Abstract
Attentional mechanisms are a crucial prerequisite to organize behavior. Most situations may be characterized by a ‘competition’ between salient, but irrelevant stimuli and less salient, relevant stimuli. In such situations top-down and bottom-up mechanisms interact with each other. In the present fMRI study, we examined how interindividual differences in resolving situations of perceptual conflict are reflected in brain networks mediating attentional selection. Doing so, we employed a change detection task in which subjects had to detect luminance changes in the presence and absence of competing distractors. The results show that good performers presented increased activation in the orbitofrontal cortex (BA 11), anterior cingulate (BA 25), inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) and visual areas V2 and V3 but decreased activation in BA 39. This suggests that areas mediating top-down attentional control are stronger activated in this group. Increased activity in visual areas reflects distinct neuronal enhancement relating to selective attentional mechanisms in order to solve the perceptual conflict. Opposed to good performers, brain areas activated by poor performers comprised the left inferior parietal lobule (BA 39) and fronto-parietal and visual regions were continuously deactivated, suggesting that poor performers perceive stronger conflict than good performers. Moreover, the suppression of neural activation in visual areas might indicate a strategy of poor performers to inhibit the processing of the irrelevant non-target feature. These results indicate that high sensitivity in perceptual areas and increased attentional control led to less conflict in stimulus processing and consequently to higher performance in competitive attentional selection.
Introduction
At any time we are only partially aware of the extensive and vivid visual impressions surrounding us. The underlying prerequisites to organize behavior are selective attentional mechanisms that help us to process a small portion of information. The information we pay attention to can be selected either by exogenous or endogenous factors [1]. For example, if we search for a particular object, attention will be focused on the target object and distracting objects in the surrounding will be ignored. Opposed to these top-down mechanisms of attention, very salient objects can also capture attention automatically (bottom-up) [2]–[4]. Previous research examined bottom-up and top-down attentional processes separately, despite several studies stressing that in most situations both processes operate simultaneously [3], [5]. This line of research suggests that bottom-up and top-down processes act in parallel to create a biased representation of the external visual world according to salience and behavioral relevance [6], [7]. More precisely, the detection of a stimulus ‘A’ is determined by (I) the relative saliency of a feature ‘B’ presented in parallel to ‘A’ (i.e., characteristics of the bottom-up channel) and (II) by intentional biases favoring stimulus ‘A’ that are simultaneously adverse for processing feature ‘B’ (i.e., characteristics of the top-down channel). Doing so, top-down biases ‘re-weight’ the relative saliency of stimuli and determine whether feature ‘A’ or ‘B’ is detected [e.g. 3], [5], [8]. Consequently, these feature-based attentional mechanisms can facilitate visual processing by enhancing perceptual representations of previously cued target features and by suppressing representations of competing non-target features [9], [10].
Evidence for this model of attentional selection has repeatedly been referred to in monkey single-cell studies as well as in functional imaging studies. Monkey studies revealed that the inferior temporal cortex (IT) is involved in selecting the objects to which we attend and is crucial for resolving competition [11]–[13]. Likewise, previous studies on humans showed that activity in the visual system is modulated by selective attention, ranging from the thalamic level (corpus geniculatum laterale; CGL) to striate (V1) and extrastriate (V2, V3, V4, MT/MST) visual areas [14]–[16]. Top-down influences that induce biasing effects in the above mentioned areas are mediated via a fronto-parietal network, including the frontal eye field (FEF), superior parietal lobule (SPL) as well as intraparietal sulcus [17]–[19]. Besides the identification of target projection sites of attentional processes, functional imaging studies indicated that top-down attentional processes are distinct neural processes where cortical responses are either enhanced or suppressed. This is comparable to the concept that task-relevant stimuli elicit an increase in cortical activation whereas a decrease in cortical activation is related to task-irrelevant stimuli [13], [20]–[21]. This concept has been corroborated for various areas of the visual cortex like areas V2, V4, the middle temporal (MT) and medial superior temporal (MST) areas, where sensory suppression takes place when stimuli compete with each other [13], [16], [22]–[25].
Within the framework of selective visual attention the role of stimulus saliency (bottom-up channel characteristics) in determining the outcome of attentional selection processes has been subject to several studies, showing that stimuli irrelevant for a given task may nevertheless capture attention when they are sufficiently salient [26]–[28]. To investigate the interaction of bottom-up and top-down attentional processes and the concomitant bias on visual selection we employed a change detection task, similar to the one used by Wascher and Beste [29]. The subject's task was to detect changes of luminance and to ignore changes of orientation during trials in which the stimulus dimensions could either change singularly or simultaneously and spatially separated or joint. If both luminance and orientation change simultaneously but spatially separated, a perceptual conflict is induced in which subjects have to enhance the processing of the less salient but task-relevant luminance change against the competing and more salient orientation change. In a recent study, Wascher and Beste [29] suggested that saliency of the distracting stimuli is predictive for the portion of detected relevant targets when both relevant and irrelevant information are spatially separated. However, while this study demonstrated a cascade of subsequently occurring attentional allocation and re-allocation processes to distractor and target stimuli using ERPs [29], they were not able to examine which brain areas and brain networks underlie mechanisms of perceptual competition as operationalized by their paradigm. Thus, it remains unclear what brain areas mainly determine performance in situations of competitive attentional selection. In other words: What brain areas are differentially activated in “good” compared to “poor performance” in situations of competitive attentional selection?
The investigation of individual differences has taken on greater significance in cognitive research and provides a powerful approach for elaborating the functional role of distinct brain regions [30]. During tasks that focus on selective attention, subjects encounter different capacity and ability constraints and thus inevitably perform differently. By identifying correlations between task performance and brain activity this interindividual variability can be explained in terms of strategic differences that are reflected in neural efficiency. Therefore, brain areas will be revealed that are necessary to perform a certain task as well as brain areas that are indicative of deficient and superior task performance.
Based on the notion that perceptual performance in the given task depends on the sensitivity for relevant features, we hypothesized that good performers will present an enhanced BOLD response in occipito-temporal areas encompassing the extrastriate cortex (BA's 18, 19) during trials, where a perceptual conflict between different stimuli is evident. Moreover, in good performers enhanced BOLD responses should be evident in fronto-parietal networks as expression of intensified top-down biasing processes resolving the perceptual conflict between the stimuli. In contrast, poor performers should most likely be influenced more by distracting stimuli which are irrelevant for the task, than by the less salient but relevant stimuli. Poor performers should therefore show a less active top-down network in comparison to good performers.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-four healthy individuals (11 females) participated in the fMRI study (mean age±SD, 23.33±2.53 years; range 20–30 years). All participants were right-handed (mean 90.29, SD = 10.04) based on Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [31], reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and had normal or corrected to normal vision. The study was approved by the Medical Council of Westfalen-Lippe in compliance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Every subject gave written informed consent and was either compensated for participation in the study with money (€20) or course credits.
Stimuli and experimental procedure
The task applied is similar to the one used by Wascher and Beste [29] and depicted in Figure 1. The stimulus material consisted of two rectangular bars which were oriented either horizontally or vertically, presented 2° visual angle left and right from a fixation cross and were either brighter or darker than the background (30 cd/m2) with a Fechner-Contrast of ±0.2.
Each trial consisted of the successive presentation of two frames. The first frame was presented for 200 ms, followed by an interstimulus interval of 50 ms, in which only the fixation cross was visible to mask the transient related to the change. Afterward, the second frame was presented for 200 ms. In order to manipulate bottom-up attentional bias luminance and orientation changed randomly between these two frames constituting four conditions: If only brightness of a stimulus changed between first and second frame, a transient of luminance occurred which will be further referred to as condition LUM. If only orientation changed, the impression of an apparent motion was given (ORI). If luminance and orientation changed at the same spatial location, both transients coincided (luminance and orientation unilateral = LOU). If luminance and orientation changed simultaneously but spatially distributed (luminance and orientation bilateral = LOB) a perceptual conflict was induced because subjects were instructed to detect the less salient luminance transient and to ignore the more salient orientation transient. To indicate the luminance change subjects responded by pressing the left button on a touch pad if the luminance change appeared on the left side and the right button if the luminance change appeared on the right side. Trials in which only orientation changed were no-go trials and subjects had to refrain from responding. Stimulus combinations were randomly intermixed and in consecutive trials the different conditions alternated in a way that no condition was presented twice in succession. Overall 640 trials were presented, 160 for each condition.
Data analysis
Behavioral data analysis
Responses were recorded from the onset of the second frame. Response times (RTs) were defined as the time between stimulus change in the second frame and button press. Error categories comprised response errors (i.e., false button presses and false alarms in the no-go condition when only orientation changed) and misses (i.e., no response was registered within 1.500 ms after stimulus onset). These behavioral parameters were then analyzed across the total sample and according to the extreme group's approach [32]. As the measurement of selective attention yields continuous scores it might be difficult to interpret the relevance of behavioral differences and the underlying neural substrates when considering the entire sample. But even if subjects are assigned to continuous subgroups based on the median score of some variable of interest, these groups may not reveal both ends of the continuum that underlie individual differences because the differentiation between good and poor performance may be blurred. Therefore, another design to maximize the power to detect individual differences and to further reduce the variance between performance subgroups relative to the variance within the entire sample is the assignment of subjects to extreme groups covering only lowest and highest scores of the variable of interest. To assess the impact of individual performance differences, we calculated a score to indicate the subject's susceptibility to distractor interference. Therefore, errors in trials in which the target stimulus was presented solely (LUM) were subtracted from errors of trials presenting a competing distractor (LOB) (LOB minus LUM). In this sense, we can provide a quantitative value to determine the subject's susceptibility to distraction and to investigate the strength of top-down attentional control allocated to the task-relevant stimulus in the absence and presence of the competing distractor by means of a relativization to the basic error rate. This performance score was then used as variable of interest to divide the total sample into extreme groups according to both extremes of the distribution curve. Thus, subjects performing 33% below the mean performance score (<11.04% errors) and 66% above mean performance score (>16.67% errors) were indicative of good and poor performers, respectively.
RTs and error rates were analyzed for all subjects by means of a repeated-measures ANOVA using within-subject factor “trial type” defining the type of stimulus change (LUM, ORI, LOU, and LOB) and for analyzing individual performance differences additionally with between-subject factor “performance” defining good and poor performance on the task. Significances were Greenhouse-Geisser corrected and additional post-hoc tests were performed and Bonferroni-corrected if necessary. All statistical tests were performed with Predictive Analytics Software (PASW) V 18.0.
fMRI data analysis
Functional MR imaging was carried out with a 3T Philips Scanner (Gyroscan; Philips, Best, Netherlands) with a standard 8-channel receiver head coil. Stimuli were presented on a monitor projecting to a mirror positioned on the head coil. Before the fMRI datasets, T1-weighted images were acquired with time of repetition TR = 3500 ms, time of echo TE = 35 ms, flip angle 90°, matrix dimensions 256×256 mm, a field of view FOV = 210 mm and 36 oblique slices. Image processing and statistical analysis of the fMRI images were done by SPM5 standard routines and templates (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, U.K.). Pre-processing steps involved realignment, normalization (resulting voxel size 2 2 2 mm3) and smoothing (8-mm isotropic Gaussian kernel). Data were filtered with a high-pass filter applying a cut-off period of 128 sec. Motion parameters of the realignment step during pre-processing were integrated into the model to control for regressive effects by motion. Events of interest were time-locked to the onset of the first frame. After pre-processing the fMRI data, individual data analysis was performed. Both correct and incorrect trials (i.e., false button presses and false alarms in the no-go condition) were entered into analysis separately. As subjects were assigned to good and poor performers on basis of their performance, the number of successful and error trials and consequently the percentage of trials in proximity to an error differed considerably. However, it has been shown that brain activation remains consistent across trials. Thus, the obtained activation patterns of certain brain regions are highly indicative of their functional relevance for cognitive mechanisms although only a small number of trials can be used for analysis [33].
For each subject four BOLD contrast differences (t-contrasts) were determined as a function of BOLD signal change compared to noise level by modeling the corresponding regressors (LUM, ORI, LOU, LOB) to obtain scaled beta weights for each conditional event [34]. These conditions of interest were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) with no derivatives and were entered into second-level analysis using the general linear model (GLM). Anatomical localization of activated brain regions was determined by reference to standard stereotaxic atlas by Talairach and Tournoux [35].
To gain valuable insights into the relationship between individual differences in task performance and brain activation we used an experimental task that aims at producing highly varying performance across subjects. Moreover, we adopted an integrative approach by modeling associations between task performance and brain activation both within and between subjects. This approach comprises random effects analyses across the total sample and performance groups, regression analyses, ANOVAs and the calculation of beta weight estimates.
Random effect analyses were performed on the individual subject's contrast images to obtain group contrast maps for conflict trials (LOB) across the entire sample and extreme groups. To test hypotheses regarding the region dependence in performance-brain activation relationships multiple regression analyses across the whole brain were conducted using performance scores as regressors. Although the use of reaction time measures as a predictor of brain activation seems to be preferable this approach has been shown to be affected by temporal BOLD summation and consequently increases the probability of type I errors [36].
To compare signal activations between good and poor performers a full factorial ANOVA was applied on both successful and error trials with “conflict trial” as factor with two levels comprising good and poor performers. For all reported analyses a Monte Carlo simulation of the brain volume was conducted to establish an appropriate voxel contiguity threshold [37]. Assuming an individual voxel type I error of p<.005, a cluster extent of 70 contiguous resampled voxels was indicated as sufficient to correct for multiple voxel comparisons at p<.005. Thus, voxels with an uncorrected significance level of p<.005 with a minimum cluster size of at least 70 voxels were reported for all analyzes.
Derived from previous neuroimaging studies [17]–[19] we have chosen three sets of regions encompassing frontal, parietal and visual regions that should capture the main contributions of bottom-up and top-down attentional control. Specific coordinates of brain regions could not be used because the task applied in this study has been developed only recently. Therefore, regions of interest (ROIs) were defined as the activation clusters obtained from regression analyses and ANOVAs. Conforming to the center coordinates of the ROIs (Table 1) indicated by results of the regression analyses and ANOVAs, contralateral ROIs were acquired additionally to consider lateralization effects. Beta weights were derived from all reported ROIs and were calculated by means of beta-weighted images for each region of interest (ROI) and compared using ANOVAs with subsequent Bonferroni-corrected pair-wise comparisons. Finally, beta weights were entered into a linear regression model to verify the association between brain activation and task performance.
Table 1. ROIs used for calculation of activation beta estimates.
Region | Talairach coordinates | ||
x | y | z | |
SUCCESSFUL TRIALS | |||
ROIs derived from regression analyses | |||
L Inferior frontal gyrus (BA 9) | −50 | 20 | 20 |
R IFG (BA 9) | 50 | 20 | 20 |
L Angular gyrus (BA 39) | −52 | −62 | 30 |
R AG (BA 39) | 52 | −62 | 30 |
L Middle temporal gyrus (BA 21) | −48 | 0 | −16 |
R MTG (BA 21) | 48 | 0 | −16 |
L Superior temporal gyrus (BA 22) | −48 | −2 | −4 |
R STG (BA 22) | 48 | −2 | −4 |
L Cingulate Gyrus (BA 24) | −6 | 18 | 26 |
R CG (BA 24) | 6 | 18 | 26 |
L Cingulate gyrus (BA 32) | −14 | 16 | 26 |
R CG (BA 32) | 14 | 16 | 26 |
L Anterior cingulate (BA 33) | −2 | 18 | 18 |
R ACC (BA 33) | 2 | 18 | 18 |
ROIs derived from ANOVA | |||
L Rectal gyrus (BA11) | −4 | 10 | −22 |
R RG (BA11) | 4 | 10 | 22 |
L Cuneus (BA 18) | −18 | −74 | 18 |
R Cun (BA 18) | 18 | −74 | 18 |
L Lingual gyrus (BA 18) | −4 | −66 | 2 |
R LG (BA 18) | 4 | −66 | 2 |
L Precuneus (BA 19) | −34 | −66 | 36 |
R Precuneus (BA 19) | 34 | −66 | 36 |
L Anterior cingulate (BA 25) | 0 | 14 | −8 |
L Middle temporal gyrus (BA 39) | −32 | 60 | 28 |
R MTG (BA 39) | 32 | −60 | 28 |
L Inferior parietal lobule (BA 39) | −50 | −62 | 38 |
R IPL (BA 39) | 50 | −62 | 38 |
L Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | −38 | −50 | 34 |
R IPL (BA 40) | 38 | −50 | 34 |
ERROR TRIALS | |||
ROIs derived from regression analyses | |||
L Medial frontal gyrus (BA 6) | −14 | −24 | 58 |
R MFG (BA 6) | 14 | −24 | 58 |
L Superior frontal gyrus (BA 8) | −14 | 46 | 42 |
R SFG (BA 8) | 14 | 46 | 42 |
L parahippocampal gyrus (BA 28) | −18 | −18 | −26 |
R PHG (BA 28) | 18 | −18 | −26 |
L Superior temporal gyrus (BA 39) | −48 | −56 | 24 |
R Superior temporal gyrus (BA 39) | 48 | −56 | 24 |
Results
Behavioral data
Figure 2 illustrates the distribution of mean response times (panel A) and mean error rates (panel B) for trial types. Corresponding descriptive statistics are listed in Table 2. Response times were negatively affected by the presence of a competing distractor (LOB trials) for both, the total sample and the performance subgroups as indicated by a main effect of trial type. This effect progressively increased in effect size from total sample (F(2,48) = 47.759, η2>.67, p<.001) to the extreme groups (F(2,28) = 42.197, η2>.75, p<.001) indicating that overall response times were lowest on LOU trials and highest on LOB trials and the difference between LUM and LOU trials being larger for extreme groups (p = .004) than for the total sample (p = .008). Moreover, the separate analysis of the extreme performance groups did not yield a significant difference between good and poor performers (F(1,14) = 2.144, p = .165) implying that better performance cannot be explained by higher response times.
Table 2. Behavioral data.
Total sample | Good performers | Poor performers | |
(N = 24) | (N = 8) | (N = 8) | |
RT (ms) LUM | 720.41±147.44 | 658.86±103.38 | 734.46±119.88 |
RT (ms) LOU | 706.12±147.52 | 641.33±94.68 | 715.79±130.19 |
RT (ms) LOB | 817.96±181.55 | 747.18±149.32 | 867.94±183.67 |
ER (%) LUM | 10.40±6.67 | 5.16±2.71 | 11.17±6.91 |
ER (%) ORI | 4.77±6.32 | 1.95±2.20 | 4.30±3.86 |
ER (%) LOU | 8.65±7.50 | 3.75±2.43 | 6.64±4.25 |
ER (%) LOB | 26.35±13.96 | 11.48±3.76 | 38.28±12.06 |
Response times and error rates across all trial types for both the total sample and performance groups.
Similarly, for error rates a highly significant main effect of trial type has been revealed across the total sample (F(3,72) = 36.126, η2 = .60, p<.001) and extreme groups (F(3,42) = 52.424, η2 = .79, p<.001) indicating that detection of the task-relevant luminance change was highly impaired on perceptual conflict (LOB) trials. Furthermore, the separate analysis of the extreme groups yielded a significant interaction between trial type and performance (F(3,42) = 24.554, η2 = .64, p<.001) indicating that a difference between good and poor performers was evident on perceptual conflict (LOB) trials (t(14) = −3.935, p = .001) but not on other trial types (all t′s(14)<.583, all p′s>.569).
fMRI data
Random effects analyses at total sample and extreme performance levels
The results of the random effect analyses on successful and error trials across the total sample (N = 24) and performance subgroups (N = 8 each) are depicted in Figure 3 (see Table 3 for voxel locations and corresponding statistical information). We have included only perceptual conflict (LOB) trials in which changes of luminance and orientation occurred simultaneously but spatially separated to emphasize the interaction between bottom-up and top-down attention and likewise because performance differences on the behavioral level were exclusively evident on these trials.
Table 3. Random effects analyses on successful trials.
Region | Talairach coordinates | Z | kE | ||
x | y | z | |||
Total sample (N = 24) | |||||
L Parahippocampal gyrus (BA 36) | −24 | −30 | −16 | 3.83 | 754 |
L Culmen | −16 | −32 | −14 | 4.23 | |
−8 | −36 | −8 | 3.73 | ||
R Caudate body | 6 | 12 | 10 | 4.80 | 127 |
Good performers (N = 8) | |||||
L Rectal gyrus (BA 11) | −2 | 8 | −26 | 3.67 | 70 |
L Medial frontal gyrus (BA 25) | −6 | 26 | −18 | 3.70 | 88 |
L Anterior cingulate (BA 25) | −4 | 18 | −8 | 3.35 | |
L Middle temporal gyrus (BA 19) | −34 | −80 | 24 | 3.56 | 95 |
L Middle temporal gyrus (BA 39) | −42 | −74 | 14 | 3.25 | |
L Cuneus (BA 18) | −10 | −68 | 14 | 3.63 | 797 |
R Posterior cingulate (BA 30) | 10 | −56 | 10 | 3.87 | |
R Culmen | 6 | −60 | −4 | 4.30 | |
L Parahippocampal gyrus (BA 28) | −18 | −14 | −24 | 3.50 | 149 |
L Parahippocampal gyrus (BA 35) | −28 | −26 | −20 | 2.78 | |
L Culmen | −24 | −32 | −24 | 3.59 |
Brain regions showing significant activations across the total sample (N = 24) and good performers (N = 8) on successful perceptual conflict trials (p<.005 uncorr., k>70 voxels). No activation clusters were evident in poor performers (N = 8).
On successful trials (Figure 3A; Table 3) the total sample (LOB) (upper panel) elicited activation in the parahippocampal gyrus (BA's 35, 36) and caudate body. By comparison, the subgroup of good performers (Figure 3A, lower left panel; Table 3) exhibited additional and more widely distributed brain activation in fronto-limbic (BA's 11, 25, 28, 30, 35), temporal (BA's 19, 39) and occipital (BA 18) areas. In contrast, poor performers (Figure 3A, lower right panel) exhibited no activation clusters within the same threshold on successful trials.
On error trials (false button presses and misses) (Figure 3B; Table 4) activation of a fronto-parietal network emerged, encompassing BA's 6, 9, 11, 32, 46, inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) and the claustrum (upper panel). Unlike successful trials, for good performers (Figure 3B, lower left panel) no activation clusters reached significance on error trials whereas the activation pattern obtained from poor performers (Figure 3B, lower right panel; Table 4) was evident in the prefrontal cortex (BA's 46, 47).
Table 4. Random effects analyses on error trials.
Region | Talairach coordinates | Z | kE | ||
x | y | z | |||
Total sample (N = 24) | |||||
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 6) | 26 | 2 | 38 | 3.31 | 162 |
R Cingulate gyrus (BA 32) | 22 | 14 | 26 | 3.20 | |
R Claustrum | 26 | 18 | 14 | 3.34 | |
R Precentral gyrus (BA 9) | 46 | 18 | 34 | 3.27 | 121 |
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 9) | 36 | 20 | 34 | 3.08 | |
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 9) | 42 | 34 | 36 | 3.31 | 78 |
50 | 30 | 30 | 2.72 | ||
R Sub-gyral (BA 10) | 42 | 40 | 0 | 3.95 | 815 |
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 11) | 26 | 38 | −8 | 3.52 | |
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 46) | 50 | 44 | 6 | 4.12 | |
L Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | −46 | −48 | 58 | 3.55 | 208 |
−58 | −38 | 36 | 3.11 | ||
−58 | −40 | 48 | 3.00 | ||
Poor performers (N = 8) | |||||
L Middle frontal gyrus (BA 46) | −48 | 44 | 8 | 3.42 | 182 |
L Middle frontal gyrus (BA 47) | −34 | 36 | −2 | 3.55 | |
−42 | 36 | −4 | 3.43 | ||
R Middle frontal gyrus (BA 46) | 50 | 44 | 8 | 2.86 | 121 |
R Inferior frontal gyrus | 48 | 42 | 0 | 3.37 |
Brain regions showing significant activations across the total sample (N = 24) and good performers (N = 8) on erroneous perceptual conflict trials (p<.005 uncorr., k>70 voxels). No activation clusters were evident in good performers (N = 8).
Correlative links between task performance and brain activation
As we aimed at linking individual performance differences to human brain functioning on perceptual conflict trials, we performed regression analyses across both successful and error trials to investigate the impact of task performance on strategic differences and efficiency of neural processes. The results of the regression analyses using subjects' error rates and regional beta weight estimates on perceptual conflict trials was characterized by both positive and negative performance-brain activation relationships in several cortical and subcortical brain regions (Figure 4 and Table 5).
Table 5. Regression analyses on successful and erroneous trials.
Region | Talairach coordinates | Z | kE | ||
x | y | z | |||
SUCCESSFUL TRIALS | |||||
Positive correlation | |||||
L Inferior frontal gyrus (BA 9) | −50 | 20 | 20 | 3.29 | 87 |
L Angular gyrus (BA 39) | −52 | −62 | 30 | 3.30 | 73 |
Negative correlation | |||||
L Anterior cingulate (BA 33) | −2 | 18 | 18 | 3.71 | 89 |
R Cingulate gyrus (BA 32) | 14 | 16 | 26 | 3.16 | |
L Cingulate Gyrus (BA 24) | −6 | 18 | 26 | 2.71 | |
R Caudate tail | 30 | −38 | 6 | 3.69 | 178 |
R Parahippocampal gyrus (BA 19) | 40 | −44 | −6 | 3.29 | |
R Middle temporal gyrus (BA 21) | 48 | 0 | −16 | 3.43 | 141 |
R Superior temporal gyrus (BA 22) | 48 | −2 | −4 | 3.10 | |
L Middle occipital gyrus (BA 19) | −46 | −82 | 18 | 3.41 | 94 |
ERROR TRIALS | |||||
Positive correlation | |||||
L Parahippocampal gyrus (BA 28) | −18 | −18 | −26 | 4.16 | 83 |
Negative correlation | |||||
R Medial frontal gyrus (BA 6) | 14 | −24 | 58 | 3.80 | 74 |
R Superior frontal gyrus (BA 8) | 12 | 44 | 46 | 3.24 | 93 |
16 | 48 | 38 | 2.63 | ||
0 | 48 | 46 | 2.63 | ||
L Superior temporal gyrus (BA 39) | −48 | −56 | 24 | 3.44 | 105 |
L Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | −52 | −58 | 36 | 2.80 | |
R Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | 54 | −52 | 44 | 2.91 | 72 |
R Angular gyrus (BA 40) | 56 | −56 | 36 | 2.79 |
Brain regions showing significant positive and negative correlations across the total sample (N = 24) on successful and erroneous perceptual conflict trials (p<.005 uncorr., k>70 voxels).
On successful trials (Figure 4A; Table 5) increased performance-related activity was seen in inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; BA 9) and angular gyrus (ANG; BA 39) (upper panel). A negative association (lower panel) was evident in limbic (BA's 19, 24, 32, 33), temporal (BA's 21, 22) and occipital brain regions (BA 19) as well as in the caudate tail.
On error trials (Figure 4D; Table 5) increase in performance was positively correlated with activation in the parahippocampal gyrus (PHG; BA 28) only (upper panel). Furthermore, with increasing performance also activation decreased in frontal (BA's 6, 8) and temporal (BA 39) brain regions (lower panel).
ROI analyses were performed for all brain regions that have been displayed by regression analyses across successful and error trials.
Across successful trials only activity in the left ANG (BA 39) yielded significance between good and poor performers (t(14) = −3.912, p = .022) (Figure 4B). Although other brain regions did not survive the statistical threshold set, additional regression analysis (Figure 4C) revealed strong relationships between performance score and beta estimates. A positive correlation with task performance, indicating that decreasing performance was associated with higher beta estimates, was evident in left ANG (BA 39) (r = .505, p = .012). Negative correlations were evident in left (r = .600, p = .002) and right (r = .414, p = .044) anterior cingulate (ACC; BA 33), left cingulate gyrus (CG; BA 24) (r = .461, p = .023), right middle temporal gyurs (MTG; BA 21) (r = .551, p = .005) and right superior temporal gyrus (STG; BA 22) (r = .538, p = .007), implying that decreasing performance was associated with lower beta estimates in these regions.
Across error trials differences between good and poor performers were not significantly different in any region (Figure 4E). However, as indicated by regression analyses (Figure 4F) task performance was negatively correlated with beta estimates in left PHG (BA 28) (r = .448, p = .028) and positively correlated in the right medial frontal gyrus (MeFG; BA 6) (r = .411, p = .046).
Distinct recruitment of neural networks by good and poor performers
Figure 5 illustrates the activation clusters obtained by good and poor performers of the extreme groups. The corresponding voxel locations and statistical information are listed in Table 6.
Table 6. Activation patterns in good and poor performers on perceptual conflict trials.
Region | Talairach coordinates | Z | kE | ||
x | y | z | |||
Main effect of Performance | |||||
L Rectal gyrus (BA 11) | −4 | 10 | −22 | 3.32 | 96 |
−6 | 18 | −20 | 2.94 | ||
L Anterior cingulate (BA 25) | 0 | 14 | −8 | 2.78 | |
L Cuneus (BA 18) | −18 | −74 | 18 | 3.93 | 301 |
L Lingual gyrus (BA 18) | −4 | −66 | 2 | 3.55 | |
R Culmen of Vermis | 6 | −62 | −2 | 3.58 | |
L Cuneus (BA 18) | 0 | −80 | 28 | 3.36 | 76 |
L Cuneus (BA 7) | −6 | −74 | 30 | 2.82 | |
Good>poor performers | |||||
L Postcentral gyrus (BA 3) | −38 | −28 | 50 | 3.26 | 90 |
L Precentral gyrus (BA 4) | −32 | −30 | 56 | 2.83 | |
L Rectal gyrus (BA 11) | −4 | 10 | −22 | 3.51 | 216 |
L Anterior cingulate (BA 25) | 0 | 14 | −8 | 3.00 | |
L Cuneus (BA 18) | −18 | −74 | 18 | 4.09 | 654 |
L Lingual gyrus (BA 18) | −4 | −66 | 2 | 3.72 | |
R Culmen of Vermis | 6 | −62 | −2 | 3.75 | |
Poor>good performers | |||||
R Middle temporal gyrus (BA 39) | 32 | −60 | 28 | 3.59 | 159 |
R Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | 38 | −50 | 34 | 3.20 | |
R Precuneus (BA 19) | 34 | −66 | 36 | 2.99 | |
L Inferior parietal lobule (BA 39) | −50 | −62 | 38 | 3.57 | 92 |
R Inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) | 50 | −50 | 44 | 2.95 | 82 |
54 | −50 | 36 | 2.85 | ||
R Supramarginal gyrus (BA 40) | 62 | −48 | 32 | 2.62 |
Brain regions significantly activated across good (N = 8) and poor performers (N = 8) on perceptual conflict trials as derived from the ANOVA (p<.005 uncorr., k>70 voxels).
On successful perceptual conflict (LOB) trials the main effect of performance yielded significant activation in fronto-limbic (BA's 11, 25) and occipital (BA's 7, 18) brain areas. Pair-wise comparisons revealed that this effect was mainly driven by the contrast of good>poor performers yielding prominent activation clusters in frontal (BA's 4, 11), limbic (BA 25), parietal (BA 3) and visual association areas (BA 18) and in the cerebellar culmen of vermis for good performers.
The reversed contrast of poor>good performers revealed a stronger activation cluster for poor performers only in temporo-parietal brain regions encompassing BA's 19, 39 and 40.
On error trials the main effect of performance yielded statistical significance neither at the same threshold nor at a lower threshold of 10 voxels uncorrected at a level of p<.001. Therefore, performance-related differences in the recruitment of neural networks seem to be based on different strategies developed for successful completion of the task at hand.
ROI analyses were conducted on the basis of the ANOVA across correct trials, yielding significant differences in beta estimates exclusively in left-lateralized fronto-parietal and occipital regions. Performance differences between good and poor performers were found to be significant in the rectal gyrus (RG; BA 11) (t(14) = 4.688, p = .005), inferior parietal lobule (IPL; BA's 39, 40) (all t(14)<4.788, p<.026), cuneus (Cun) and lingual gyrus (LingG; BA 18) (all t(14)<5.860, p<.005) and marginally in the ACC (BA 25) (t(14) = 3.348, p = .094). One sample t-tests revealed significant activation for good performers only left-lateralized in the RG (BA 11) (t(7) = 4.971, p = .024), Cun (BA 18) (t(7) = 5.271, p = .017) and the precuneus (PreCun; BA 19) (t(7) = 4.829, p = .029). Significant deactivations for good performers were evident only in IPL (BA 39) (t(7) = −4.359, p = .05). In contrast, for poor performers the LingG (BA 18) yielded significant deactivation (t(7) = −4.625, p = .036) as well as the left IPL (BA 40) (t(7) = −4.288, p = .054).
Discussion
In the current study we investigated neural correlates of perceptual conflict that determine performance in competitive attentional selection. Participants were instructed to detect changes in luminance (task-relevant stimulus) accompanied by contralateral changes in orientation (task-irrelevant stimulus), inducing a perceptual conflict [29]. Our results support theories that claim an interaction of bottom-up and top-down processes mediated by distinct attentional networks [14], [17], [38]–[41].
Both, our behavioral and fMRI data indicated that detection of the task-relevant luminance change was influenced by the simultaneously presented distractor of orientation change if both were spatially separated (see also [29]). The behavioral data yielded faster reaction times on non-conflict trials of LUM and LOU (for the ORI condition no response was required) and higher accuracy in all non-conflict conditions of LUM, ORI and LOU as compared to the perceptual conflict trials (LOB) with good performers outperforming the poor performers concerning error rates (Table 2). Accordingly, the fMRI data indicated that interindividual differences are accompanied by distinct activation patterns during successful performance on a competitive attentional task (Figure 5). The results of the random effects analyses, ANOVA and beta estimates across the performance groups presented areas that are activated to varying degrees by good and poor performers. Good performers showed strong neuronal enhancement of fronto-parietal and visual brain areas and a decrease in brain activation in parietal areas involving BA 39. In contrast, poor performers depicted a reversed activation pattern, showing neuronal enhancement only in BA 39 and neuronal suppression in fronto-parietal and visual brain areas whereby the modulation of neuronal enhancement and suppression was not as strong as it has been shown by the good performers. Together, these results indicate a distinct recruitment of fronto-parietal and visual areas that contribute differentially to varying degrees of visual processing capacities and top-down control.
Top-down attentional processes have repeatedly been hypothesized to recruit a network of fronto-parietal areas [5], [17]–[18], [42], in which prefrontal areas and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) may provide a source of top-down attentional control [26], [43]–[47]. Our results revealed a strong engagement of the orbitofrontal cortex (BA 11), cingulate and anterior cingulate cortex in good performers which is in accordance with this assumption. The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has dense reciprocal connections with prefrontal, limbic and sensory areas and is most heavily linked to the medial and inferior temporal lobe [48]–[52], areas known to be part of the ventral processing stream [53]–[54]. Similarly, the ACC integrates incoming inputs and has strong reciprocal interconnections with lateral prefrontal, parietal cortex, premotor and supplementary motor areas [55]. Therefore, increased activity in OFC and ACC obtained by good performers relative to poor performers may reflect differences in recruiting top-down attentional control. Moreover, the OFC and ACC act upon brain regions that modulate sensory processing [56]–[57] which may be reflected in our activation pattern found in extrastriate visual areas (BA's 18, 19). Thus, it can be assumed that increased brain activation in OFC and ACC revealed by good performers reflect a stronger recruitment of top-down attentional control to support processing of the task-relevant stimulus [58]–[59]. In line with this interpretation, poor performers showed less recruitment of the OFC and ACC as indicated by decreased beta estimates. Decreased brain activation in poor performers might reflect less focused attention and thus weaker top-down attentional control to resolve the perceptual conflict.
Besides the involvement of OFC and ACC in top-down control and sensory modulation, alternative hypotheses relate OFC and ACC function to reward [60]–[63], effort computation [61], [64], and performance monitoring [65]. Although we did not offer reward for good task performance some participants may have encouraged and maintained task compliance in a way that they experienced reward even only by complying with the task instruction or felt a rewarding effect of success. This would be in accordance with the incentive salience hypothesis, which combines both perceptual and motivational features by attributing motivational values to neutral stimuli that previously carried purely perceptual information [66]–[67]. In this sense, the attribution of incentive salience to the task-relevant change of luminance could have led to differential recruitment of the OFC.
Another approach that results in different performance outcomes involves models of effort computation and decision making. Decision making implies a selection of an option over another on the basis of preferences and the balance between costs and benefits associated with pursuing one option [68]. As subjects experience cost-benefit decision differently, some may exert higher effort to obtain a desired option than other subjects. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the ACC evaluates the benefit of an option by integrating both the expected outcome and the effort needed [61], [67]–[70]. Therefore, differences in ACC activation may reflect different levels of effort invested by good and poor performers. Together, these hypotheses provide an insight into orbitofrontal and ACC function that could be the consequence rather than the cause of successful trials.
In line with the modulation of activity found in frontal areas, the results pattern in the inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) similarly revealed an increase in activation for good performers and a decrease in activation for poor performers. As proposed by Posner and Petersen [40] the posterior attention system operates upon the ventral stream which is required for detailed object identification. Accordingly, this model has been confirmed by previous research implicating the inferior parietal cortex in top-down attentional control [57], [71], especially in target detection [72]–[73].
In addition to fronto-parietal projection sites of top-down attentional control, good performers revealed higher activation in visual areas, compared to poor performers. Recent work has shown that neural mechanisms of visual selective attention operate at cortical and subcortical stages in the visual system [8], [15], [19], [74] and also the biased competition account assumes that perceptual conflicts are resolved in visual cortical areas [3], [5]. The observed increased brain activity in areas V2 and V3 in good performers and decreased activity in these areas for poor performers is in line with the above findings and may additionally contribute to overt performance differences. As revealed by one-sample t-tests, beta estimates for good performers in V2 and V3 increased significantly whereas beta estimates for poor performers decreased and showed only significant activity in the left lingual gyrus. This is in line with the findings from fronto-limbic areas indicating that increasing activation in prefrontal areas and the ACC provide stronger top-down attentional control that exert effects on visual areas to resolve perceptual conflict.
In contrast to increasing activation in visual areas V2 and V3, good performers presented significant deactivation in BA 39 that corresponds to the monkey inferotemporal cortex, an area known to be involved in high-level visual processing, especially in internal representation and target selection [11]. Moreover, the angular gyrus has been suggested to house a salience map [17], [47] with a winner-take-all process selecting the most salient stimulus from the current visual set and shifting attention towards this location [75]–[77]. In this sense, a strong deactivation as reflected by good performers may indicate an active suppression of the task-irrelevant change of orientation which has been accounted as the most salient. Poor performers did not reveal such a deactivation of BA 39. It is therefore conceivable that deactivation of BA 39 is part of a ‘strategy’ utilized by good performers to reduce processing of the salient, but irrelevant orientation change, which causes the competition with the less salient, but behaviorally relevant luminance change.
In summary, the results show a correlation between brain activation and individual performance differences in competitive attentional selection. Good performance on the task was reflected in higher accuracy and increased activation of fronto-parietal and visual areas V2 and V3, but also in decreased activation in BA 39. The latter deactivation may be part of a ‘strategy’ to reduce influences of the salient distractor in the bottom-up channel. On a related note, deactivations in areas that have been related to top-down attentional control were closely tied to poor performance that may indicate deficient top-down attentional control. These results confirm previous studies emphasizing the importance of a fronto-parietal network that has been shown to be necessary to exert top-down attention control. With our study we extended the available expertise by highlighting the varying recruitment of the fronto-parietal network during situations of perceptual conflict.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (http://www.dfg.de/index.jsp) BE 4045/6-1 (to C.B.) and PF 244/13 (to B.P.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Yantis S (2000) Goal-directed and stimulus-driven determinants of attentional control. In Monsell S and Driver J, editors. Attention and performance. Cambridge: MIT Press. 73–103.
- 2. Egeth HE, Yantis S (1997) Visual attention: Control, representation, and time courses. Annu Rev Psychol 48: 269–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Knudsen EI (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 30: 57–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Yantis S, Jonides J (1984) Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10(5): 601–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18: 193–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Beste C, Wascher E, Güntürkün O, Dinse HR (2011) Improvement and impairment of visually guided behavior through LTP- and LTD-like exposure-based visual learning. Curr Biol 21(10): 876–882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Shipp S (2004) The brain circuitry of attention. Trend Cogn Sci 8(5): 223–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Stokes M, Thompson R, Nobre AC, Duncan J (2009) Shape-specific preparatory activity mediates attention to targets in human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(46): 19569–19574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Maunsell JH, Treue S (2006) Feature-based attention in visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 29(6): 317–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Treue S, Martinez-Trujillo JC (2007) Attending to features inside and outside the spotlight of attention. Neuron. 55(2): 174–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R (1993) A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363(6427): 345–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Chelazzi L, Duncan J, Miller EK, Desimone R (1998) Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search. J Neurophysiol 80(6): 2918–2940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Moran J, Desimone R (1985) Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229(4715): 782–784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kastner S, De Weerd P, Pinsk MA, Elizondo MI, Desimone R, et al. (2001) Modulation of sensory suppression: Implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 86(3): 1398–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Kastner S, Pinks MA (2004) Visual attention as a multilevel selection process. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4(4): 483–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Poghosyan V, Shibata T, Ioannides AA (2005) Effects of attention and arousal on early responses in striate cortex. Eur J Neurosci 22(1): 225–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(3): 201–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: 315–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Serences JT, Yantis S (2006) Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 10(1): 38–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Chadick JZ, Gazzaley A (2011) Differential coupling of visual cortex with default or frontal-parietal network based on goals. Nat Neurosci 14(7): 830–832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Gazzaley A, Cooney J, Rissmann J, D′Esposito M (2005) Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat Neurosci 8(10): 1298–1300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Beck DM, Kastner S (2009) Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Res 49(10): 1154–1165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Kastner S, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (1998) Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. Science 282(5386): 108–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Miller EK (1993) Suppression of visual responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque by addition of a second stimulus. Brain Res 616(1–2): 25–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R (1999) Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci 19(5): 1736–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hickey C, McDonald JJ, Theeuwes J (2006) Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. J Cogn Neurosci 18(4): 604–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Schreij D, Owens C, Theeuwes J (2008) Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Percept Psychophys 70(2): 208–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Theeuwes J (2004) Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonom Bulle Rev 11(1): 65–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Wascher E, Beste C (2010) Tuning perceptual competition. J Neurophysiol 103(2): 1057–1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gruska A, Matthews G, Szynura B (2010) Handbook of individual differences in cognition. New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London: Springer. 600 p.
- 31. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edingburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1): 97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, MacCullum RC, Nicewander WA (2005) Use of the extreme groups approach: a critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychol Methods. 10(2): 178–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Windischberger C, Lamm C, Bauer H, Moser E (2002) Consistency of inter-trial activation using single-trial fMRI: assessment of regional differences. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 13(1): 129–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ward B (2002) Deconvulation analysis of fMRI time series data. Available: http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/manual/Deconvolvem.pdf. Accessed 2011 May 26.
- 35.Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme. 122 p.
- 36. Yarkoni T, Barch DM, Gray JR, Conturo TE, Braver TS (2009) BOLD correlates of trial-by-trial reaction time variability in gray and white matter: a multi-study fMRI analysis. PLoS ONE 4(1): e4257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Slotnick SD, Moo LR, Segal JB, Hart J Jr (2003) Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 17: 75–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Hahn B, Ross TJ, Stein EA (2006) Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom-up and top-down processes of visuospatial selective attention. Neuroimage 32(2): 842–853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Hilimire MR, Mounts JR, Parks NA, Corballis PM (2010) Event-related potentials dissociate effects of salience and space in biased competition for visual representation. PLoS One 5(9): e12677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13: 25–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Proulx MJ, Egeth HE (2008) Biased competition and visual search: The role of luminance and size contrast. Psychol Res 72(1): 106–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L (2004) Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 611–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Duncan J (2001) An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(11): 820–829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Ramnani N, Owen AM (2004) Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 5(3): 184–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Rossi AF, Pessoa L, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (2008) The prefrontal cortex and the executive control of attention. Exp Brain Res 192(3): 489–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Hung J, Driver J, Walsh V (2011) Visual Selection and the Human Frontal Eye Fields: Effects of Frontal Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on Parietal Report Analyzed by Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention. J Neurosci 31(44): 15904–15913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Zenon A, Filali N, Duhamel JR, Olivier E (2010) Salience representation in the parietal and frontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 22(5): 918–930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Barbas H (1988) Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 276(3): 313–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Carmichael ST, Price JL (1995a) Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 363(4): 615–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Carmichael ST, Price JL (1995b) Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 363(4): 642–664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Cavada C (2000) Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo RJ, Reinoso-Suarez F (2000) The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex 10(3): 220–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Webster MJ, Bachevalier J, Ungerleider LG (1994) Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. Cereb Cortex 4(5): 470–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15(1): 20–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG (1982) Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav Brain Res 6(1): 57–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA (1995) Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 118(Pt 1): 279–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Chawla D, Rees G, Friston KJ (1999) The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas. Nat Neurosci 2(7): 671–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR (2000) The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci 3(3): 284–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald III AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, et al. (2004) Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303(5660): 1023–1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Rolls ET (2004) The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn 55(1): 11–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. O′Doherty JP (2004) Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14(6): 769–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Prévost C, Pessiglione M, Météreau E, Cléry-Melin ML, Dreher JC (2010) Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. J Neurosci 30(42): 14080–14090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Rolls ET (2000) The orbitofrontale cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex 10(3): 284–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontale cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 86(3): 216–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Kennerley SW, Behrens TE, Wallis JD (2011) Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontale and anterior cingulate neurons. Nat Neurosci 14(12): 1581–1589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD (1998) Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280(5364): 747–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Berridge KC, Robinson TE (1998) What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev 28(3): 309–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Berridge KC (2012) From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. Eur J Neurosci 35(7): 1124–1143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Fishburn PC (1970) Utility theory for decision making. Publications in Operations Research, No. 18. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
- 69. Croxson PL, Walton ME, O′Reilly JX, Behrens TE, Rushworth MF (2009) Effort-based cost-benefit valuation and the human brain. J Neurosci 29(14): 4531–4541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM, Phillips PE, Rushworth MF (2006) Weighing up the benefits of work: behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making. Neural Netw 19(8): 1302–1314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Hung J, Driver J, Walsh V (2005) Visual selection and posterior parietal cortex: effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on partial report analyzed by Bundesen's theory of visual attention. J Neurosci 25(42): 9602–9612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL (2000) Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 3(3): 292–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Perry RJ, Zeki S (2000) The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. Brain 123(Pt 11): 2273–2788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Gilbert CD, Sigman M (2007) Brain states: Top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54(5): 677–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Itti L, Koch C (2000) A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Res 40(10–12): 1489–1506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Itti L, Koch C (2001) Computational modeling of visual attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 194–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Koch C, Ullman S (1985) Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. Hum Neurobiol 4(4): 219–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]