Skip to main content
Autism Research and Treatment logoLink to Autism Research and Treatment
. 2012 Jul 5;2012:146132. doi: 10.1155/2012/146132

The Development of Executive Function in Autism

Elizabeth Pellicano 1,2,*
PMCID: PMC3420556  PMID: 22934168

Abstract

Autism is a common and often highly debilitating neurodevelopmental condition, whose core behavioral features are believed to be rooted in disrupted neurocognitive processes, including especially “executive function.” Researchers have predominantly focused upon understanding the putative causal relationship between difficulties in EF and autistic symptomatology. This paper suggests, however, that the effects of individual differences in EF should be more far-reaching, playing a significant part in the real-life outcomes of individuals with autism, including their social competence, everyday adaptive behavior, and academic achievement. It further considers the nature of the EF-outcome relationship, including the possible determinants of individual differences in EF, and makes several recommendations for future research.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum conditions (hereafter, “autism”) are a set of common, lifelong neurodevelopmental conditions that involve substantial heterogeneity at numerous levels, including etiology, neurobiology, cognition, and especially behavior. Long-term follow-up studies show that the developmental outcomes of autistic individuals are highly variable, even for individuals at the more intellectually able end of the autism spectrum. While some individuals go on to live independently and obtain qualifications, the majority fail to achieve independence, to attain full-time employment, or to enjoy friendships [15] (though see [6]). Explaining this variability is of critical import: to discover why developments take place in some areas and not in others, and especially in some individuals and not in others.

Researchers have little understanding of the factors underpinning this heterogeneity, due in part to a dearth of longitudinal studies tracing the development of autism and to the preponderance of studies using a case-control design, focusing on group rather than on individual differences. This paper aims to address this limitation. It identifies one potential source of this variability, namely, autistic children's emerging “executive function” (EF), those higher-order processes, closely associated with the prefrontal cortex, which are necessary for regulating and controlling behavior (see Table 1). Specifically, it suggests that individual differences in the growth trajectories of autistic children's EF skills could explain some of the variability in children's functional outcomes, including their social awareness, real-life adaptive behavior, and readiness to learn in school, both in the shorter term and in the longer term.

Table 1.

Definition of key constructs and examples of tasks used to test these skills in preschool and primary school-age children.

Construct Component Definition Description of an example task
Working memory The ability to hold information “on-line” and manipulate it. Working memory is differentiated according to whether the information is verbal or spatial in nature. Verbal working memory can be tested using the Digit Span task from the Wechsler Intelligence Scales [44] in which participants are asked to repeat number sets, which progressively increase in complexity, back to the experimenter either in the same (forwards) or reverse (backwards) order. Spatial working memory can be tested using a Spatial Span or Corsi Blocks task [45] in which the child is shown a block board containing 9 cubes in fixed positions. The experimenter taps the wooden blocks (1–9) in a sequence and child is asked to repeat the sequence in the same (forwards) or reverse order (backwards). Backward span for both tasks requires manipulating the to-be-remembered elements and thus targets working memory.
Inhibitory control The capacity to hold a rule in mind, responding according to this rule, and resist a prepotent response. In Luria's hand-game [46, 47], a test of motor inhibition, the child first imitates the experimenter's hand movements (make a fist or point a finger). Next, the child must execute the opposite action (i.e., when the experimenter makes a fist, the child points finger, and vice versa). Success on this task demands that the child both hold in mind an arbitrary rule and inhibit the prepotent tendency to copy the experimenter's gesture.
Executive function Set-shifting The ability to shift flexibly one's attentional focus. Card-sorting tasks require children to switch cognitive set in response to verbal feedback. The child is shown a set of cards and must sort the cards according to one dimension (e.g., shape). The rule then changes and the child must shift to sort according to new dimension (e.g., colour). In some tasks (e.g., in the Dimensional Change Card Sort task [48]), children are explicitly told the rule change, while in others (e.g., in the teddy bear set-shifting task [26]), the rule change is implicit. Children who can switch flexibly between cognitive sets make fewer “post-switch” errors.
Planning The ability to formulate a plan (including selecting appropriate goals and sub-goals) and executive this plan effectively. In the Tower of London task [26, 49], children are shown a peg-board containing three vertical pegs of increasing size and three beads (red, white, black) arranged in a particular configuration (“start state”). Children are then given a picture showing the beads in a different configuration (“goal state”) and instructed to move the beads from the start state to the goal state within the minimum number of moves possible while observing various rules. Success on this task requires children to plan ahead and to generate and maintain a series of moves.

Theory of mind The ability to infer the mental states (e.g., beliefs, desires) of others in order to make predictions about their behavior. In the now-classic false-belief paradigm [50, 51], children watch one character (Sally) place an object (e.g., a ball) in one location (a basket) and leave the room. While the main character is absent, another character (Ann) surreptitiously moves the object from one location to another. Children are then asked to predict the main character's behavior (“Where will Sally look for her marble?”). Successful performance on such a task involves children predicting an action based on an attributed false belief.
Social competence Joint attention Requires the triadic coordination or sharing of attention with another person around an object or event. Semi-structured observational schedules like the Early Social Communication Scales [52] are designed to elicit joint attention behaviours, including children's ability to (a) respond to the experimenter's eye gaze or gestures (e.g., pointing) to share a common point of reference and (b) initiate joint attention using eye contact and gestures to direct the experimenter's attention to the point of interest.

Adaptive behavior Those skills necessary for individuals to live independently and to function well in real-life personal and social settings. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior ScalesSecond Edition (Vineland-II) [53] is one standardized parent-report measure designed to assess a variety of typical developmental milestones with respect to social and communicative competence and real-life daily living skills.

2. Executive Functions in Autism

EF has received extensive attention in the autism literature for many years largely due to the influential proposal that the inherent rigidity and invariance of autistic behaviors could be explained by a primary impairment in executive control [7, 8]. EF problems have been demonstrated consistently in school-age children, adolescents, and adults with autism [8, 9], as well as relatives of individuals with autism albeit to a lesser degree [10, 11] (see [12, 13] for reviews). These problems typically manifest as perseverative responses (i.e., getting “stuck” performing the same action) and difficulties switching flexibly between response sets. Furthermore, problems in specific components of EF have been shown to distinguish autism from other developmental conditions, such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [14, 15] (though see [16]).

Despite the theory's apparent face validity, several negative findings have cast doubt over the possibility that a primary problem in EF can explain all autistic symptoms. Not all individuals show EF difficulties (e.g., [1719]), and investigations of EF in young children with autism have failed to find evidence of autism-specific difficulties [20, 21]. Consequently, researchers have shifted away from a framework that emphasizes a single, primary neurocognitive atypicality (such as EF) as the underlying cause of autism to one that encompasses multiple cognitive atypicalities [22, 23].

3. Why Focus on Autistic Children's Developing Executive Function?

While there is general consensus that EF problems are unlikely to play a primary causal role in autism, it remains possible that the degree of difficulties in EF could play a substantial role in autistic children's developmental outcomes—including their social competence (those skills, including “theory of mind,” which allow individuals to evaluate social situations and respond effectively; see Table 1), their adaptive behavior (those skills necessary for individuals to live independently and to function well in real-life settings), and their success in school. Indeed, clinicians and those who care for individuals with autism often associate some individuals' inability to achieve independence with persistent difficulties in regulating behavior and adapting flexibly to change (e.g., [2]). Therefore, whether poor EF plays a fundamental role in the emergence of core autistic features or, instead, is a consequence of early atypical input from another cognitive system, it is nevertheless likely to place the child with autism at risk for a poor developmental outcome either directly or indirectly.

Substantial recent progress in understanding the typical development of EF provides further reason to investigate individual differences in EF as a possible source of the heterogeneity in outcomes in autism. First, it is clear that EF is intimately tied to another aspect of neurocognitive development known to develop atypically in autism: theory of mind (ToM: mental state awareness). Numerous studies report robust correlations between individual differences in tasks tapping ToM and EF, independent of the effects of age and IQ, in typical preschoolers [2427] and toddlers [28, 29]. Several theorists have further proposed a direct, functional link between EF and ToM. They assert that the abilities to monitor one's actions and to act with volition are critical for reflecting on the mental states of self and other, that is, that ToM emerges from EF [30, 31]. Research findings have repeatedly shown that early EF skills are predictive of later ToM but early ToM skills are not predictive of later EF [2729], providing overwhelming support for the view that EF is critical to developmental changes in ToM.

Second, early EF is also predictive of typical preschoolers' later school readiness [32, 33] and academic success in reading and mathematics [3436]. The transition to school relies on mastery of basic EF skills, including remembering and following instructions, completing tasks independently and smoothly transitioning between tasks, and inhibiting inappropriate behaviors. EF, therefore, is held to play an important role in the acquisition of knowledge; the better children are at focusing and refocusing their attention, holding information in mind and manipulating it and resisting distraction, the better placed children should be to acquire knowledge and skills in the classroom.

Third, it is well known that the prefrontal cortex, which mediates EF, shows a protracted development trajectory: it begins to develop very early in life, has a boost during the preschool period, and continues to develop well into adolescence [37]. The extended postnatal developmental of prefrontal cortical networks means that they should be particularly sensitive to exogenous influences. Several studies now report direct evidence of the malleability of EF: “exercising” young children's EF skills results in substantial improvements in their ability to regulate their behavior in both at-risk [38] and typical [39] populations.

Taken together, these findings provide compelling grounds for suggesting that one source of the heterogeneity in autistic individual's functional outcomes are individual differences in emerging EF. Some researchers have begun to speak to this possibility, with several studies reporting links between EF and a range of concurrent behaviors, including social competence (e.g., [18, 40]) and everyday adaptive behavior (e.g., [41, 42]). Yet full consideration of this developmental issue has not yet been possible due to a paucity of studies analyzing individual differences within a longitudinal design. The handful of studies using such an approach have done so, though, with dramatic success. Griffith et al. [20] found links between early executive skills (performance on a spatial reversal task tapping cognitive flexibility) and sociocommunicative behavior (bids for joint attention) one year later in young children with autism, but not in nonautistic children with developmental delay. Also, individual differences in set-shifting ability predicted the social competence scores of cognitively able adults with autism 3 years later in one study [43] and, in another, predicted real-life adaptive skills between 11 and 27 years later [5].

A recent study has further demonstrated the influence of autistic children's early EF skills on their sociocognitive awareness. Pellicano [18] investigated the EF and ToM skills of 37 cognitively able children with autism and 31 typical children (M age = 5 yrs 6 mths). As expected, children with autism showed difficulties in both domains compared to typical children. There were also significant EF-ToM correlations, suggestive of a functional link between domains. Analysis of patterns of “atypicalities,” however, revealed striking dissociations in one direction only: poor ToM coupled with intact EF [18]. In line with Russell [31], these findings suggested that EF skills might be one important ingredient for the development of ToM but that ToM does not play this role for the development of EF.

Three-year follow-up data on the same samples of children further supported this claim [54]. For children with autism, individual differences in early EF skills (Time 1) were longitudinally predictive of developmental change in ToM skills (Time 2), independent of age, language, nonverbal intelligence, and early ToM skills. Yet there were no predictive relations in the opposite direction—a finding that is entirely consistent with longitudinal studies of the EF-ToM relation in typical children [2729]. These results provide initial evidence that autistic children's cognitive skills emerge within a dynamic developing system where EF skills play a critical role in shaping the developmental trajectories of ToM.

In sum, accumulating evidence of the important contribution of EF in typical development, together with promising findings from studies with individuals with autism, provides good reason to suspect that individual differences in the development of EF might critically influence autistic children's developmental trajectories and could account, at least in part, for the heterogeneity in their sociocognitive, behavioural and academic success.

4. How Is EF Organized in Autism and Which Factors Drive Its Growth?

Determining the precise nature of the developmental course of EF in autism and also of the potential causal links between EF and other social and learning outcomes demands greater understanding of the nature of EF itself and of the mechanisms underpinning its growth. Although there is no question that EF plays a vital role in well-regulated, organized behavior [55], there has been much disagreement regarding the characterization of the EF construct. Like adult models [56, 57], competing developmental accounts differ with respect to which they emphasize the unitary [58, 59] or fractionated [60, 61] nature of EF. Empirical work with adults, using sophisticated statistical techniques like confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), has reported evidence of three latent EF variables or component processes—set-shifting, updating (working memory), and inhibitory control—which are partially independent but still intercorrelated [62].

Studies using similar methods with school-age children in part support this integrated framework [63, 64], although recent studies with 2- to 6-year-old children have instead reported evidence in line with a unitary model of EF [65, 66]. These latter results question the apparent continuity in the structure of EF during development but are consistent with a dynamic, neuroconstructivist approach in which cognitive functions begin relatively undifferentiated and become progressively modularised or specialised over time [67] (see Figure 1). This framework suggests that individual differences in development itself might be key to explaining the wide variation in findings both within [68] and across [12, 16] studies on EF in autism.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A simplified model showing a unitary executive function (EF) construct early in development and an emergent fractionated construct of EF with development with latent EF variables of working memory (WM), inhibitory control (IC), and set-shifting (SS).

No study has yet explicitly investigated the nature of EF in autism. Early fractionation of EF makes it plausible for distinct EF components, such as cognitive flexibility, to be specifically affected in autism. Yet if, as the evidence suggests, EF is a single, unitary construct during early childhood (at least in typical children), then it becomes more difficult to see how a distinct profile of EF difficulties might emerge in autism. Given the prolonged development of EF and the degree of neural plasticity during childhood [69, 70], it is likely that emerging prefrontal cortical networks affect, and are affected by, the development of other key cognitive functions. In this case, then, disruption to distinct EF components in autism might be driven by other factors.

One goal therefore should be to understand precisely which mechanism(s) drives the development of EF in typical children and in children with autism. Some theorists propose that progress in EF occurs via the development of the prefrontal cortex [60] and the strengthening of prefrontal representations [58] in an experience-dependent manner [71]. An influential yet contrasting view suggests that development in children's attentional control—the ability to focus on a task and ignore irrelevant information—is the source of common variance in EF [61, 72]. Posner et al. have demonstrated significant advances during the preschool period in the central “attention network,” which includes alerting, orienting, and executive attention processes [73, 74]. Developmental changes in attention are considered to provide children with greater executive control over action. On this view, then, rate of growth in EF should be predicted by developments in attentional capacities (Figure 2(a)) and such developments might even mediate the fractionation of EF.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Three simplified models showing executive function (EF) development. Model (a) includes a latent construct of attention that influences baseline levels (EF intercept) and rate of growth of EF (EF slope). Model (b) shows a latent construct of language mediating baseline EF and its developmental trajectory. Model (c) includes latent constructs of attention and language, which both influence the development of EF. These models could be tested using latent growth curve modelling.

Impairments in core attentional processes have been reported in autism, including problems with disengagement or so-called “sticky” attention [7577]. Fundamental problems in critical aspects of attention could therefore place limits on the rate of EF development, which could in turn hinder the emergence of autistic children's social and learning outcomes. It is of course possible that the causal relationship might exist in the opposite direction, such that early developments in EF might influence the emergence of attentional networks. The relationship between aspects of attention and components of executive control has, however, been hitherto unaddressed in autism.

Yet Posner and Rothbart's [72] model (see also [61]) neglects the potentially mediating role of another key function: language. Children's verbal skills can affect the expression of EF, for example, by limiting their ability to store phonological information in working memory [78]. Yet language might play a more fundamental role, affecting the very development of EF. For several theorists [31, 7981], language provides an internal plan for behavior. Vygotsky [81] emphasized the importance of self-directed speech during early childhood, which becomes increasingly internalized during the preschool years, allowing children (verbally) to “think through” problems and to guide future-oriented behavior. Similarly, Zelazo et al. [59] stress that language is the medium through which higher-order (if-if-then) rules are formulated and is key to recursive thought. Developmental gains in young children's language skills (specifically their ability to formulate hierarchical rules) are therefore directly implicated in the rate of EF development (Figure 2(b)).

Difficulties with communication are a core characteristic of autism [82] and has been previously implicated as a potential limiting factor on the development of EF (e.g., [83]). Furthermore, children with autism are less likely to use verbal rehearsal strategies on executive tasks [8486] suggesting that they, unlike typical children, may not be using internal language in the service of executive control. Pellicano's [54] longitudinal work showed that autistic children's initial receptive-vocabulary skills were not predictive of EF performance three years later, suggesting further that verbal skills may not influence the emergence of EF in autism as they do in typical development [26, 28]. Individual differences in the growth trajectories of autistic children's verbal skills therefore might partially mediate (or fail to mediate at all) the rate of progress of EF in autism. Further still, it is possible that attention and language could mediate the development of EF. In this case, both functions could make independent contributions to the rate of growth of EF (Figure 2(c)) and both could be limiting factors in autism. Importantly, evidence for any one of these patterns (Figures 2(a)2(c)) would suggest that the potential influence of EF on children's functional outcomes is indirect rather than direct.

All of these models suggest that the development of EF itself might be shaped by certain endogenous factors, which in turn could mediate children's developmental outcomes. Yet the developmental trajectory of EF, and its resulting neurocognitive architecture, will be an emergent property of interactions within the children and between the children and their environment. Alternative explanations therefore place the development of EF squarely in the social realm. For example, Luria emphasized that “we must go beyond the limits of the individual organism and examine how volitional processes are formed for the child in his/her concrete contacts with adults” [87, page 89] (see also [88]). Hughes [27] (see also [80]) extended this view to suggest that the effect of EF upon the development of ToM should be indirectly influenced by the child's social environment. Since negotiating social interactions requires children to regulate their own behaviors (e.g., turn-taking, following rules in games), peer relations are likely to have positive effects on children's developing executive skills, which in turn will foster their developing ToM.

There has been renewed interest in the sociocultural predictors of EF development, which so far include socioeconomic status [89], parent scaffolding [90, 91], and parent-child interactions [92]. These exogenous factors are also likely to influence the development of EF in children with autism. Contrary to popular opinion, children with autism do not grow up in a social vacuum. Rather, they can show attachment security with caregivers [93], can engage in positive and collaborative interactions with siblings (e.g., [94]), and actively seek out their nonautistic peers [4]. It is therefore plausible that social contact could influence autistic children's developing EF, which in turn might exert its effects on key real-life outcomes.

5. Future Perspectives

There is much awareness of the huge variability in autistic children's developmental outcomes but there is very little empirical knowledge of the underlying causes of this variability. Early individual differences in EF represent one candidate source of this heterogeneity. To address this issue, however, we need a richer understanding of the causal determinants of EF growth in autism, which will require prospective longitudinal studies and carefully designed training studies. Well-designed cognitive training studies will need to disentangle whether any gains are genuinely “raising the ceiling” of performance or just “speeding up development” to reach the child's given ability. Such knowledge will lead ultimately to a more nuanced theoretical, and distinctly developmental, perspective of EF in autism. Elucidating whether EF has direct or indirect longitudinal effects on children's functional outcomes is vital for knowing whether to directly “exercise” autistic children's EF skills or to concentrate instead on bootstrapping those factors (attention/language/social environment) through which EF influences children's outcomes.

References

  • 1.Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45(2):212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M. Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2000;41(5):561–578. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kanner L. Childhood Psychosis: Initial Studies and New Insights. New York, NY, USA: Winston/Wiley; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sigman M, Ruskin E. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial no. 256. Vol. 4. Chicago, Ill, USA: University of Chicago Press; 1999. Social competence in children with autism, Down syndrome and other developmental delays: a longitudinal study. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Szatmari P, Bartolucci G, Bremner R, Bond S, Rich S. A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1989;19(2):213–225. doi: 10.1007/BF02211842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Farley MA, McMahon WM, Fombonne E, et al. Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Research. 2009;2(2):109–118. doi: 10.1002/aur.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hughes C, Russell J. Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29(3):498–510. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ozonoff S, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1991;32(7):1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hughes C, Russell J, Robbins TW. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32(4):477–492. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90092-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hughes C, Leboyer M, Bouvard M. Executive function in parents of children with autism. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27(1):209–220. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796004308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hughes C, Plumet MH, Leboyer M. Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1999;40(5):705–718. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review. 2004;24(2):189–233. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Russo N, Flanagan T, Iarocci G, Berringer D, Zelazo PD, Burack JA. Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition. 2007;65(1):77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Geurts HM, Verté S, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA. How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45(4):836–854. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00276.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ozonoff S, Jensen J. Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1999;29(2):171–177. doi: 10.1023/a:1023052913110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Geurts HM, Corbett B, Solomon M. The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(2):74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liss M, Fein D, Allen D, et al. Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2001;42(2):261–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pellicano E. Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(4):974–990. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pellicano E. The development of core cognitive skills in Autism: a 3-year prospective study. Child Development. 2010;81(5):1400–1416. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Griffith EM, Pennington BF, Wehner EA, Rogers SJ. Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development. 1999;70(4):817–832. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yerys BE, Hepburn SL, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function in preschoolers with autism: evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(6):1068–1079. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0250-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dawson G, Munson J, Estes A, et al. Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Development. 2002;73(2):345–358. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9(10):1218–1220. doi: 10.1038/nn1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development. 2001;72(4):1032–1053. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Frye D, Zelazo PD, Palfai T. Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development. 1995;10(4):483–527. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hughes C. Executive function in preschoolers: links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1998;16(2):233–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hughes C. Finding your marbles: does preschoolers’ strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? Developmental Psychology. 1998;34(6):1326–1339. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.6.1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Carlson SM, Mandell DJ, Williams L. Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(6):1105–1122. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hughes C, Ensor R. Executive function and theory of mind: predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(6):1447–1459. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Moses LJ. Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Development. 2001;72(3):688–690. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Russell J. How executive disorders can bring about an adequate theory of mind. In: Russell J, editor. Autism as an Executive Disorder. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1997. pp. 256–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Blair C. School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002;57(2):111–127. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.57.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary and maths skills. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(4):947–959. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007;78(2):647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bull R, Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001;19(3):273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2006;59(4):745–759. doi: 10.1080/17470210500162854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Diamond A. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In: Stuss D, Knight R, editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 466–503. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. The early years: preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318(5855):1387–1388. doi: 10.1126/science.1151148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science. 2009;12(4):F9–F15. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Osterling J, Rinaldi J. Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Development. 1998;69(5):1276–1285. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Black DO, Wagner AE, Sirian L. Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8(4):241–248. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.241.13504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Happé F, Booth R, Charlton R, Hughes C. Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition. 2006;61(1):25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Berger HJC, Aerts FHTM, Van Spaendonck KPM, Cools AR, Teunisse JP. Central coherence and cognitive shifting in relation to social improvement in high-functioning young adults with autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(4):502–511. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.4.502.13870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wechsler D. Wechsler Scales of Intelligence. 4th edition. San Antonio, Tex, USA: The Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Milner B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin. 1971;27(3):272–277. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hughes C. Control of action and thought: normal development and dysfunction in autism: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1996;37(2):229–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Luria AR, Pribram KH, Homskaya ED. An experimental analysis of the behavioral disturbance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal endothelioma (meningioma) Neuropsychologia. 1964;2(4):257–280. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zelazo PD. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols. 2006;1(1):297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 1982;298(1089):199–209. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition. 1983;13(1):103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mundy P, Hogan A, Doehring P. A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social-Communication Scales. Coral Gables, Fla, USA: University of Miami; 1996. http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/ourteam/faculty_staff/escs.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sparrow S, Cicchetti D, Balla D. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. 2nd edition. Minneapolis, Minn, USA: Pearson Assessment; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Pellicano E. Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(2):530–544. doi: 10.1037/a0018287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Duncan J. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2(11):820–829. doi: 10.1038/35097575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences. 2000;23(10):475–483. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain. 1991;114(2):727–741. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.2.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Munakata Y. Graded representations in behavioral dissociations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(7):309–315. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01682-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2003;68(3):1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976x.2003.00260.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Diamond A. The early development of executive functions. In: Bialystock E, Craik FIM, editors. Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 70–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(1):31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW. Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(11):2017–2036. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Lehto JE, Juujärvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2003;21(1):59–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Wiebe SA, Espy KA, Charak D. Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: i. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(2):575–587. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wiebe SA, Sheffield T, Nelson JM, Clark CAC, Chevalier N, Espy KA. The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108(3):436–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Karmiloff-Smith A. Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(1):56–63. doi: 10.1037/a0014506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Towgood KJ, Meuwese JDI, Gilbert SJ, Turner MS, Burgess PW. Advantages of the multiple case series approach to the study of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2981–2988. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Huttenlocher PR. Neural Plasticity: The Effects of the Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, Mass, USA: Harvard University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Nelson CA, Thomas KM, de Haan M. Neuroscience and Cognitive Development: The Role of Experience and the Developing Brain. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS. Experience and brain development. Child Development. 1987;58(3):539–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12(3):427–441. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(8):1029–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Burack JA. Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103(3):535–543. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.3.535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Landry R, Bryson SE. Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with austism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45(6):1115–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00304.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mann TA, Walker P. Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2003;44(2):274–284. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Baddeley A. Working Memory. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Luria AR. The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior. London, UK: Pergamon Press; 1961. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Luria AR. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. New York, NY, USA: Basic Books; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Vygotsky L. Thought and Language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.) Cambridge, Mass, USA: MIT Press; 1962. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Russell J. Agency: Its Role in Mental Development. Hove, UK: Erlbaum; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Joseph RM, Steele SD, Meyer E, Tager-Flusberg H. Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory? Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(10):1400–1411. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Wallace GL, Silvers JA, Martin A, Kenworthy LE. Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in Autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39(12):1735–1739. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0802-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Whitehouse AJO, Maybery MT, Durkin K. Inner speech impairments in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006;47(8):857–865. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01624.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Luria AR. Language and Cognition. New York, NY, USA: John Wiley & Sons; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Vygotsky L. Mind and Society: The Development of Higher Mental Processes. Cambridge, Mass, USA: Harvard University Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science. 2005;8(1):74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Bernier A, Carlson SM, Whipple N. From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development. 2010;81(1):326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Hughes CH, Ensor RA. How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. 2009;2009(123):35–50. doi: 10.1002/cd.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Rhoades BL, Greenberg MT, Lanza ST, Blair C. Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108(3):638–662. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, van Berckelaer-Onnes IA. Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45(6):1123–1134. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00305.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Knott F, Lewis C, Williams T. Sibling interaction of children with autism: development over 12 months. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(10):1987–1995. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0347-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Autism Research and Treatment are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES