes in language areas linked to formal thought disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;194(2):130–138. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.045633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) Iowa City, Iowa, USA: The University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Van Petten C. Words and sentences: event-related brain potential measures. Psychophysiology. 1995;32(6):511–525. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01228.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nathaniel-James DA, Fletcher P, Frith CD. The functional anatomy of verbal initiation and suppression using the Hayling Test. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(4):559–566. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Allen P, Mechelli A, Stephan KE, et al. Fronto-temporal interactions during overt verbal initiation and suppression. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20(9):1656–1669. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Annett M. A classification of hand preference by association analysis. The British journal of psychology. 1970;61(3):303–321. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01248.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nelson HE, Willison JR. National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. 2nd edition. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) Iowa City, Iowa, USA: The University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arcuri SM, Rabe-Hesketh S, Morris RG, McGuire PK. Regional variation of cloze probabilities for sentence contexts. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 2001;33(1):80–90. doi: 10.3758/bf03195350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Norris D, McQueen JM, Cutler A. Bias effects in facilitatory phonological priming. Memory and Cognition. 2002;30(3):399–411. doi: 10.3758/bf03194940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Arcuri SM. Neural and cognitive studies of thought disorder in schizophrenia. London, UK: King’s College, University of London; 2003. Ph.D. thesis. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bullmore E, Long C, Suckling J, et al. Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: resampling methods in time and wavelet domains. Human Brain Mapping. 2001;12(2):61–78. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200102)12:2<61::AID-HBM1004>3.0.CO;2-W. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RSJ, Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1996;35(3):346–355. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bullmore E, Brammer M, Williams SCR, et al. Statistical methods of estimation and inference for functional MR image analysis. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1996;35(2):261–277. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, NY, USA: Thieme; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Rabe-Hesketh S, et al. Methods for diagnosis and treatment of stimulus-correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fMRI. Human Brain Mapping. 1999;7(1):38–48. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:1<38::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kadota H, Sekiguchi H, Takeuchi S, Miyazaki M, Kohno Y, Nakajima Y. The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the inhibition of stereotyped responses. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;203(3):593–600. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2269-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Novick JM, Kan IP, Trueswell JC, Thompson-Schill SL. A case for conflict across multiple domains: memory and language impairments following damage to ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2009;26(6):527–567. doi: 10.1080/02643290903519367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Homae F, Hashimoto R, Nakajima K, Miyashita Y, Sakai KL. From perception to sentence comprehension: the convergence of auditory and visual information of language in the left inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2002;16(4):883–900. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(12):2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, et al. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage. 2006;30(4):1414–1432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Graves WW, Desai R, Humphries C, Seidenberg MS, Binder JR. Neural systems for reading aloud: a multiparametric approach. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20(8):1799–1815. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Experimental Brain Research. 2000;133(1):3–11. doi: 10.1007/s002210000395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Barch DM, Braver TS, Sabb FW, Noll DC. Anterior cingulate and the monitoring of response conflict: evidence from an fMRI study of overt verb generation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12(2):298–309. doi: 10.1162/089892900562110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303(5660):1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ghatan PH, Hsieh JC, Petersson KM, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. Coexistence of attention-based facilitation and inhibition in the human cortex. NeuroImage. 1998;7(1):23–29. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kuhl BA, Dudukovic NM, Kahn I, Wagner AD. Decreased demands on cognitive control reveal the neural processing benefits of forgetting. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10(7):908–914. doi: 10.1038/nn1918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kircher TTJ, Brammer M, Andreu NT, Williams SCR, McGuire PK. Engagement of right temporal cortex during processing of linguistic context. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(8):798–809. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.de Zubicaray GI, Zelaya FO, Andrew C, Williams SCR, Bullmore ET. Cerebral regions associated with verbal response initiation, suppression and strategy use. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(9):1292–1304. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00026-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129(3):564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wimber M, Bäuml KH, Bergström Z, Markopoulos G, Heinze HJ, Richardson-Klavehn A. Neural markers of inhibition in human memory retrieval. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28(50):13419–13427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1916-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fusar-Poli P, Howes OD, Allen P, et al. Abnormal frontostriatal interactions in people with prodromal signs of psychosis: a multimodal imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):683–691. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kuhl BA, Kahn I, Dudukovic NM, Wagner AD. Overcoming suppression in order to remember: contributions from anterior cingulate and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2008;8(2):211–221. doi: 10.3758/cabn.8.2.211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Metzler C. Effects of left frontal lesions on the selection of context-appropriate meanings. Neuropsychology. 2001;15(3):315–328. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.15.3.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Burgess PW, Shallice T. Response suppression, Initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34(4):263–272. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]