Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Aug 29.
Published in final edited form as: Ear Hear. 2000 Feb;21(1):70–78. doi: 10.1097/00003446-200002000-00010

Cognitive Factors and Cochlear Implants: Some Thoughts on Perception, Learning, and Memory in Speech Perception

David B Pisoni 1
PMCID: PMC3429935  NIHMSID: NIHMS400228  PMID: 10708075

Abstract

Over the past few years, there has been increased interest in studying some of the cognitive factors that affect speech perception performance of cochlear implant patients. In this paper, I provide a brief theoretical overview of the fundamental assumptions of the information-processing approach to cognition and discuss the role of perception, learning, and memory in speech perception and spoken language processing. The information-processing framework provides researchers and clinicians with a new way to understand the time-course of perceptual and cognitive development and the relations between perception and production of spoken language. Directions for future research using this approach are discussed including the study of individual differences, predicting success with a cochlear implant from a set of cognitive measures of performance and developing new intervention strategies.


We are now beginning to see several important changes in the direction and nature of research on cochlear implants, particularly in research on very young prelinguistically deaf children. As cochlear implants improve and speech-processing strategies become more sophisticated, many more deaf children are able to derive benefits from their implants. A large number of these children display substantial gains in speech perception, word recognition and language development (Fryauf-Bertschy, Tyler, Kelsay, & Gantz, 1992; Fryauf-Bertschy, Tyler, Kelsay, Gantz, & Woodworth, 1997; Miyamoto, Kirk, Robbins, Todd, Riley, & Pisoni, 1997; Miyamoto, Svirsky, & Robbins, 1997; Waltzman et al., 1997). And, many prelinguistically deafened children with cochlear implants somehow learn to produce intelligible speech within a year or so of implantation and appear to be well on their way to acquiring spoken language via their cochlear implant (Miyamoto et al., 1997; Robbins, Svirsky, & Kirk, 1997; Svirsky, Robbins, Kirk, Pisoni, & Miyamoto, in press). How do they accomplish this difficult task?

As performance levels on standardized audiological tests continue to improve, a number of researchers have turned their efforts to gaining a better understanding of more basic questions surrounding how cochlear implants function to facilitate cognitive and linguistic development in deaf children (Kirk, Pisoni, & Miyamoto, in press; Kirk, Pisoni, & Osberger, 1995; Miyamoto et al., 1997; Robbins et al., 1997). These new research questions deal with a variety of issues involving the perception of speech and spoken language understanding. Interest has started to shift from issues of “efficacy” of cochlear implants to questions of how deaf children encode and process information using a cochlear implant. These new research questions concern fundamental issues of human information processing and involve topics such as perception, learning, memory, attention, and language processing, research areas that have traditionally been in the mainstream of Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Science (Ashcraft, 1989; Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Crowder, 1976; Gardner, 1985; Haber, 1969).

Much of the past research on cochlear implants has been concerned with questions of assessment and device efficacy using outcome measures that were based on traditional audiological criteria. These measures included a variety of hearing tests, speech discrimination, word recognition and comprehension tests, as well as some standardized vocabulary and language assessments. The major focus of this research over the past 10 to 15 yr has been concerned with the study of demographic variables as predictors of these outcome measures. The available evidence to date demonstrates that age at onset of deafness, length of deprivation and age at implantation all play substantial roles in predicting many of the standard outcome measures (Fryauf-Bertschy et al., 1997; Kirk et al., in press; Osberger, Todd, Berry, Robbins, & Miyamoto, 1991; Staller, Pelter, Brimacombe, Mecklenberg, & Arndt, 1991; Waltzman, Cohen, Gomolin, Shapiro, Ozdaman, & Hoffman, 1994; Waltzman et al., 1997).

What happens if the demographic variables are eliminated from these research designs? What is left over to study? We suggest there are a number of “process” variables or cognitive factors that are related to learning, memory, attention and language processing that have been ignored over the years. The study of these particular psychological processes may provide new insights into how children acquire language through a cochlear implant and may offer researchers and clinicians a new way of explaining the enormous individual differences observed among prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants.

Numerous studies have shown that the nature of the child’s early sensory experience plays a significant role in predicting outcome measures. It should not come as a surprise to anyone that deaf children from “oral-only” programs do consistently better on auditory-based tests of speech perception and spoken language performance than deaf children from total communication (or TC, i.e., manually coded language plus speech) programs (Kirk, Reference Note 1; Miyamoto, Kirk, Robbins, Todd, Riley, & Pisoni, 1997; Robbins, Kirk, Osberger, & Ertmer, 1995). The study of demographic variables and the focus on traditional audiological outcome measures in these children are only a small part of the story of what is actually going on under the surface. To gain a better understanding of what these children are learning via their implant, it is necessary to approach this problem from an entirely different theoretical perspective and look more closely at the content and flow of information within the processing system and how it changes over time. The focus of research on cochlear implants in children needs to shift from an emphasis on the study of audiologically based outcome measures to the investigation of the underlying neural, psychological and linguistic processes that mediate speech perception and production.

Little, if any, of the previous research on cochlear implants has been concerned with studying what the children are learning via their implant, how they are going about the process of acquiring a grammar from the ambient language or how they are able to develop both receptive and expressive language abilities. Moreover, until recently there have been very few attempts to study the language development of children with cochlear implants and compare their linguistic knowledge and performance with normal-hearing children or with other hearing-impaired children (Miyamoto et al., 1997; Robbins & Kirk, 1996). These are important new research questions that go beyond surface issues of assessment, device efficacy, or simply predicting outcome measures; they are fundamental questions that deal with the “effectiveness” of cochlear implants outside the special conditions of the clinic or the research laboratory (Seligman, 1996). The major emphasis on assessment-based clinical research and the prediction of outcome measures appears to be changing now. Several recent papers have already reported new findings on some of these issues (Pisoni, Svirsky, Kirk, & Miyamoto, Reference Note 3; Zwolan, Zimmerman-Phillips, Asbaugh, Hieber, Kileny, & Telian, 1997) and other studies are underway now at a number of research centers.

To pursue these new research questions and to move beyond the study of demographics and the traditional issues surrounding assessment and prediction of outcome measures, it is necessary to look to other allied disciplines such as Cognitive Psychology. New experimental methods and techniques are available to study the emergence of these fundamental underlying cognitive and neural processes and how these processes change over time after implantation. Fortunately, many useful experimental procedures have already been developed by cognitive psychologists to study perception, attention, learning and memory within the framework of human information processing (Haber, 1969; Lachman, Lachman, & Butterfield, 1979; Neisser, 1967). More importantly, this approach has also provided a variety of conceptual tools for thinking about the structures and processes involved in cognitive activity and the underlying psychological phenomena (Lindsay & Norman, 1977; Reitman, 1965).

“Information processing” is a generic label that describes a broad approach to the study of complex psychological processes such as perception, cognition and thought (Haber, 1969; Neisser, 1967). Information-processing theories are concerned with an analysis of “central processes” of large complex systems (such as human cognition) used in visual object recognition, perceptual learning and memory, speech perception, and various aspects of language processing such as comprehension or speech production. A common goal of this approach is to examine the representations, elementary psychological processes and cognitive structures used in these kinds of cognitive activities and to trace out the time-course of these processing operations (Haber, 1969; Lachman et al., 1979; Sternberg, 1966, 1969).

In the sections below, I first give a very brief overview of the major theoretical assumptions of this approach to cognition. Then, I briefly examine several new directions for research on cochlear implants that are motivated by the major assumptions of the information-processing framework. I believe it may now be possible to understand and explain the large individual differences observed in children and adults with cochlear implants by studying the underlying psychological and cognitive factors and the component subsystems used in perception, attention, learning and memory. Individual differences is one of many problems that can be approached within this theoretical framework.

Overview of the Information-Processing Approach

Assumption I: Perception is Not Immediate

One of the fundamental principles of information-processing theory is that sensation, perception, memory, thought and other complex activities like language and problem solving should be viewed as representing a continuum of cognitive processing (Haber, 1969; Neisser, 1967). These activities are assumed to be mutually interdependent and cannot be divided up into separate subsystems. Furthermore, an analysis of one subsystem, such as perception, cannot take place without an appreciation and awareness of the contribution of the other major subsystems, such as memory, attention or learning (Haber, 1969; Neisser, 1967; Reitman, 1965).

The information-processing approach to cognition assumes that processing activity goes through several successive stages of analysis. One goal of information-processing theory is to specify the component operations that occur between the presentation of a stimulus and the response of the observer. The processing stages between input and output are typically represented by a flow chart with structures and processes organized in a block diagram. The flow of information is marked by arrows connecting these structures (Haber, 1969).

These hypothesized processing stages also take time. It is assumed that processing times reflect distinct operations that occur at each stage (Baddeley, 1986). By looking at the correlations between the contents of the stimulus and the contents of the observer’s response at various times after stimulation, insights can be gained about the flow of information within the system and the nature of the psychological operations that are carried out at each stage of processing (Haber, 1969; Neisser, 1967; Reitman, 1965).

Finally, the information-processing approach assumes that psychological processes such as sensation, perception, attention, learning and memory are organized hierarchically. More complex cognitive processes that occur later in the flow of information are critically dependent on earlier more elementary psychological processes (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977).

Assumption II: Capacity Limitations on Processing Information

A second principle of information-processing theory is that the human observer has finite information-processing capabilities and displays severe channel capacity limitations in a variety of tasks (Broadbent, 1958; Miller, 1956; Waugh & Norman, 1965). Years ago when this approach was first developed, it was believed that the nervous system was not large enough to maintain all aspects of stimulation permanently and that raw sensory information needed to be transformed, reduced and recoded into a more efficient symbolic form for storage in memory (Neisser, 1967). A central problem in information-processing theory is identifying the “locus” of where recoding takes place in the processing system and describing the nature of these processing operations (Kahnman, 1973; Shiffrin, 1988). Thus, research in cognitive psychology on topics such as selective attention (Cherry, 1953; Lindsay & Norman, 1977; Shiffrin, 1988) and immediate memory span (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Miller, 1956; Waugh & Norman, 1965) demonstrate that not all aspects of the stimulus environment are encoded or processed by the nervous system or stored in permanent memory for later retrieval (Cowan, 1988; Navon & Gopher, 1979). Understanding the process of “information reduction” and “recoding” by the nervous system has been a long-standing problem that cuts across several domains including perception, attention, learning and categorization (Baddeley, 1990; Posner, 1969).

Assumption III: Commonality of Perception and Memory

A third principle of in formation-processing theory is that all aspects of cognitive activity—ranging from sensation and perception to learning and thought—involve some kind of storage or memory system that preserves selected aspects of the initial sensory stimulation (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Lindsay & Norman, 1977; Posner, 1969; Waugh & Norman, 1965). Thus, the nature of the neural representations of the stimulus in memory and the organization of this information is a fundamental problem in all information-processing analyses. The operations involved in encoding, rehearsal, storage and retrieval of information occur at all stages of processing (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968). This theoretical approach also assumes that it is not possible to separate the processes that support perception from those that, support memory. The two processes are mutually dependent. Memory is therefore one of the central problems to be studied in understanding psychological activities within information-processing theory. Encoding, storage, retrieval and rehearsal of the stimulus input all take place within a common system (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Baddeley, 1986, 1990; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Posner & Mitchell, 1967).

Goals of Information-Processing Approach

The information-processing approach is concerned primarily with the “central” cognitive processes used in large systems to carry out complex activities such as perception, learning, memory, language processing and problem solving. The goals of this approach follow from the three principles described earlier. First, information-processing theory focuses research on describing the sequence of operations, or “stages of processing,” used in a particular task. Second, this approach is concerned with identifying the “locus” of capacity limitations in processing information. Third, this approach attempts to trace the “time-course” of perceptual processing from the stimulus input that impinges on the nervous system to the observer’s overt response. Researchers working within the information-processing approach attempt to construct process models and computer simulations of various subsystems to formalize and make precise quantitative statements about their performance (Hunt, 1978; Lindsay & Norman, 1977; McClelland & Elman, 1986; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1986; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986). These models provide detailed explanations of phenomena and make explicit predictions. Finally, over the past few years, researchers working within this general framework have tried to establish the neural plausibility of their models (e.g., McClelland & Rumelhart, 1986; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986). In the past, most cognitive psychologists expressed little, if any, interest in brain modeling. Their concern was with the flow of abstract symbolic information within the information-processing system and with the correlations between stimulus and response at various points in time after stimulation terminated. This emphasis is changing now as new concepts and techniques from neural networks and connectionist models become available to permit the construction of “neurally inspired” models that have some relationship to what is currently known about the nervous system and brain function.

Methodology of Converging Operations

An important methodological principle of the information-processing approach is the use of “converging operations” (Garner, Hake, & Eriksen, 1956; Haber, 1969), The goal here is to obtain data on the flow and content of information within the processing system using a variety of experimental techniques and research designs and to search for commonalties across different tasks. It is also assumed that the processing activities at different stages can be revealed by two general measures of an observer’s behavior: the accuracy of performance and the time required to perform a given task (Hunt, 1978; Lachman et al, 1979; Sternberg, 1966, 1969). Both measures, response accuracy and response latency, have a long history in the field of experimental psychology and both measures have provided a great deal of valuable information about the component elementary psychological operations used to carry out a particular task. Researchers have also examined the incorrect responses of subjects in a variety of experimental paradigms to analyze error patterns. Errors are often quite systematic and provide new insights into the way human perceivers use partial or degraded stimulus information to structure their responses. All of these measures provide valuable new information about the underlying cognitive processes being studied.

Some New Directions on Cochlear Implants in Children

Language Development versus Hearing

The bulk of the research on cochlear implants has been carried out by audiologists and hearing scientists who have been concerned with the sensory coding of speech by the peripheral nervous system (Boothroyd, 1997a, 1997b). Only recently have researchers begun to examine the effects of cochlear implants on specific aspects of language development. One very important area of research on language development concerns the nature of the child’s phonological system, which encodes and represents the inventory of sounds the child has acquired and the rules used to produce the sound contrasts of the ambient language (Chin & Kirk, in press; Chin & Pisoni, in press).

For a child to produce intelligible speech, he must have an organized system for encoding and representing sound patterns in memory, a phonology, and a set of procedures to translate these phonological representations and rules into sensory-motor commands and gestures that can control the vocal tract and articulators in speech production. It is generally assumed that the child has one common phonological system of representation that is used for both speech perception and speech production. Unfortunately, very little is currently known about the phonological systems of prelinguistically deaf children with cochlear implants. Research on this topic has not generated much interest among audiologists, who have been concerned primarily with hearing and receptive language functions. Only recently have clinical phonologists begun to study this problem in deaf children with cochlear implants (Chin & Pisoni, in press). Recent findings indicate that deaf children with cochlear implants display several commonalties with normal-hearing children in terms of their inventory of sounds and the patterning of these sounds in production.

The same situation is also true for the study of spoken word recognition and lexical access, two subcomponents of the language comprehension system. Little, if any, research has been done on the organization of the child’s developing lexicon or the nature of the lexical representations of words that are constructed by deaf children with cochlear implants (see, however, Kirk, Reference Note 1; Kirk et al., 1995; Kirk, Eisenberg, Martinez, & Hay-McCutcheon, 1999). Some recent evidence suggests that deaf children with cochlear implants perceive and represent spoken words in terms of broad phonetic categories or functional equivalence classes that reflect their inability to reliably discriminate fine phonetic differences in place and voicing (Pisoni et al., Reference Note 3). Difficulty in perceiving and encoding phonetic distinctions among sound patterns would in all likelihood influence the organization and structural arrangement of words in the lexicon (Chin, Meyer, Hay-McCutcheon, Wright, & Pisoni, in press), and would no doubt produce parallel changes in speech and language production as well. These are two topics that are being explored in our research laboratory.

At the present time, very little research has been carried out on morphological and syntactic development in deaf children with cochlear implants. This is a critical area of language development that needs to be investigated in greater depth to determine how children acquire abstract linguistic knowledge about the grammar of the target language through a processing device that presents their nervous system with a highly degraded and impoverished electrical signal. The key question here is whether deaf children can acquire the full range of morphological contrasts and structural regularities of English when they can only encode and represent the sound contrasts of the language in terms of broad manner classes. What kind of morphological system will these children actually come up with and how is their system different from the system normal-hearing children develop? This is an interesting and important research problem in language development because it deals with the interface between phonology and syntax.

Perception and Production

Historically, the study of speech perception and production developed independently of each other. In deaf children who have received cochlear implants, it may be necessary to study the development of both processes together to gain insights into the underlying linguistic system of the child. Recent studies have reported unusually high correlations between open-set word recognition scores and measures of speech intelligibility (Pisoni et al., Reference Note 3; Svirsky et al., in press). We need to learn more about the child’s acquisition and use of phonology. Specifically, we want to know whether these phonological systems reflect language universals or coding limitations of the cochlear implant regarding certain phonetic features of the speech signal like place and voicing. Recent findings suggest a common source of variance underlying word recognition and speech intelligibility that involves the phonological representations of words and the mapping of sound patterns onto meanings in memory (Pisoni et al., Reference Note 3). Whatever linguistic skills or abilities these children employ in recognizing words in isolation (i.e., without any context or retrieval cues) also appear to he recruited in speech production. The pattern of intercorrelations among measures of perception and production that we found in our recent analysis of the exceptionally good users—the “Stars”—suggests a common underlying representational system for phonological knowledge in memory.

Multimodal Speech Perception

Although many researchers and theorists have traditionally viewed speech perception and spoken language processing as purely acoustic or auditory processes, recent findings on multimodal speech perception have provided many reliable demonstrations of the visual/optical correlates for speech perception as represented in the dynamic changes in the talker’s face and lips (Massaro, 1998). The topic of multimodal perception has many implications for the hearing-impaired because these perceivers often rely heavily on information in the optical display of a talker’s face as an aid to speech perception. If speech is viewed more broadly within the theoretical framework of event perception as a perceptual system having both acoustic and optical correlates (Auer & Bernstein, 1997; Gaver, 1993), then our view of the task confronting the perceiver must be modified accordingly to fully acknowledge the multimodal properties of speech and the lawful complementary relations between auditory and visual speech cues. Normal-hearing listeners often have difficulty dissociating these two sensory inputs and normally respond in ways suggesting an integrated perceptual pattern (McGurk & MacDonald, 1976; Sumby & Pollack, 1954).

These observations about multimodal speech perception are relevant to several recent findings showing interference and inhibition effects of manual communication skills in TC children who are learning oral language via their cochlear implant. In TC children, knowledge and use of manual language apparently competes with the dominant mode of processing speech via the auditory modality. Differences in input modality between sign language and speech make it difficult for TC children to integrate common gestural information across the two sensory modalities. This mismatch increases the processing load on working memory that is assumed to play a major role in language comprehension and word recognition (Baddeley, Gathercole & Papagno, 1998). Thus, TC children generally show lower scores on a variety of assessments that measure auditory-only or auditory + visual language processing skills.

Perceptual and Cognitive Development

Many basic questions about perceptual and cognitive development have not yet been studied in deaf children with cochlear implants. At this time, we know very little about the perceptual learning abilities of these children or how auditory and visual attention is shaped and modified by awareness of sound and perception of speech after long periods of sensory deprivation (Lenneberg, 1967). Almost no research has been done on perceptual learning, categorization or concept development in these children. Similarly, we know almost nothing about their working memory systems, a key factor in acquiring new words and producing spoken language using phonological knowledge previously stored in memory (Baddeley et at, 1998; Pisoni & Geers, Reference Note 2). Finally, we currently have no systematic knowledge or information about the metalinguistic abilities of deaf children with cochlear implants. It seems reasonable at this point to wonder if the children who have acquired some rudimentary language skills via their implant also have explicit metalinguistic abilities to reason about spoken language as an abstract symbolic system. Results from the reading literature suggest that metalinguistic awareness is a strong predictor of early reading success. Is the same relationship true of deaf children with cochlear implants? We simply do not know at this time.

Looking at the “Stars”—Studies of the Exceptional Users of Cochlear Implants

The published literature on cochlear implants has consistently reported large individual differences among users. Some prelinguistically deaf children do exceptionally well with their cochlear implants and go on to acquire spoken language and produce intelligible speech. Other children, however, develop only an awareness of sound and never appear to acquire language fully or produce intelligible speech to the same degree or proficiency as the exceptionally good users. We are now just beginning to examine the performance of the exceptionally good users of cochlear implants, the so-called “Stars,” on a variety of behavioral measures including open- and closed-set speech perception tests, word recognition, and vocabulary tests, as well as expressive and receptive language development (Pisoni et al., Reference Note 3). Our analyses of the intercorrelations of these measures suggest that the “Stars” have developed a representational system, that is, a phonology and a lexicon for mapping sounds onto meanings. Their exceptionally good performance in recognizing spoken words in isolation is not only restricted to open-set word recognition tasks. The “Stars” display outstanding performance on several other tasks, all of which apparently require access to and use of words stored in the lexicon. The pattern of intercorrelations with other behavioral measures was extremely strong for the Reynell expressive and receptive language scales (Pisoni et al., Reference Note 3). These analyses revealed that the children who do well on open-set speech perception and word recognition skills also do well on other language measures. Finally, one of the most important discoveries from our analysis of the “Stars” was the high intercorrelations of the open-set word recognition tests with measures of speech intelligibility. Whatever skills, abilities or processes these children use to recognize isolated words in an open-set format, these same processes also appear to be recruited in speech production in tasks that require the child to access sensory-motor patterns from memory to imitate spoken words.

Assessment-Based versus Theory-Driven Research

The primary focus of most of the past research on cochlear implants in children has been on device efficacy and in predicting outcome measures using standardized audiological tests. Because of these clinical goals, researchers have tended to concentrate on the study of demographic variables as predictors of success with a cochlear implant. Until recently, these were the only independent variables that were included in the research designs used to study the performance of children with cochlear implants. The situation is now changing in several respects as the research questions focus in on a variety of new issues surrounding what the child is learning via the cochlear implant and how the cochlear implant works in a functional way to control behavior in a specific information-processing task.

As we noted earlier, these are new research questions that deal with psychological processing activities underlying the actual use of the cochlear implant. Fundamental questions about perception, learning, memory, attention, and language that lie outside the domain of clinical audiology or hearing Science all can be approached within the framework of information-processing theory because of its concern with describing the underlying psychological processes and mechanisms that intervene between stimulus input and response output. Viewed within this broader theoretical context, many of the difficult “central processing” issues surrounding topics such as individual differences, language development, predicting outcome performance, and the relations between speech perception and speech production can now be approached using a variety of new concepts and experimental techniques. The emphasis on demographics no longer has to be the primary focus of research on deaf children with cochlear implants. There are many other important new questions to study in this clinical population.

The shift from “assessment-based” research to “hypothesis testing” and theory-driven research represents a natural progression as researchers move from simple description and device “efficacy” questions to explanation, prediction, and “effectiveness” issues (Seligman, 1996). Fundamentally, we want to know what deaf children are learning via their cochlear implant and how they manage to accomplish the task of developing language from a highly degraded and impoverished sensory input. Answers to these basic questions about underlying psychological process may have broad implications for new approaches to processor design, intervention techniques, predicting outcome, and decision making with prelinguistically deaf children.

The findings that some deaf children with cochlear implants can perceive speech and produce spoken language is very encouraging because it demonstrates device “efficacy.” That is, cochlear implants work with some deaf children, and these children appear to acquire spoken language in spite of using a highly degraded and impoverished electrical signal. However, we do not know how this is actually accomplished in the exceptionally good children like the “Stars” nor do we know why other children have much more difficulty in reaching these goals. This is the problem of trying to predict outcome from measures obtained before or immediately after implantation. If we had some better ideas and specific hypotheses about what psychological processes and mechanisms were responsible for the exceptionally good performance of the “Stars,” it might be possible to develop new intervention techniques to accelerate the perceptual learning and language development of the “average” users of cochlear implants. It is very unlikely that changes like this would ever come about by continuing to do descriptive assessment-based research with these children using the measurement techniques from hearing science and clinical audiology. What is needed now is an integrated theoretical framework for studying information processing in these children and relating Endings on individual differences to performance on speech and language tests, the traditional outcome measures used in the field of cochlear implants.

We believe that the information-processing approach to complex psychological activities has a great deal to offer at this time. We are encouraged already by several new findings on the “Stars” who display exceptionally good performance on a wide variety of behavioral tests of speech perception and language processing. The “Stars” no longer need to be viewed as anomalies, but may instead provide important new insights into the underlying cognitive processes that are responsible for their superior performance across many different behavioral tests. The information-processing framework also provides us with new ways to study and understand the time-course of perceptual and cognitive development and the interrelations between speech perception and production in these children. We are confident that these new research findings will help us to understand the contribution of the environment and the effects of early experience on language development during the critical period when the child’s nervous system is still amenable to change.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH-NIDCD Research Grants DC0064, DC00423, and DC00111 to Indiana University. I thank Karen Iler-Kirk, Steven Chin, and Mario Svirsky for their help and advice on the ideas presented in this paper.

Footnotes

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Vth International Cochlear Implant Conference at New York University, May 1–3, 1997, New York, NY.

Reference Notes

1

Kirk, K. I. (1996). Lexical discrimination and age at implantation. A first report. Paper presented at the 131st meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Indianapolis, IN.

1

Pisoni, D. B., & Geers, A. (1998). Working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants: Correlations between digit span and measures of spoken language processing. Paper presented at the 7th Symposium on Cochlear Implants in Children, Iowa City, IA, June 1998.

1

Pisoni, D. B., Svirsky, M. A., Kirk, K. I., & Miyamoto, R. T. (1997). Looking at the “starts”: A first report on the intercorrelations among measures of speech perception, intelligibility and language in pediatric cochlear implant users. Paper presented at the Vth International Cochlear Implant Conference at New York University, May 1–3, 1997, New York, NY.

References

  1. Ashcraft M II, editor. Human Memory and Cognition. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and Company; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  2. Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence KW, Spence JT, editors. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press; 1968. pp. 89–195. [Google Scholar]
  3. Auer ET, Bernstein LE. Speechreading and the structure of the lexicon: Computationally modeling the effects of reduced phonetic distinctiveness and lexical uniqueness. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1997;102:3704–3709. doi: 10.1121/1.420402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baddeley AD. Working Memory. New York: Oxford University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley AD. HUMAN memory: Theory and Practice. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley A, Gathercole S, Papagno C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review. 1998;105:158–173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working memory. In: Bower GA, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press; 1974. pp. 47–90. [Google Scholar]
  8. Boothroyd A. Auditory development of the hearing child. Scandinavian Audiology. 1997a;26:9–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boothroyd A. Auditory capacity of hearing-impaired children using hearing aids and cochlear implants: Issues of efficacy and assessment. Scandinavian Audiology. 1997b;26:17–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broadbent DE. Perception and Communication. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1958. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cherry EC. Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1953;25:975–979. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chin SB, Kirk KI. Consonant feature production by children with multichannel cochlear implants, hearing aids, and tactile aids. In: Waltzman S, Cohen N, editors. Proceedings of the Vth International Cochlear Implant Conference. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers; (in press). [Google Scholar]
  13. Chin SB, Meyer TA, Hay-McCutcheon M, Wright GA, Pisoni DB. Understanding the structure of the mental lexicons of children who use cochlear implants. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology. doi: 10.1177/0003489400109s1249. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chin SB, Pisoni DB. A phonological system at two years after cochlear implantation. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. doi: 10.1080/026992000298940. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cowan N. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;104:163–191. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1972;11:671–684. [Google Scholar]
  17. Crowder RG. Principles of Learning and Memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  18. Fryauf-Bertschy H, Tyler RS, Kelsay D, Gantz BJ. Performance over time of congenitally and postlingually deafened children using a multichannel cochlear implant. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1992;35:913–920. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3504.913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fryauf-Bertschy H, Tyler RS, Kelsay D, Gantz BJ, Woodworth GG. Cochlear implant use by prelingually deafened children: The influences of age at implant and length of device use. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:183–199. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4001.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gardner H. The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Basic Books; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  21. Garner WR, Hake HW, Eriksen CW. Operationism and the concept of perception. Psychological Review. 1956;63:317–329. doi: 10.1037/h0042992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gaver WW. What in the world do we hear? An ecological approach to auditory event perception. Ecological Psychology. 1993;5:1–29. [Google Scholar]
  23. Haber RN, editor. Information-Processing Approaches to Visual Perception. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hunt EB. Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological Review. 1978;85:109–130. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kahnman D. Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kirk KI, Eisenberg LS, Martinez AS, Hay-McCutcheon M. Lexical neighborhood test: Test-retest reliability and interlist equivalency. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 1999;10:113–123. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Miyamoto RT. Lexical discrimination by children with cochlear implants: Effects of age at implantation and communication mode. In: Waltzman S, Cohen N, editors. Proceedings of the Vth International Cochlear Implant Conference. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers; (in press). [Google Scholar]
  28. Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Osberger MJ. Lexical effect on spoken word recognition by pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear and Hearing. 1995;16:470–481. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199510000-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lachman R, Lachman JL, Butterfield EC. Cognitive Psychology and Information Processing: An Introduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  30. Lenneberg E. Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  31. Lindsay PH, Norman DA. Human Information Processing: An Introduction to Psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  32. Massaro DW. Perceiving Talking Faces: From Speech Perception to a Behavioral Principle. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller GA. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review. 1956;63:81–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McClelland JL, Elman JL. The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology. 1986;18:1–86. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. Vol. 2: Psychological and Biological Models. Cambridge, MA: Bradford; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  36. McGurk H, MacDonald J. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature. 1976;264:746–748. doi: 10.1038/264746a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Miyamoto RT, Kirk KI, Robbins AM, Todd S, Riley A, Pisoni DB. Speech perception and speech intelligibility in children with multichannel cochlear implants. In: Honjo I, Takahashi H, editors. Cochlear Implant and Related Sciences Update. Advances in Otorhinolaryngology. Basel, Switzerland: Karger; 1997. pp. 198–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Miyamoto RT, Svirsky MA, Robbins AM. Enhancement of expressive language in prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants. Acta Otolaryngologica. 1997;117:154–157. doi: 10.3109/00016489709117758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Navon D, Gopher D. On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review. 1979;86:214–255. [Google Scholar]
  40. Neisser U. Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  41. Osberger MJ, Todd SL, Berry SW, Robbins AM, Miyamoto RT. Effect of age of onset of deafness on children’s speech perception abilities with a cochlear implant. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology. 1991;100:883–888. doi: 10.1177/000348949110001104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Posner MI. Abstraction and the process of recognition. In: Bower GH, Spence JT, editors. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press; 1969. pp. 43–100. [Google Scholar]
  43. Posner MI, Mitchell RF. Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review. 1967;74:392–409. doi: 10.1037/h0024913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Reitman WR. Cognition and Thought: An Information Processing Approach. New York: John Wiley #x00026; Sons; 1965. [Google Scholar]
  45. Robbins A, Kirk KI. Speech perception assessment and performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Seminars in Hearing. 1996;17:353–369. [Google Scholar]
  46. Robbins AM, Kirk KI, Osberger MJ. Speech intelligibility of implanted children. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology. 1995;104:399–401. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robbins A, Svirsky M, Kirk KI. Children with implants can speak, but can they communicate? Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery. 1997;117:155–160. doi: 10.1016/s0194-5998(97)70168-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rumelhart DE, McClelland JL, editors. Parallel Distributed Processing: Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  49. Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review. 1977;84:1–66. [Google Scholar]
  50. Seligman MEP. Science as an ally of practice. American Psychologist. 1996;51:1072–1088. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.51.10.1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shiffrin RM. Attention. In: Atkinson RC, Hernstein RJ, Lindzey G, Luce RD, editors. Steven’s Handbook of Experimental Psychology: Vol. 2. Learning and Cognition. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 1988. pp. 739–811. [Google Scholar]
  52. Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review. 1977;84:127–190. [Google Scholar]
  53. Staller SJ, Pelter AL, Brimacombe JA, Mecklenberg D, Arndt P. Pediatric performance with the Nucleus 22-Channel Cochlear Implant System. American Journal of Otology. 1991;12:126–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sternberg S. High-speed scanning in human memory. Science. 1966;153:652–654. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3736.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sternberg S. Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist. 1969;57:421–457. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sumby WH, Pollack I. Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1954;26:212–215. [Google Scholar]
  57. Svirsky MA, Robbins AM, Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Miyamoto RT. Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychological Science. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00231. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Waltzman SB, Cohen NL, Gomolin RH, Green JE, Shapiro WH, Hoffman RA, Roland JT., Jr Open set speech perception in congenitally deaf children using cochlear implants. American Journal of Otology. 1997;12:342–349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Waltzman SB, Cohen NL, Gumolin RH, Shapiro WH, Ozdaman SR, Hoffman RA. Long-term results of early cochlear implantation in congenitally and prelingually deafened children. American Journal of Otology. 1994;15:9–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Waugh NC, Norman DA. Primary memory. Psychological Review. 1965;72:89–104. doi: 10.1037/h0021797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Zwolan TA, Zimmerman-Phillips S, Asbaugh CJ, Hieber SJ, Kileny PR, Telian SA. Cochlear implantation of children with minimal open-set speech recognition skills. Ear and Hearing. 1997;18:240–251. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199706000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES