Skip to main content
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA logoLink to Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA
. 2012 Aug 22;2(1):343–352. doi: 10.1159/000341782

Roles of Education and IQ in Cognitive Reserve in Parkinson's Disease-Mild Cognitive Impairment

MJ Armstrong a,b,*, G Naglie c,d,e, S Duff-Canning f, C Meaney g, D Gill h, PJ Eslinger h, C Zadikoff i, M Mapstone j, KL Chou k, C Persad l, I Litvan m, BT Mast n, S Fox a,b, DF Tang-Wai b,o, C Marras a,b
PMCID: PMC3435526  PMID: 22962558

Abstract

Background/Aims

The role of cognitive reserve in Parkinson's disease (PD)-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is incompletely understood.

Methods

The relationships between PD-MCI, years of education, and estimated premorbid IQ were examined in 119 consecutive non-demented PD patients using logistic regression models.

Results

Higher education and IQ were associated with reduced odds of PD-MCI in univariate analysis. In multivariable analysis, a higher IQ was associated with a significantly decreased odds of PD-MCI, but education was not.

Conclusion

The association of higher IQ and decreased odds of PD-MCI supports a role for cognitive reserve in PD, but further studies are needed to clarify the interaction of IQ and education and the impact of other contributors such as employment and hobbies.

Key Words: Parkinson's disease, Mild cognitive impairment, Education, IQ, Cognitive reserve

Introduction

Cognitive reserve is the concept that individual differences in neural pathways and cognitive processes may affect a person's ability to compensate for brain pathology. Certain factors and life experiences such as IQ, education, employment, and leisure activities are proposed to increase cognitive reserve and are frequently used as indicators or markers of cognitive reserve. While much cognitive reserve research has been in Alzheimer's disease (AD), the concept is also applicable to other neurological diseases [1]. Studies describing the effect of education on Parkinson's disease (PD)-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have produced mixed results [2, 3, 4, 5]. We aimed to investigate the relationship between a diagnosis of PD-MCI and (1) education level (the primary variable of interest) and (2) IQ (the secondary variable of interest) as theorized indicators of cognitive reserve in a well-characterized cohort of non-demented PD patients.

Patients and Methods

Subjects

Consecutive English-speaking non-demented PD patients were enrolled at six North American movement disorders centers as part of an ongoing prospective longitudinal study of PD-MCI screening measures. Recruitment started in December 2008 and continued through June 2011; annual follow-up visits are ongoing. Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of PD according to the United Kingdom PD Society Brain Bank Criteria, age ≥60 years, English as first language, grade 8 or higher education, and a standard score of at least 80 on the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR), a standardized estimate of premorbid IQ. Language and minimum education/IQ inclusion criteria were prerequisites chosen for accurate interpretation and comparison of neuropsychological testing results.

Exclusion criteria included evidence of impairments in activities of daily living or instrumental activities of daily living related to cognition on a modified Disability Assessment for Dementia (DAD), medication changes over the past month, planned medication changes over the next 3 weeks, color blindness, a score of ≥5 on the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale, currently unstable psychiatric disorder, history of prior stroke, brain tumor, hospitalization for head injury, or active central nervous system illness (e.g. multiple sclerosis), prior or planned neurosurgical intervention (e.g. deep brain stimulation), enrollment in another interventional or observational clinical trial using neuropsychological testing, neuropsychological testing in the past 12 months, and lack of consent from either the subject or an identified close contact. Enrolled subjects received a clinical evaluation followed 1–3 weeks later by formal neuropsychological testing performed blinded to clinical results.

Each participating institution received local research ethics board approval prior to study enrollment. Written informed consent was obtained from all study subjects and participating close contacts (defined as contact at least twice weekly) prior to formal screening and study visits.

Clinical Assessment

The clinical evaluation included the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Neurobehavioral Signs and Symptoms Abbreviated Inventory (NBI) (© Professional Resources and Technologies, Westtown, Pa., USA), and Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS). Historical information was provided by the subject. The number of years of education (YOE) was assigned by patient report of the highest grade attended (up to grade 12 or 13 for secondary school) plus any additional years for post-secondary training. Post-secondary degrees were assigned YOE based on typical completion time after secondary school. Bachelor's degrees were coded as 16 YOE, master's degrees as 18 YOE, and doctorates as 20 YOE.

A consenting close contact completed the NBI and a modified version of the DAD in reference to the subject. The modified DAD added a question regarding whether any reported impairment was related to cognitive difficulties or the physical impairments of PD.

Neuropsychological Testing

Neuropsychological testing was performed 1–3 weeks following the clinical evaluation to minimize the risks of status changes in the intervening time and potential interference from similar tasks on clinical and neuropsychological cognitive evaluations. The WTAR was used as an estimate of premorbid IQ. The neuropsychological battery included the Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS-III) Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing Test [6], the Trail Making Test [7], Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Color-Word Interference Test and verbal fluency subtest [8], Benton Judgment of Line Orientation (JLO) [9], Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial [10], California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) [11], modified conditional associative learning test [12], Visual Verbal Test (VVT) abbreviated 10-item version [13], Frontal Systems Behavior Scale [14], and the 30-item Boston Naming Test (odd version) [15].

Diagnosis of PD-MCI

Diagnosis of PD-MCI was made by consensus conference based on modified Petersen criteria [2] and required (1) a cognitive complaint from the subject or close contact on the NBI, (2) no functional impairment related to cognition (as assessed by the modified DAD), and (3) performance at least 1.5 SD below the age-adjusted mean on at least one of eight core neuropsychological variables designated a priori. Core variables were WMS-III Digit Span longest span forward (attention), D-KEFS time to complete Condition 1 from the Color-Word Interference Test (processing speed), CVLT-II long delay free recall score (learning and memory), D-KEFS total score for category fluency condition from the verbal fluency subtest (language), JLO total correct (visuospatial), WMS-III total score from the Letter-Number Sequencing Test (executive: working memory), VVT total number of shifts (executive: cognitive flexibility), and D-KEFS time to complete Condition 3 from the Color-Word Interference Test (executive: inhibition). CVLT-II and JLO scores were adjusted for gender in addition to age, and VVT norms were from a control group with similar age and education to the study population.

Data Analysis

Baseline demographic characteristics were summarized as medians and interquartile ranges (IQR) or percentages as appropriate. Spearman's correlation coefficients were calculated for the relationships between each demographic variable and YOE, the primary variable of interest.

Logistic regression was performed to estimate the effect of YOE (the primary variable of interest) and IQ (the second variable of interest) on a diagnosis of PD-MCI in both univariate and multivariable models. Variables considered a priori for the multivariable model were YOE, IQ, age, age at PD diagnosis, PD duration, gender, PD severity as assessed by the MDS-UPDRS total and motor scores, postural instability and gait disorder (PIGD) subtype [calculated as a 5-point score of the sum of walking difficulty and freezing by history and gait, freezing, and postural stability by examination, where each was dichotomized as 0 (normal) or 1 (abnormal >0)], levodopa-equivalent units, and the presence of comorbidities occurring in over 10% of subjects: hypertension, hyperlipidemia, coronary artery disease, or any one of these three (a combined cardiovascular risk variable). An interaction between YOE and IQ was hypothesized, and a product term including these two variables was tested for significance. The number of independent variables included in the final model was reduced based on the available sample size [16, 17]. Variable reduction was performed by the investigation of the multicollinearity and application of the Harrell variable reduction method using YOE as the independent variable of interest [18]. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% CIs were calculated from the final logistic regression model.

For the secondary analysis, subjects were categorized into low- and high-education groups according to whether they attained post-secondary schooling or not. This division is common in the literature and is a relevant cutoff for North American populations. The low-education group included subjects whose highest education was up to high school graduation (grade 12 or 13 depending on location) and the high-education group included subjects with at least 1 year of post-secondary schooling. The univariate and multivariable logistic regression models developed above were re-derived replacing YOE with high versus low education.

Secondary outcomes were PD-MCI defined without the criterion of a cognitive complaint and performance on individual neuropsychological measures. For the association between education and PD-MCI without the requirement of a cognitive complaint (i.e. cognitive impairment as determined by scores ≥1.5 SD below the mean on at least one core neuropsychological measure without evidence of impairment in function related to cognition), the univariate and multivariable regression models were re-derived for IQ, YOE, and dichotomized education level. For the association of the core neuropsychological measures with education, median and IQR scores for the low- and high-education groups were compared using Wilcoxon rank sum statistics. Spearman's correlation coefficient was calculated for each core measure and YOE.

Complete data were available for every variable of interest except for PIGD, for which the analysis was performed without the single missing subject. Significance was assigned p < 0.05. All analyses were performed using SAS statistical software version 9.2 (SAS Institute).

Results

Baseline Characteristics

In total, 157 consecutive non-demented PD patients consented to participate in the study. Thirty (19%) did not meet the inclusion criteria, and an additional 8 (3%) were excluded after their first clinical assessment (fig. 1). At the time of analysis, 119 subjects had complete year-1 assessments. YOE was significantly correlated with age, gender, age at PD diagnosis, and IQ, though all Spearman's correlation coefficients had an absolute value of 0.35 or less except for the coefficient for IQ (table 1). Uncorrected MMSE and MoCA scores had low but significant correlations with YOE (table 1), but the MoCA correlation was no longer significant when the extra point was given for ≤12 years of formal education as recommended in the scoring instructions [19].

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study enrollment flow chart. MSA = Multiple System Atrophy

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics

Variable Total population Correlation with YOEa Two-sided p value
Patients 119
Age, years 71 (67–75) −0.23 0.01
Male gender 69% 0.35 0.001
Age at PD diagnosis, years 66 (62–71) −0.19 0.04
Years since diagnosis 4 (2–8) 0.03 0.74
YOE 16 (15–18) 1.0
Premorbid IQ 116 (109–122) 0.64 <0.0001
Total MDS-UPDRS score 42 (32–54) 0.11 0.24
Motor MDS-UPDRS score 26 (20–34) −0.14 0.13
PIGD scoreb (maximum 5) 2 (1–3) −0.15 0.11
Total LEU, mg 450.0 (300.0–787.5) −0.02 0.85
Hypertension 34% −0.04 0.64
Hyperlipidemia 28% 0.068 0.46
Coronary artery disease 18% −0.064 0.49
Any of the 3 cardiovascular risk factors 49% −0.065 0.48
MoCA score (without EL bonus point) 26 (23–27) 0.22 0.02
MoCA score (with EL bonus point) 26 (23–27) 0.17 0.07
MMSE score 29 (27–30) 0.18 0.05

V alues are medians (IQR, 25th and 75th percentiles) or percentages.

LEU = Levodopa-equivalent units; EL = education level.

a

a Spearman's correlation coefficient.

b

118 subjects.

Univariate Modeling of PD-MCI

A total of 61 of 119 subjects (51%) were diagnosed with PD-MCI, and 77 subjects (65%) were categorized as PD-MCI if no cognitive complaint was required. Univariate ORs for the associations of education and IQ with both diagnoses are presented in table 2.

Table 2.

Odds of PD-MCI (diagnosed with and without the requirement for a cognitive complaint) based on education and IQ

Variable Odds ratio of PD-MCI with a cognitive complaint (95% CI); p value Odds ratio of PD-MCI without a cognitive complaint (95% CI); p value


univariable analysis multivariable analysisa univari able analysis multivariable analysisa
YOE (1-year increment) 0.83 (0.71–0.97); 0.02 1.06 (0.85–1.31); 0.63 0.84 (0.71–0.99); 0.03 0.94 (0.75–1.17); 0.57
High educationb 0.19 (0.06–0.61); 0.005 0.46 (0.12–1.78); 0.26 0.15 (0.03–0.69); 0.01 0.24 (0.04–1.30); 0.10
IQ (5-point increment) 0.61 (0.47–0.79); 0.0002 YOE model: 0.60 (0.44–0.83); 0.002 High-education model: 0.67 (0.50–0.89); 0.006 0.68 (0.52–0.88); 0.004 YOE model: 0.67 (0.47–0.94); 0.02 High-education model: 0.70 (0.51–0.96); 0.03
a

Variables in the model included education level (YOE or high education), IQ, age, gender, and total MDS-UPDRS score.

b

High education is defined as 1 year of post-secondary education or more.

Multivariable Modeling of PD-MCI

The final multivariable model for PD-MCI included YOE, IQ, and the clinically relevant variables age, gender, and total MDS-UDPRS score. During Harrell variable reduction [18], IQ was the only variable that significantly modified the relationship between YOE and MCI. The interaction term of YOE and IQ was not significant and was not included in the final model.

In the final multivariable model, IQ significantly decreased the odds of PD-MCI in both the YOE and high-education multivariable models (table 2). The association between higher education and decreased odds of PD-MCI was no longer significant. IQ and YOE were highly correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient 0.64; p < 0.0001]). When IQ was removed from the final model to test if YOE was non-significant due to the inclusion of both YOE and IQ, the smaller (nested) model had a higher Akaike Information Criteria [20] value, suggesting that the full model including IQ and YOE had better relative goodness of fit than the model excluding IQ. Thus, the 5-variable model was maintained.

When MCI was defined without requiring a cognitive complaint, IQ was again the only significant predictor in the 5-variable model. The ORs for PD-MCI without the requirement of a cognitive complaint are presented in table 2. A higher IQ again reduced the odds of PD-MCI, while the benefit of higher education seen in univariate analysis was lost in the multivariable models.

Neuropsychological Measures and Education Level

The relationship between the core neuropsychological variables and education, the primary variable of interest, is shown in table 3. Patients in the low-education group scored significantly lower on the core test for executive cognitive flexibility, which was also the only test that correlated significantly with YOE. There was a trend for lower visuospatial scores in the low-education group (table 3).

Table 3.

A ssociation between core neuropsychological measures and level of education

Domain Test Low education (n = 21) High education (n = 98) Two-sided p value Correlation with YOEa
Attention WMS-III Digit Span longest span forward 0.51 (–0.05 to 0.88) 0.62 (–0.10 to 1.21) 0.68 0.11 (p = 0.23)
Processing speed D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test time to complete Condition 1 (color naming) 0.33 (–0.33 to 0.67) 0.0 (–0.67 to 0.67) 0.32 −0.13 (p = 0.17)
Learning and memory CVLT-II – long delay free recall 0.0 (–1.0 to 1.0) 0.0 (–0.5 to 1.0) 0.55 0.14 (p = 0.14)
Language D-KEFS verbal fluency test category fluency 0.0 (–0.67 to 0.67) 0.33 (–0.33 to 0.99) 0.14 0.033 (p = 0.72)
Visuospatial Benton JLO −0.70 (−1.97 to −0.07) −0.27 (−1.12 to 1.00) 0.06 0.10 (p = 0.27)
Executive (working memory) WMS-III Letter-Number Sequencing 0.33 (−0.67 to 0.99) 0.33 (0.00 to 0.99) 0.45 0.060 (p = 0.52)
Executive (cognitive flexibility) VVT – total number of shifts −1.99 (−4.88 to −1.99) −1.03 (−1.99 to −0.07) 0.001 0.30 (p = 0.001)
Executive (inhibition) D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test time to complete Condition 3 (inhibition) 0.33 (−0.67 to 0.99) 0.33 (−0.67 to 0.99) 0.59 −0.026 (p = 0.78)

V alues are median z-scores (IQR, 25th and 75th percentiles); median scores compared with Wilcoxon rank sum test.

a

Spearman's correlation coefficient (p value).

Discussion

A higher level of education was associated with decreased odds of PD-MCI in univariate analyses, but not in multivariable logistic regression models controlling for IQ, age, gender, and total MDS-UPDRS score. In these models, the point ORs suggested that higher education may decrease the odds of PD-MCI, but the results were not statistically significant. Estimated premorbid IQ, however, was a strong predictor of PD-MCI in all models, a relationship reported only once previously [5]. This is consistent with recent theories of cognitive reserve, where some AD studies have shown that IQ is a more powerful predictor of cognitive reserve than YOE [21], but education and other experiences impart additional benefit on top of IQ alone [1].

Prior studies with education levels similar to those in our study are mixed regarding whether education is associated with the development of PD-MCI. The lack of consensus regarding PD-MCI criteria, methods to operationalize the commonly used Petersen criteria, and selection of neuropsychological domains, tests, and cutoffs for diagnosing PD-MCI currently limits the comparability of studies. Keeping these limitations in mind, some studies found no association between YOE and PD-MCI [2, 3, 4], while another found that PD-MCI patients had significantly lower YOE [5]. Importantly, the latter was the only study that also estimated premorbid IQ. While ORs were not reported, IQ and YOE were both significantly associated with PD-MCI in univariate analysis [5]. Multivariable analysis with these variables was not performed. Thus, our study is the first, to our knowledge, to investigate the association between education and PD-MCI taking premorbid IQ into account.

Our results and those of studies enrolling similar patients are applicable only to well-educated populations. However, they should be generalizable within the United States and Canada, where 88–89% of adults have completed at least an upper secondary (high school) education and 41–50% have completed a post-secondary degree [22]. The literature in populations with less education is also mixed but generally suggestive of an association between education level and cognitive performance in at least some domains [23, 24, 25, 26]. Most standardized cognitive tests require some degree of literacy, limiting the ability to apply the measures to the lowest end of the educational spectrum. Additionally, most standardized cognitive tests, including the ones used in this study, do not have education-stratified norms for total and/or subdomain scores [7]. Available norms are likely relevant for our population as norms for most of the large cognitive batteries (e.g. WMS-III) were developed using stratified samples based on the US census [7] and education levels in the United States are generally similar to those in our study [22].

One challenge in studies attempting to evaluate a potential link between education and PD-MCI is discerning whether observed differences between education groups reflect different odds of cognitive decline or simply baseline differences in neuropsychological test performance. Longitudinal studies show that PD subjects with fewer YOE have greater cognitive decline over time [24], suggesting that differences in cognitive outcomes according to education level are not due to baseline testing differences alone. However, diagnosing PD-MCI currently requires test scores to fall a certain standard deviation (usually ≥1.5 SD) below the adjusted mean, regardless of the baseline education level. This requires patients with higher baseline test scores to fall farther before reaching the impaired range. How this affects the diagnosis or interpretation of PD-MCI is unknown and cannot be determined from the current data.

Of our core neuropsychological measures, only the VVT z-scores, representing the executive function/cognitive flexibility domain, were significantly different between education levels. Although significant, the correlation between VVT scores and YOE was low (0.30). There was also a trend for higher JLO z-scores in the high-education group. In other studies investigating the effect of education on neuropsychological test scores in non-demented PD patients, subjects with higher education levels tended to perform better particularly in frontal/executive and visuospatial domains [27, 28], consistent with our findings. The implications of this for diagnosing MCI deserve further study. It remains unclear whether education protects against decline in executive function or whether baseline and non-progressive difficulties with executive tasks in low-education groups result in a higher proportion being labeled as MCI based on these tests. Longitudinal studies would be needed to answer this question.

In addition to broad challenges in diagnosing PD-MCI and interpreting neuropsychological results as discussed above, specific limitations to our study include the use of an available cohort for analysis rather than recruiting a representative cross-sectional population. While the study design cannot prove a protective effect of IQ, this is supported by the fact that premorbid IQ necessarily precedes the development of PD and PD-MCI. The WTAR is an estimate of premorbid IQ but is validated for this use [7]. The sample size may have limited the ability to detect significant interactions between variables. As discussed above, our results may only be generalized to similarly educated populations. No subject was excluded based on WTAR standard scores, though 1 subject refused to take the WTAR and withdrew consent. The range of WTAR standard scores of our subjects (80–126) is consistent with predicted WTAR scores for US adults with ≤8 to ≥17 YOE [29]. Thus, the WTAR inclusion criterion does not substantially limit the North American generalizability of the findings.

Given the broad CIs for ORs in the multivariable model, our study lacked the statistical precision to confirm or refute the protective effect of higher education suggested by univariate analysis and by the OR point estimates of education when considered as a dichotomous variable. In contrast, the beneficial association seen for IQ was robust. Education and estimated premorbid IQ were the only measured contributors to cognitive reserve. Data regarding type of employment and hobbies, other theorized contributors to cognitive reserve, were not collected and may have contributed further to our understanding of cognitive reserve and PD-MCI.

Our study found that higher education was associated with decreased odds of PD-MCI in univariate analysis, but not multivariable analysis. Estimated premorbid IQ was associated with substantially decreased odds of PD-MCI across all models. This finding is consistent with the cognitive reserve theory in the context of PD-MCI but suggests that IQ may have more influence than education level. Future studies should report estimated premorbid IQ in addition to education level and should also investigate whether other factors hypothesized to influence cognitive reserve, such as employment and/or hobbies, may also have protective roles, likely requiring a large sample size. Whether there is any protective benefit of IQ and other factors associated with cognitive reserve in delaying the progression from PD-MCI to PD dementia remains to be established.

Disclosure Statement

No author has conflicts of interest related to this paper.

Acknowledgements

The study ‘Validating the Montreal Cognitive Assessment for the diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson's disease’ is funded by The Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research. Melissa J. Armstrong received support as an Edmond J. Safra fellow at Toronto Western Hospital while working on this project. Dr. Naglie receives funding from the University of Toronto Mary Trimmer Chair in Geriatric Medicine and research support from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (IAO-90399). Dr. Duff-Canning has research support from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, National Parkinson Foundation, and Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Dr. Eslinger has research support from the NIH (NS060722, ES019672, AG027771) and the Pennsylvania Department of Health. Dr. Zadikoff has received consultancy and honoraria from Teva, GSK, Merz, Allergan, Ipsen, and Abbott. She received research support from Parkinson Alliance. Dr. Mapstone has received honoraria from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, and funding from the NIH, Michael J. Fox Foundation, Neurologix, Inc., and Advanced Neuromodulation System, Inc. Dr. Chou receives research support from the NIH (NS44504-08), participates as a site-PI in clinical trials sponsored by the Huntington Study Group (PHAROS, 2CARE, and CREST-HD), receives royalties from UpToDate, and has received honoraria from Allergan for speaking engagements, and from Medtronic and Merz Pharmaceuticals for consulting work. Dr. Persad is a co-investigator on NIH grant P01 NS015655-26A1 and co-PI on MCASTL RFP2010-04. Dr. Litvan receives research support from the NIH (R01 PAS-03-092 NIA), National Parkinson Foundation, Parkinson Support Center of Kentuckiana, CurePSP, Noscira, Allon Therapeutics, and Parkinson Study Group. Dr. Fox has research support from the Parkinson Society Canada, Michael J. Fox Foundation, and NIH. She has served as a consultant for Teva, Novartis, Santhera, Merz, Merck Serono, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. Dr. Marras is a recipient of a New Investigator Award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. She also receives funding from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, National Parkinson Foundation, Merck Serono, and Allon Therapeutics. The authors would like to thank Dr. Nancy Kennedy and Dr. Paul J. Eslinger, who both served as local investigators, and Brandon Rothberg, who provided database and administrative assistance throughout the study.

References

  • 1.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Caviness JN, Driver-Dunckley E, Connor DJ, Sabbagh MN, Hentz JG, Noble B, Evidente VG, Shill HA, Adler CH. Defining mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2007;22:1272–1277. doi: 10.1002/mds.21453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mamikonyan E, Moberg PJ, Siderowf A, Duda JE, Have TT, Hurtig HI, Stern MB, Weintraub D. Mild cognitive impairment is common in Parkinson's disease patients with normal Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009;15:226–231. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sollinger AB, Goldstein FC, Lah JJ, Levey AI, Factor SA. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: subtypes and motor characteristics. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2010;16:177–180. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.McKinlay A, Grace RC, Dalrymple-Alford JC, Roger D. Cognitive characteristics associated with mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;28:121–129. doi: 10.1159/000235247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wechsler D. 3. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. 3. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2001. Examiner's Guide for the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Benton AL, Sivan AB, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O. 2. Orlando: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1994. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995. Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial – Professional Manual. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. 2. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2002. Manual for the California Verbal Learning Test. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Taylor AE, Saint-Cyr JA, Lang AE. Memory and learning in early Parkinson's disease: evidence for a ‘frontal lobe syndrome’. Brain Cogn. 1990;13:211–232. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(90)90051-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wicklund AH, Johnson N, Weintraub S. Preservation of reasoning in primary progressive aphasia: further differentiation from Alzheimer's disease and the behavioral presentation of frontotemporal dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004;26:347–355. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Grace J, Malloy PF. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resource; 2001. Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe) Professional Manual. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Williams BW, Mack W, Henderson VW. Boston Naming Test in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:1073–1079. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90186-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Babyak MA. What you see may not be what you get: a brief, nontechnical introduction to overfitting in regression-type models. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:411–421. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000127692.23278.a9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Peduzzi P, Concato J, Kemper E, Holford TR, Feinstein AR. A simulation study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:1373–1379. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(96)00236-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Harrell FE, Jr, Lee KL. Califf RM. Pryor DB. Rosati RA. Regression modelling strategies for improved prognostic prediction. Stat Med. 1984;3:143–152. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780030207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Automat Control. 1974;19:716–723. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Alexander GE, Furey ML, Grady CL, Pietrini P, Brady DR, Mentis MJ, Schapiro MB. Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:165–172. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.OECD Education at a Glance 2011. OECD Publishing. 2011 [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Janvin C, Aarsland D, Larsen JP, Hugdahl K. Neuropsychological profile of patients with Parkinson's disease without dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2003;15:126–131. doi: 10.1159/000068483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Muslimovic D, Schmand B, Speelman JD, de Haan RJ. Course of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13:920–932. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707071160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pai MC, Chan SH. Education and cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease: a study of 102 patients. Acta Neurol Scand. 2001;103:243–247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kummer A, Harsányi E, Dias FM, Cardoso F, Caramelli P, Teixeira AL. Depression impairs executive functioning in Parkinson disease patients with low educational level. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2009;22:167–172. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3181b278f9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Green J, McDonald WM, Vitek JL, Evatt M, Freeman A, Haber M, Bakay RA, Triche S, Sirockman B, DeLong MR. Cognitive impairments in advanced PD without dementia. Neurology. 2002;59:1320–1324. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031426.21683.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cohen OS, Vakil E, Tanne D, Nitsan Z, Schwartz R, Hassin-Baer S. Educational level as a modulator of cognitive performance and neuropsychyatric features in Parkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2007;20:68–72. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3180335f8e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2001. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading Manual. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA are provided here courtesy of Karger Publishers

RESOURCES