Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Aug 27.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Neurol. 2010 Jan 21;25(7):856–863. doi: 10.1177/0883073809351313

Dorso-Lateral Prefrontal Cortex MRI Measurements and Cognitive Performance in Autism

Jessica Griebling 1, Nancy J Minshew 2, Kimberly Bodner 3, Robin Libove 4, Rahul Bansal 5, Prasad Konasale 6, Matcheri S Keshavan 7, Antonio Hardan 8
PMCID: PMC3428128  NIHMSID: NIHMS357810  PMID: 20097663

Abstract

This study examined the relationships between volumetric measurements of frontal lobe structures and performance on executive function tasks in individuals with autism. MRI scans were obtained from 38 individuals with autism and 40 matched controls between the ages of 8 and 45 years. Executive function was assessed using neuropsychological measures including the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Tower of Hanoi. Differences in performance on the neuropsychological tests were found between the two groups. However, no differences in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes were observed between groups. No correlations between volumetric measurements and performance on the neuropsychological tests were found. Findings from this study suggest that executive function deficits observed in autism are related to functional but not anatomical abnormalities of the frontal lobe. The absence of correlations suggests that executive dysfunction is not the result of focal brain alterations but, rather, is the result of a distributed neural network dysfunction.

Keywords: autism, frontal lobe, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, magnetic resonance imaging, executive function

Introduction

Autistic disorder is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by social impairments, communication deficits, and idiosyncratic or stereotypic behaviors with evidence of wide-spread cognitive deficits.1 Neuropsychological studies have provided evidence of disruption in executive functioning in individuals with autism.2-5 Executive functioning includes cognitive processes such as working memory, planning, inhibition of responses, and cognitive flexibility.6,7 Previous studies have shown that individuals with autism have deficits in some or all of these domains leading to characteristic behavioral impairments,8-13 but it remains unclear whether or not all aspects of these cognitive functions are affected.

The most common neuropsychological tests used to assess executive functioning have been the Tower of Hanoi 14 and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.15,16 The Tower of Hanoi task challenges problem solving and concept formation and has been shown to activate prefrontal and parietal areas in healthy individuals.17 Several investigations have reported that individuals with autism show impaired performance on this task over time.18-21 Using a simplified version of the Tower of Hanoi, the Tower of London, Hughes et al.22 and Barnard et al.23 found that participants with autism performed significantly worse on this test compared to controls, indicating deficits in forward planning. These observations are consistent with several other studies that have used the Tower of London and Tower of Hanoi to examine executive functioning deficits in autism.24,25 Similarly, other investigations have found individuals with autism to have less efficient or impaired abilities to plan and organize tasks or activities.3,20,26-30 The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test is another executive function test, but unlike the Tower tasks, it assesses cognitive flexibility, or shifts in planning, and measures difficulties with conceptualization, perseveration, and inefficient learning.16 Studies have shown that individuals with autism demonstrate some rigidity and difficulties developing problem-solving strategies.20-24,29,31-34 Investigations have also found that individuals with autism without intellectual disabilities have a higher percentage of perseverative responses on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test compared to matched controls with learning disabilities3 and typically developing controls,35,36 indicating a deficit in cognitive flexibility regardless of IQ.6,24 Although the relationship between performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and IQ is unclear in the typical population,16,37-44 performance on this test has been found to be related to both age and education level.16,37,45,46 High-functioning individuals with autism, however, perform poorly on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test with increased perseverative errors despite controlling for age, education level, and IQ.47 Furthermore, performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, as reflected by the number of perseverative errors, appears to be related to the severity of clinical features,35 which indicates difficulties in flexibly applying categorical concepts. Overall, the executive functioning deficits that have consistently been reported in autism provide evidence of frontal lobe in this disorder.48

Furthermore, there is substantial evidence regarding the role of the prefrontal cortex and its connections to executive functioning.49 After two decades of volumetric MRI studies, the consensus appears to be that there is a general increase in cortical gray matter volume with a rostral caudal gradient that is manifest by about 9 months of postnatal age and persists until about 4-6 years of age.50 Neuroimaging studies of the frontal lobe in autism have reported several abnormalities implicating this structure in the pathophysiology of this disorder. A review of various anatomical studies by Brambilla et al.51 indicates conflicting results among multiple studies of the frontal lobe with some investigations,52-59 but not all,60-63 reporting increase in volume. Varying results across these studies are most likely due to methodological differences, particularly related to the differing age of subjects studied, suggesting the presence of developmental effect. In contrast, functional imaging studies have been more consistent in their findings, perhaps because they are confined to older, more cooperative subjects. These investigations have frequently implicated underconnectivity of cerebral cortical structures, particularly the frontal cortex, and underlying cognitive impairments observed in autism.64-72 In fact, a recent study reported abnormal activation in the frontal lobe in individuals with autism while performing Tower of Hanoi executive functioning tasks.66 These studies have suggested that there is a lower level of functional coordination among brain areas in autism48,73 and warrant further investigations of the size of the frontal lobe and its subdivisions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, using large sample sizes.

The purpose of this study was to examine performance on executive function tasks, the size of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the relationship between these measures in a group with autism and age-, gender-, and IQ-matched healthy controls. We hypothesized that structural abnormalities would be observed in the patient group and differences in executive function abilities would be found between the two groups. We also predicted the existence of a relationship between structural alterations and performance on the neuropsychological tests.

Methods

Participants

Subjects were 38 individuals with high-functioning autism and 40 age-, gender-, and IQ-matched controls between the ages of 8 and 45 years. The sample was restricted to individuals with autism without mental retardation (e.g., Full-Scale, Performance, and Verbal IQ scores ≥ 80) (hereafter referred to as having High Functioning Autism) to ensure cooperation for scanning and cognitive testing, matching to normal controls, and low likelihood of associated disorders. All participants in both groups were prescreened for history of metal, claustrophobia, or weight ≥ 114 kg. All participants in both groups were administered the age-appropriate version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised or Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised to measure Full-Scale, Performance, and Verbal IQ. All autism participants and most controls were recruited through the Subject Core of the University of Pittsburgh Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (University of Pittsburgh- Carnegie Mellon University CPEA) funded by the National Institutes of Health. The methodology of the study, including MRI for minors, was approved by the institutional review boards at the University of Pittsburgh and Carnegie Mellon University. Procedures were fully explained to all subjects and, when appropriate, to their parent or legal guardian. Written informed consent was obtained from subjects and/or their guardians.

Subjects with autism represented all consecutive community referrals to a research clinic who met the criteria for participation in the study. The diagnosis of autism was established through expert clinical evaluation in accordance with published clinical descriptions of high-functioning individuals with autism74 and two structured research diagnostic instruments, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised 75 and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.76 The individuals with High Functioning Autism, recruited from autism conferences and parent support groups, were medically healthy and had no identifiable genetic, metabolic, or infectious etiology for their disorder. Their personal and family health histories were evaluated in the initial screening interview and in the medical review portion of the Autism Diagnostic Interview 75. All participants with autism met all criteria on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule76 for autism, e.g., for Communication (cutoff, 3; range, 3–7), Reciprocal Social Interaction (cutoff, 6; range, 7–13) and Total (cutoff, 10; range, 10–18) algorithm scores. Additionally, these participants met autism criteria on the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised75 including age of onset. The diagnosis of autism was confirmed by expert opinion (N.J.M.). Additionally, subjects meeting the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule criteria for autism but without delayed or abnormal language development were considered to have Asperger’s disorder and were excluded from this study.

Controls were recruited from the community through advertisements in areas socio-economically comparable to those of the families of origin of the subjects with autistic disorder and were chosen to individually match the autism participants. All subjects were medically healthy and had a Wechsler Full-Scale and Verbal IQ of 80 or higher. Candidates were prescreened with questionnaires regarding current/past personal and family history of medical/neurological/psychiatric disorders. Inclusion criteria were good physical health, no regular CNS medications, good school/job record, and good peer relationships based on parent- or self-report and staff observations during eligibility testing.

Potential control and autistic subjects were excluded if found to have evidence of an associated infectious, genetic, or metabolic disorder (e.g., fragile-X syndrome or tuberous sclerosis), birth asphyxia, head injury, or a seizure disorder. Exclusions were based on neurologic history and examination, physical examination, and chromosomal analysis or, metabolic testing if indicated. Potential control subjects were also screened to exclude those with a family history of autism in first-, second-, or third-degree relatives; developmental cognitive disorder; learning disability; schizophrenia or obsessive-compulsive disorder, or other neurologic or psychiatric disorders thought to have a genetic component, including first-degree family history of developmental cognitive disorders or mood/anxiety disorders (other than a single episode of situational depression in one first-degree relative). The socioeconomic status of the family of origin was assessed using the Hollingshead method.77

Procedures

Neuropsychological Battery

All participants were given a battery of neuropsychological tests, including the Tower of Hanoi 14 and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.15 Performances on these tests were correlated with frontal structural measurement.

Tower of Hanoi

The Tower of Hanoi assesses the ability to plan and decide which steps are necessary to complete a desired goal. The participant is presented with five rings of different sizes on one of three pegs that appear on a computer screen. The participant’s goal is to move all five rings from the first peg to the third empty peg by moving one ring at a time and by not placing a larger ring on top of a smaller ring in the fewest moves possible.14

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test assesses the capacity for identifying inherent rules and flexibly changing from one rule to another in response to environmental cues. The participant is presented with a deck of cards and four stimulus cards. The participant then has to decide how to sort and match each card in the deck to one of the four stimulus cards either by color, number, or shape while the criterion continually changes.15,16

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Neuroimaging scans were obtained using a General Electric (Milwaukee) 1.5-T Tesla Signa scanner. The imaging protocol consisted of two T1-weighted (TR=500, TE=20) series: a sagittal series of 3mm slice thickness parallel to the midline structure, and an axial series of 5 mm slice thickness. An additional 1.5 mm SPGR (spoiled gradient recalled echo in steady state) coronal series (TR = 35; TE = 5, NEX = 1, flip angle = 45°) was collected, which was used for all the measurements reported in this study.78 All images were transferred from the acquisition facility to the image analysis laboratory via File Transfer Protocol and archived on CD-ROM disks. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were identified by scan number in order to retain blindness.

Tracing Guidelines

Dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. The tracing of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was done using Brain Research Analysis of Images, Networks, and Systems 2 (BRAINS2, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA) software package.79 Measurements were done on coronal slices while observing the outline of the tracings on axial and sagittal sections. The boundaries were determined initially on the coronal slice and confirmed by its correspondence to the anatomical landmark on axial and sagittal sections. The tracing started posteriorly at the posterior end of the genu of the corpus callosum one slice anterior to the point where septum pellucidum disappears. The inferior limit was the Sylvian fissure posteriorly and horizontal ramus of the lateral fissure anteriorly, in order to exclude Broadman areas 47, 45 and 44. The superior delimitation was superior frontal sulcus from the posterior most extent of the tracing to the anterior tip of the genu of the corpus callosum; from the anterior end of the genu, the superior boundary was the interhemispheric fissure that included the superior frontal gyrus until the anterior end of the tracing. This was done in order to exclude Brodmann area 8 as much as possible. Anteriorly, the tracing ended at the tip of the horizontal ramus of the lateral fissure so that most of area 10 could be excluded. Medially, the tracings were divided into the part of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that lies along the extent of the genu of the corpus callosum where the inferior tracings were extended to the tip of the lateral fissure/ horizontal ramus, while the superior tracing extended to the very depths of the superior frontal sulcus and the medial most points were connected. Anterior to the tip of the genu, while the guideline for the inferior tracing remained the same, the superior tracing extended up to the cingulate sulcus along the interhemispheric sulcus and the medial-most points were connected. Wherever paracingulate sulcus was detected, the supero-medial tracing extended up to the paracingulate sulcus only.

Total Brain Volume. Measurements were made on a Gateway 2000 graphics workstation (N. Sioux City, SD) using locally developed custom graphics software.79 A semi-automated thresholding procedure was used for segmenting brain from cerebrospinal fluid and extra-cerebral tissue, as described elsewhere.80 Measurements were performed blind to diagnosis. Intra-rater reliability for obtaining brain volumes with this procedure yielded an intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.99 on 10 brains. Since two different programs were used to conduct the morphometric studies, total brain volume measurements were obtained from 10 scans using both software and revealed high reliability between the two programs (0.95) and acceptable intraclass correlation coefficient (R=0.85).

Data Analysis

A two-tailed statistical significance level was set at p<0.05 for all analyses. All volumetric measurements and frontal lobe test performances from participants with autism were compared with controls using Student’s t-test. An analysis of covariance was used to compare the two groups on structure volumes while controlling for total brain volume. Regression analyses were conducted to examine the relationships between the two groups and performance on the Tower of Hanoi and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Results

The demographic characteristics of the total sample were previously published.81 Neuropsychological tests and good quality MRI scans were available on subgroups of the original sample. The subgroups demographic variables remained unchanged with no differences between the autism group and controls on any of the characteristics (age, gender, socioeconomic status, and Full Scale IQ). Summary of the demographic of the different subgroups are included in Table 1 and 2. On the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, individuals with autism committed significantly more total errors and preservative errors when compared to controls (Table 1). Participants with autism had difficulties on this test and several were not able to complete the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test because of perseveration. On the Tower of Hanoi, individuals with autism performed significantly more slowly than the healthy control group and required significantly more moves to achieve a solution (Table 1). However, no volumetric differences were found in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex structures between the two groups before and after controlling for total brain volume (Table 2). No relationships between performance on the neuropsychological tests and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume were observed.

Table 1.

Performance on Wisconsin cart sorting test and tower of Hanoi task in autism and controls:

WCST1 Autism
N=24
Mean
SD Controls
N=38
Mean
SD t df p
TOTERR 30.04 18.733 19.18 15.691 2.461 60 .017
PERSERR 15.38 9.969 8.74 6.717 3.136 60 .003
NPERSERR 14.67 10.520 10.45 10.093 1.577 60 .120

TOH2 N = 37
Mean
SD N = 38
Mean
SD t df p

T1 Move 48.76 26.665 36.74 22.495 2.14 73 .036
T1 Time 240.3 266.487 137.56 100.002 2.247 73 .028
T2 Move 41.27 28.276 27.21 12.218 2.84 73 .006
T2 Time 197.73 186.184 105.97 45.251 2.987 73 .004
T3 Move 37.32 21.951 23.62 11.35 3.446 73 .001
T3 Time 163.89 152.224 90.36 33.44 2.944 73 .004
T4 Move 35.89 20.386 25.33 13.923 2.649 73 .010
T4 Time 141.00 105.923 89.24 48.69 2.731 73 .008
Total Move 163.38 61.337 112.9 44.515 4.122 73 .045

WCST: Wisconsin card sorting test; 1. Sample characteristics, FSIQ: autism=104 ± 15, controls =104 ± 10; age: autism=17.9 ± 10 years, controls =18.6 ± 9 years; gender: autism= 2 females, controls =2 females; socioeconomic status: autism=3.7 ± 1.4, controls =3.5 ± 1.2; TOH: tower of Hanoi; 2. Sample characteristics, FSIQ: autism=104 ± 15, controls =104 ± 10; age: autism=19.1 ± 9 years, controls =18.8 ± 9 years; gender: autism= 2 females, controls =2 females; socioeconomic status: autism=3.8 ± 1.5, controls =3.5 ± 1.2; TOTERR: total numbers of errors; PERSERR: perseverative errors; NPERSERR: non perseverative errors.

Table 2.

Volumetric Measurements for Grey and White Matter DLPFC and Frontal Lobe

Autism Control TBV as covariate

DLPFC1 n = 33
Mean
SD n = 37
Mean
SD t P F P
TBV 1315.4 124.5 1349.8 136.5 1.155 .726 .124 .252
DLPC Grey 27.85 8.08 26.22 8.45 .873 .388 .343 .560
DLPFC White 13.58 4.55 12.89 4.32 .688 .999 .078 .781
Total DLPFC 41.44 12.22 39.11 12.56 .828 .441 .245 .622

DLPFC: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 1: Sample characteristics, FSIQ: autism=103 ± 14, controls =104 ± 10; age: autism=19.8 ± 9 years, controls =18.3 ± 8 years; gender: autism= 2 females, controls =2 females; socioeconomic status: autism=3.8 ± 1.6, controls =3.6 ± 1.2; TBV: total brain volume.

Discussion

In the present investigation, the autism group was found to have evidence of executive functioning deficits based on their performance on neuropsychological tests commonly used to characterize executive functioning by assessing rule learning, flexibility, and concept formation. Participants with autism were generally able to identify rules inherent in a task, but had difficulties with cognitive flexibility and with forming concepts. Interestingly, no volumetric alterations in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex were observed between the autism and control groups, and no associations were found between the frontal lobe volume and performance on the neuropsychological measures in either group.

Findings from the cognitive testing in the present study are consistent with previous investigations reporting deficits in executive functioning in children and adults with autism and implicating these abnormalities in characteristic behaviors in autism.3-5,8-10 These results are concordant with findings from an article reviewing the literature by Pennington and Ozonoff,7 which found that in 13 out of 14 studies deficits in at least one measure of executive functioning were observed in subjects with autism, and in 25 out of 32 executive functioning tasks, individuals with autism displayed deficits when compared to controls. However, it is important to note that these deficits have also been reported in several other neuropsychiatric disorders including schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),30,82,83 suggesting that executive functioning deficits are not specific to autism. Interestingly, emerging evidence examining attention abnormalities in several neuropsychiatric disorders suggest that executive functioning abnormalities differ among these disorders with lack of deficits in sustained attention in autism.84 Therefore, further investigations examining the nature of these deficits and the underlying neural networks are warranted to determine whether or not executive functioning deficits, as observed in different disorders, share the same neurobiological underpinnings.

Despite the deficits in executive functioning, no volumetric alterations were found in the present investigations of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a brain region within the frontal lobe well-known to be related to mental planning and cognitive flexibility. Additionally, no relationships were observed between volumetric measurements and performance on cognitive tasks. The absence of volumetric alterations observed here is consistent with one previous study describing an increase in the parietal, occipital and temporal but not the frontal lobes in autism.86 However, our findings are not concordant with several other investigations reporting structural abnormalities in the frontal lobe in individuals with autism.86-89 Increased total frontal lobe volume was found in a study of young children with autism which was related to the enlargement of grey and white matter in the dorsolateral and medial frontal cortices.86,88 Consistent with this finding, increased grey matter volume was observed in a sample of 21 medication-naive children with autism between 7 and 15 years of age when compared to 21 gender-, age-, and IQ-matched controls.87 In contrast, a decrease in frontal lobe parenchyma was observed in a recent study examining this structure in 9 individuals with autism between 29 and 47 years of age compared to matched controls.89 The discrepancies are most likely related to differences in the morphometric methodology used and to differences in the samples’ characteristics included in each study.

This study is limited by the relatively small number though carefully diagnosed subjects with autism who span a narrow IQ range but wide age range. While it is possible that a much larger number of subjects might reveal differences between the groups on volumetric measures or correlations between frontal volume and executive function, the existing structural MRI literature in autism provides substantial evidence that gross changes in brain size are a function of age, and are most prominent in the first few years of life in autism.50 There is also substantial accumulated evidence to document rather wide-spread abnormalities in the functional connectivity of frontal cortex predominantly in subjects over the age of 10 years. Collectively, these studies strongly suggest that the neural basis of the cognitive dysfunction in this developmental disorder will not be found in gross structural changes as it is in acquired brain injury. More detailed investigation of local and distant functional and structural connectivity of cortex as a function of both age and gene status will likely provide the greatest insight into the neurobiologic basis of cognitive dysfunction in autism.90-92

Conclusions

Findings from this study suggest that the executive functioning deficits observed in this sample of children and adults with autism are probably not associated with gross anatomical abnormalities of the frontal lobe. However, they are consistent with the numerous fMRI studies demonstrating reduced functional connectivity of frontal cortex with other cortical and subcortical regions during a variety of tasks. Conclusions from this investigation are limited by the small sample size and should be considered with caution in light of its methodological limitations such as the broad age range of the sample, the exclusion of lower functioning individuals, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex measurement methodology, and the lack of adjustment of the p values due to the multiple comparisons. Therefore, additional research is needed to examine the frontal lobe in autism while using multimodal imaging techniques (structural MRI, diffusion tensor imaging, functional MRI, and magnetic resonance spectroscopy) that include the capacity to assess different aspects of connectivity and molecular pathophysiology in autism. These strategies will lead to a better understanding of the relationships between executive functioning deficits and brain structure and function, including the neural network connections that underlie these cognitive abnormalities critical to adaptive function.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by NIMH grants MH 083972 and 64027 to Dr. Hardan and NICHD grant HD 055748 to Dr. Minshew. This study was also supported by an NICHD Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (CPEA).

From the University of Pittsburgh-Carnegie Mellon University Autism Center of Excellence and the Autism and Developmental Disabilities Research Program at Stanford University. This work was completed at Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, Pittsburgh, PA, USA

Footnotes

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose with regard to this article.

Contributor Information

Jessica Griebling, Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, grieblingj@upmc.edu.

Nancy J. Minshew, Departments of Psychiatry & Neurology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, minshewnj@upmc.edu.

Kimberly Bodner, University of Pittsburgh Autism Center of Excellence, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, bodnerke@upmc.edu.

Robin Libove, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, rlibove@stanford.edu.

Rahul Bansal, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University, rbansal@med.wayne.edu.

Prasad Konasale, Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, prasadkm@upmc.edu.

Matcheri S. Keshavan, Department of Psychiatry, Beth Israel and Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, mkeshava@bidmc.harvard.edu.

Antonio Hardan, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, hardanay@stanford.edu.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. text revision. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Seigel DJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1997;3:303–316. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ozonoff S, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32:1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pennington BF. Dimensions of executive functions in normal, abnormal development. In: Krasnegor N, Lyon G, Goldman-Rakic P, editors. Development of the prefrontal cortex. Baltimore: Paul Brooks; 1997. pp. 265–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Russell J. Autism as an Executive Disorder. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ozonoff S. Components of executive function in autism and other disorders. In: Russell J, editor. Autism as an Executive Disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997. pp. 179–211. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37:51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Christ SE, Holt DD, White DA, Green L. Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1155–1165. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0259-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McGonigle-Chalmers M, Bodner K, Fox-Pitt A, Nicholson L. Size sequencing as a window on executive functioning in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1382–1390. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0396-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Joseph R. Neuropsychological frameworks for understanding autism. Int Rev Psychiatry. 1999;11:309–325. doi: 10.1080/09540269974195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Liss M, Fein D, Feinstein C, et al. Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:261–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rinehart NJ, Bellgrove MA, Tonge BJ, et al. An examination of movement kinematics in young people with high-functioning autism and asperger’s disorder: Further evidence for a motor planning deficit. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:757–767. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0118-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Solomon M, Ozonoff SJ, Cummings N, Carter CS. Cognitive control in autism spectrum disorders. Int J Devl Neuroscience. 2008;26:239–247. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Borys SV, Spitz HH, Dorans BA. Tower of Hanoi performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children as a function solution length and goal state. J Exp Child Psychol. 1982;33:87–110. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(82)90008-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Grant DA, Berg EA. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Newman SD, Carpenter PA, Varma S, Just MA. Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1668–1682. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00091-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hill F. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cog Sci. 2004a;8:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hill F. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Dev Rev. 2004b;24:189–233. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ozonoff S, McEvoy RE. A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Dev Psychopathol. 1994;6:415–3. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Prior MR, Hoffmann W. Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990;20:581–590. doi: 10.1007/BF02216063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hughes C, Russell J, Robbins TW. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:477–92. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90092-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barnard L, Muldoon K, Hasan R, et al. Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism. 2008;12:125–141. doi: 10.1177/1362361307088486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Geurts HM, Verte S, Oosterlaan J, et al. How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:836–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00276.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ozonoff S, Rogers S, Pennington BF. Asperger’s syndrome: Evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32:1107–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00352.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Minshew NJ, Goldstein G. The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:1095–1101. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Renner P, Klinger LG, Klinger MR. Implicit and explicit memory in autism: is autism an amnesic disorder? J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:3–14. doi: 10.1023/a:1005487009889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in children autism: further evidence for disordered complex information processing. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12:279–298. doi: 10.1080/09297040600681190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bennetto L, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996;67:1816–1835. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ozonoff S, Jensen J. Brief report: Specific executive functioning profiles in three Neurodevelopmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:171–177. doi: 10.1023/a:1023052913110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rumsey JM. Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. J Autism Dev Disord. 1985;15:23–36. doi: 10.1007/BF01837896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD. Neuropsychological findings in high functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1988;10:201–221. doi: 10.1080/01688638808408236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD. Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990;20:155–68. doi: 10.1007/BF02284715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Szatmari P, Tuff L, Finlayson MAJ, Bartolucci G. Asperger’s syndrome and autism: Neurocognitive aspects. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;29:130–6. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199001000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ozonoff S. Executive functions in autism. In: Schopler E, Mesibov GB, editors. Learning and cognition in autism (Current issues in autism) New York: Plenum Press; 1995. pp. 199–219. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ozonoff S, Cook I, Coon H, et al. Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:139–50. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022605.81989.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Boone KB, Ghafferian S, Lessner IM, et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in healthy, older adults: Relationship to age, sex, education, and IQ. J Clin Psychol. 1993;49:54–60. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199301)49:1<54::aid-jclp2270490108>3.0.co;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Welsh MC, Pennington BF, Grossier DB. A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Dev Neuropsychol. 1991;7:131–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Arffa S, Lovell M, Podell K, Goldberg E. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in above average and superior school children: Relationship to intelligence and age. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13:713–720. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Klonoff H, Low M. Disordered brain function in young children and early adolescents: Neuropsychological and electroencephalographic correlates. In: Reitan RM, Davison LA, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and Applications. Washington, DC: Winston; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Parsons EJC. An investigation of appropriate neuropsychological assessment procedures with adults in the higher intelligence ranges. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1984;45:1592B. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Riccio CA, Hall J, Morgan A, et al. Executive function and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Relationship with behavioral ratings and cognitive ability. Dev Neuropsychol. 1994;10:215–229. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Romine CB, Lee D, Wolfe ME, et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test with children: A meta-analytic study of sensitivity and specificity. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19:1027–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Seidenberg M, Giordani B, Berent S, Boll T. IQ level and performance of the Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery for older children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983;51:406–413. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.51.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Chelune GJ, Baer RA. Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1986;8:219–228. doi: 10.1080/01688638608401314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Levin HS, Culhane KA, Hartmann J, et al. Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning. Dev Neuropsychol. 1991;7:377–395. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Minshew NJ, Meyer J, Goldstein G. Abstract reasoning in autism: a dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:327–334. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Just MA, Cherkassky V, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004;127:1811–1821. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Goldman-Rakic PS. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1988;11:137–156. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lainhart JE. Advances in autism neuroimaging research for the clinician and geneticist. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 2006;142:33–39. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Brambilla P, Hardan A, Ucelli di Nemi S, et al. Brain anatomy and development in autism: review of structural MRI studies. Brain Res Bull. 2003;61:557–569. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, et al. Links effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology. 2002;59:175–183. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.2.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bailey A, Luthert P, Bolton P, et al. Autism and megalencephaly. Lancet. 1993;341:1225–1226. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)91065-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Davidovitch M, Patterson B, Gartside P. Head circumference measurements in children with autism. J Child Neurol. 1996;11:389–393. doi: 10.1177/088307389601100509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Fidler DJ, Bailey JN, Smalley SL. Macrocephaly in autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42:737–740. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200001365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Fombonne E, Roge B, Claverie J, et al. Microcephaly and macrocephaly in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:113–119. doi: 10.1023/a:1023036509476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gillberg C, deSouza L. Head circumference in autism, Asperger syndrome, and ADHD: A comparative study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:296–300. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201002110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lainhart JE, Piven J, Wzorek M, et al. Macrocephaly in children and adults with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolescence Psychiatry. 1997;36:282–290. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Miles JH, Hadden LL, Takahashi TN, Hillman RE. Head circumference is an independent finding associated with autism. Am J Med Genet. 2000;95:339–350. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Goldstein J, et al. MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology. 1999;53:2145–2150. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.9.2145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Elia M, Ferri R, Musumeci SA, et al. Clinical correlates of brain morphometric features of subjects with low-functioning autistic disorder. J Child Neurol. 2000;15:504–508. doi: 10.1177/088307380001500802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gaffney GR, Kuperman S, Tsai LY, et al. Midsagital magnetic resonance imaging of autism. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;151:831–833. doi: 10.1192/bjp.151.6.831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Garber HJ, Ritvo ER. Magnetic resonance imaging of the posterior fossa in autistic adults. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149:245–247. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.2.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.George MS, Costa DC, Kouris K, et al. Cerebral blood flow abnormalities in adults with infantile autism. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1992;180:413–417. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199207000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Grady CL, Rapoport SI. The cerebral metabolic landscape in autism: Intercorrelations of regional glucose utilization. Arch Neurol. 1988;45:749–755. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520310055018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Just MA, Cherkassky V, Keller TA, et al. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, et al. Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain. 2006;129:2482–2493. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Koshino H, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ, et al. Functional connectivity in an fmri working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage. 2005;24:810–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Koshino H, Kana RK, Keller TA, et al. FMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:289–300. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Luna B, Minshew NJ, Garver KE, et al. Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: an fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology. 2002;59:917–922. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.6.834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Villalobos ME, Mizuno A, Dahl BC, et al. Reduced functional connectivity between V1 and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. Neuroimage. 2005;25:916–925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zilbovicius M, Garreau B, Samson Y, et al. Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:248–252. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.2.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Minshew NJ, Williams DL. The new neurobiology of autism: Cortex, connectivity, and neuronal organization. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:945–950. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.7.945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Minshew N. Autism. In: Berg BO, editor. Principles of Child Neurology. New York: McGraw Hill; 1996. pp. 1713–1729. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24:659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. Yale University, Department of Sociology; New Haven, CT: 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Patel VH, Friedman L. MRI of the Brain, Normal Anatomy, and Normal Variants. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Magnotta VA, Harris G, Andreasen NC, et al. Structural MR image processing using the BRAINS2 toolbox. Computerized Medical Imaging and Graphics. 2002;26:251–264. doi: 10.1016/s0895-6111(02)00011-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Aylward EH, Andersen NB, Bylsma FW, et al. Frontal lobe volume in patients with Huntington’s disease. Neurology. 1998;50:252–258. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.1.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hardan AY, Kilpatrick M, Keshavan MS, Minshew NJ. Motor performance and anatomic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the basal ganglia in autism. J Child Neurol. 2003;18:317–324. doi: 10.1177/08830738030180050801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Johnson KA, Kelly SP, Bellgrove MA, et al. Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:630–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Pennington BF, Welsh M. Neuropsychology and developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology. 1995. pp. 254–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Greene C, Braet W, Johnson KA, Bellgrove MA. Imaging the genetics of executive function. Biol Psychol. 2008;79:30–42. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Andreasen N. Regional brain enlargement in autism: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescence Psychiatry. 1996;35:530–536. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199604000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Carper RA, Moses P, Tigue ZD, Courchesne E. Cerebral lobes in autism: early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. Neuroimage. 2002;16:1038–1051. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Palmen SJMC, van Engeland H, Hof PR, Schmitz C. Neuropsychological findings in autism. Brain. 2004;127:2572–2583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Carper RA, Courchesne E. Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Schmitz N, Daly E, Murphy D. Frontal anatomy and reaction time in autism. Neuroscience Letters. 2007;412:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Wassink TH, Hazlett HC, Epping EA, et al. Cerebral cortical gray matter overgrowth and functional variation of the serotonin transporter gene in autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:709–717. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Conturo TE, Williams DL, Smith CD, et al. Neuronal fiber pathway abnormalities in autism: An Initial MRI diffusion tensor tracking study of hippocampo-fusiform and amygdalo-fusiform pathways. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14:933–946. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH. Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nature Reviews. 9:341–355. doi: 10.1038/nrg2346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES