Abstract
Although sensitive maternal behaviors improve later quality of mother-infant interaction and subsequently infant development, little is known regarding how an intervention might promote early premature infant social interactive behavior. This study compared the frequency of premature infant engagement and disengagement behaviors during two maternally administered interventions, the multi-sensory auditory, tactile, visual and vestibular intervention (ATVV) and kangaroo care (KC) for 26 infants between 31 and 46 weeks PMA. The ATVV intervention elicited more disengagement (M = 24 vs. 12, p = .0003), trended toward more engagement (M = 21 vs. 15.7, p = .06) and more potent engagement (M = 24 vs. 12, p = .0003), subtle disengagement (M = 25 vs. 11.9, p < .0001), and potent disengagement (M = 22.9 vs. 14, p = . 006) behaviors than did KC. The ATVV intervention may be an intervention to promote the infant’s learning how to regulate engagement and disengagement behaviors.
Keywords: VLBW Infant, Premature Infant, Infant Behavior, Social Interaction, ATVV Intervention, Kangaroo Care, Social Competence, Mother-Infant Interaction
While sensitive maternal behaviors and mother-infant synchrony are known to improve later quality of mother-infant interactions and subsequently infant development, [1] little is known about how interventions might promote emerging premature infant social interactive behavior, thus facilitating the infant’s role in the developing pattern of mother-infant interaction. [2] Two maternally administered interventions - the multi-sensory auditory, tactile, visual and vestibular intervention (ATVV) and kangaroo care (KC) - have been shown to support sleep and alert states in preterm infants as well as early growth and development. [3–7] However, few studies have examined the effects of these interventions on early infant social interactive behaviors that are likely to affect subsequent mother-infant interactions. This study compared the frequency of infant engagement and disengagement behavioral cues during two maternally administered interventions, the ATVV intervention and KC, among very low birth weight (VLBW) premature infants.
Social interactive behaviors start emerging around 32 weeks post-menstrual age (PMA) for infants born prematurely. [8] One of the benefits of social interactive behaviors is that they reinforce the mother’s social actions and encourage her during early interactions. [9] For example, smiling, a potent engagement behavioral cue, emerges out of attentive engagement with an interactive caregiver. [10] In premature infants, early smiling most often occurs during REM sleep, [11, 12] yet our clinical experience has been that mothers interpret the smile as an engagement cue and are likely to respond by smiling back at their sleeping infants. In older infants, smiling is a potent engagement behavior that indicates the infant’s interest in initiating/maintaining social interaction. [13]
The infant’s capacity to become actively engaged in a social interaction is linked to his/her ability to regulate behavior by gazing away (disengaging or withdrawing) from their interactive partners and then re-engaging. [10] Ultimately, the infant’s ability to control attention during social interaction supports the infant's use of smiles to initiate early communication with a partner. This engagement behavior may be an example of the emergence of early social behaviors that support dyadic responsivity and later patterns of social competence.
Early interventions that support development and theoretically the emergence of social interactive behaviors include the ATVV intervention and KC. The ATVV intervention (as described in more detail below) modulates infant behavioral state by influencing sleep-wake behaviors and increasing the frequency of alertness in premature infants between 32 and 35 weeks PMA. [5–7, 14–17] The ATVV intervention also has been found to improve patterns of early mother-infant interaction. [2] When performed by the mother, the ATVV intervention improved maternal sensitivity towards her infant’s cues and cognitive-growth-fostering behaviors. Additionally, infant clarity of cues and responsiveness were significantly improved after the ATVV intervention. During the ATVV intervention, the mother provides social interaction (auditory and visual cues) and learns to modify her behavior based on the infant’s engagement and disengagement cues. [9] The ATVV also offers infants the opportunity to learn social skills. Past studies have also shown that the ATVV has promising short-term benefits, including improved feeding progression leading to reduced length of hospital stay. [6]
KC, or holding the infant between the mother’s breasts in skin-to-skin contact, is another popular neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) intervention. [18] KC is a safe intervention [19–23] and results in better head growth, more positive parenting, less distress and negative physiological responses to painful procedures, and possibly better development than hospital care. [24–30] In developed countries, KC is used as an intervention for sicker and smaller preterm infants that offers parents increased contact during visits. KC has been shown to have immediate behavioral benefits for the infant, including more sleep (especially quiet sleep) and less crying, as well as greater respiratory regularity than when the infant was in the incubator [4, 21, 31–33] though these effects were not maintained after the end of KC. [34] Thus, KC has an immediate effect of promoting sleep but does not appear to affect overall sleep-wake patterns. When KC was used throughout hospitalization, mothers were more likely to breastfeed. [35, 36]
The ATVV intervention provides infants with a greater variety of stimuli than does KC and promotes alertness, the optimal sleep-wake state for social interaction; whereas KC promotes sleep. Thus, the ATVV intervention provides more opportunities for infants to practice responding and communicating with their mothers and potentially to improve their neurological development. [9] Considering the effects of ATVV and KC, it was hypothesized that the ATVV intervention would elicit more engagement and disengagement behavioral cues in the infants while KC would elicit fewer of these behaviors. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to compare the frequency of premature infant engagement and disengagement behavioral cues during two maternally administered interventions, the ATVV intervention and KC.
Methods
Design
This report is part of a larger study that tested the effects of two maternally administered interventions for VLBW preterm infants on infant health and development, maternal psychological well-being, and the maternal-infant relationship. Infants and mothers were enrolled from the time the infant was no longer critically ill until the infant was 12 months of age corrected for prematurity. Mothers and infants were randomly assigned to the ATVV intervention, KC, or an attention control group. Mothers in the ATVV intervention or KC groups administered the intervention a minimum of three times weekly and were video recorded weekly during intervention sessions. Mothers in the attention control group (education about supplies needed for home care) were not included in this secondary analysis because their infants did not experience an intervention.
Setting and Sample
The research was conducted at the neonatal intensive care and intermediate care units of four hospitals: Brenner Children's Hospital in Winston-Salem, NC; Duke University Health System in Durham, NC; Mt. Sinai Hospital in Chicago, IL; and Stroger Hospital in Chicago, IL. These sites were selected because they serve different populations: Brenner and Duke are Southern and serve urban, suburban, and rural populations of diverse socioeconomic status, whereas Mt. Sinai and Stroger Medical Centers serve a Northern, urban, and low socioeconomic status population. Three of the hospitals have inborn infants and all of the nurseries received infants by transport. The physical settings of the four nurseries were similar. The intensive care rooms held between 10 – 18 infants in an open room design. The convalescent rooms were also open rooms and designed to care for 6 – 8 in one room.
Infants were enrolled when they were no longer critically ill. Gestational age at birth was assessed by mother’s dates and exam. [37] If a discrepancy was identified between the mother’s dates and the gestational age examination, gestational age was determined by the examination. For this secondary analysis, the first 26 mother-infant pairs were analyzed.
Data from thirteen mother infant dyads from North Carolina (7 KC and 6 ATVV) and thirteen mother-infant dyads from Chicago (6 KC and 7 ATVV) participated in this analysis. The participants ranged in age at birth from 21 to 32 weeks gestation with a mean of 27 weeks. Birth weight ranged from 490 to 1470 grams with a mean of 900 grams. PMA at the time of the videotapes ranged from 31 to 46 weeks during this pilot study. Weight at entry into the study ranged from 1030 to 2693 grams with a mean of 1650 grams. Twelve infants were female and fourteen infants were male. Eighteen Black, four Caucasian and four “Other” mothers participated in the research. Maternal age ranged from 18 to 41 years with a mean of 26 years. Seventeen mothers were primiparous and six were multiparous. Maternal educational level ranged from 11 to 18 years with a mean of 13 years of completed education. More mothers delivered via cesarean section (n = 19) than normal spontaneous vaginal deliveries (n = 7). There were no significant differences between the ATVV and KC groups on these variables (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Group 1 – ATVV Intervention (n = 13) |
Group 2 – Kangaroo care (n = 13) |
|
---|---|---|
GA at birth (weeks) | 26.31 (21–31)* | 26.77 (23 – 32) |
Birth weight (grams) | 830.15 (490 – 11470) | 999.17 (500 – 1465) |
PMA at entry into the study (weeks) | 33.84 (28.14 – 39.71) | 34.56 (31.14 – 39.00) |
Weight at entry into the study (grams) | 1603 (1030 – 2693) | 1663.46 (1132 – 2435) |
Infant Gender (M/F) | 6/7 | 8/5 |
Maternal Race (Black/White/Other) | 7/4/2 | 11/0/2 |
Maternal Age | 26.54 (19–36) | 26.85 (18–41) |
Type of Delivery (Vaginal/C-Section) | 2/11 | 5/8 |
Maternal Parity (Primip/Multip) | 10/2** | 7/4** |
Maternal Educational Level (in years) | 13.85 (11–16) | 13.31 (10–18) |
Mean (range)
Missing Data
Developmental Interventions
The ATVV intervention. The ATVV intervention incorporates normal maternal behaviors with standardized stimuli. [38] The 15-minute intervention starts with auditory stimulation via infant-directed talk by the mother. The infant-directed talk (motherese) continues throughout the 15 minutes and is performed using a higher pitched voice with pauses, offering the infant the opportunity to respond. Following at least 30 seconds of talk, the infant is then placed in a supine position for a 10-minute moderate pressure massage. First the head is massaged, followed by strokes on the chest and abdomen, the arms from shoulder to wrist, and the legs from thigh to ankle. The infant is repositioned to the prone position and the back is massaged using straight continuous strokes and circular strokes directly over the spine. The head is then massaged from the hairline to the nape of the neck. Following massage, the infant is swaddled and rocked horizontally for the final 5 minutes. Throughout the entire 15-minute ATVV intervention, the mother attempts eye contact with the infant when the infant is alert. Furthermore, the intervention is designed to be contingent on infant behavior. [38] Mothers are taught how to recognize subtle and potent engagement and disengagement behavioral cues and how to respond to them. As a result, if the infant expresses disengagement cues during a particular component of the intervention, the mother pauses allowing the infant to disengage and then re-engage. When the infant is re-engaged, the massage continues. Infant individuality is continually assessed, e.g., some infants prefer different components of the massage. When the mother has determined which tactile components receive a positive response from the infant, these tactile components are repeated, and the tactile components that elicit potent disengagement cues are discontinued.
Mothers were taught the ATVV intervention and asked to administer the intervention for a minimum of three times weekly. The nurse taught the mother the intervention on a doll and also demonstrated the ATVV intervention on the infant for the mother to observe. Mothers gave a return demonstration to assure they were able to administer the ATVV intervention to criterion (85% or greater reliability with the ATVV intervention checklist).
Kangaroo care. KC is skin-to-skin contact between the mother and her infant. The infant, clad in a diaper only, is placed on the mother’s chest, usually between the breasts, and then the mother is wrapped in a blanket for warmth and privacy. Mothers were taught this intervention and asked to administer KC for a minimum of three times weekly for 1.5 hours. Mothers were given support and their questions answered by the research nurse. Staff nurses were also trained in the administration of KC so that they could also offer support to others with handling and transfer of the infants from the incubator to the KC position.
Nursery environment and intervention fidelity. The interventions were provided in the NICU. Per our stress reduction protocol and to provide a similar nursery environment among the nurseries, the lights were dimmed and sound levels kept to a minimum prior and during the intervention and throughout the assessment period. Fidelity of both interventions was assessed by the research team via the video recordings used in this secondary analysis.
Outcome Measures
Infant behavioral cues were measured by the frequency of infant engagement and disengagement cues. [13] Engagement cues are behavioral cues that are indicators of the infant’s interest in participating in social interaction while disengagement cues are indicators of the infant needing to break away from social interaction. Examples of engagement cues include facial gaze, mutual facial gaze, eyes brightening, and relaxed posture. Examples of disengagement cues include yawning, looking away (gaze aversion), or crying. The engagement and disengagement cues were further categorized by subtle and potent behavioral cues (see Table 2 for the cues identified in our sample of VLBW infants). Engagement and disengagement behavioral cues were selected because previous research has shown that parents who can read subtle behavioral cues are better able to read and respond to their infants and are more sensitive in their interactions with their infants. [13]
Table 2.
Behavior | Definition |
---|---|
Subtle Engagement | |
Facial brightening | infant moves mouth or cheeks as if smiling |
Hands open | fingers apart from palms but not stiff |
Potent Engagement | |
Facial gaze/focus | infant looks towards mother's face without making direct eye contact |
Mutual gaze | infant makes direct eye contact with mother |
Cyclic extremity movement | infant moves arms and/or legs in a slow, fluid manner |
Relaxation posture | awake infant (behavioral state =1) lies still with extremities relaxed |
Subtle Disengagement | |
Eyes clinched | eyes narrow or shut tightly |
Gaze aversion | infant breaks eye contact with mother or looks purposefully away |
Finger splay | infant opens hands fully and stiffens fingers |
Struggling movements | infant jerks extremities or tries to avoid mother's contact by moving away |
Finger extension | infant extends and stiffens a single finger |
Hunger posture | infant lies with arms drawn up to sides, fists clenched beside shoulders |
Potent Disengagement | |
Crying | infant wails loudly |
Whining | infant makes soft cries or whimpers |
Fussing | infant makes distressed sounds quieter than crying |
Cry face | infant wrinkles face, clenches eyes as if crying but without making sound |
Halt hand | infant stiffens hand as with finger splay but in addition extends it towards the mother as if trying to block something |
Maximum lateral gaze aversion | infant turns head to the side to avoid mother's face/gaze |
Two research assistants were trained to criterion on the infant behavioral cues. One research assistant coded all of the video recordings. The second research assistant coded 25% of the video recordings. Since the coders viewed the two interventions during coding, the second research assistant was blinded to the purpose of the study. Initial inter-rater reliability between the two coders was established by measuring the intra-class correlation between the two coders. The intra-class correlation was maintained at .713 (df = 8, p = .012) for the total engagement behaviors and .649 (df = 8, p = .012) for the total disengagement behaviors, documenting significant inter rater agreement. [39, 40]
The frequency of engagement and disengagement behaviors were coded for each 15-second interval for a total of 15 minutes by the trained rater. In preparation for analysis, the frequencies for each behavioral cue were totaled for each 2.5 minute period and then grouped into engagement and disengagement behaviors. The behaviors were then totaled for the four different categories: subtle or potent and engagement or disengagement behaviors.
Procedure
The study was granted human subjects approval by the Institutional Review Boards at the respective universities and the clinical sites. After informed consent was obtained from the mother, the mother and her infant were randomly assigned to the ATVV intervention (Group 1), KC (Group 2) or the attention control groups. Mothers were taught their respective interventions and were asked to administer the intervention for a minimum of three times per week. The mothers and infants were video recorded weekly over the course of the infants’ hospital stay while the mother was administering either the ATVV intervention or KC. These video recordings were used for this secondary analysis.
The interventions began when the infants were at least 1000 grams and were assessed as clinically stable. Per protocol, the ATVV Intervention was provided 30 minutes prior to the next anticipated feeding. If the mother missed the feeding, the intervention began 30 minutes after completion of the feeding. KC was provided at the same time intervals. Infants in the study were already on monitoring equipment and thus were continuously monitored during the intervention. Nurses also evaluated the infant’s vital signs and temperature before and after the intervention session. Neither intervention was administered if the baby was scheduled for medical procedures or eye exams or if the baby was receiving mechanical or positive pressure ventilation.
Data Analysis
Sample characteristics were analyzed using descriptive statistics. To determine whether any significant differences existed between infants receiving different interventions, the frequency of the engagement and disengagement behavioral cues were analyzed using the Wilcoxon Two-Sample Rank Sum Test. The Wilcoxon Two Sample Rank Sum test was conducted for the total frequency of engagement and disengagement behavior as well as for the subtle and potent engagement and disengagement behavior scores.
Results
A total of 36 video recordings were coded. Frequency of the total engagement cues ranged from 4–65 and the total disengagement cues ranged from 17–122 for infants assigned to the ATVV group compared to 1–129 and 2–139 respectively for infants in the KC group. For infants assigned to the ATVV group, the frequency of subtle engagement cues ranged from 3–39, potent engagement cues ranged from 1–62, subtle disengagement cues ranged from 17–105, and potent disengagement cues ranged from 1–38. For infants assigned to the KC group, the frequency of subtle engagement cues ranged from 1–78, potent engagement cues ranged from 0–51 (twelve infants exhibited no potent engagement cues), subtle disengagement cues ranged from 1–99, and potent disengagement cues ranged from 0–47 (six infants exhibited no potent disengagement cues).
A one-sided Wilcoxon Two-Sample Rank Sum Test (normal approximation) was used for the analysis. The ATVV intervention elicited more disengagement (mean of 24.6 vs. 12.4, p = .0003) and a trend toward more engagement behaviors (mean of 21.3 vs. 15.7, p = .06) than did KC. Separate analysis of the subtle and potent behavioral cues revealed that the ATVV intervention elicited significantly more potent engagement (24.4 vs.12.6), subtle disengagement (25.1 vs. 11.9), and potent disengagement (22.9 vs. 14.1) behaviors from infants than did KC. Table 3 summarizes the mean, median, and statistical significance between the two groups.
Table 3.
Category | ATVV (n = 18) |
KC (n = 18) |
Wilcoxon Two- Sample |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rank Sum | |||||||
mean | median | mean | median | Statistic | p value | ||
Total | 21.3 | 31.0 | 15.7 | 20.5 | 383.0 | .06 | |
Engagement | |||||||
Total | 24.6 | 65.0 | 12.4 | 13.0 | 443.0 | .0003 | |
Disengagement | |||||||
Subtle | 18.6 | 18.5 | 18.4 | 17.0 | 334.5 | .49 | |
Engagement | |||||||
Potent | 24.4 | 11.5 | 12.6 | 0.0 | 440.0 | .0003 | |
Engagement | |||||||
Subtle | 25.1 | 51.0 | 11.9 | 8.5 | 451.0 | < .0001 | |
Disengagement | |||||||
Potent | 22.9 | 16.5 | 14.1 | 2.0 | 412.0 | .006 | |
Disengagement |
One-sided analysis with Wilcoxon Two-Sample Rank Sum Test (Normal approximation)
Discussion
VLBW premature infants in this research study exhibited a higher frequency of disengagement behavioral cues (subtle and potent behaviors combined) during the ATVV intervention than during KC. Infants also trended toward more engagement behaviors in response to the ATVV intervention. When the two categories of engagement and disengagement cues were further categorized into the subcategories of potent and subtle cues and then analyzed separately, infants receiving the ATVV intervention exhibited more potent engagement and subtle and potent disengagement behaviors when compared with KC. Thus, our findings show that the ATVV intervention offers an environment in which the infant is able to respond to the mother’s behavior. The mother then has the opportunity to adapt or synchronize her behavior to the infant’s cues. When the infant exhibits an engagement cue, a positive maternal response reinforces engagement. Likewise, when the infant exhibits a disengagement cue, the mother’s sensitive response to the cue facilitates the infant’s ability to regulate her behavior. Disengagement should not be considered a negative finding, rather it shows that the infant is learning how to regulate interaction – how to maintain attention to engage and then withdraw (disengage) and break away. Although additional research with a larger sample size is warranted to strengthen the validity of these findings, our findings support the use of ATVV in cultivating preterm infant social interactive behaviors.
VLBW infants face multiple challenges, including the risk of long-term medical and neurodevelopmental problems, decreased cognitive abilities, attentional deficits, and internalizing behavioral difficulties. [41–43] Positive mother-infant interaction has the ability to mitigate the adverse effects of prematurity on at-risk infants. [44] Mother-infant synchrony, the ability of the parent and child to reciprocate a shared behavior and affect, provides a stable foundation for growth and development. [45] As an active player in a synchronous mother-infant interaction, the infant’s state and behavior elicit both positive and negative responses from the mother. Compared to term infants, premature infants are less responsive and clear in their cues, [46] which correlate with fewer maternal cognitive growth fostering behaviors. [47] Also, the premature infant’s immature autonomic system and decreased time spent in the alert state has been correlated with decreased maternal behavior. [48] Furthermore, the ability of the infant to self-regulate his/her behavior and the ability of infant and mother to synchronize their behavior influences infant attachment. [49] Interventions that foster synchronous mother-infant interactions have the ability to reduce the risk factors associated with prematurity, which lays the groundwork for future cognitive, social and emotional development. [50] Our findings show that the ATVV intervention elicits both positive and negative cues from the infant and our prior research shows that ATVV promotes the quiet alert state. [6] By facilitating the infant’s behavior self-regulation and promoting the alert state, the ATVV intervention has the potential to strengthen mother-infant social interaction and attachment and improve infant development.
Based on our findings KC elicited fewer engagement and disengagement cues during the intervention, potentially limiting the opportunity for mother-infant social engagement. Previous research has shown KC to improve infant self-regulation, [51] reduce infant negative emotions, increase maternal sensitive behaviors [52] and increase infant alertness while reducing gaze aversion. [50] However, these results were obtained weeks to months following the actual implementation of KC, whereas our findings examine the influence of KC on immediate infant behaviors. While previous research has documented the benefits of KC, this intervention might best be used to decrease infant irritability, and promote infant sleep and comfort. Further research is needed to better understand the differing effect of KC and ATVV on infant social behavior.
This research has several limitations including a small sample size, conduct of the research in the NICU as opposed to a more controlled environment, and coding by the primary coder who was aware of the purpose of the larger study. The small sample size limits generalizability of the findings, yet they provide new data on infant behavioral responses to the ATVV intervention and KC. All mothers performed their assigned interventions in a NICU, thus the environment was not strictly controlled and may have provided additional stimuli unrelated to the interventions. The NICU environment may have elicited behavioral cues from the infants that were not directly related to KC or the ATVV intervention. However, the NICU is the natural environment in which mothers perform these interventions. Thus, by studying them in the NICU, we sacrificed some control in order to gain ecological validity. Although 25% of the videos were coded by a second coder who was blind to the purpose of this pilot study and the larger primary study, a larger scale clinical trial should be conducted with coders that are blind to the purpose of the study. However, true blinding will never be possible since coders will inevitably see the mothers performing different interventions.
Despite these limitations, this study is the first to compare KC and the ATVV interventions in regard to emerging social interactive behaviors. Future prospective research with a larger sample is warranted that addresses the benefits of the two interventions on neurobehavioral organization. Such information can be used to enhance existing protocols for promoting healthy development of preterm infants. Furthermore, research examining the impact of the ATVV intervention on social interactive behaviors at 3 and 6 months could strengthen our understanding of its short-term effects on social engagement. The potential for the ATVV intervention as a clinical application to teach parents how to recognize and respond to their premature infant’s behavioral cues may lead to better support for the infants’ emerging social interactions and promote developmental outcomes.
Acknowledgments
The authors acknowledge Janet Levy and John Boling for statistical consultation, the mothers and infants who participated in this research, and the nursing and medical staff at Stroger County Medical Center, Mount Sinai Medical Center, Duke University Hospital, and Brenner Childrens’ Hospital. This research was funded by the National Institutes of Health, the National Institute of Nursing, 1 R01 NR009418 to the fourth author and the Harris Foundation to the first author.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Rosemary White-Traut, Dr. Rosemary White-Traut, University of Illinois at Chicago College of Nursing, Department Head of Women, Children, and Family Health Science, 845 South Damen Avenue (M/C 802), Chicago, IL 60612-7350.
Teresa Wink, Associate Dean for Research Affairs, Marcus E. Hobbs Distinguished Professor of Nursing.
Tali Minehart, Associate Dean for Research Affairs, Marcus E. Hobbs Distinguished Professor of Nursing.
Diane Holditch-Davis, Associate Dean for Research Affairs, Marcus E. Hobbs Distinguished Professor of Nursing.
References
- 1.Forcada-Guex M, Pierrehumbert B, Borghini A, Moessinger A, Muller-Nix C. Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):E107–E114. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.White-Traut RC, Nelson MN. Maternally administered tactile, auditory, visual, and vestibular stimulation: Relationship to later interactions between mothers and premature infants. Res Nurs Health. 1988;11(1):31–39. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770110106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ludington SM. Energy conservation during skin-to-skin contact between premature infants and their mothers. Heart Lung. 1990 Sep;19(5 Pt 1):445–451. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Scher MS, Ludington-Hoe S, Kaffashi F, Johnson MW, Holditch-Davis D, Loparo KA. Neurophysiologic assessment of brain maturation after an 8-week trial of skin-to-skin contact on preterm infants. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009 Oct;120(10):1812–1818. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Patel M, Vasan U, Han BK, et al. Developmental intervention for preterm infants diagnosed with periventricular leukomalacia. Res Nurs Health. 1999;22(2):131–143. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-240x(199904)22:2<131::aid-nur5>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Vasan U, Littau S, Meleedy-Rey P, et al. Effect of auditory, tactile, visual, and vestibular intervention on length of stay, alertness, and feeding progression in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44(2):91–97. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201001736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.White-Traut RC, Pate CM. Modulating infant state in premature infants. J Pediatr Nurs. 1987;2(2):96–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Als H. Neurobehavioral organization of the newborn: Opportunity for assessment and intervention. NIDA Res Monogr. 1991;114:106–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.White-Traut R, Norr K. An ecological model for premature infant feeding. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN / NAACOG. 2009;38(4):478–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2009.01046.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Messinger D, Fogel A. The interactive development of social smiling. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2007;35:327–366. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-009735-7.50014-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Emde RN, McCartney RD, Harmon RJ. Neonatal smiling in REM states, IV. Premature study. Child Dev. 1971 Nov;42(5):1657–1661. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Holditch-Davis D, Edwards LJ, Helms RW. Modeling development of sleep-wake behaviors: I. Using the mixed general linear model. Physiol Behav. 1998;63(3):311–318. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00459-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Barnard KE. What the NCAST feeding scale measures. In: Sumner G, Spietz A, editors. NCAST caregiver/Parent-child interaction feeding manual. Seattle, WA: NCAST Publications, University of Washington School of Nursing; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dieter JN, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Emory EK, Redzepi M. Stable preterm infants gain more weight and sleep less after five days of massage therapy. J Pediatr Psychol. 2003;28(6):403–411. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Diego M, Feijo L, Vera Y, Gil K. Massage therapy by parents improves early growth and development. Infant Behavior and Development. 2004;27(4):435–442. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kelmanson IA, Adulas EI. Massage therapy and sleep behaviour in infants born with low birth weight. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice. 2006;12(3):200–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2005.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Patel MK, Kilgallon D. Patterns of physiologic and behavioral response of intermediate care preterm infants to intervention. Pediatr Nurs. 1993;19(6):625–629. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Whitelaw A, Sleath K. Myth of the marsupial mother: Home care of very low birth weight babies in Bogota, Colombia. Lancet. 1985 May 25;1(8439):1206–1208. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(85)92877-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bohnhorst B, Heyne T, Peter CS, Poets CF. Skin-to-skin (kangaroo) care, respiratory control, and thermoregulation. J Pediatr. 2001 Feb;138(2):193–197. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2001.110978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bystrova K, Widstrom AM, Matthiesen AS, Ransjo-Arvidson AB, Welles-Nystrom B, Wassberg C, et al. Skin-to-skin contact may reduce negative consequences of "the stress of being born": A study on temperature in newborn infants, subjected to different ward routines in St. Petersburg. Acta Paediatr. 2003;92(3):320–326. doi: 10.1080/08035250310009248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Chwo MJ, Anderson GC, Good M, Dowling DA, Shiau SH, Chu DM. A randomized controlled trial of early kangaroo care for preterm infants: Effects on temperature, weight, behavior, and acuity. J Nurs Res. 2002 Jun;10(2):129–142. doi: 10.1097/01.jnr.0000347592.43768.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fohe K, Kropf S, Avenarius S. Skin-to-skin contact improves gas exchange in premature infants. J Perinatol. 2000 Jul-Aug;20(5):311–315. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ludington-Hoe SM, Nguyen N, Swinth JY, Satyshur RD. Kangaroo care compared to incubators in maintaining body warmth in preterm infants. Biological Research for Nursing. 2000 Jul;2(1):60–73. doi: 10.1177/109980040000200107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Charpak N, Ruiz-Pelaez JG, Figueroa de CZ, Charpak Y. A randomized, controlled trial of kangaroo mother care: Results of follow-up at 1 year of corrected age. Pediatrics. 2001 Nov;108(5):1072–1079. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.5.1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gathwala G, Singh B, Balhara B. KMC facilitates mother baby attachment in low birth weight infants. Indian J Pediatr. 2008 Jan;75(1):43–47. doi: 10.1007/s12098-008-0005-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Johnston CC, Stevens B, Pinelli J, Gibbins S, Filion F, Jack A, et al. Kangaroo care is effective in diminishing pain response in preterm neonates. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003 Nov;157(11):1084–1088. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.11.1084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kambarami RA, Chidede O, Pereira N. Long-term outcome of preterm infants discharged home on kangaroo care in a developing country. Ann Trop Paediatr. 2003 Mar;23(1):55–59. doi: 10.1179/000349803125002931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rojas MA, Kaplan M, Quevedo M, Sherwonit E, Foster LB, Ehrenkranz RA, et al. Somatic growth of preterm infants during skin-to-skin care versus traditional holding: A randomized, controlled trial. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003 Jun;24(3):163–168. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200306000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tessier R, Cristo MB, Velez S, Giron M, Nadeau L, Figueroa de Calume Z, et al. Kangaroo Mother Care: A method for protecting high-risk low-birth-weight and premature infants against developmental delay. Infant Behavior and Development. 2003;26(3):384–397. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Warnock FF, Castral TC, Brant R, Sekilian M, Leite AM, Owens Sde L, et al. Brief report: Maternal kangaroo care for neonatal pain relief: A systematic narrative review. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010 Oct;35(9):975–984. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ludington-Hoe SM, Anderson GC, Simpson S, Hollingsead A, Argote LA, Rey H. Birth-related fatigue in 34–36-week preterm neonates: Rapid recovery with very early kangaroo (skin-to-skin) care. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 1999 Jan-Feb;28(1):94–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.1999.tb01970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Messmer PR, Rodriguez S, Adams J, Wells-Gentry J, Washburn K, Zabaleta I, et al. Effect of kangaroo care on sleep time for neonates. Pediatr Nurs. 1997 Jul-Aug;23(4):408–414. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Velandia M, Matthisen AS, Uvnas-Moberg K, Nissen E. Onset of vocal interaction between parents and newborns in skin-to-skin contact immediately after elective cesarean section. Birth. 2010 Sep;37(3):192–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-536X.2010.00406.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Constantinou JC, Adamson-Macedo EN, Stevenson DK, Mirmiran M, Fleisher BE. Effects of skin-to-skin holding on general movements of preterm infants. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 1999 Aug;38(8):467–471. doi: 10.1177/000992289903800805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Furman L, Minich N, Hack M. Correlates of lactation in mothers of very low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 2002 Apr;109(4):e57. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.4.e57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hake-Brooks SJ, Anderson GC. Kangaroo care and breastfeeding of mother-preterm infant dyads 0–18 months: A randomized, controlled trial. Neonatal Netw. 2008 May-Jun;27(3):151–159. doi: 10.1891/0730-0832.27.3.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ballard JL, Khoury JC, Wedig K, Wang L, Eilers-Walsman BL, Lipp R. New Ballard Score, expanded to include extremely premature infants. J Pediatr. [Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S.] 1991 Sep;119(3):417–423. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(05)82056-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Burns K, Cunningham N, White-Traut R, Silvestri J, Nelson MN. Infant stimulation: Modification of an intervention based on physiologic and behavioral cues. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 1994 Sep;23(7):581–589. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.1994.tb01924.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977 Mar;33(1):159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979 Mar;86(2):420–428. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009 Aug;124(2):717–728. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A meta-analysis. JAMA. 2002;288(6):728–737. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Eichenwald EC, Stark AR. Management and outcomes of very low birth weight. N Engl J Med. 2008 Apr 17;358(16):1700–1711. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0707601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Neonatal state organization, neuromaturation, mother-infant interaction, and cognitive development in small-for-gestational-age premature infants. Pediatrics. 2006;118(3):e869–e878. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Treyvaud K, Anderson VA, Howard K, Bear M, Hunt RW, Doyle LW, et al. Parenting behavior is associated with the early neurobehavioral development of very preterm children. Pediatrics. 2009 Feb;123(2):555–561. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.McGrath MM, Sullivan MC, Seifer R. Maternal interaction patterns and preschool competence in high-risk children. Nurs Res. 1998 Nov-Dec;47(6):309–317. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199811000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Singer LT, Fulton S, Davillier M, Koshy D, Salvator A, Baley JE. Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003 Aug;24(4):233–241. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200308000-00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Dev Psychobiol. 2007 Apr;49(3):290–302. doi: 10.1002/dev.20220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Beebe B, Jaffe J, Markese S, Buck K, Chen H, Cohen P, et al. The origins of 12-month attachment: A microanalysis of 4-month mother-infant interaction. Attach Hum Dev. 2010 Jan;12(1–2):3–141. doi: 10.1080/14616730903338985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Feldman R, Eidelman AI, Sirota L, Weller A. Comparison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: Parenting outcomes and preterm infant development. Pediatrics. 2002 Jul;110(1 Pt 1):16–26. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Neu M, Robinson J. Maternal holding of preterm infants during the early weeks after birth and dyad interaction at six months. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2010 Jul;39(4):401–414. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2010.01152.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003 Apr;45(4):274–281. doi: 10.1017/s0012162203000525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]