Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jun 6.
Published in final edited form as: Physiol Behav. 2012 Jan 12;106(3):337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.007

Cognitive and Neuronal Systems Underlying Obesity

Scott E Kanoski 1
PMCID: PMC3437324  NIHMSID: NIHMS354679  PMID: 22266286

Abstract

Since the late 1970’s obesity prevalence and per capita food intake in the USA have increased dramatically. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the hyperphagia that drives obesity requires focus on the cognitive processes and neuronal systems controlling feeding that occurs in the absence of metabolic need (i.e., "non-homeostatic” intake). Given that a portion of the increased caloric intake per capita since the late 1970’s is attributed to increased meal and snack frequency, and given the increased pervasiveness of environmental cues associated with energy dense, yet nutritionally deplete foods, there’s a need to examine the mechanisms through which food-related cues stimulate excessive energy intake. Here, learning and memory principles and their underlying neuronal substrates are discussed with regard to stimulus-driven food intake and excessive energy consumption. Particular focus is given to the hippocampus, a brain structure that utilizes interoceptive cues relevant to energy status (e.g., neurohormonal signals such as leptin) to modulate stimulus-driven food procurement and consumption. This type of hippocampal-dependent modulatory control of feeding behavior is compromised by consumption of foods common to Western diets, including saturated fats and simple carbohydrates. The development of more effective treatments for obesity will benefit from a more complete understanding of the complex interaction between dietary, environmental, cognitive, and neurophysiological mechanisms contributing to excessive food intake.

Keywords: hippocampus, learning, rat, leptin, ghrelin, saturated fat, Western diet, VTA

1. Introduction

The need to more fully understand the neuronal substrates underlying food intake control is highlighted by the profound increase in obesity prevalence that has emerged in the USA and other developed countries across the past few decades [13]. Food intake and energy balance are regulated, in part, by neuronal processing in the hypothalamus and the brainstem. A great deal of research has focused on neurohormonal and neurotransmitter systems in these brain regions that regulate what has been called “homeostatic”, or energy deficit-driven feeding [46]. Much less is known, however, about the neurochemical and psychological factors that underlie food intake that occurs in the absence of metabolic need, or “non-homeostatic” food intake. Substantial progress in research aimed at treating obesity can be made through a deeper understanding of the neuronal systems that control non-homeostatic-driven feeding. This notion is highlighted by two important points: 1) the excessive energy intake that drives human obesity is clearly not based on metabolic need, and 2) decisions about eating or not eating, or how much food is consumed undoubtedly involves neuronal processing in higher-order (extrahypothalamic and extrahindbrain) brain regions that control cognitive processes.

Given the rarity of monogenetic causes of obesity [7] and the extreme unlikelihood that the the human genome has changed substantially in the past 30 years, the exponential increase in obesity prevalence is largely attributed to environment factors present in modernized Western cultures [8, 9]. While specific causal environmental factors are difficult to identify, it is likely that changes in diet composition in Western cultures (e.g., more energy dense and highly processed foods), the easy availability of these “Western” foods, as well as the abundance of cues in the modern environment associated with this type of food are contributing to the alarming increase in obesity. Increased feeding stimulated by exposure to these types of environmental food-related cues involves associative learning mechanisms. This review discusses learning principles and their underlying neuronal substrates in relation to excessive energy consumption, and considers the perspective that one antecedent fueling the hyperphagia driving obesity is dietary-induced disruption of the higher-order learned controls of feeding behavior.

2. Basic Conditioning Mechanisms and Feeding

2.1. Learned associations

Learning about the relationships between food-related cues (e.g., visual, olfactory, gustatory) and postingestive consequences undoubtedly influences feeding behavior. Powerful demonstrations of this are found in rodent models of conditioned taste aversion/avoidance (CTA) learning [10], in which animals will subsequently avoid or reject neutral or preferred flavor cues that have been paired with visceral malaise, commonly induced experimentally by noxious agents such as lithium chloride. Similarly, neutral flavors can be conditioned to be subsequently preferred through pairing with intragastric nutrient infusion [1114]. Conditioned aversions and preferences involve the formation of learned associations between conditioned stimuli, or CSs (flavors), and unconditioned stimuli, or USs (postingestive malaise or nutrient absorption).

The modern environment in Western industrialized countries is flooded with food-related cues such as fast food signs, television commercials, catchy logos and images on food packaging, vending machines, etc. Human studies show that food intake can be elevated by experimentally manipulating food-relevant external cues, including the time/clock [15] and meal vs. snack-related cues (e.g., ceramic vs. paper plates) [16, 17]. Evidence from animal models also shows that the presence of conditioned food-associated cues can stimulate feeding. Weingarten demonstrated that discrete stimuli (e.g., tones, lights) previously paired with access to a meal when rats were food restricted would later stimulate increased eating even when the rats were food-sated and would not otherwise eat [18]. Petrovich and colleagues developed a similar “cue-potentiated eating” paradigm in which food-deprived rats were trained in conditioning boxes with discrete cues that signal either food access (CS+) or no food access (CS-) [19]. When the rats were later tested in a nondeprived (food-sated) state, presentation of the CS+ evoked elevated feeding compared to the CS−. In a series of studies this lab elucidated part of the neuronal circuitry that mediates this phenomenon [1922], which includes neuronal communication between the basolateral amygdala (BLA) and lateral hypothalamus (LH) [see [23, 24] for reviews]. It is unclear whether this type of cue-driven feeding phenomenon is augmented in obese and obesity-prone individuals, as has been suggested by Schachter [25] and others [26, 27], and whether pharmacological treatments targeting specific neurohormonal systems have the potential to alleviate obesity and hyperphagia by reducing the ability of conditioned cues to drive excessive food intake.

2.2. “Reward”-driven feeding

It is clear that some foods are more sought after and enjoyable than other foods, and are hence more likely to be consumed independent of need or hunger. Which foods are more reinforcing/rewarding than others is a dynamic individual-specific state modulated by physiological status (e.g., hunger, overall health), recent consumption history (e.g., sensory specific satiety), previous experience (e.g., CTA), and various other factors [28]. The neuronal system mediating the hedonic aspects of consumption begins in the brainstem, as illustrated by pioneering work from Grill and Norgren showing that the isolated brainstem is capable of eliciting basic appetitive (e.g., ingestion) and aversive (e.g., rejection) facial expressions to sweet and bitter tastes [29]. Hedonic “liking” of certain tastes and foods is thought to be largely mediated by opioid peptide signaling in a distributed CNS network including hindbrain, midbrain, and forebrain regions, such as the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) [30], the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) [3133], the amygdala [34, 35], the ventral tegmental area (VTA) [36], and hypothalamic nuclei [LH, paraventricular hypothalamus(PVH)] [34, 35]. Activation or blockade of CNS opioid receptor signaling can increase or decrease feeding even for foods that are considered bland and not palatable (standard lab chow) [3739]. However, for the most part, results support the hypothesis that opioid effects on feeding are larger with preferred foods [4045], which for both humans and animal models are typically foods that contain fats and/or simple (mono- and di-saccharides), sweet carbohydrates.

A related construct associated with feeding that is linked with brain “reward” circuitry is incentive motivation (i.e., wanting) in which cues associated with rewarding foods can act as incentive motivators for food independent of basic homeostatic drive (e.g., hunger) [46, 47]. The neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) is a critical player in the neurochemical controls of incentive motivation. The mesolimbic/mesocortical DA neurons originate in the midbrain (substantia nigra and VTA) and project to the NAcc, prefrontal cortex (PFC), hypothalamus, and amygdala (see [48] for review). The mesolimbic DA system regulates neuronal processing of natural rewards such as feeding and sex, as well as pharmacological stimuli (addictive drugs) that hijack this system [49, 50]. Intake of a preferred food increases DA levels in the NAcc [51, 52]. Further, pharmacological manipulations that increase DA signaling in the NAcc (e.g., dopamine receptor agonists, amphetamine, etc.) increase the extent to which an animal will work for food in operant lever pressing paradigms [53, 54], yet typically do not alter total food intake in a free-feeding situation [53]. According to Berridge [48], environmental cues associated with appetitive reinforcement induce burst-firing and phasic DA release in the mesolimbic DA system, which in turn increase goal-directed behavior. Interestingly, the mesolimbic DA control of incentive motivation for food reinforcement is modulated by an array of neurohormonal signals relevant to energy balance, particularly leptin and ghrelin, a topic reviewed in detail elsewhere [5557].

The neuronal systems and neurochemical players mediating the overconsumption of palatable foods are well-investigated, yet the underlying cognitive mechanisms remain poorly understood. Given that previous experience with food undoubtedly influences which types of foods are more preferred, most clearly illustrated by the fact that saccharine and sucrose can easily be conditioned to be aversive [58, 59], learning and memory principles may offer some insight regarding the cognitive/psychological mechanisms underlying overconsumption of palatable foods. From a learning theory perspective, the magnitude of the reinforcer (e.g., US magnitude) is one of the most important determinants of learning, influencing the rate at which learning occurs as well as the asymptote, or maximum level of conditioning possible [6062]. Some foods (e.g., sweeter foods) appear to inherently have a greater reinforcing capacity than others, supported by findings from Sclafani’s lab showing a direct relationship between sucrose concentration and the amount of operant licks to obtain sucrose in a progressive ratio reinforcement schedule [63]. Consumption of these more reinforcing foods represents a stronger US, perhaps due in part to greater elevations in endogenous CNS opioid signaling and altered mesolimbic DA neuronal firing during or following consumption. Compared with bland foods, which represent a weaker US, these palatable foods are more easily and strongly conditioned to environmental cues (increased learning rate and asymptote). Based on this stronger learned ‘CS-US’ association, exposure to environmental cues linked with palatable food therefore evokes a more powerful US memory and triggers greater procurement and consumption of these foods relative to cues linked with bland foods. Within this framework, environmental stimuli associated with palatable, preferred foods are particularly adept at having stimulus control over feeding behavior based on more powerful learned associations (e.g., golden arches of McDonald’s).

These learning principles can account for how some foods (preferred foods) acquire and maintain greater stimulus control over food-directed behavior compared to less preferred foods; yet, reinforcement principles do not offer insight into the mechanisms underlying why/how some foods are initially more preferred (i.e., more reinforcing) than others. Unfortunately constructs such as reinforcement, reward, motivation, and palatability offer no real explanatory potential regarding psychological principles underlying the phenomenon that some foods are over-consumed to a greater extent than others. Further complicating our understanding of reward-based feeding and overconsumption is the fact that which specific foods are preferred relative to others is individual-specific and extremely dynamic.

3. Higher-order learning processes and feeding

3.1. Modulatory control of learned associations

While the learned CS-US associations produced from conditioned flavor aversion/preference and cue-potentiated feeding training yield powerful alterations in feeding behavior, animals encountering food in the natural environment are not allowed the luxury of making decisions about feeding behavior based solely on approach vs. avoidance of preferred or nonpreferred foods. Rather, decisions about whether to feed or not to feed, or about the continuation vs. cessation of an ongoing meal are made within the framework of a larger context. Contextual factors that influence feeding decisions include external environmental context cues, such as the presence or absence of predators and the location and accessibility of food, etc. In addition to these types of physical background cues, feeding behavior is also modulated by internal contextual cues, which can include interoceptive signals informing about general health, overall energy balance status (e.g., circulating nutrients, adipose reserves), and those that relate to ongoing and recent nutrient consumption and absorption (i.e., satiation and satiety cues) [64, 65].

Contextual stimuli play a modulatory role in influencing conditioned behavior in the sense that contexts do not always have a direct stimulatory (or inhibitory) influence on responding, but rather modulate the ability of other cues (e.g., discrete cues) to evoke conditioned responding [6669]. One manner in which internal contextual cues influence feeding behavior is by modulating the mnemonic strength of learned CS-US food-related associations, or put differently, by modulating how effectively food-associated cues (CS) evoke food memory and subsequent food-directed responding (conditioned response, or CR) [64, 65]. Within this framework, the presence of neurohormonal signals that inform about sufficient long-term energy status, such as the adipostat hormone leptin [70], and signals informing about recent or ongoing nutrient ingestion, including the gastrointestinally-derived hormones cholecystokinin (CCK) [71] and glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) [72], will reduce the effectiveness of food-associated cues to evoke food procurement and consumption. On the other hand, ghrelin, a gut peptide which increases food intake via activation of CNS growth hormone secretagogue receptor (GHSR) [73], will presumably increase the strength of food-related CS-US associations. Thus, the internal milieu of hormonal and metabolic signals informing about energy status modulates how effectively environmental cues associated with food reinforcement (e.g., fast food sign) will trigger the procurement of food.

The notion that neurohormonal signals provide an internal context that modulates food-directed responding is supported by studies employing the deprivation intensity discrimination paradigm developed by Davidson [69, 74]. In this paradigm rats are trained to use a high (24hr) or low (0hr) level of food deprivation as discriminative internal cues for a food reward. Rats receive one of two contingencies: 1) a food reward is given on 24hr but not 0hr food-deprived training days (24+ contingency), or 2) the opposite contingency (0+; food only on 0hr deprived days). Discrimination learning is shown as heightened anticipatory appetitive responding (e.g., food cup approach) on rewarded compared to nonrewarded deprivation state conditions. In rats trained in this paradigm, peripheral CCK or leptin administration produced internal contextual cues that generalized to an energy replete state [75], whereas peripheral and ICV ghrelin [76], but not ICV administration of the orexigenic hormone, neuropeptide Y (NPY) [77] produced internal context cues that generalized to an energy deprived state. An important point derived from these studies is that exogenous administration of these peptides does not simply drive appetitive responding in a general direction consistent with known anorectic/orexigenic properties (e.g., leptin reduces appetitive responding, ghrelin increases). Rather, the ability of leptin or ghrelin to influence appetitive behavior depends on previous learned relationships between internal context cues and food access. This notion is best exemplified by the fact that leptin increased appetitive responding in rats trained with the 0+ contingency, yet it decreased responding for 24+ trained rats [75], whereas ghrelin produced the opposite pattern [76]. In other words, leptin and ghrelin modulate food procurement based on conditioned/learned mechanisms rather than simple unconditioned approach vs. avoidance of food. The idea that learning has a profound influence on the nature through which energy status cues guide feeding behavior is further supported by work from Dickinson and Balleine (e.g., [47, 78, 79]) showing that the ability of a food deprivation or repletion state to influence operant responding for food reinforcement is highly dependent on whether the animals had previously consumed the specific food reinforcer under that deprivation (or repletion) state.

In a free-feeding situation, the internal hunger or satiation/satiety context influences feeding by increasing or decreasing, respectively, how effectively environmental food-associated cues trigger food procurement and consumption. This occurs through mechanisms akin to a type of modulatory associative learning process known as occasion setting, in which stimuli (discrete or contextual) modulate the strength of CS-US learned associations. This model of food intake control has been presented in detail elsewhere [64, 6581]. The take-home point is that the internal energy status context, which is largely derived from vagally mediated, as well as circulating meal and adiposity-related neurohormonal signals, controls feeding based on learned relationships between environmental food cues and food-based reinforcement, rather than unconditionally influencing approach vs. avoidance of feeding relevant behaviors.

3.2. The Hippocampus and modulatory control of feeding

The hippocampus is a brain structure that is strongly linked with contextual learning and memory processes [82]. Regarding external contextual cues, an abundance of data demonstrate that the hippocampus is critical for integrating learned information with representations of the spatial external environment [8385]. All regions of the hippocampus contain populations of place-modulated neurons with distinct firing fields depending on an animal’s precise location within a larger contextual realm [86, 87]. Further, selective damage to the hippocampus in animals produces profound impairments in spatial learning and memory task such as the Morris water maze, and in paradigms such as contextual fear conditioning that involve incorporating external contextual information into learned associations (see [88, 89] for reviews). This type of spatial/external contextual hippocampal-dependent learning has relevance to food procurement, which is evident from lesion studies showing that selective hippocampal damage either increases or decreases (depending on which subregion is lesioned) learning a “place preference” for a context paired with food access [90]. Further, hippocampal lesions impair learning and retention of spatial food location in a radial arm maze paradigm [91], in which rats learn which of various arms in an elevated maze are consistently baited with food based on external visuospatial cues located outside the maze.

The hippocampus is also critical for memory processes involving the utilization of internal contextual information. For instance, selective neurotoxic hippocampal lesions impair the ability of rats to use interoceptive cues arising from different levels of food deprivation to guide food-directed behavior in the deprivation intensity discrimination paradigm described above [9294]. Similarly, Kennedy and Shapiro observed that the pattern of hippocampal (CA1 cell field) neuronal firing is dependent upon previous learned relationships between external and internal (food or water deprivation) contextual cues [95]. In humans, amnesic patients with hippocampal damage will consume a second meal immediately after consuming a full meal, and do not appropriately adjust hunger/satiety ratings following a meal [9698]. This suggests that these amnesic patients are impaired in detecting and utilizing internal satiation cues (including stomach distention, changes in circulating nutrient and hormone concentrations, etc.) arising from recently consumed nutrients. Higgs and colleagues provided data indicating that this phenomenon may also be based on impaired episodic memory for recent eating episodes [99, 100].

Evidence has been quickly amassing over the past decade that hippocampal-dependent processes involving the integration of internal energy status relevant signals with learned information is critical for the normal control of feeding behavior (see [65, 80, 81, 64] for reviews). Human and rodent imaging studies show that the hippocampus is activated following food consumption [101103] and by experimental manipulations that mimic aspects of nutrient intake, including gastric distention [104] and gastric electrical stimulation of the vagus nerve [105], the primary sensory conduit of information communicated from the gastrointestinal tract to the brain. Studies from rodents employing selective neurotoxic hippocampal lesions also support a role for this structure in food intake control. Relative to intact controls, hippocampal-lesioned rats show increased appetitive responding (e.g., lever pressing for food, food cup approach) when they are food-sated [106108], increased meal frequency [109], and increased overall energy intake and body weight gain [65, 92, 110]. Thus, one role of the hippocampus in the control of food intake appears to involve anorectic/inhibitory control. Given that the hippocampus integrates and utilizes interoceptive signals relevant to energy status, it stands to reason that neurohormonal players involved in feeding behavior may signal in the hippocampus to influence food-directed behavior and energy intake.

3.3. Neurohormonal signaling in the hippocampus and feeding

The hippocampus contains receptors for several hormonal signals of relevance to energy status, including leptin [111, 112], ghrelin [113, 114], GLP-1 [115], and insulin [116, 117]. Previous work has shown that all of these hormones improve hippocampal-dependent spatial or contextual learning (using nonappetitive memory paradigms), and also facilitate molecular and cellular processes that are thought to underlie memory formation (e.g., long-term potentiation, neurogenesis) [118126]. However, these reports did not address the possibility that these energy balance relevant signals influence feeding behavior through signaling in the hippocampus. In a recent study our lab examined the role of leptin signaling in the hippocampus in food intake and in memory processes related to food procurement [127]. Results showed that doses of leptin that are without intake effects when given ICV reduced 24hr food intake and body weight in rats when administered directly to either the dorsal region of the hippocampus (DHPC), which is most strongly linked with spatial learning, or the ventral hippocampus (VHPC), which is most strongly linked with learning processes that have a motivational or emotional component (see[128] for review of dorsal/ventral hippocampal function). Intake suppression was notably larger following VHPC relative to DHPC leptin delivery, ranging between ~11–15% compared to 6–10% suppression, respectively. Other findings from this study demonstrated that VHPC (but not DHPC) leptin administration reduced the expression of a conditioned place preference for a location/context previously associated with food reinforcement, and reduced latency to run for food in a runway paradigm. These results suggest that leptin signaling in the VHPC may be reducing food intake via downstream signaling in brain regions associated with reward/motivational processing. This, notion is consistent with neuroanatomical data showing that the VHPC projects directly to, and in some cases receives direct projections from nuclei embedded within the brain reward circuit, including the VTA [129, 130], NAcc [131, 132] , LH [133], amygdala [134, 135], and PFC [136].

VHPC leptin signaling may also reduce appetitive and consummatory behavior by modulating which types of environmental cues are learned about and remembered. Leptin administered to the VHPC after rats learned the spatial location of food in an elevated plus maze paradigm blocked memory consolidation for the spatial location of food (assessed 7-days later in a retention test), whereas VHPC leptin had no effect on memory consolidation of an appetitive nonspatial response task [127]. However, Farr and colleagues [120] using a comparable dose of leptin demonstrated that post-training dorsal hippocampal leptin administration improved memory consolidation for a task that requires animals to associate a context with an aversive US (foot shock). That leptin can both decrease and increase memory consolidation depending on the type of reinforcement and the hippocampal subregion suggests that leptin signaling in this brain structure modulates what types of environmental cues are learned about and remembered, reducing resources invested into learning about food-relevant cues in favor of other cues when energy reserves are sufficient and endogenous leptin levels are elevated.

The food intake-stimulatory gut peptide ghrelin also appears to influence food intake through signaling on its receptor (growth-hormone secretagogue receptor, or GHSR) in the hippocampus. Preliminary unpublished data from our lab show that ghrelin administered to the VHPC, but not DHPC stimulates food intake in rats during a period when rats normally are not eating (during the light cycle). The mechanisms through which hippocampal ghrelin signaling stimulates feeding remain to be established. Data from both humans [137, 138] and animal models [126, 139, 140] are consistent with the notion that ghrelin increases food intake by acting as a signal for meal initiation. Given that the hippocampus is necessary for utilizing interoceptive energy status cues to modulate food-directed/appetitive responding, and given that hippocampal damage profoundly increases meal frequency [109], GHSR signaling in the hippocampus may modulate feeding by increasing how effectively environmental cues trigger food-related memories and stimulate meal initiation.

4. Obesity: A Learning and Memory Problem

4.1. Environmental food cues and obesity

As discussed above, exposure to external food-associated cues can increase feeding in both human and animal models under experimental conditions. This phenomenon may also contribute to hyperphagia and obesity in the normal environment by either, 1) stimulating extra meals or snacks, and/or 2) directing meals and snacks towards foods that are more energy dense and reinforcing. A recent study from Duffey and Popkin suggests that the former possibility may be a contributing factor [141]. They reported that since the late 1970’s, which is approximately when the slope of obesity prevalence began to dramatically shift upwards [142], average per capita energy intake in the USA has risen by approximately 570 kcal/day. To elucidate what is driving the increased intake, the authors utilized cross-sectional survey data to evaluate the contribution of changes in energy density, portion size, and number of eating occasions (meals or snacks). They reported that the increase in overall energy intake observed between the late 1970’s and mid 1990’s was attributable to both increased portion size and increased number of eating occasions. However, from the mid 1990’s to 2006, portion size no longer contributed to increased overall energy intake, whereas increased number of eating occasions continued to have a large contribution (+ ~39 kcal/day). While this type of cross-sectional national survey analysis is limited in regard to establishing direct causal factors, their findings highlight increased number of meals and snacks as being an important variable correlated with the sharp rise in obesity prevalence seen across the past few decades. The increase in meal and snack frequency observed since the late 1970’s (+ ~1.1 eating occasions [141]) is likely based, in part, on the heightened prevalence of cues in Western cultures that are associated with energy dense foods. In fact, the number of fast-food restaurants [143] and money spent on fast-food advertising [144] in the USA have more than doubled since this time, as has the number of television commercials that advertise foods with minimal nutritional value (e.g., candy, cereal, fast-food) [145].

The greater prevalence of environmental cues linked with rewarding foods does not necessarily predict increased hyperphagia and meal/snack frequency, as higher-order brain regions involved with learning and cognitive function, including the hippocampus, should exert inhibitory control over the ability of these cues to stimulate feeding at inappropriate times and/or when energy reserves are sufficient. However, recent studies show that dietary factors that are particularly prevalent in modern Western diets, including simple carbohydrates (mono and disaccharides) and saturated fatty acids (SFA), disrupt hippocampal function, which in turn may reduce the effectiveness of anorectic neurohormonal signals (e.g., leptin) to negatively modulate food procurement and consumption.

4.2. Western diets impair learned controls of feeding

Obesity and Type II diabetes mellitus are both strongly linked with cognitive impairment and dementia (see [146, 147] for reviews). Recent findings show that specific dietary factors can also produce cognitive impairment, in some cases independent of their effects on body weight gain and obesity. Human population-based prospective studies show that high intakes of SFA, but not total fat, over several years leads to a greater risk for the development of Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment [148150]. A recent study reported that human subjects with high self-reported levels of saturated fat and refined sugar intake are impaired in memory problems (particularly hippocampal-dependent memory) relative to subjects reporting less saturated fat and refined sugar intake [151]. These findings are corroborated by reports in rats, showing that maintenance on a high SFA diet, but not a diet high in unsaturated fatty acids, impairs learning and memory function [152]. Research from rodent models also shows that long-term intake of fructose, a simple monosaccharide common in Western diets, can produce hippocampal insulin resistance and impair hippocampal-dependent memory function [153, 154]. Further, excessive sucrose intake in rats disrupts hippocampal function independent of dietary fat intake [155].

A series of studies by Kanoski and colleagues examined the effects of Western diet (one containing high SFA and glucose) intake in rats on learning and memory processes that differ in sensitivity to hippocampal damage. Results show that simple conditioning processes (e.g., formation of CS-US associations) that do not require an intact hippocampus are minimally affected by long-term (> 90 days) maintenance on a Western diet. However, higher-order modulatory learning processes that do rely on the hippocampus, such as a negative occasion setting task where a discrete stimulus signals when another stimulus will not be followed by food reinforcement, are profoundly impaired by Western diet maintenance [156, 157]. Importantly, the impairment was expressed as increased appetitive responding to conditioned cues on nonreinforced trials, suggesting that the Western diet consumption disrupted learned inhibitory/anorectic control of appetitive responding. Another study demonstrated that hippocampal-dependent spatial memory function, assessed in an appetitive radial arm maze task, is impaired after as little as three days of consuming a Western diet, whereas for the same rats it took a much longer-term maintenance period (more than 60-days) to disrupt nonspatial memory processes that do not rely on the hippocampus [158]. Thus, hippocampal-dependent learning and memory processes, including those involving modulation of feeding behavior, are particularly susceptible to disruption by intake of SFA and simple sugars, a finding that is consistent with the notion that this brain region is especially vulnerable to various disease and age-related insults [159]. Disruption of hippocampal inhibitory control over behaviors directed at obtaining food can yield further overconsumption of the same foods that contributed to hippocampal dysfunction in the first place, a “vicious circle” model of energy dysregulation that has also been discussed elsewhere [65, 81, 64, 110, 156, 160].

The neurophysiological mechanisms underlying diet-induced impairment in hippocampal function include (but are not limited to) reductions in hippocampal levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [156, 161], impaired blood-brain barrier integrity (increased permeability, reduced expression of tight junction proteins, impaired BBB leptin transport) [157, 162], elevated levels of circulating triglycerides and cholesterol [163, 164], and neuronal insulin resistance in the hippocampus [165, 166] (see [160] for review). Western diet-induced hippocampal dysfunction may also involve impaired leptin (LepRb) and ghrelin (GHSR) receptor signaling in this brain region. LepRb “resistance” occurs in the hypothalamus in diet-induced obese rodents, which is evident from behavioral (reduced anorectic effects of CNS leptin delivery) and molecular (reduced leptin-induced activation of phosphorylation of the signal transducer and activator or transcription, or PSTAT-3) measures (see [167, 168] for reviews). Obese rodents also show GHSR resistance, illustrated by a blunted food intake increase following peripheral ghrelin administration [169] and reduced CNS ghrelin-induced activation of NPY/AgRP neurons in the hypothalamus [170]. Yet unknown is whether Western diet-induced impairments in hippocampal-dependent modulatory control over appetitive behavior is based, in part, on LepRb and/or GHSR resistance in this brain region.

5. Conclusions

The hyperphagia driving obesity undoubtedly involves neuronal processing in extrahypothalamic and extrahindbrain “higher-order” brain regions that control learning and cognitive processes. The dramatic elevations in food intake and obesity prevalence in the USA since the late 1970’s are partially attributed to increased per capita daily number of meals and snacks consumed since that time [141]. This phenomenon may be based on increased pervasiveness of environmental cues associated with energy dense, yet nutritionally deplete foods. The hippocampus is a brain region that functions to modulate the effectiveness of food-related cues to stimulate food procurement and consumption via the detection and utilization of neurohormonal signals of relevance to energy balance. This type of modulatory control is disrupted by dietary factors common in modern Western diets, including simple carbohydrates (mono- and disaccharides) and saturated fatty acids. Thus, consumption of these dietary factors can have a detrimental impact on modulatory learning processes that normally function to curb excessive energy consumption. Research targeting obesity treatment will benefit from deeper understanding of the influence of dietary and environmental factors on the neuronal systems that control non-homeostatic food intake control.

Highlights.

  • Environmental cues associated with food stimulate feeding

  • Stimulus control over feeding is modulated by hippocampal-dependent learning

  • Energy status cues (eg, leptin, ghrelin) signal in the hippocampus to control feeding

  • Consumption of a Western diet compromises hippocampal modulation of food intake

Acknowledgements

I thank Harvey Grill, Terry Davidson, Matthew Hayes, Terry Powley, and Peter Urcuioli for invaluable career guidance, Jessica Kanoski and Samantha Fortin for conceptual and editorial contribution to this review, and the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behavior.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Ogden CL, Lamb MM, Carroll MD, Flegal KM. Obesity and socioeconomic status in adults: United States, 2005–2008. NCHS Data Brief. 2010:1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Ogden CL, Curtin LR. Prevalence and trends in obesity among US adults, 1999–2008. Jama. 2010;303:235–241. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Berghofer A, Pischon T, Reinhold T, Apovian CM, Sharma AM, Willich SN. Obesity prevalence from a European perspective: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2008;8:200. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kenny PJ. Reward mechanisms in obesity: new insights and future directions. Neuron. 2011;69:664–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grill H, Skibicka K, Hayes M. Imaging obesity: fMRI, food reward, and feeding. Cell Metab. 2007;6:423–425. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2007.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Berthoud HR. Neural control of appetite: cross-talk between homeostatic and non-homeostatic systems. Appetite. 2004;43:315–317. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ramachandrappa S, Farooqi IS. Genetic approaches to understanding human obesity. J Clin Invest. 2011;121:2080–2086. doi: 10.1172/JCI46044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Berthoud HR. Homeostatic and non-homeostatic pathways involved in the control of food intake and energy balance. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006;14 Suppl 5:197S–200S. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Berthoud H, Morrison C. The brain, appetite, and obesity. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:55–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Garcia J, Kimeldorf DJ, Koelling RA. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science. 1955;122:157–158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sclafani A. Post-ingestive positive controls of ingestive behavior. Appetite. 2001;36:79–83. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sclafani A, Cardieri C, Tucker K, Blusk D, Ackroff K. Intragastric glucose but not fructose conditions robust flavor preferences in rats. Am J Physiol. 1993;265:R320–R325. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1993.265.2.R320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sclafani A, Mann S. Carbohydrate taste preferences in rats: Glucose, sucrose, maltose, fructose and polycose compared. Physiol Behav. 1987;40:563–568. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90097-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sclafani A, Nissenbaum JW. Robust conditioned flavor preference produced by intragastric starch infusions in rats. Am J Physiol. 1988;255:R672–R675. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1988.255.4.R672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schachter S, Gross LP. Manipulated time and eating behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1968;10:98–106. doi: 10.1037/h0026285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shimizu M, Payne CR, Wansink B. When snacks become meals: How hunger and environmental cues bias food intake. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:63. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wansink B, Payne CR, Shimizu M. "Is this a meal or snack?" Situational cues that drive perceptions. Appetite. 2010;54:214–216. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Weingarten HP. Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: Basic behavioral properties. Appetite. 1984;5:147–158. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(84)80035-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Petrovich GD, Setlow B, Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdalo-hypothalamic circuit allows learned cues to override satiety and promote eating. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8748–8753. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-19-08748.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Holland PC, Petrovich GD, Gallagher M. The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats. Physiol Behav. 2002;76:117–129. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00688-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Petrovich GD, Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdalar and prefrontal pathways to the lateral hypothalamus are activated by a learned cue that stimulates eating. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8295–8302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2480-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Holland PC, Gallagher M. Medial prefrontal cortex is necessary for an appetitive contextual conditioned stimulus to promote eating in sated rats. J Neurosci. 2007;27:6436–6441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5001-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Holland PC, Petrovich GD. A neural systems analysis of the potentiation of feeding by conditioned stimuli. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:747–761. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Learned contextual cue potentiates eating in rats. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:362–367. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schachter S. Obesity and eating. Internal and external cues differentially affect the eating behavior of obese and normal subjects. Science. 1968;161:751–756. doi: 10.1126/science.161.3843.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Nisbett RE. Hunger, obesity, and the ventromedial hypothalamus. Psychol Rev. 1972;79:433–453. doi: 10.1037/h0033519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Herman CP, Polivy J. External cues in the control of food intake in humans: the sensory-normative distinction. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:722–728. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Olszewski PK, Alsio J, Schioth HB, Levine AS. Opioids as facilitators of feeding: can any food be rewarding? Physiol Behav. 2011;104:105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Grill HJ, Norgren R. The taste reactivity test. II. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in chronic thalamic and chronic decerebrate rats. Brain Res. 1978;143:281–297. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90569-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Giraudo SQ, Kotz CM, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Association between the amygdala and nucleus of the solitary tract in mu-opioid induced feeding in the rat. Brain Res. 1998;802:184–188. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00602-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kelley AE, Bakshi VP, Haber SN, Steininger TL, Will MJ, Zhang M. Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum. Physiol Behav. 2002;76:365–377. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00751-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zhang M, Kelley AE. Intake of saccharin, salt, and ethanol solutions is increased by infusion of a mu opioid agonist into the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;159:415–423. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0932-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zhang M, Gosnell BA, Kelley AE. Intake of high-fat food is selectively enhanced by mu opioid receptor stimulation within the nucleus accumbens. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics. 1998;285:908–914. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Stanley BG, Lanthier D, Leibowitz SF. Multiple brain sites sensitive to feeding stimulation by opioid agonists: a cannula-mapping study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1988;31:825–832. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(88)90391-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Glass MJ, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Naltrexone administered to central nucleus of amygdala or PVN: neural dissociation of diet and energy. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2000;279:R86–R92. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.1.R86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kim EM, Quinn JG, Spanswick D, O'Hare E. Feeding association between the nucleus of the solitary tract and the ventral tegmental area. Appetite. 2009;53:457–460. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Olszewski PK, Shaw TJ, Grace MK, Hoglund CE, Fredriksson R, Schioth HB, et al. Complexity of neural mechanisms underlying overconsumption of sugar in scheduled feeding: involvement of opioids, orexin, oxytocin and NPY. Peptides. 2009;30:226–233. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kirkham TC, Blundell JE. Effects of naloxone and naltrexone on meal patterns of freely-feeding rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1987;26:515–520. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(87)90158-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kirkham TC, Blundell JE. Effect of naloxone and naltrexone on the development of satiation measured in the runway: comparisons with d-amphetamine and d-fenfluramine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;25:123–128. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90241-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gosnell BA, Krahn DD, Majchrzak MJ. The effects of morphine on diet selection are dependent upon baseline diet preferences. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:207–212. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90322-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Naleid AM, Grace MK, Chimukangara M, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Paraventricular opioids alter intake of high-fat but not high-sucrose diet depending on diet preference in a binge model of feeding. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2007;293:R99–R105. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00675.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Woolley JD, Lee BS, Kim B, Fields HL. Opposing effects of intra-nucleus accumbens mu and kappa opioid agonists on sensory specific satiety. Neuroscience. 2007;146:1445–1452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Woolley JD, Lee BS, Fields HL. Nucleus accumbens opioids regulate flavor-based preferences in food consumption. Neuroscience. 2006;143:309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Woolley JD, Lee BS, Taha SA, Fields HL. Nucleus accumbens opioid signaling conditions short-term flavor preferences. Neuroscience. 2007;146:19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Glass MJ, Grace M, Cleary JP, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Potency of naloxone's anorectic effect in rats is dependent on diet preference. Am J Physiol. 1996;271:R217–R221. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.271.1.R217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bindra D. A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychol Rev. 1974;81:199–213. doi: 10.1037/h0036330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dickinson A, Balleine B. Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1994;22:1–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine's role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Fibiger HC, Phillips AG. Mesocorticolimbic dopamine systems and reward. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;537:206–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb42107.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Berridge KC. Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1996;20:1–25. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Food reward and cocaine increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens as measured by microdialysis. Life Sci. 1988;42:1705–1712. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(88)90036-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Liang NC, Hajnal A, Norgren R. Sham feeding corn oil increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006;291:R1236–R1239. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00226.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Baldo BA, Kelley AE. Discrete neurochemical coding of distinguishable motivational processes: insights from nucleus accumbens control of feeding. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:439–459. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0741-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wirtshafter D, Stratford TR. Evidence for motivational effects elicited by activation of GABA-A or dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2010;96:342–346. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Vucetic Z, Reyes TM. Central dopaminergic circuitry controlling food intake and reward: implications for the regulation of obesity. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Syst Biol Med. 2010;2:577–593. doi: 10.1002/wsbm.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Skibicka KP, Dickson SL. Ghrelin and food reward: The story of potential underlying substrates. Peptides. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2011.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Figlewicz DP, Benoit SC. Insulin, leptin, and food reward: update 2008. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2009;296:R9–R19. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.90725.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Taylor KM, Mark GP, Hoebel BG. Conditioned taste aversion from neostigmine or methyl-naloxonium in the nucleus accumbens. Physiol Behav. 2011;104:82–86. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.McDonald RV, Parker LA, Siegel S. Conditioned sucrose aversions produced by naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from acutely administered morphine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58:1003–1008. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00313-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Prokasy ABWF, editor. Classical conditioning: Current theory and research. New York: Appleton, Century-Crofts; 1972. pp. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mackintosh N. A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 1975;82:276–298. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Pearce JM, Hall G. A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev. 1980;87:532–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sclafani A, Ackroff K. Reinforcement value of sucrose measured by progressive ratio operant licking in the rat. Physiol Behav. 2003;79:663–670. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Schier LA, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. A potential role for the hippocampus in energy intake and body weight regulation. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007;7:613–616. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Walls EK, Jarrard LE. Memory inhibition and energy regulation. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:731–746. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chan KH, Morell JR, Jarrard LE, Davidson TL. Reconsideration of the role of the hippocampus in learned inhibition. Behav Brain Res. 2001;119:111–130. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00363-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bouton ME, King DA. Effect of context on performance to conditioned stimuli with mixed histories of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1986;12:4–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and classical conditioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1994;3:49–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Davidson TL, Benoit SC. The learned function of food-deprivation cues: A role for conditioned modulation. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1996;24:46–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, Friedman JM. Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue.[see comment][erratum appears in Nature 1995 Mar 30;374(6521):479] Nature. 1994;372:425–432. doi: 10.1038/372425a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Smith GP, Gibbs J, Jerome C, Pi-Sunyer FX, Kissileff HR, Thornton J. The satiety effect of cholecystokinin: a progress report. Peptides. 1981;2(Suppl 2):57–59. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(81)90011-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Holst JJ. The physiology of glucagon-like peptide 1. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:1409–1439. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00034.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Tschop M, Smiley DL, Heiman ML. Ghrelin induces adiposity in rodents. Nature. 2000;407:908–913. doi: 10.1038/35038090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Davidson TL. Learning about deprivation intensity stimuli. Behav Neurosci. 1987;101:198–208. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.101.2.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Kanoski SE, Walls EK, Davidson TL. Interoceptive "satiety" signals produced by leptin and CCK. Peptides. 2007;28:988–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Tracy AL, Walls EK, Clegg D, Benoit SC. The interoceptive cue properties of ghrelin generalize to cues produced by food deprivation. Peptides. 2005;26:1602–1610. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Seeley RJ, Benoit SC, Davidson TL. Discriminative cues produced by NPY do not generalize to the interoceptive cues produced by food deprivation.[erratum appears in Physiol Behav 1996 Apr-May;59(4–5):1015] Physiol Behav. 1995;58:1237–1241. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02074-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dickinson A, Balleine B. The role of learning in the operation of motivational systems. In: Pashler Hal, Gallistel Randy., editors. 2002:motivation, and emotion. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Balleine B. Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1992;18:236–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: Links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Benoit SC, Davis JF, Davidson TL. Learned and cognitive controls of food intake. Brain Res. 2010;1350:71–76. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Holland PC, Bouton ME. Hippocampus and context in classical conditioning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9:195–202. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)80027-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.O'Keefe J. A computational theory of the hippocampal cognitive map. Prog Brain Res. 1990;83:301–312. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)61258-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.O'Keefe J, Nadel L. Precis of O'Keefe and Nadel's The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Behavioral & Brain Sciences. 1979;2:487–533. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Okeefe J, Nadel L. The Cognitive Map As A Hippocampus. Behavioral And Brain Sciences. 1979;2:520–528. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Kjelstrup KB, Solstad T, Brun VH, Hafting T, Leutgeb S, Witter MP, et al. Finite scale of spatial representation in the hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:140–143. doi: 10.1126/science.1157086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Muller RU, Bostock E, Taube JS, Kubie JL. On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:7235–7251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07235.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Myers CE, Gluck MA. Context, conditioning, and hippocampal rerepresentation in animal learning. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:835–847. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.5.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Honey RC, Ward-Robinson J. Transfer between contextual conditional discriminations: an examination of how stimulus conjuctions are represented. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2001;27:196–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Ferbinteanu J, McDonald RJ. Dorsal/ventral hippocampus, fornix, and conditioned place preference. Hippocampus. 2001;11:187–200. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Jarrard LE, Davidson TL, Bowring B. Functional differentiation within the medial temporal lobe in the rat. Hippocampus. 2004;14:434–449. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Chan K, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC, Jarrard LE. Hippocampal lesions impair retention of discriminative responding based on energy state cues. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:97–105. doi: 10.1037/a0018402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Hock BJ, Jr, Bunsey MD. Differential effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:7027–7032. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-07027.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Davidson TL, Jarrard LE. A role for hippocampus in the utilization of hunger signals. Behav Neural Biol. 1993;59:167–171. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90925-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Kennedy PJ, Shapiro ML. Retrieving memories via internal context requires the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:6979–6985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1388-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Hebben N, Corkin S, Eichenbaum H, Shedlack K. Diminished ability to interpret and report internal states after bilateral medial temporal resection: case H.M. Behav Neurosci. 1985;99:1031–1039. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.99.6.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Rozin P, Dow S, Moscovitch M, Rajaram S. What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychol Sci. 1998;9:392–396. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Higgs S, Williamson AC, Rotshtein P, Humphreys GW. Sensory-specific satiety is intact in amnesics who eat multiple meals. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:623–628. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02132.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Higgs S. Cognitive influences on food intake: the effects of manipulating memory for recent eating. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:734–739. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Higgs S. Memory for recent eating and its influence on subsequent food intake. Appetite. 2002;39:159–166. doi: 10.1006/appe.2002.0500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.DelParigi A, Chen K, Salbe AD, Hill JO, Wing RR, Reiman EM, et al. Persistence of abnormal neural responses to a meal in postobese individuals. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2004;28:370–377. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.DelParigi A, Chen K, Salbe AD, Reiman EM, Tataranni PA. Sensory experience of food and obesity: a positron emission tomography study of the brain regions affected by tasting a liquid meal after a prolonged fast. Neuroimage. 2005;24:436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Gautier JF, Del Parigi A, Chen K, Salbe AD, Bandy D, Pratley RE, et al. Effect of satiation on brain activity in obese and lean women. Obes Res. 2001;9:676–684. doi: 10.1038/oby.2001.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Min DK, Tuor UI, Chelikani PK. Gastric distention induced functional magnetic resonance signal changes in the rodent brain. Neuroscience. 2011;179:151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Wang G-J, Yang J, Volkow ND, Telang F, Ma Y, Zhu W, et al. Gastric stimulation in obese subjects activates the hippocampus and other regions involved in brain reward circuitry. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:15641–15645. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601977103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Benoit SC, Davidson TL, Chan KH, Trigilio T, Jarrard LE. Pavlovian conditioning and extinction of context cues and punctate CSs in rats with ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus. Psychobiology. 1999;27:26–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Davidson TL, Jarrard LE. The hippocampus and inhibitory learning: a 'Gray' area? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:261–271. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Schmelzeis MC, Mittleman G. The hippocampus and reward: effects of hippocampal lesions on progressive-ratio responding. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:1049–1066. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.5.1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Clifton PG, Vickers SP, Somerville EM. Little and often: Ingestive behavior patterns following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:502–511. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.3.502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Davidson TL, Chan K, Jarrard LE, Kanoski SE, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. Contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to energy and body weight regulation. Hippocampus. 2009;19:235–252. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Huang XF, Koutcherov I, Lin S, Wang HQ, Storlien L. Localization of leptin receptor mRNA expression in mouse brain. Neuroreport. 1996;7:2635–2638. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199611040-00045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Scott MM, Lachey JL, Sternson SM, Lee CE, Elias CF, Friedman JM, et al. Leptin targets in the mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. 2009;514:518–532. doi: 10.1002/cne.22025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Zigman JM, Jones JE, Lee CE, Saper CB, Elmquist JK. Expression of ghrelin receptor mRNA in the rat and the mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. 2006;494:528–548. doi: 10.1002/cne.20823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Guan X, Yu H, Palyha O, McKee K, Feighner S, Sirinathsinghji D, et al. Distribution of mRNA encoding the growth hormone secretagogue receptor in brain and peripheral tissues. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1997;48:23–29. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(97)00071-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Merchenthaler I, Lane M, Shughrue P. Distribution of pre-pro-glucagon and glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor messenger RNAs in the rat central nervous system. J Comp Neurol. 1999;403:261–280. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990111)403:2<261::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Zahniser NR, Goens MB, Hanaway PJ, Vinych JV. Characterization and regulation of insulin receptors in rat brain. J Neurochem. 1984;42:1354–1362. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1984.tb02795.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Pratchayasakul W, Kerdphoo S, Petsophonsakul P, Pongchaidecha A, Chattipakorn N, Chattipakorn SC. Effects of high-fat diet on insulin receptor function in rat hippocampus and the level of neuronal corticosterone. Life Sci. 2011;88:619–627. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Harvey J. Novel actions of leptin in the hippocampus. Annals of Medicine. 2003;35:197–206. doi: 10.1080/07853890310008251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Shanley LJ, Irving AJ, Harvey J. Leptin enhances NMDA receptor function and modulates hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:RC186. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-j0001.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Farr SA, Banks WA, Morley JE. Effects of leptin on memory processing. Peptides. 2006;27:1420–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Diano S, Farr SA, Benoit SC, McNay EC, da Silva I, Horvath B, et al. Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:381–388. doi: 10.1038/nn1656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Craft S, Asthana S, Cook DG, Baker LD, Cherrier M, Purganan K, et al. Insulin dose-response effects on memory and plasma amyloid precursor protein in Alzheimer's disease: Interactions with apolipoprotein E genotype. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28:809–822. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00087-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Park CR, Seeley RJ, Craft S, Woods SC. Intracerebroventricular insulin enhances memory in a passive-avoidance task. Physiol Behav. 2000;68:509–514. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00220-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Watson GS, Craft S. Modulation of memory by insulin and glucose: neuropsychological observations in Alzheimer's disease. Eur J Pharmacol. 2004;490:97–113. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.02.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.During MJ, Cao L, Zuzga DS, Francis JS, Fitzsimons HL, Jiao X, et al. Glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor is involved in learning and neuroprotection. Nat Med. 2003;9:1173–1179. doi: 10.1038/nm919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Davis JF, Choi DL, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. Signaling through the ghrelin receptor modulates hippocampal function and meal anticipation in mice. Physiol Behav. 2011;103:39–43. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Kanoski SE, Hayes MR, Greenwald HS, Fortin SM, Gianessi CA, Gilbert JR, et al. Hippocampal Leptin Signaling Reduces Food Intake and Modulates Food-Related Memory Processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011 doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron. 2010;65:7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Gasbarri A, Packard MG, Campana E, Pacitti C. Anterograde and retrograde tracing of projections from the ventral tegmental area to the hippocampal formation in the rat. Brain Res Bull. 1994;33:445–452. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90288-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Gasbarri A, Verney C, Innocenzi R, Campana E, Pacitti C. Mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons innervating the hippocampal formation in the rat: a combined retrograde tracing and immunohistochemical study. Brain Res. 1994;668:71–79. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90512-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Pennartz CM, Groenewegen HJ, Lopes da Silva FH. The nucleus accumbens as a complex of functionally distinct neuronal ensembles: an integration of behavioural, electrophysiological and anatomical data. Prog Neurobiol. 1994;42:719–761. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(94)90025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Mulder AB, Hodenpijl MG, Lopes da Silva FH. Electrophysiology of the hippocampal and amygdaloid projections to the nucleus accumbens of the rat: convergence, segregation, and interaction of inputs. J Neurosci. 1998;18:5095–5102. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-13-05095.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Cenquizca LA, Swanson LW. Analysis of direct hippocampal cortical field CA1 axonal projections to diencephalon in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;497:101–114. doi: 10.1002/cne.20985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Muller M, Faber-Zuschratter H, Yanagawa Y, Stork O, Schwegler H, Linke R. Synaptology of ventral CA1 and subiculum projections to the basomedial nucleus of the amygdala in the mouse: relation to GABAergic interneurons. Brain Struct Funct. 2011 doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0326-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Canteras NS, Swanson LW. Projections of the ventral subiculum to the amygdala, septum, and hypothalamus: a PHAL anterograde tract-tracing study in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1992;324:180–194. doi: 10.1002/cne.903240204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Swanson LW. A direct projection from Ammon's horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res. 1981;217:150–154. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90192-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Cummings DE, Purnell JQ, Frayo RS, Schmidova K, Wisse BE, Weigle DS. A preprandial rise in plasma ghrelin levels suggests a role in meal initiation in humans. Diabetes. 2001;50:1714–1719. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.50.8.1714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Frecka JM, Mattes RD. Possible entrainment of ghrelin to habitual meal patterns in humans. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol. 2008;294:G699–G707. doi: 10.1152/ajpgi.00448.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Verbaeys I, Tolle V, Swennen Q, Zizzari P, Buyse J, Epelbaum J, et al. Scheduled feeding results in adipogenesis and increased acylated ghrelin. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2011;300:E1103–E1111. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00551.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Blum ID, Patterson Z, Khazall R, Lamont EW, Sleeman MW, Horvath TL, et al. Reduced anticipatory locomotor responses to scheduled meals in ghrelin receptor deficient mice. Neuroscience. 2009;164:351–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Duffey KJ, Popkin BM. Energy density, portion size, and eating occasions: contributions to increased energy intake in the United States, 1977–2006. PLoS Med. 2011;8:e1001050. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Overweight and Obesity: Data and Statistics. Center for Disease Control and Prevention; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Chou SY, Grossman M, Saffer H. An economic analysis of adult obesity: results from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. J Health Econ. 2004;23:565–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2003.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Gallo AE. Food advertising in the United States. In: Frazao E, editor. America's eating habits: changes and consequences. Washington, DC: United States Department of Agriculture; 1999. pp. 173–180. [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Kunkel D. Children and television advertising. In: Singer D, editor. Handbook of children and the media. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001. pp. 375–393. [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Cholerton B, Baker LD, Craft S. Insulin resistance and pathological brain ageing. Diabet Med. 2011 doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2011.03464.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Crichton GE, Elias MF, Buckley J, Murphy KJ, Bryan J, Frisardi V. Metabolic Syndrome, Cognitive Performance, and Dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011 doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Tangney CC, Wilson RS. Dietary fat intake and 6-year cognitive change in an older biracial community population. Neurology. 2004;62:1573–1579. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000123250.82849.b6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Tangney CC, Bennett DA, Aggarwal N, et al. Dietary fats and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:194–200. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.2.194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Eskelinen M, Ngandu T, Helkala E, Tuomilehto J, Nissinen A, Soininen H, et al. Fat intake at midlife and cognitive impairment later in life: a population-based CAIDE study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:741–747. doi: 10.1002/gps.1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Francis HM, Stevenson RJ. Higher reported saturated fat and refined sugar intake is associated with reduced hippocampal-dependent memory and sensitivity to interoceptive signals. Behav Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0025998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Greenwood CE, Winocur G. Cognitive impairment in rats fed high-fat diets: a specific effect of saturated fatty-acid intake. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:451–459. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.3.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Ross AP, Bartness TJ, Mielke JG, Parent MB. A high fructose diet impairs spatial memory in male rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92:410–416. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Mielke JG, Taghibiglou C, Liu L, Zhang Y, Jia Z, Adeli K, et al. A biochemical and functional characterization of diet-induced brain insulin resistance. J Neurochem. 2005;93:1568–1578. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03155.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Jurdak N, Lichtenstein A, Kanarek R. Diet-induced obesity and spatial cognition in young male rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2008;11:48–54. doi: 10.1179/147683008X301333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Kanoski SE, Meisel RL, Mullins AJ, Davidson TL. The effects of energy-rich diets on discrimination reversal learning and on BDNF in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2007;182:57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Kanoski SE, Zhang Y, Zheng W, Davidson TL. The effects of a high-energy diet on hippocampal function and blood-brain barrier integrity in the rat. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;21:207–219. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Different patterns of memory impairments accompany short- and longer-term maintenance on a high-energy diet. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2010;36:313–319. doi: 10.1037/a0017228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Small SA, Schobel SA, Buxton RB, Witter MP, Barnes CA. A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:585–601. doi: 10.1038/nrn3085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav. 2011;103:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Molteni R, Barnard RJ, Ying Z, Roberts CK, Gomez-Pinilla F. A high-fat, refined sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor, neuronal plasticity, and learning. Neuroscience. 2002;112:803–814. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00123-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Banks WA, Farr SA, Morley JE. The effects of high fat diets on the blood-brain barrier transport of leptin: failure or adaptation? Physiol Behav. 2006;88:244–248. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Farr S, Yamada K, Butterfield D, Abdul H, Xu L, Miller N, et al. Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive impairment. Endocrinology. 2008;149:2628–2636. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Stranahan AM, Cutler RG, Button C, Telljohann R, Mattson MP. Diet-induced elevations in serum cholesterol are associated with alterations in hippocampal lipid metabolism and increased oxidative stress. J Neurochem. 2011;118:611–615. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07351.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Mielke J, Nicolitch K, Avellaneda V, Earlam K, Ahuja T, Mealing G, et al. Longitudinal study of the effects of a high-fat diet on glucose regulation, hippocampal function, and cerebral insulin sensitivity in C57BL/6 mice. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175:374–382. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.McNay EC, Ong CT, McCrimmon RJ, Cresswell J, Bogan JS, Sherwin RS. Hippocampal memory processes are modulated by insulin and high-fat-induced insulin resistance. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010;93:546–553. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Munzberg H. Differential leptin access into the brain--a hierarchical organization of hypothalamic leptin target sites? Physiol Behav. 2008;94:664–669. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Munzberg H, Bjornholm M, Bates SH, Myers MG., Jr Leptin receptor action and mechanisms of leptin resistance. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2005;62:642–652. doi: 10.1007/s00018-004-4432-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Perreault M, Istrate N, Wang L, Nichols AJ, Tozzo E, Stricker-Krongrad A. Resistance to the orexigenic effect of ghrelin in dietary-induced obesity in mice: reversal upon weight loss. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2004;28:879–885. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Briggs DI, Enriori PJ, Lemus MB, Cowley MA, Andrews ZB. Diet-induced obesity causes ghrelin resistance in arcuate NPY/AgRP neurons. Endocrinology. 2010;151:4745–4755. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-0556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES